Abstract
Recent experimental observations and theoretical advances have indicated that the homeostatic equilibrium for sleep/wake regulation—and thereby sensitivity to neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss—is modulated by prior sleep/wake history. This phenomenon was predicted by a biomathematical model developed to explain changes in neurobehavioral performance across days in laboratory studies of total sleep deprivation and sustained sleep restriction. The present paper focuses on the dynamics of neurobehavioral performance within days in this biomathematical model of fatigue. Without increasing the number of model parameters, the model was updated by incorporating time-dependence in the amplitude of the circadian modulation of performance. The updated model was calibrated using a large dataset from three laboratory experiments on psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) performance, under conditions of sleep loss and circadian misalignment; and validated using another large dataset from three different laboratory experiments. The time-dependence of circadian amplitude resulted in improved goodness-of-fit in night shift schedules, nap sleep scenarios, and recovery from prior sleep loss. The updated model predicts that the homeostatic equilibrium for sleep/wake regulation—and thus sensitivity to sleep loss—depends not only on the duration but also on the circadian timing of prior sleep. This novel theoretical insight has important implications for predicting operator alertness during work schedules involving circadian misalignment such as night shift work.
Citation:
McCauley P; Kalachev LV; Mollicone DJ; Banks S; Dinges DF; Van Dongen HPA. Dynamic circadian modulation in a biomathematical model for the effects of sleep and sleep loss on waking neurobehavioral performance. SLEEP 2013;36(12):1987-1997.
Keywords: Fatigue and performance models, Sleep deprivation, sleep homeostasis, allostatic regulation, dynamic circadian amplitude, alertness, Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC), sigmoidal dynamics, nonlinear interaction, model-based fatigue risk management
INTRODUCTION
Building on the seminal two-process model of sleep regulation,1,2 biomathematical models for the prediction of fatigue, behavioral alertness and performance during periods of sleep deprivation and/or circadian misalignment instantiate a homeostatic process seeking to balance time asleep with time awake, and a circadian process seeking to place wakefulness during the day and sleep during the night.3 Such fatigue models have become useful tools in modern fatigue risk management systems.4 To capture the temporal dynamics of waking neurobehavioral performance across a wide range of sleep/wake schedules, we recently introduced a new class of fatigue models, formulated in terms of coupled first-order ordinary differential equations (ODEs).5 A specific implementation of the new model class, using scheduled time in bed (TIB) as input, was calibrated and validated5 with large datasets of neurobehavioral performance from laboratory-based dose-response studies of sleep loss.6,7
The dynamics of this new fatigue model5 indicated that prior sleep history modulates the homeostatic equilibrium state for sleep/wake regulation,8,9 and therefore the magnitude of the effect of subsequent sleep restriction on neurobehavioral performance.10 This prediction has since been corroborated experimentally.11,12 The dependence on sleep history of the effects of sleep loss implies that sleep homeostatic effects on performance show integration of both chronic and acute sleep loss over time and should be modeled with (at least) two state variables. In our model,5 the state variables are p representing performance impairment over time (hours to days), and u modulating the sleep homeostatic equilibrium over time (days to weeks). The interplay of these two state variables in the model exhibits a bifurcation: for daily scheduled amounts of wakefulness less than a critical threshold estimated to be 20.2 h (i.e., more than 3.8 h TIB per day), performance deficits are predicted to converge across days to an asymptotically stable state of equilibrium; whereas for daily wakefulness extended beyond this critical threshold, performance deficits are predicted to escalate through divergence from an unstable state of equilibrium.5 This provides a unified explanation for the experimentally observed, differential dynamics of neurobehavioral performance under conditions of total sleep deprivation versus sustained nocturnal sleep restriction.7,13
The general mathematical framework of our published model5 focused primarily on the temporal dynamics between sleep/wake cycles (i.e., across days). The dynamics of waking neurobehavioral performance within sleep/wake cycles are driven by an emergent property involving nonlinear interaction between the homeostatic process described by the homogeneous part of the differential equations and the circadian process encompassed in the non-homogeneous part.14–16 It was shown that as long as the non-homogeneous part contains only oscillatory functions such as circadian rhythm, the dynamics across sleep/wake cycles do not depend on it. Recognizing this property, a detailed examination of the non-homogeneous part was left to be addressed in follow-up research.5
Inspired by the significant contributions of other groups to fatigue modeling,2,17–23 we present the results of this follow-up research. Dropping the constraint for the non-homogeneous part to contain only oscillatory functions, we generalized the applicability of our model to also include night shift schedules and nap sleep scenarios. This was accomplished without increasing the net number of model parameters, by introducing time-dependence in the amplitude of the circadian modulation of neurobehavioral performance.
METHODS
Model Reformulation
The ODE system modeling neurobehavioral performance during wakefulness and (notionally) during sleep is given by Eqs. (1) in Table 1 (cf. Eqs. (9), (21) and (26) in ref. 5). These equations describe the two state variables p and u. The first state variable, p, represents the primary outcome variable, which denotes predicted performance impairment. This variable rises in a saturating exponential manner towards an upper asymptote during wakefulness and falls exponentially towards a lower asymptote during sleep, while being modulated continuously by circadian rhythm. The second state variable, u, represents a slow dynamic process that causes allostatic modulation of p over time (days to weeks). Specifically, during wakefulness u produces a slow exponential increase of the asymptote towards which p rises, and during sleep u produces a slow exponential decrease of the asymptote towards which p falls.
Table 1.
In Eqs. (1) in Table 1, the parameters α and σ in the homogeneous part of the ODE system govern the homeostatic changes in p and u and their interaction during wakefulness and sleep, respectively. The function c is a skewed sinusoidal (i.e., 5-harmonic oscillatory) function representing the circadian process of the two-process model.24 In the model formulation of Eqs. (1), c is scaled by κ, offset by μ, and shifted in time by φ (see footnote of Table 1). Also, note that u is offset by a constant δ (not explicitly shown) during wakefulness compared to sleep5 to distinguish the upper and lower asymptotes to which p rises and falls, resulting in a discontinuity in the model at each sleep/wake and wake/ sleep transition.
Without changing any aspect of the model, Eqs. (1) can be written as a coupled non-homogeneous first-order ODE system for p (see supplemental material, section 1), which is given in Eqs. (2) and (3) in Table 1 (where the subscripts “w” and “s” stand for wakefulness and sleep, respectively). The parameters in this model formulation are described in Table 2. The functions gw and gs are bounded, and are described in more detail in the next section. In the formulation of Eqs. (2) and (3), both p and u are continuous over time across wake/sleep and sleep/ wake transitions, removing the discontinuity embedded in Eqs. (1). Note that Eqs. (2) in Table 1 can be reduced to the original homeostatic process of the two-process model24 by setting αw = −1/τr, αs = −1/τd, βw = 0, βs = 0, gw = −αw, and gs = 0.
Table 2.
As briefly discussed above, the model exhibits a bifurcation when comparing daily scheduled wakefulness greater versus smaller than a critical threshold. This critical threshold is given by Ac = Wc / T = ηs / (ηs – ηw). The published value for Wc is 20.2 h for a day length T of 24 h,5 such that Ac = 0.84. Postulating that this bifurcation is a generalizable neurobiological property with constant value of Ac, it follows that ηs can be derived from ηw through Eq. (4) in Table 1. This reduces the number of free parameters by one without changing the model.
Improved Circadian Dynamics
The functions gw and gs in Eqs. (2) in Table 1 are the main focus of this paper. They represent the non-homogeneous part of the ODE system, which includes circadian rhythm. Recent laboratory studies, in particular from simulated shift work schedules,25 suggested that improvement was needed in the dynamics of the non-homogeneous part to better track circadian influences on performance.
In Eqs. (5) in Table 1 we introduce minor, yet important, changes in the mathematical form for the non-homogeneous part. Here the circadian process is represented by the function c(t) given in Eq. (6), with τ = 24 h representing the circadian period and time being denoted by t (expressed in hours). By including the term αs βs / ηs, Eq. (5b) is formulated such that performance impairment p in Eq. (2b) tends to zero for increasing sleep duration (see supplemental material, section 2). This constrains the model to converge to zero PVT lapses under the hypothetical condition of infinite sleep duration, as should theoretically be the case, and helps to improve the estimation of initial values for studies with different amounts of baseline sleep.
To capture findings that sleep/wake state moderates the electrophysiological activity level of the circadian pacemaker26 and that the circadian modulation of neurobehavioral performance is small in well-rested individuals and larger in sleep-deprived individuals,16,27 the time-dependent function κ in Eqs. (5) modulates the amplitude of the circadian process as a function of sleep/wake state. The function is specified in Eqs. (7) in Table 1, and produces a sigmoidal rise in circadian amplitude during wakefulness and an exponential fall during sleep. In future work, this dynamic behavior may be further refined to include a limit-cycle oscillator, in agreement with published biomathematical models of the human circadian pacemaker,28 but this is not needed for the present purposes.
Datasets
Using data from previously published studies, we compiled two large datasets. The first, comprised of studies A1-A3, was used for estimation of the model parameters (i.e., for model calibration). The second, comprised of studies B1-B3, was used for model validation.
Study A1
47 healthy young adults were subjected to 1 of 4 different laboratory sleep deprivation protocols.7 Each protocol began with several baseline days involving 8 h TIB. Subsequently, 13 subjects were kept awake for 3 days continuously (i.e., 0 h TIB), for a total of 88 h of total sleep deprivation. Afterwards, they received varied amounts of recovery sleep (not considered here). The other 34 subjects underwent 1 of 3 doses of sustained sleep restriction for 14 consecutive days, followed by 2 recovery days with 8 h TIB. The sleep restriction schedule involved 4 h TIB per day for 13 subjects; 6 h TIB per day for another 13 subjects; and 8 h TIB per day for the remaining 8 subjects (control condition). Awakening was scheduled at 07:30 for every sleep period.
Performance on a 10-minute psychomotor vigilance test (PVT)29,30 was tested every 2 h, starting at 07:30, during scheduled waking periods. The first 2 test bouts of each waking period were omitted in order to avoid confounds from sleep inertia.31
Study A2
27 healthy young adults participated in a laboratory study of simulated shift work.25 Thirteen subjects were randomized to a night shift schedule, which began with a baseline day with 10 h TIB (22:00-08:00) and a subsequent prophylactic nap of 5 h TIB (15:00-20:00). Subjects were then on a night shift schedule for 5 days, with diurnal sleep of 10 h TIB (10:00-20:00) after the first 4 shifts and a transition nap of 5 h TIB (10:00-15:00) after the last. The transition nap commenced a 34 h “restart” or recycling break which also included a nocturnal sleep period of 10 h TIB (22:00-08:00) and a prophylactic nap of 5 h TIB (15:00-20:00). Subjects were then on a night shift schedule again for 5 days, which was identical to the first 5 days on night shift and ended with a transition nap of 5 h TIB (10:00-15:00). They subsequently had a final recovery sleep period of 10 h TIB (22:00-08:00).
The other 14 subjects in this study were randomized to a day shift schedule (control condition), which was equivalent to the night shift schedule but involved nocturnal sleep with 10 h TIB (22:00-08:00) every day and no napping. The second day in the laboratory was considered a rest day, not a shift day. Cumulative TIB during the study was identical between the 2 conditions.
In both conditions, performance on the 10-min PVT was tested before and after a 30-min session on a driving simulator occurring every 3 h during the baseline day (practice) and during each of the shift days. Only the pre-driving PVT bouts on the shift days are considered here—4 bouts daily starting at 21:00 or 22:00 (randomized over subjects) in the night shift condition, and at 09:00 or 10:00 (randomized over subjects) in the day shift condition. PVT performance was also tested once every 3 h during the first 5 h after baseline sleep (at 09:30 and 12:30), during scheduled wakefulness in the “restart” break (at 09:30, 12:30, 16:30, and 19:30), and after the final recovery sleep period (at 09:30 and 12:30). Sleep inertia was not an issue, as there was no significant sleep loss25 and no test bout occurred close to awakening.
Study A3
90 healthy young adults were subjected to one of 18 different laboratory sleep restriction protocols.32 Each protocol began with 2 baseline days involving 8.2 h TIB (21:54-06:06). Subjects were then randomly assigned to one of 18 sleep restriction conditions, each involving a specific sleep regimen that was maintained for 10 consecutive days, the first 8 of which had sufficiently complete data to be considered here. The sleep regimen involved one of four nocturnal sleep durations: 8.2 h TIB (control condition), 6.2 h TIB, 5.2 h TIB, or 4.2 h TIB (all centered around 02:00), of which the 3 shorter ones were followed by one of 7 diurnal nap durations: 2.4, 2.0, 1.6, 1.2, 0.8, or 0.4 h TIB (all centered around 14:00) or 0.0 h TIB (i.e., no nap). The 2.4-h nap duration was paired only with the 4.2-h nocturnal sleep duration; the 2.0-h and 1.6-h nap durations were paired only with the 5.2-h and 4.2-h nocturnal sleep durations. Each of the 18 conditions had 5 subjects, which sufficed for the study's original objective to investigate dose-response relationships across different sleep regimens.
Performance on the 10-min PVT was tested every 2 h during scheduled wakefulness, starting at 04:10, 06:10, or 08:10, depending on the study condition. Focusing on test bouts that the conditions had in common and that were not potentially confounded by sleep inertia, only tests administered at 08:10, 10:10, 12:10, 16:10, 18:10, and 20:10 were considered.
Study B1
Twelve healthy young adults participated in a laboratory study of total sleep deprivation.33 The study involved 2 baseline days each with 10 h TIB (22:00-08:00), then 62 h of sustained wakefulness, and then 2 recovery days each with 10 h TIB (22:00-08:00).
Performance on the 10-min PVT was tested every 2 h, starting at 09:30, throughout most of scheduled wakefulness. The first test bout of each waking period was omitted in order to avoid confounds from sleep inertia. Note that the presence of recovery days distinguishes the data of this study from the data of the total sleep deprivation condition in study A1.
Study B2
Fifteen healthy young adults were subjected to a laboratory study condition identical to the 88 h sustained wakefulness condition in study A1, except they were given a nap of 2 h TIB every 12 h during the 88 h period.34 Nap start times alternated between 02:45 and 14:45.
Performance on the 10-min PVT was tested every 2 h, starting at 07:30, during scheduled wakefulness. The first 2 test bouts after each awakening were omitted in order to avoid confounds from sleep inertia.
Study B3
As part of a larger study, 142 healthy young adults were involved in a laboratory sleep restriction protocol similar to the 4 h TIB condition of study A1.35 The study began with 2 baseline days each with 10 h TIB (22:00-08:00). Subsequently, subjects were given 4 h TIB (04:00-08:00) each day for 5 consecutive days. They were then randomized to one of 6 doses of sleep, all ending at 08:00 the next day: 0 h TIB (13 subjects), 2 h TIB (27 subjects), 4 h TIB (29 subjects), 6 h TIB (25 subjects), 8 h TIB (21 subjects), or 10 h TIB (27 subjects).
Performance on the 10-min PVT was tested every 2 h, starting at 08:00, during scheduled wakefulness. Data points through to 20:00 on the day after the night with the randomized sleep dose were considered. The first 2 test bouts after each awakening were omitted in order to avoid confounds from sleep inertia.
Model Evaluation
For each PVT test bout of every subject in studies A1-A3 and B1-B3, the number of lapses (reaction times > 500 ms)36 was assessed. Day 0 was defined as the study day leading into the night when sleep loss first began, or the equivalent day for control conditions without sleep loss. Starting with day 0, the number of lapses was averaged over subjects by time point, separately for each condition in each study. The number of data points was 427 in study A1 (4 conditions), 96 in study A2 (2 conditions), 972 in study A3 (18 conditions), 31 in study B1 (1 condition), 28 in study B2 (1 condition), and 317 in study B3 (6 conditions).
Model parameter estimates and their standard errors (indicating the magnitude of the uncertainty in the parameter estimates) were obtained using the data from studies A1-A3 and applying state-of-the-art Metropolis-Hastings-type Markov chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) fitting37,38 (with a chain length of 10,000). We posited that βw = βs and λw = −λs, leaving 9 free parameters to be estimated (see Table 2). For determining the initial values, we assumed steady state for the sleep/wake schedule of the baseline days in each study (see supplemental material, section 3).
For studies A1-A3 (calibration) and B1-B3 (validation), model goodness-of-fit was quantified using root mean square errors (RMSE) and explained variance. Statistical comparisons with our earlier model version5 were made using Akaike's Information Criterion (AIC).
RESULTS
Table 2 and Figures 1 and 2 show the results of parameter estimation for the model, Eqs. (2)-(7), based on the data from studies A1-A3. Judging by the standard errors (Table 2), all parameters are well defined by the data. This is largely corroborated by their pairwise correlations (see supplemental material, section 4).
The estimated values for αw and αs are comparable to the corresponding parameters in the two-process model of sleep regulation,24 −τr-1 = −1 / 18.2 = −0.055 and −τd-1 = −1 / 4.2 = −0.24. The estimate for the parameter ηw, which does not exist in the two-process model, is nearly identical to the equivalent parameter in our earlier model version,5 α22 = 0.00743. The estimate for φ is also nearly identical to its equivalent in the earlier model,5 namely θ + t0 = 12.7 + 8.6 = 21.3 (with θ = 12.7 taken from ref. 5 and t0 = 8.6 defined in ref. 24). The other parameters in Table 2 are not directly comparable to either the two-process model parameters or the parameters in our earlier model version. Note that the new model version presented in this paper has only 9 free parameters (see Table 2), whereas the earlier model version had 10.
Figure 1 shows the data of the 4 conditions of study A1 (top panels), which had also been used for the calibration of our earlier model version,5 and the predictions of the new version of the model (bottom panels). The earlier model captured as much as 73.2% of the variance (RMSE: 3.06), leaving little room for improvement (see supplemental material, section 5). The new model, calibrated to fit the collective data of studies A1-A3, still captures almost the same amount of variance in study A1, namely 71.6% (RMSE: 3.15). Goodness-of-fit for sustained sleep restriction and recovery days, which was the main scientific advance achieved with the earlier model, is preserved in the new model (Figure 1, right panels).
Figure 2 shows the data and the predictions of the new model for the two conditions of study A2, which compares a night shift scenario (left panel) to a day shift scenario (right panel). The observed performance impairment in this study was remarkably low even in the night shift condition.25 Nonetheless, although the model slightly overpredicts the magnitude of impairment, it captures the relative dynamics in the 2 conditions well. The dynamics are also well captured in the nap sleep conditions of study A3 (not shown in a figure because there were 18 different conditions; but see supplemental material, section 6).
For study A2, the new model represents a notable improvement over our earlier model version (see supplement material, section 5). The earlier model would have captured only 15.5% of the variance in study A2, but the new model captures 81.9% of the variance. For study A3, the new model likewise represents an improvement. The earlier model would have mispredicted the data and captured none of the variance in study A3, but the new model captures 34.9% of the variance. Indeed, considering studies A1-A3 together, the new model outperforms our earlier model version significantly. Whereas the earlier model would have captured 34.2% of the overall variance (RMSE: 4.08) in dataset A, the new model captures 60.0% of the variance (RMSE: 3.18, smaller is better).
It should be noted that besides model goodness-of-fit, variance explained and RMSE also reflect the magnitude of the variance in the experimental data (as affected, e.g., by the range of sleep loss and the circadian variability) and the signal-to-noise ratio. Even so, the improvement of the new model over the earlier model is confirmed by the AIC values, which are 4222.0 for the earlier model versus 3477.8 for the new model (smaller is better). For comparison, a null model predicting a straight line at the grand mean (predicting 0% of the variance) would have an AIC of 4830.3. AIC values cannot be interpreted in an absolute sense, but the relative improvement achieved with the new model version is substantial by any standard.
Figures 3 and 4 show the results of validation of the new model using the data from studies B1-B3. In study B1 (Figure 3, left panel), there is slight underprediction of the highest levels of impairment during total sleep deprivation (as expected based on metric nonlinearity39), and slight overprediction of residual impairment following recovery. Peak impairment in the first 2 days of study B2 is also overpredicted (Figure 3, middle panel), which could potentially be overcome by dropping the assumption that λw = −λs (but more data would be needed to support the ensuing increase in the number of parameters). Regardless, the model does capture the relative dynamics within and between studies. Furthermore, the model predicts the recovery sleep dose-response curve for study B3 particularly well (Figure 4).
Overall, the model performs well on the validation dataset, relatively closely matching the data of all 3 studies B1-B3. The model captures 75.1% of the variance in this dataset, although that number should be interpreted carefully—the model parameters are not re-estimated to fit the validation data and explained variance is then not a reliable statistic.40 The excellent goodness-of-fit is confirmed by the RMSE though, which is 2.29 and near theoretical lower bounds derived in earlier fatigue model validation efforts.40
DISCUSSION
Here we refined our previously published model5 on the temporal dynamics of neurobehavioral performance under conditions of sleep loss and circadian misalignment. A key feature retained from the earlier model version is the instantiation of a slow process (in the order of days to weeks) that modulates the waking neurobehavioral expression of sleep/wake homeostasis as a function of the prevailing sleep/wake ratio,5 with prior sleep insufficiency increasing the sensitivity to neurobehavioral impairment during subsequent sleep deprivation. The dynamics of the model indicate that prior sleep history modulates the homeostatic equilibrium state for sleep/wake regulation allostatically.8,9 This implies that “optimal” neurobehavioral functioning is a moving target depending on prior sleep history6 as has been confirmed experimentally.11,12,41,42 Such dynamics could potentially explain the discrepancy of temporal profiles in sustained sleep restriction scenarios7 between objective measurements of neurobehavioral performance and self-reports of subjective sleepiness. That is, the former are typically expressed in terms of absolute impairment and therefore display the effects of both short-term homeostatic and longer-term allostatic regulation, whereas the latter seem to be dominated by acute change and may primarily reflect short-term homeostatic regulation in response to deviations from the homeostatic equilibrium state.
The main refinement introduced in the present paper concerns the circadian modulation of neurobehavioral performance, for which we improved the dynamics and the prediction accuracy without increasing the total number of model parameters. To better capture the nonlinear interaction between circadian and homeostatic influences on neurobehavioral performance,14,15 where the circadian modulation of neurobehavioral performance is small in well-rested individuals and larger in sleep-deprived individuals,16,43 we implemented a sigmoidal rise in circadian amplitude during wakefulness and an exponential decline in circadian amplitude during sleep. The introduction of sigmoidal time-dependence for the circadian amplitude during wakefulness generalizes the scope of the model to include night shift schedules and nap sleep scenarios. Moreover, through the nonlinear interaction between circadian and homeostatic influences on neurobehavioral performance, the new model also produces good predictions for the neurobehavioral effects of recovery sleep following sustained sleep restriction (Figure 4).
The time it takes for the circadian amplitude to rise to and saturate near its asymptotic maximum during wakefulness, when initially well-rested, is estimated to be about 20 h (as derived by entering the estimate for λw in Table 2 into Eq. (7a) in Table 1). Sigmoidal dynamics for waking neurobehavioral functioning have been proposed previously,44 with similar estimated saturation time, but were presumed to be associated principally with a decline in homeostatic influence on waking neurobehavioral performance.
Goodness-of-fit statistics for both the calibration dataset and the independent validation dataset indicate that prediction accuracy across the extended scope of the new model is high. The model is grounded in mathematical characterization of the emergent dynamic behavior5 and in experimental studies and theory of the regulatory neurobiology. The recently proposed ATP-cytokine-adenosine hypothesis45,46—the first theoretical advance to offer a neurobiological explanation for the cumulative deficits associated with sustained sleep restriction—has suggested specific connections between the time-dependent variables of our model and the neurobiology of sleep and wakefulness. These hypothesized connections are shown in Figure 5 (black ovals), placed in the framework of a broader theory proposing that sleep is fundamentally use-dependent and local to cortical columns and other neuronal assemblies.47–49 Within that framework, we posit that p(t) corresponds to extra-cellular adenosine production rate in neuronal assemblies critically involved in task performance,49 which may be proportional to the probability of local sleep and thereby underlie performance variability and impairment.50 We posit that u(t) reflects the regulation of post-synaptic adenosine receptor density,48 changes in which may embody allostatic control of the homeostatic equilibrium for sleep/wake regulation.9 We posit that c(t) represents global modulation of basal synaptic activity,51 which may be driven by peripheral oscillators in the brain52 orchestrated by the circadian pacemaker in the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN).53 Lastly, we posit that κ(t) captures the influence of sleep homeostasis on SCN activity,26 as may be induced by sleep regulatory substances and subcortical sleep regulatory circuits49 (see Figure 5).
As the underlying neurobiology is increasingly well understood, confidence grows that predictions of our model generalize to sleep/wake/work scenarios not already included in the calibration and validation datasets.54 Some specific predictions regarding the dynamics of neurobehavioral performance, in scenarios for which carefully controlled studies have yet to be published, are worth discussing. Consider, for example, the dynamics of neurobehavioral performance impairment across days of sustained sleep restriction when the restricted sleep is placed during the day. It could be expected that the build-up of neurobehavioral deficits across days of sleep restriction is accelerated under such conditions of circadian misalignment. However, that is not what the model predicts—see Figure 6. Although neurobehavioral performance is worse overall when sleep is placed during the day and wakefulness during the night, the predicted rate for the build-up of impairment is essentially the same regardless of the circadian placement of the restricted sleep. Preliminary analyses of laboratory data addressing this issue appear to tentatively confirm this.55
The situation seems more complex following a “restart” (recycling) break with nocturnal recovery sleep—see Figure 7. Initially, the nocturnal recovery sleep is predicted to restore degraded performance more when prior restricted sleep is placed during the day (red trajectory) than during the night (blue trajectory), to about the same absolute level of performance. The relatively greater restorative benefit of nocturnal recovery sleep following restricted diurnal sleep appears to be short-lived, though—after a few days, predicted neurobehavioral performance is again worse overall when sleep is placed during the day, and with the same predicted rate for the build-up of impairment. In contrast, nocturnal recovery sleep immediately following a short period of acute sleep loss (with only a brief nap; black trajectory) is predicted to have a relatively large and more enduring restorative effect. The trajectories in the figure suggest that sustained circadian misalignment, similar to sustained sleep restriction, allostatically modulates the homeostatic equilibrium for sleep/wake regulation.
The present version of our model may be suitable for use in model-based fatigue risk management,4 comparable to if not more precise than other fatigue and performance models56 currently in use for that purpose. Our model is distinct from other models in this context in that it uses time in bed (TIB) as a basis for predicting waking neurobehavioral function, and does not require information about or estimation of actual physiological sleep obtained. That is, homeostatic, circadian, and allostatic effects on sleep efficiency are implicitly accounted for in the predictions for neurobehavioral performance through the parameter estimates of the model. It should be noted that external factors that could markedly reduce sleep efficiency (such as environmental noise) cannot be accounted for this way unless actual sleep obtained is measured (e.g., through actigraphy). A reparameterization of the model to use measured sleep times as a basis for prediction is possible, and will be considered in the future.
Even though our model predicts performance impairment quantitatively, model predictions should generally not be interpreted as absolute measures of impairment, especially when used in sleep/wake scenario evaluation and model-based fatigue risk management.4 One reason is that there is increasing recognition of quantitative and qualitative differences in the effects of sleep loss across different tasks.57,58 This issue can be partially addressed by building on a primary strength of fatigue and performance models, which is their focus on the temporal dynamics of neurobehavioral impairment. Model predictions can accordingly be used to compare the relative impairment associated with different sleep/wake schedules (see footnote following this section)—and select a schedule that is comparatively favorable with respect to fatigue and other operationally relevant criteria. Following the recent promulgation by the U.S. Federal Aviation Administration of a new rule on duty and rest requirements for flight crew members in commercial aviation (14 Code of Federal Regulations Parts 117, 119, and 121), which contains a provision for requests for exceptions from duty hour restrictions based on safety-relevant evidence including predictions from fatigue and performance models, the stakes are high to further develop model-based fatigue risk management methodology along these lines.4
While the work presented here constitutes a considerable expansion of the scope of our model, further improvements will be pursued. Data from additional studies on the effectiveness of recovery sleep that may become available in the future will be used to re-estimate the parameters of the model. In doing so, the correlation among some of the model parameters (see supplemental material, section 4) will likely be significantly reduced. Prediction of the effects of sleep inertia on neurobehavioral performance will be included in the model pending the better development of a theoretical basis for the phenomenon. Finally, there is a need for additional improvements to the circadian component of the model, particularly with regard to phase adjustments after circadian desynchronization. Although informative data on this issue relevant to real-world scenarios are scarce, it is a priority for our next model development efforts. Until then, the model in its present form should not be applied to scenarios involving transmeridian travel or otherwise systematically shifted circadian rhythmicity.
FOOTNOTE
Schedule-based comparisons of predicted fatigue by time point and by time spent above some threshold level4 are nominally invariant to what measure of fatigue is considered, provided a monotonic relationship between different fatigue measures may be assumed. Caution is warranted if schedule-based comparisons rely on temporal integration of predicted fatigue67 (e.g., average or area under the curve), because outcomes may then vary depending on the fatigue measures considered if relationships between them are nonlinear.
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
This was not an industry supported study. This work was supported by Air Force Office of Scientific Research grant FA9550-09-1-0136 (to Dr. Van Dongen); and in part by Office of Naval Research contracts N00014-10-C-0392 and N00014-11-C-0592 (to Dr. Mollicone), Naval Medical Logistics Command contract N62645-12-C-4004 (to Dr. Mollicone), Naval Warfare Development Command contract N65236-09-D-3809 (to Dr. Mollicone), National Institutes of Health grant NR04281 (to Dr. Dinges), Air Force Office of Scientific Research grant F49620-95-1-0388 (to Dr. Dinges), Federal Motor Carrier Safety Administration award DTMC75-07-D-00006 (to Dr. Van Dongen), Congressionally Directed Medical Research Program award W81XWH-05-1-0099 (Washington State University, Spokane, WA), and the National Space Biomedical Research Institute through NASA NCC 9-58 (to Dr. Dinges). This work was performed at Washington State University, the University of Montana, and the University of Pennsylvania. The paper does not address any off-label or investigational use.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors thank Mark McCauley for help with the mathematics and computer programming, and an anonymous reviewer for helpful suggestions for improving the manuscript.
SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL
1. Invariant reformulation of Eqs. (1) as a system of coupled non-homogeneous first-order ODEs
Reintroducing previously used notation where p and u are replaced by q and v and where wake/sleep cycles are indexed by n, we start with Eqs. (1) in the following form1:
where tn denotes the beginning of scheduled wakefulness in the nth wake/sleep cycle, Wn is the duration of scheduled wakefulness in the nth cycle, Tn is the total duration of the nth cycle (such that tn+1 = tn + Tn), and γ = 0. Eqs. (S1) are linked within and across days by:
The link between un(t) and vn(t) in Eqs. (S2) can be made continuous by setting γ = −σ22 δ in Eqs. (S1) and dropping δ from Eqs. (S2). However, the equation for dqn(t)/dt then has to be adjusted as follows:
Normalizing un(t) and vn(t) through division by −σ22 δ and making a number of trivial parameter substitutions puts the model in the following form:
which is equivalent to the form given in Eqs. (2) and (3).
2. Proof that performance impairment in Eq. (2b) tends to zero for increasing sleep duration
The solutions for Eqs. (7b) and (3b) with arbitrary initial conditions have the form:
respectively. Substituting Eqs. (S5) and (S6) into Eq. (2b), and taking into account Eqs. (5b) and (6), we arrive at:
or
where the non-homogeneous term is a linear combination of the functions eηst, eλst, and eλst .
The solution of Eq. (S8) with arbitrary initial condition has the form:
where F(t) is a particular solution of the non-homogeneous Eq. (S8) which, under conditions αs ≠ βs and αs ≠ λs, is also a linear combination of eηst, eλst, and eλst . If αs = βs and/or αs = λs, then F(t) also contains terms proportional to teηst and/or teλst. Since αs < 0, βs < 0 and λs < 0, each term on the right hand side of Eq. (S9) tends to zero as t → ∞, and thus limt → ∞ p(t) = 0. That is, p(t) tends to zero for increasing time asleep.
3. Initial values for the ODE system of Eqs. (2), (3) and (7)
For a wake/sleep schedule of constant wake duration W and constant day length T, where T = τ (i.e., circadian entrainment), we assume steady state (of a periodic map with period T). The initial values for p(t) and u(t) in Eqs. (2a) and (3a) at the onset of scheduled wakefulness t0 are then given by1:
where Ф(t) = φ(t)φ-1 (t0 + W) and Ψ(t) = ψ(t)ψ-1(t0) with
and
The corresponding initial value for κ(t) in Eq. (7a) is given by:
Note that under the steady state (of the periodic map), the time-dependent trajectory for κ(t) is bounded and oscillatory, and thus the non-homogeneities given by Eqs. (5) are bounded and oscillatory and the integrals in Eq. (S12) are constant. Previously provided proofs2 about states of equilibrium, stability, and the bifurcation property of the model are therefore applicable to the present version of the model.
4. Pairwise correlations among the estimated model parameters
Figure S1 shows the one- and two-dimensional marginal probability distributions of the model parameters, obtained through MCMC, and gives the pairwise correlations among the estimated model parameters. While some correlation among model parameters is unavoidable in non-linear models, substantive correlations were found only among αs, βw and ηw. It suggests some potential redundancy in the model parameterization given the data at hand, which may be resolvable with the inclusion of data from additional studies focusing on recovery sleep.
5. Predictions by the earlier version of the biomathematical model
In the publication on the earlier version of our biomathematical model,1 we focused primarily on the temporal dynamics between sleep/wake cycles (i.e., across days). Figure S2 shows the predictions of the earlier model version for study A1, which had also been used for the calibration of that model version. Comparison with the predictions of the new model version (main paper, Figure 1, bottom panels) illustrates that goodness-of-fit between sleep/wake cycles has been preserved from the earlier version to the new version of the model.
It was demonstrated that as long as the non-homogeneous part of the model contains only oscillatory functions such as circadian rhythm, the dynamics across sleep/wake cycles do not depend on it.1 A detailed examination of the non-homogeneous part of the earlier model version was left to be addressed in the present paper, and involved inclusion of additional calibration data sets such as those of study A2. The earlier model version was not meant to be used for the night shift schedule of study A2. For comparison with the predictions of the new model version, however, Figure S3 shows the predictions of the earlier model version for study A2. The magnitude of the changes within sleep/wake cycles in the night shift condition was much too large in the earlier model version; this has been fixed in the new model version by means of the time-dependent function κ, which modulates the amplitude of the circadian process as a function of sleep/wake state.
6. Dose response curve for build-up of impairment across days with restricted, split sleep
In study A3, a dose-response experiment involving 18 different conditions with chronically restricted nocturnal sleep augmented with a diurnal nap, it was found that the rate of PVT performance degradation across days is primarily a function of total TIB per 24 h—in the range from 4.2 h to 8.2 h—regardless of whether and how sleep is split between a nocturnal sleep period and a diurnal nap.3 Based on previously developed methodology for statistical modeling of changes in average performance impairment across days,4 the data of each condition were normalized to baseline and then fitted with a function β tθ, where t denotes time in days, θ is a curvature parameter, and β is an index of the rate of the (non-linear) build-up of performance impairment across days. The study revealed that β could be adequately described by a linear function of total TIB per 24 h.3
We applied the same analytical strategy to our new model estimates for study A3, averaging them over the time points 08:10, 10:10, 12:10, 16:10, 18:10, and 20:10 for each of days 0 (baseline) through 8 (see study A3 description in the methods section of the paper). After subtracting the day 0 average from the other day averages, β was assessed for each study condition by fitting the function β tθ (where θ was shared among conditions and estimated to be θ = 0.74, indicating near-linearity across days as previously reported3,4). The results for β are shown as a function of total TIB per 24 h in Figure S4 (blue dots).
To derive a dose-response curve approximation, the analysis of the model estimates was repeated with β constrained as a parabolic function of total TIB per 24 h. Figure S4 (blue dashed curve) shows that the dose-response curve approximation is near-linear, reaching zero (i.e., no build-up of impairment across days) near 8.2 h TIB (in agreement with an earlier estimate4 based on statistical modeling in study A1).
The originally estimated dose-response relationship (Figure S4, black dashed line),3 based on statistical modeling of the study A3 data, was linear. It was anchored at nearly the same β value for the lowest sleep dose of 4.2 h TIB in the experiment. However, the improvement as a linear function of increasing total TIB per 24 h was underestimated – seemingly implying that much more than 9 h TIB per day would be needed to maintain baseline performance across days, which is inconsistent with other laboratory study findings.4,5 Here, the model-based estimates appear to reflect a more realistic account of the dose-response relationship between daily sleep amounts and the build-up of performance impairment across days with restricted, split sleep.
SUPPLEMENTAL REFERENCES
- 1.McCauley P, Kalachev LV, Smith AD, Belenky G, Dinges DF, Van Dongen HPA. A new mathematical model for the homeostatic effects of sleep loss on neurobehavioral performance. J Theor Biol. 2009;256:227–39. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2008.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.McCauley P. Missoula, MT: University of Montana; 2009. Fatigue risk management: modeling the sleep/wake-based dynamics of performance. PhD thesis, ISBN 9781109550894. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Mollicone DJ, Van Dongen HPA, Rogers NL, Dinges DF. Response surface mapping of neurobehavioral performance: testing the feasibility of split sleep schedules for space operations. Act Astronaut. 2008;63:833–40. doi: 10.1016/j.actaastro.2007.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Van Dongen HPA, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003;26:117–28. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.2.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Belenky G, Wesensten NJ, Thorne DR, et al. Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose-response study. J Sleep Res. 2003;12:1–12. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00337.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
REFERENCES
- 1.Borbély AA. A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol. 1982;1:195–204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Daan S, Beersma DGM, Borbély AA. Timing of human sleep: recovery process gated by a circadian pacemaker. Am J Physiol. 1984;246:R161–78. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1984.246.2.R161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hursh SR, Van Dongen HPA. Fatigue and performance modeling. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles and practice of sleep medicine. 5th ed. St. Louis, MO: Elsevier Saunders; 2010. pp. 745–52. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Van Dongen HPA, Belenky G. Model-based fatigue risk management. In: Matthews G, Desmond PA, Neubauer C, Hancock PA, editors. The handbook of operator fatigue. Farnham, England: Ashgate Publishing; 2012. pp. 487–506. [Google Scholar]
- 5.McCauley P, Kalachev LV, Smith AD, Belenky G, Dinges DF, Van Dongen HPA. A new mathematical model for the homeostatic effects of sleep loss on neurobehavioral performance. J Theor Biol. 2009;256:227–39. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2008.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Belenky G, Wesensten NJ, Thorne DR, et al. Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose-response study. J Sleep Res. 2003;12:1–12. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00337.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Van Dongen HPA, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003;26:117–28. doi: 10.1093/sleep/26.2.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Aeschbach D, Cajochen C, Landolt H, Borbély AA. Homeostatic sleep regulation in habitual short sleepers and long sleepers. Am J Physiol. 1996;270:R41–53. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1996.270.1.R41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Grant DA, Van Dongen HPA. Individual differences in sleep duration and responses to sleep loss. In: Shaw PJ, Tafti M, Thorpy MJ, editors. The genetic basis of sleep and sleep disorders. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; in press. [Google Scholar]
- 10.McCauley P, McCauley ME, Van Dongen HPA. Mathematical model of sleep loss. Sleep Res Soc Bull. 2009;15:17–8. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rupp TL, Wesensten NJ, Bliese PD, Balkin TJ. Banking sleep: realization of benefits during subsequent sleep restriction and recovery. Sleep. 2009;32:311–21. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.3.311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Rupp TL, Wesensten NJ, Balkin TJ. Sleep history affects task acquisition during subsequent sleep restriction and recovery. J Sleep Res. 2010;19:289–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00800.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Van Dongen HPA, Dinges DF. Sleep debt and cumulative excess wakefulness. Sleep. 2003;26:249. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Dijk DJ, Duffy JF, Czeisler CA. Circadian and sleep/wake dependent aspects of subjective alertness and cognitive performance. J Sleep Res. 1992;1:112–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1992.tb00021.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Van Dongen HPA, Dinges DF. Investigating the interaction between the homeostatic and circadian processes of sleep-wake regulation for the prediction of waking neurobehavioural performance. J Sleep Res. 2003;12:181–7. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00357.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Mollicone DJ, Van Dongen HPA, Rogers NL, Banks S, Dinges DF. Time of day effects on neurobehavioral performance during chronic sleep restriction. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2010;81:735–44. doi: 10.3357/asem.2756.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Achermann P, Borbély AA. Simulation of daytime vigilance by the additive interaction of a homeostatic and a circadian process. Biol Cybern. 1994;71:115–21. doi: 10.1007/BF00197314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Jewett ME, Kronauer RE. Interactive mathematical models of subjective alertness and cognitive throughput in humans. J Biol Rhythms. 1999;14:588–97. doi: 10.1177/074873099129000920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hursh SR, Redmond DP, Johnson ML, et al. Fatigue models for applied research in warfighting. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2004;75:A44–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Johnson ML, Belenky G, Redmond DP, et al. Modulating the homeostatic process to predict performance during chronic sleep restriction. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2004;75:A141–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Roach GD, Fletcher A, Dawson D. A model to predict work-related fatigue based on hours of work. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2004;75:A61–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Åkerstedt T, Ingre M, Kecklund G, Folkard S, Axelsson J. Accounting for partial sleep deprivation and cumulative sleepiness in the three-process model of alertness regulation. Chronobiol Int. 2008;25:309–19. doi: 10.1080/07420520802110613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Postnova S, Layden A, Robinson PA, Phillips AJK, Abeysuriya RG. Exploring sleepiness and entrainment on permanent shift schedules in a physiologically based model. J Biol Rhythms. 2012;27:91–102. doi: 10.1177/0748730411419934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Borbély AA, Achermann P. Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep regulation. J Biol Rhythms. 1999;14:557–68. doi: 10.1177/074873099129000894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Van Dongen HPA, Belenky G, Vila BJ. The efficacy of a restart break for recycling with optimal performance depends critically on circadian timing. Sleep. 2011;34:917–29. doi: 10.5665/SLEEP.1128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.DeBoer T, Vansteensel MJ, Détári L, Meijer JH. Sleep states alter activity of suprachiasmatic nucleus neurons. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:1086–90. doi: 10.1038/nn1122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lo JC, Groeger JA, Santhi N, et al. Effects of partial and acute total sleep deprivation on performance across cognitive domains, individuals and circadian phase. PLoS One. 2012;7:e45987. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Indic P, Forger DB, St. Hilaire MA, et al. Comparison of amplitude recovery dynamics of two limit cycle oscillator models of the human circadian pacemaker. Chronobiol Int. 2005;22:613–29. doi: 10.1080/07420520500180371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lim J, Dinges DF. Sleep deprivation and vigilant attention. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129:305–22. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Basner M, Dinges DF. Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep. 2011;34:581–91. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dinges DF. Are you awake? Cognitive performance and reverie during the hypnopompic state. In: Bootzin RR, Kihlstrom JF, Schacter DL, editors. Sleep and cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1990. pp. 159–75. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Mollicone DJ, Van Dongen HPA, Rogers NL, Dinges DF. Response surface mapping of neurobehavioral performance: testing the feasibility of split sleep schedules for space operations. Act Astronaut. 2008;63:833–40. doi: 10.1016/j.actaastro.2007.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Tucker AM, Whitney P, Belenky G, Hinson JM, Van Dongen HPA. Effects of sleep deprivation on dissociated components of executive functioning. Sleep. 2010;33:47–57. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.1.47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Doran SM, Van Dongen HPA, Dinges DF. Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: evidence of state instability. Arch Ital Biol. 2001;139:253–67. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Banks S, Van Dongen HPA, Maislin G, Dinges DF. Neurobehavioral dynamics following chronic sleep restriction: dose-response effects of one night for recovery. Sleep. 2010;33:1013–26. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.8.1013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Dorrian J, Rogers NL, Dinges DF. Psychomotor vigilance performance: neurocognitive assay sensitive to sleep loss. In: Kushida CA, editor. Sleep deprivation. Clinical issues, pharmacology, and sleep loss effects. New York, NY: Marcel Dekker; 2005. pp. 39–70. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Haario H, Laine M, Mira A, Saksman E. DRAM: efficient adaptive MCMC. Stat Comp. 2006;16:339–54. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Brooks S, Gelman A, Jones GL, Meng XL, editors. Handbook on Markov Chain Monte Carlo. London, UK: CRC Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 39.Olofsen E, Van Dongen HPA, Mott CG, Balkin TJ, Terman D. Current approaches and challenges to development of an individualized sleep and performance prediction model. Open Sleep J. 2010;3:24–43. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Van Dongen HPA. Comparison of mathematical model predictions to experimental data of fatigue and performance. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2004;75:A15–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Roehrs T, Timms V, Zwyghuizen-Doorenbos A, Roth T. Sleep extension in sleepy and alert normals. Sleep. 1989;12:449–57. doi: 10.1093/sleep/12.5.449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Roehrs T, Shore E, Papineau K, Rosenthal L, Roth T. A two-week sleep extension in sleepy normals. Sleep. 1996;19:576–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Zhou X, Ferguson SA, Matthews RW, et al. Sleep, wake and phase dependent changes in neurobehavioral function under forced desynchrony. Sleep. 2011;34:931–41. doi: 10.5665/SLEEP.1130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Jewett ME, Dijk DJ, Kronauer RE, Czeisler CA. Sigmoidal decline of homeostatic component in subjective alertness and cognitive throughput. Sleep. 1999;22:S94–5. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Krueger JM, Taishi P, De A, et al. ATP and the purine type 2 X7 receptor affect sleep. J Appl Physiol. 2010;109:1318–27. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00586.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Sengupta P, Roy S, Krueger JM. The ATP-cytokine-adenosine hypothesis: how the brain translates past activity into sleep. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2011;9(Suppl. 1):29–33. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Krueger JM, Obál F. A neuronal group theory of sleep function. J Sleep Res. 1993;2:63–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1993.tb00064.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Krueger JM, Rector DM, Roy S, Van Dongen HPA, Belenky G, Panksepp J. Sleep as a fundamental property of neuronal assemblies. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:910–9. doi: 10.1038/nrn2521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Van Dongen HPA, Belenky G, Krueger JM. A local, bottom-up perspective on sleep deprivation and neurobehavioral performance. Curr Top Med Chem. 2011;11:2414–22. doi: 10.2174/156802611797470286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Jackson ML, Gunzelmann G, Whitney P, et al. Deconstructing and reconstructing cognitive performance in sleep deprivation. Sleep Med Rev. 2013;17:215–25. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2012.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Windels F. Neuronal activity: from in vitro preparation to behaving animals. Mol Neurobiol. 2006;34:1–26. doi: 10.1385/mn:34:1:1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mendoza J, Challet E. Brain clocks: from the suprachiasmatic nuclei to a cerebral network. Neuroscientist. 2009;15:477–88. doi: 10.1177/1073858408327808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Edgar DM, Dement WC, Fuller CA. Effect of SCN lesions on sleep in squirrel monkeys: evidence for opponent processes in sleep-wake regulation. J Neurosci. 1993;13:1065–79. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-03-01065.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Kronauer RE, Gunzelmann G, Van Dongen HPA, Doyle FJ, III, Klerman EB. Uncovering physiologic mechanisms of circadian rhythms and sleep/wake regulation through mathematical modeling. J Biol Rhythms. 2007;22:233–45. doi: 10.1177/0748730407301237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Rogers NL, Van Dongen HPA, Powell JW, et al. Neurobehavioural functioning during chronic sleep restriction at an adverse circadian phase. Sleep. 2002;25:A126–7. (Abstract Supplement) [Google Scholar]
- 56.Mallis MM, Mejdal S, Nguyen TT, Dinges DF. Summary of the key features of seven biomathematical models of human fatigue and performance. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2004;75:A4–A14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Dinges DF. Critical research issues in development of biomathematical models of fatigue and performance. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2004;75:A181–91. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Jackson ML, Van Dongen HPA. Cognitive effects of sleepiness. In: Thorpy MJ, Billiard M, editors. Sleepiness: causes, consequences and treatment. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 2011. pp. 72–81. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Van Dongen HPA, Olofsen E, Dinges DF, Maislin G. Mixed-model regression analysis and dealing with interindividual differences. Methods Enzymol. 2004;384:139–71. doi: 10.1016/S0076-6879(04)84010-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Van Dongen HPA, Belenky G, Krueger JM. Investigating the temporal dynamics and underlying mechanisms of cognitive fatigue. In: Ackerman PL, editor. Cognitive fatigue. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2010. pp. 127–47. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Wesensten NJ, Belenky G, Thorne DR, Kautz MA, Balkin TJ. Modafinil vs. caffeine: effects on fatigue during sleep deprivation. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2004;75:520–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Elmenhorst D, Meyer PT, Winz OH, et al. Sleep deprivation increases A1 adenosine receptor binding in the human brain: a positron emission tomography study. J Neurosci. 2007;27:2410–5. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5066-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Churchill L, Yasuda K, Yasuda T, et al. Unilateral cortical application of tumor necrosis factor α induces asymmetry in Fos- and interleukin-1β-immunoreactive cells within the corticothalamic projection. Brain Res. 2005;1055:15–24. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.06.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Yasuda K, Churchill L, Yasuda T, Blindheim K, Falter M, Krueger JM. Unilateral cortical application of interleukin-1β (IL1β) induces asymmetry in Fos- and IL1β-immunoreactivity: implication for sleep regulation. Brain Res. 2007;1131:44–59. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.11.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Fuller PM, Gooley JJ, Saper CB. Neurobiology of the sleep-wake cycle: sleep architecture, circadian regulation, and regulatory feedback. J Biol Rhythms. 2006;21:482–93. doi: 10.1177/0748730406294627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Starzyk J, Van Dongen HPA, Rogers NL, Dinges DF. Circadian adaptation during chronic sleep restriction at different circadian phases. Sleep. 2004:27, A74–5. (Abstract Supplement) [Google Scholar]
- 67.Rangan S, Van Dongen HPA. Quantifying fatigue risk in model-based fatigue risk management. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2013;84:155–7. doi: 10.3357/asem.3455.2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.