Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2015 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Med. 2013 Apr 11;44(3):10.1017/S0033291713000585. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713000585

The boundaries of the internalizing and externalizing genetic spectra in men and women

K S Kendler 1,2,3,*, J Myers 1,2
PMCID: PMC3825740  NIHMSID: NIHMS467449  PMID: 23574685

Abstract

Background

The distribution and co-morbidity of common psychiatric disorders can be largely explained as manifestations of two broad psychopathological spectra of internalizing and externalizing disorders. Twin studies suggest that these spectra arise from genetic factors.

Method

Structural equation twin modeling was applied to interview and questionnaire data on personality traits and lifetime psychiatric disorders on more than 5300 members of male–male (MM) and female–female (FF) twin pairs.

Results

The best-fitting models for both the externalizing and internalizing spectra differed significantly in males and females. In males, the externalizing genetic common factor was best indexed by four disorders in the following order: antisocial personality disorder (ASPD), drug abuse/dependence (DAD), alcohol abuse dependence (AAD) and conduct disorder (CD). In females, the four disorders most closely related to the externalizing common factor were, in order: DAD, AAD, nicotine dependence (ND) and ASPD. Personality traits of novelty seeking (NS) and extraversion (E) better indexed the genetic externalizing spectrum in females than in males. In both males and females, major depression (MD) and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) best indexed the genetic internalizing common factor. Panic disorder (PD) and agoraphobia (AgP) better reflected the internalizing genetic common factor in women, and neuroticism (N) in men. Genetic correlations between the two spectra were estimated at +0.53 in males and +0.52 in females.

Conclusions

The disorders that optimally index the genetic liability to externalizing and internalizing disorders in the general population differ meaningfully in men and women. In both sexes, these genetic spectra are better assessed by psychiatric disorders than by personality traits.

Keywords: Externalizing disorders, genetics, internalizing disorders, twins

Introduction

Multivariate analyses of epidemiological samples studied both cross-sectionally (Krueger, 1999; Krueger et al. 2001) and longitudinally (Krueger et al. 1998) suggest that the distribution of common psychiatric and drug abuse disorders, and the observed patterns of co-morbidity, can be largely explained as resulting from two broad psychopathologic dimensions typically called ‘internalizing’ and ‘externalizing’. Population-based twin studies in Virginia and Norway have verified the existence of these dimensions and found that they result largely from the operation of genetic factors (Kendler et al. 2003, 2011). A range of prior studies have attempted to define the boundaries of these spectra from a phenotypic perspective (e.g. Krueger et al. 2007; Venables & Patrick, 2012). One large-scale twin study reported a multivariate genetic analysis of the relationship between internalizing disordersandthe personality trait of neuroticism (Hettema et al. 2006). However, we are unaware of a previous focused effort to define which disorders and traits are central versus more peripheral to genetically defined internalizing and externalizing spectra. This is an important question because it addresses how, for genetic, epidemiological or clinical studies, we might be best able to assess in patients or subjects their overall genetic propensity to internalizing or externalizing psychopathology.

In this study, to address this question, we examined whether the pattern of loadings of disorders and traits on these spectra differ across the two sexes, which we know differ substantially in the overall prevalence of internalizing versus externalizing disorders (Wender, 1971; Kessler et al. 1994, 2005). After optimally assessing a core set of disorders or traits for each spectrum, we then examined the correlation between their genetic liabilities.

Method

Sample

Participants derived from two inter-related studies of Caucasian same-sex twin pairs from the Virginia Adult Twin Study of Psychiatric and Substance. Use Disorders (VATSPSUD; Kendler & Prescott, 2006). Subjects were ascertined from the population-based Virginia Twin Registry formed from a review of all birth certificates in the Commonwealth of Virginia. Female–female (FF) twin pairs, born 1934–1974, were eligible if both members responded to a mailed questionnaire in 1987–1988. Nearly all the variables used in these analyses were collected at the fourth interview wave (FF4), conducted in 1995–1997, with cooperation in earlier waves ranging from 88% to 92% (Kendler & Prescott, 2006). For this wave, we succeeded in interviewing 85% of eligible twins.

Data on the male–male pairs (MM) came from a sample (birth years 1940–1974) initially ascertained, with a 72% cooperation rate, directly from registry records. The first interview (MM1) was completed largely by telephone in 1993–1996 and was followed by a second wave of interviews (MM2), conducted in 1994–1998, with a response rate of 83%. The third wave (MM3) was completed from 1998 to 2004 in 77.8% of the eligible sample. Zygosity was determined by discriminate function analyses using standard twin questions validated against DNA genotyping in 496 pairs (Kendler & Prescott, 1999). The mean (S.D.) age and years of education of the twins were 36.3 (8.2) and 14.3 (2.2) at the FF4 interview, and 37.0 (9.1) and 13.6 (2.6) at the MMMF2 interview.

Measures and sample sizes

Unless noted otherwise, the lifetime prevalence of psychiatric and substance use disorders was assessed at personal interview by trained mental health professionals, who were blind to the status of the co-twin, using modifications of the SCID and DSM-IV criteria (APA, 1994; Spitzer & Williams, 1985). The names and abbreviations used for all the disorders and traits examined in this paper are given in Table 1 and Table 2.

Table 1.

Loadings on externalizing disorders and traits from the genetic common factor in males and females from the AE independent pathway model

Path estimates
from the genetic
common factor
Males Females
Antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) +0.67 +0.48
Drug abuse/dependence (DAD) +0.67 +0.67
Alcohol abuse/dependence (AAD) +0.66 +0.65
Conduct disorder (CD) +0.56 +0.46
Nicotine dependence (ND) +0.48 +0.49
Major depression (MD) +0.33 +0.28
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) +0.30
Neuroticism (N) +0.25 +0.21
Novelty seeking (NS) +0.24 +0.48
Sensation seeking (SS) +0.17
Caffeine dependence (CaD) +0.13 +0.18
Extraversion (E) +0.02 +0.30

Table 2.

The proportion of heritability of the individual putative externalizing disorders and traits that is shared with the genetic common factor in the AE independent pathway model

% Heritability
from common
factor
Variable Males Females
Antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) 92 57
Alcohol abuse/dependence (AAD) 74 80
Conduct disorder (CD) 73 47
Drug abuse/dependence (DAD) 62 74
Major depression (MD) 37 18
Novelty seeking (NS) 36 58
Nicotine dependence (ND) 33 37
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) 24
Sensation seeking (SS) 11
Neuroticism (N) 17 14
Caffeine dependence (CaD) 04 11
Extraversion (E) 00 23

MM twins

Lifetime major depression (MD) and self-esteem (SE) wereobtainedfrom thefirstwavedata(n=3483). The following disorders were assessed at the second wave interview (n~2900): alcohol abuse or dependence (AAD), drug abuse or dependence (DAD), nicotine dependence (ND), panic disorder (PD), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD); and five types of phobias: agoraphobia (AgP), social (SoP), situational (SiP), animal (AnP), and blood-injury (BiP). Both AAD and DAD were modeled as trichotomous variables: no disorder, abuse only, dependence (with or without abuse). ND was assessed by the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (Heatherton et al. 1991) for the lifetime period of heaviest smoking. We trichotomized the scores: 0–3, 4–6 and ≥7. Lifetime non-smokers were assumed to have a zero score. Phobias were assessed by modified DSM-III criteria (APA, 1980). For GAD we required a minimum duration of 1 month. Conduct disorder (CD), antisocial personality disorder (ASPD), neuroticism (N), extraversion (E) and novelty seeking (NS) were assessed by a self-report questionnaire associated with the MM2 study (n~2740). SE was measured by the Rosenberg scale (Rosenberg, 1965), N and E by the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire short-form (Eysenck et al. 1985), and NS by a shortened version of the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ) NS scale (Cloninger et al. 1991). N was developed by Eysenck to reflect an individual’s propensity to negative affect or emotionality. Typical items were ‘Are you the type of person (i) whose mood often goes up and down, (ii) whose feeling are easily hurt, and (iii) who is tense or highly strung?’ E was developed by Eysenck to reflect an individual’s sociability and the pleasure obtained from interpersonal interactions. Typical items were ‘Are you the type of person (i) who is rather lively, (ii) who likes mixing with people, and (iii) who can get a party going?’ NS was developed by Cloninger to assess impulsiveness and the desire for high levels of stimulation. Two representative items were: ‘Are you the type of person (i) who often tries new things for the fun or thrills, even if most people think it is a waste of time, and (ii) who, when nothing new is happening, usually starts looking for something that is thrilling or exciting?’ OurASPDmeasure assessed only adult antisocial traits.

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), caffeine dependence (CaD) and sensation seeking (SS) were assessed only at the MM3 interview (n~1750). As outlined elsewhere (Edwards & Kendler, 2012), ADHD was assessed retrospectively by 11 items that reflected DSM-IV criteria reported up to age 18 with the scores broken down into four approximately equal categories. As described previously (Kendler et al. 2008), CD was assessed by 11 items from the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA) interview using a four-point scale (Bucholz et al. 1994) operationalizing DSM-III-R criteria, excluding the highly deviant A criteria 9 (forcing someone into sexual activity) and 13 (physically cruel to people). We used a criteria count for CD divided into four roughly equal groups. Caffeine dependence was defined using symptoms of caffeine tolerance from the SCID and caffeine withdrawal as defined by DSM-IV (Kendler et al. 2007). SS was assessed by a shortened version of the Zuckerman scale, which, similar to the NS scale, attempts to assess an individual’s propensity to seek out novel and exciting experiences (Zuckerman, 1994). Three representative items were (i) I ‘like wild uninhibited parties’, (ii) I ‘sometimes like to do “crazy” things just for fun’, and (iii) I ‘like doing things just for the thrill of it’.

FF twins

All of the variables chosen for analysis were obtained from the FF4 interview (n=1900), with the exception of CD, ASPD and NS, which were assessed in the self-report questionnaire associated with the FF4 (n~1470); SE, which was assessed at the third wave interview (n=1887); and panic disorder, which was assessed at the second wave interview (n=1994). We had no measures of ADHD or SS in the FF twin pairs.

Statistical methods

The aim of these analyses was to apply, using structural equations, a model containing a single genetic common factor to putative externalizing and internalizing disorders, and traits in MM and FF twin pairs, to determine their relative loadings on the common factor. To improve our power, many of the diagnostic categories were treated as polychotomous rather than dichotomous, which allowed us to use information about subthreshold levels of symptoms. To ensure that this was a plausible model, we first tested the fit of an independent pathway model containing a single genetic and a single unique environmental common factor, in addition to genetic and unique environmental variable-specific loadings, against a model that contained only a single shared and a single unique environmental common factor (and shared and unique environmental variable-specific loadings). Analyses were performed in the Mx software package using full information maximum likelihood (Neale et al. 2003). The fit of the models was assessed by Akaike’s information criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1987), which performs well for models of intermediate complexity as used in this report (Williams & Holahan, 1994).

We then compared the fit of a model that constrained all parameters to equality across the sexes versus allowed them to differ. If the latter model fitted better, this meant that globally, across all the disorders and traits, males and females differed in the patterns of loadings on the internalizing and externalizing factors.

For computational reasons, we were limited to fitting nine variables at a time in our structural models. We therefore chose, for both the externalizing and internalizing spectra, nine a priori variables that reflected a broad range of potentially relevant disorders and traits. Once this first model was fitted, we then picked one variable with a relatively weak loading on the common factor and substituted for that variable, one at a time, other additional variables until we had exhausted the set of variables we wanted to examine. In this way, we were able to obtain results that should closely resemble those that we would find if we could fit simultaneously a single model encompassing all the variables of interest. Finally, we chose four high-loading variables on the genetic common factor for the internalizing and externalizing spectra, and fit a model examining the genetic and individual-specific environmental correlations between them.

Results

The lifetime prevalence rates for the major psychiatric and substance use disorders examined in this report are shown, separately for males and females, in Table 3. As expected, internalizing disorders are consistently more common in the female than in the male twins. The opposite pattern is seen for externalizing disorders.

Table 3.

The prevalence rates of the major internalizing and externalizing disorders used in these analyses in males and females

Disorder Sample size % Affected
Females
  Major depression disorder (MDD) 1917 30.62
  Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) 1920 24.11
  Panic disorder (PD) 1994 5.47
  Agoraphobia (AgP) 1917 6.10
  Social phobia (SoP) 1917 9.18
  Situational phobia (SiP) 1917 12.99
  Animal phobia (AnP) 1917 10.38
  Blood-injury phobia (BiP) 1917 6.05
  Alcohol abuse or dependence (AAD) 1917 15.44
  Drug abuse or dependence (DAD) 1917 11.01
  Nicotine dependence (ND) 1893 6.50
Males
  Major depression disorder (MDD) 3483 26.59
  Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) 2901 18.44
  Panic disorder (PD) 2895 2.04
  Agoraphobia (AgP) 2896 4.07
  Social phobia (SoP) 2896 6.28
  Situational phobia (SiP) 2895 9.40
  Animal phobia (AnP) 2895 5.25
  Blood-injury phobia (BiP) 2895 5.77
  Alcohol abuse or dependence (AAD) 2895 40.24
  Drug abuse or dependence (DAD) 2909 22.38
  Nicotine dependence (ND) 2897 18.29

Externalizing spectrum

We examined the fit of an independent pathway model with additive genetic effects and individual specific environment (hereafter AE) (with the seven a priori externalizing disorders and traits assessed in both males and females: ASPD, AAD, DAD, CD, ND, NS and E). The fit of an AE model with separate parameters in males and females (AIC= −209.0) was substantially better than that of a model with shared and individual specific environment (hereafter CE) (AIC= −14.3) or an AE model that forced the parameters to be the same in males and females (AIC= −188.2). Therefore, in the following analyses, we examined AE models separately in MM and FF pairs. In subsequent versions of this model, we substituted the traits N, MD and CaD for E.

Table 1 shows the magnitude of the loading of these eight disorders and four personality traits for males on the genetic common externalizing factor. The three disorders of ASPD, DAD and AAD had nearly identical and high loadings (around +0.67), followed by CD (+0.56), ND (+0.48), MD (+0.33) and ADHD (+0.30). CaD had the lowest loading of any disorder (+0.13). The personality trait with the highest loading on the genetic common externalizing factor was N (+0.25), followed closely by NS (+0.24) and SS (+0.17). E was essentially uncorrelated with the externalizing genetic common factor.

An alternative way to characterize the degree to which individual disorders or traits reflect the genetic common factor is to calculate the percentage of their heritability shared with the common factor. These results are shown in Table 2. Using this measure, ASPD is the disorder most strongly related to the externalizing factor in males followed by AAD, CD and DAD. MD, ND and ADHD are distinctly lower on this index, and CaD is near zero. Of the personality traits, the highest value is obtained by NS, followed by N and SS. E scores a zero.

The estimated loadings of seven disorders and three personality traits on the genetic common externalizing factor in females are shown in Table 1. The two disorders of DAD and AAD had nearly identical and high loadings (around +0.66), followed by ND (+0.49), ASPD (+0.48) and CD (+0.46). CaD had the lowest loading of any disorder examined (+0.18). The personality trait with the highest loading on the genetic common externalizing factor was NS (+0.48), followed by E (+0.30) and N (+0.21).

We also examined, in Table 2, the proportion of the heritability of these disorders or traits that is shared, in women, with the externalizing genetic common factor. Using this measure, AAD is the best index of the genetic common factor in females, followed by DAD, NS and ASPD.

Internalizing spectrum

We examined the fit of an AE independent pathway model with nine a priori internalizing disorders and traits assessed in both males and females (MD, GAD, PD, AgP, SoP, SiP, AnP, N and E). The fit of an AE model assuming separate parameters in males and females (AIC= −41317.2) was substantially better than that of a CE model (AIC= −41240.6), and also better than that of an AE model that forces all the parameters to be the same in males and females (AIC= −41314.1).

We therefore proceeded to examine the AE model separately in MM and FF pairs. To obtain parameter estimates from our two remaining internalizing variables (BiP and SE), we substituted these variables in two separate AE models for E. The parameter estimates of the AE model for all 11 variables in males are given in Table 4. The three variables MD, GAD and N stood out as having loadings in excess of +0.45 on the genetic common factor. Three further variables had more moderate loadings (0.25–0.35): SE, AgP and PD. All the other variables had fairly modest loadings on the genetic common factor (i.e.<+0.20).

Table 4.

Loadings on internalizing disorders and traits from the genetic common factor in males and females from the AE independent pathway model

Path estimates
from the genetic
common factor
Variable Males Females
Major depression (MD) +0.55 +0.70
Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) +0.53 +0.72
Neuroticism (N) +0.48 +0.30
Self-esteem (reverse coded) (SE) +0.32 +0.26
Panic disorder (PD) +0.27 +0.47
Agoraphobia (AgP) +0.26 +0.44
Social phobia (SoP) +0.17 +0.21
Blood-injury phobia (BiP) +0.10 +0.15
Animal phobia (AnP) +0.09 +0.03
Situational phobia (SiP) +0.08 +0.25
Extroversion (reverse coded) (E) +0.03 –0.01

Table 5 shows the proportion of genetic risk for each trait or disorder that results from the genetic common factor in males. By this measure, MD and GAD were the best indices of the genetic common factor for the internalizing spectrum, with all of their genetic variance shared with the common factor. Only two other traits in males shared more than half of their genetic variance with the common internalizing factor: SoP and N.

Table 5.

The proportion of heritability of the individual putative internalizing disorders and traits that is shared with the genetic common factor in the AE independent pathway model

% Heritability from
common factor
Variable Males Females
Major depression (MD) 100 100
Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) 100 100
Neuroticism (N) 68 32
Self-esteem (reverse coded) (SE) 25 19
Panic disorder (PD) 21 96
Agoraphobia (AgP) 43 55
Social phobia (SoP) 53 15
Blood-injury phobia (BiP) 6 11
Animal phobia (AnP) 3 2a
Situational phobia (SiP) 3 20
Extraversion (reverse coded) (E) 0 0
a

Very low total heritability. Results somewhat suspect.

As shown in Table 5, for females, the two disorders of MD and GAD stood out as having by far the highest loading on the genetic common factor of around +0.70. Two other disorders had moderate loadings of around +0.45: PD and AgP. N and reverse coded SE had loadings of +0.25 to +0.30. In females, three disorders stand out with an especially high proportion (>90%) of their genetic risk deriving from the genetic common factor: MD, GAD and PD (Table 5). Two other disorders/traits share more than 30% of their genetic variance with the internalizing common factor in females: N and AgP.

Correlation between the internalizing and externalizing spectra

We indexed the externalizing spectrum in males by ASPD, DAD, AAD and CD, and the internalizing spectrum by MD, GAD, N and PD. Fitting an AE model to both spectra, the genetic and environmental correlations between the common factors of these spectra were estimated at +0.53 and +0.30 respectively. In females, we indexed the externalizing spectrum by of DAD, AAD, ND and ASPD, and the internalizing spectrum by MD, GAD, PD and AgP. The genetic and environmental correlations between the common factors of these spectra were estimated at +0.52 and +0.48 respectively.

Discussion

These analyses, conducted in a large general population twin sample, had three major aims. The first was to determine whether the broad structure of the disorders and the traits that index the genetic common factors underlying the externalizing and inzing spectra were the same in males and females. The second aim was to characterize in detail the pattern of disorders and traits, and their loadings on these two genetic common factors. The third was to determine, once we knew how to index them, the correlation between the genetic common factors for the externalizing and the internalizing spectra. We review these results in turn.

For both the externalizing and internalizing spectra, we could not constrain the parameters of our multivariate twin model to equality across the sexes. In addition to the reliably observed large prevalence differences in the sexes for these two broad classes of condition (internalizing disorders are consistently more common in females than males and the reverse for externalizing disorders) (Wender, 1971; Kessler et al. 1994, 2005), the nature of the disorders and traits that optimally index the genetic and environmental liabilities to these spectra also differ in the sexes. These results are at odds with prior analyses of internalizing and externalizing disorder in our twins, where we found that the same parameters could be applied to both sexes (Kendler et al. 2003). However, that study examined only seven disorders across both spectra and so had much less power to detect sex differences than the present investigation.

Several interesting results emerged from our efforts to characterize the disorders and personality traits that best reflected these two genetic spectra. First, contrary to expectations, psychiatric and drug use disorders consistently outperformed personality traits as indices for these genetic common factors. The two best-performing personality traits were N for the internalizing spectra (which had the third highest loading in males and the fifth in females) and NS for the externalizing spectra (which had the fourth highest loading in females but the ninth in males, behind N). If we consider instead the percentage of heritability shared with the genetic common factors, the results are similar.

Despite the presence in our analyses of personality traits widely seen as good indices of the internalizing and externalizing dimensions of psychopathology, the hypothesis that these spectra might be best captured at the level of personality or temperament was not supported empirically (Krueger & Tackett, 2003; Clark, 2005). This is unfortunate because such scales are more informative about the broad range of vulnerability than a dichotomous diagnosis, and easier and cheaper to measure. For the externalizing dimension, these results might have differed if we had had other personality measures that more directly assessed disinhibition/impulsivity or meanness/callousness (Patrick et al. 2009). Our results are also consistent with a prior multivariate twin analysis in the sample showing that a substantial proportion of the genetic variation for internalizing disorders was not captured by N (Hettema et al. 2006).

Second, interesting sex differences emerged in the externalizing spectra. ASPD and CD more poorly indexed the genetic externalizing dimension in females than in males. Whereas ASPD was the ‘anchoring’ phenotype for the genetic common factor for the externalizing spectrum in men, in women it was AAD and DAD. These results provide empirical support for the claim that the criteria for these disorders are ‘male-centric’ and do not optimally assess female externalizing behaviors (Hartung & Widiger, 1998; Ohan & Johnston, 2005).

Third, the degree to which the abuse/dependence of individual psychoactive substances indexed the externalizing genetic common factor differed dramatically across substances (Kendler et al. 2007). In both males and females, DAD and AAD strongly, and ND moderately, reflected this genetic common factor. By contrast, in both sexes, CaD poorly indexed the genetic risk to the externalizing spectrum. It is intriguing to ponder the degree to which this difference arises from innate pharmacological features of the drugs themselves versus how they are viewed and controlled by our society (Courtwright, 2001).

Fourth, less pronounced sex differences were seen in the internalizing spectrum than in the externalizing spectrum. In both men and women, the internalizing genetic common factor was best tapped by a lifetime history of MD and GAD. The largest sex difference was for PD, which was a much better index of the genetic risk for the internalizing dimension in women than in men. In addition, surprisingly, N was a better index of the genetic common factor for the internalizing spectra in males.

Fifth, of the five phobias we examined, only AgP loaded substantially on the internalizing genetic common factor. Consistent with prior studies that suggest that the internalizing spectrum may contain a second partially independent ‘fear-based’ factor (Krueger, 1999; Vollebergh et al. 2001), we found that SoP, SiP, BiP and AnP made only very modest contributions to the overall internalizing genetic common factor. However, our results for PD, at least in females, were not fully consistent with this hypothesis.

Turning to the third aim of this study, we found very similar and substantial genetic correlations between the externalizing and internalizing genetic common factors in men and women. These correlationswere higher than observed between the Axis I and Axis II internalizing and externalizing genetic factors identified in a recent multivariate analysis of a Norwegian cohort (Kendler et al. 2011) (estimated at +0.16 and +0.36 respectively). However, the correlations we observed were broadly consistent with phenotypic correlations between internalizing and externalizing common factors observed in the USA (+0.51) (Krueger, 1999), New Zealand (+0.45) (Krueger et al. 1998) and The Netherlands (+0.56) (Vollebergh et al. 2001).

The high genetic correlations between these two spectra might arise from a range of non-specific risk genes that broadly predispose to any form of psycho-pathology. Alternatively, part of these correlations might result from causal pathways through the clinical syndromes themselves. For example, internalizing risk genes might predispose to MD or GAD, which in turn might increase the risk for AAD or DAD. Alternatively, genetic variants for externalizing traits might predispose to ASPD or AAD, which often lead to major relationship and work problems that in turn increase the risk for MD or anxiety disorders.

We examined the relationship between individual disorders and traits, and the genetic common factors in two ways. The most common-sense approach, emphasized in our discussion, is the coefficient on the path from the common genetic factor to the disorder. However, we also presented the proportion of disorder or trait heritability due to the common factor because this measure has at least one conceptual and one practical advantage. Conceptually, our commonsense approach ignores the impact of genetic effects specific to the disorder. Consider two disorders. The first has a 0.5 loading on the common genetic factor (so that 0.52=25% of the variance is due to genes shared with the common factor) but also has 25% of its variance due to disorder-specific genes. The second disorder has the same 0.5 loading on the common genetic factor and zero disorder-specific genes. Although the two disorders have the same magnitude of path from the common factor, in some senses, the genetic risk for the second disorder is a ‘purer’ index of the genetic common factor than is the genetic for the first disorder. In practice, unreliability places an upper limit on any estimates of genetic effects. Thus the loading on the genetic common factor for disorder A may be higher than disorder B because we can measure disorder A much more reliably. Our second measure, the proportion of genetic variance shared with the common factor, ‘corrects’ for unreliability as its effects would be similar in the numerator and denominator of the ratio, hence canceling out.

These results can be usefully compared to two recent modeling studies of the externalizing spectrum. In the Minnesota twin family study, Hicks et al. (2004) examined four disorders as indices of a genetic externalizing spectrum and found a pattern of loadings not dissimilar to those found in our sample: ASPD (+0.79), followed by AAD (+0.72), DAD (+0.59) and CD (+0.56). In a large male prisoner sample, Venables & Patrick (2012) compared various individual interview-based measures to the total phenotypic score of their extensive externalizing spectrum inventory (ESI). Their highest correlations (in order) were similar to those observed in our genetic analyses of the externalizing spectra: drug dependence, adult antisocial behavior and conduct disorder, followed by lower correlations with alcohol dependence and nicotine use disorder.

Although not a prime focus, the results from this study provide further support for dimensional models of psychopathology (Krueger & Tackett, 2003; Clark, 2005). A substantial proportion of the genetic risks for a relatively wide range of disorders and traits can be parsimoniously captured by two common genetic factors. These results raise the question of whether the focus for gene finding efforts for common psychiatric and substance use disorders should remain on the individual disorders or instead shift to focusing on broader spectra (Dick et al. 2008).

Limitations

These results should be interpreted in the context of four potentially important methodological limitations. First, the subjects in this study were all white twins born in Virginia. The degree to which these results would extrapolate to other samples is currently unknown. Second, to improve our statistical power with several of our measures, we examined subthreshold symptom levels rather than only diagnostic dichotomies. Thus, a reasonable amount of the information we used was from individuals with sub-diagnostic levels of symptoms. Third, the results of any multivariate analysis are keenly sensitive to the variables included. We lacked information on some potentially key diagnoses and personality traits. If we had been able to include information on obsessive–compulsive, bipolar illness or post-traumatic stress disorders, personality disorders such as avoidant or narcissistic or personality measures for psychopathy or impulsivity, our results might have differed meaningfully. Fourth, we used global model comparisons across the sexes and on that basis interpreted the resulting parameters as differing in males and females. However, we could have tested each individual parameter for equality across the sexes, letting the other parameters be free to differ. We did not do this because we were interested in global sex effects. Furthermore, with complex models like these, free parameters can sometimes ‘absorb’ the effects of constrained parameters, making interpretation of global changes in model fitting problematic. Thus, some of the individual factor loadings that we interpret as different across sexes might not have been significantly different had they been tested individually in this way.

Conclusions

We attempted, in this study, to identify those disorders and traits that optimally reflect the underlying genetic risk to internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. These results have potential practical implications in clinical or research settings where it might be of importance to assess an individual’s vulnerability to these two important psychopathological dimensions. Disorders differed widely in the degree to which they indexed the underlying genetic risk. For the externalizing trait, the best indices were four disorders: ASPD, AAD, DAD and CD. For the internalizing trait, the best indices were two disorders: MD and GAD. Importantly, the disorders and traits that optimally indexed the genetic liability to externalizing and internalizing disorders in the general population differed meaningfully in men and women. Finally, in both sexes, these genetic spectra are better assessed by psychiatric disorders than by personality traits.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grants P20 AA107828 and R37 AA011408. The Mid-Atlantic Twin Registry is supported by NIH grant UL1RR031990.

Footnotes

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

  1. Akaike H. Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika. 1987;52:317–332. [Google Scholar]
  2. APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  3. APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bucholz KK, Cadoret R, Cloninger CR, Dinwiddie SH, Hesselbrock VM, Nurnberger JI, Jr., Reich T, Schmidt I, Schuckit MA. A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: a report on the reliability of the SSAGA. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1994;55:149–158. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1994.55.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Clark LA. Temperament as a unifying basis for personality and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:505–521. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM. The Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire: U.S. normative data . Psychological Reports. 1991;69:1047–1057. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1991.69.3.1047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Courtwright DT. Forces of Habit: Drugs and the Making of the Modern World. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  8. Dick DM, Aliev F, Wang JC, Grucza RA, Schuckit M, Kuperman S, Kramer J, Hinrichs A, Bertelsen S, Budde JP, Hesselbrock V, Porjesz B, Edenberg HJ, Bierut LJ, Goate A. Using dimensional models of externalizing psychopathology to aid in gene identification. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2008;65:310–318. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.3.310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Edwards AC, Kendler KS. Twin study of the relationship between adolescent attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder and adult alcohol dependence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2012;73:185–194. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Eysenck SBG, Eysenck HJ, Barrett P. A revised version of the psychoticism scale. Personal Individual Differences. 1985;6:21–29. [Google Scholar]
  11. Hartung CM, Widiger TA. Gender differences in the diagnosis of mental disorders: conclusions and controversies of the DSM-IV. Psychological Bulletin. 1998;123:260–278. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.123.3.260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerström KO. The Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence: a revision of the Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire. British Journal of Addiction. 1991;86:1119–1127. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hettema JM, Neale MC, Myers J, Prescott CA, Kendler KS. A population-based twin study of the relationship between neuroticism and internalizing disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163:857–864. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.5.857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Iacono WG, McGue M, Patrick CJ. Family transmission and heritability of externalizing disorders: a twin-family study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61:922–928. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Kendler KS, Aggen SH, Knudsen GP, Roysamb E, Neale MC, Reichborn-Kjennerud T. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for syndromal and subsyndromal common DSM-IV Axis I and all Axis II disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2011;168:29–39. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10030340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kendler KS, Jacobson K, Myers JM, Eaves LJ. A genetically informative developmental study of the relationship between conduct disorder and peer deviance in males. Psychological Medicine. 2008;38:1001–1011. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. endler KS, Myers J, Prescott CA. Specificity of genetic and environmental risk factors for symptoms of cannabis, cocaine, alcohol, caffeine, and nicotine dependence. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:1313–1320. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.11.1313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kendler KS, Prescott CA. A population-based twin study of lifetime major depression in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56:39–44. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.1.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kendler KS, Prescott CA. Genes, Environment, and Psychopathology: Understanding the Causes of Psychiatric and Substance Use Disorders. New York: Guilford Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  20. Kendler KS, Prescott CA, Myers J, Neale MC. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factorsv for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:929–937. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao S, Nelson CB, Hughes M, Eshleman S, Wittchen HU, Kendler KS. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1994;51:8–19. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950010008002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Krueger RF. The structure of common mental disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56:921–926. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.10.921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Krueger RF, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Silva PA. The structure and stability of common mental disorders (DSM-III-R): a longitudinal-epidemiological study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1998;107:216–227. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.2.216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Krueger RF, Markon KE, Patrick CJ, Benning SD, Kramer MD. Linking antisocial behavior, substance use, and personality: an integrative quantitative model of the adult externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116:645–666. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.4.645. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Krueger RF, McGue M, Iacono WG. The higher-order structure of common DSM mental disorders: internalization, externalization, and their connections to personality. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30:1245–1259. [Google Scholar]
  27. Krueger RF, Tackett JL. Personality and psychopathology: working toward the bigger picture. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2003;17:109–128. doi: 10.1521/pedi.17.2.109.23986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Neale MC, Boker SM, Xie G, Maes HH. Mx: Statistical Modeling. Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University Medical School, Box 980126; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  29. Ohan JL, Johnston C. Gender appropriateness of symptom criteria for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, oppositional-defiant disorder, and conduct disorder. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 2005;35:359–381. doi: 10.1007/s10578-005-2694-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Patrick CJ, Fowles DC, Krueger RF. Triarchic conceptualization of psychopathy: developmental origins of disinhibition, boldness, and meanness. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:913–938. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Rosenberg M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1965. [Google Scholar]
  32. Spitzer RL, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID) New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  33. Venables NC, Patrick CJ. Validity of the Externalizing Spectrum Inventory in a criminal offender sample: relations with disinhibitory psychopathology, personality, and psychopathic features. Psychological Assessment. 2012;24:88–100. doi: 10.1037/a0024703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Vollebergh WA, Iedema J, Bijl RV, de Graaf R, Smit F, Ormel J. The structure and stability of common mental disorders: the NEMESIS study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001;58:597–603. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.6.597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Wender PH. Minimal Brain Dysfunction in Children. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc; 1971. [Google Scholar]
  36. Williams L, Holahan P. Parsimony-based fit indices for multiple-indicator models: do they work? Structural Equation Modeling. 1994;1:161–189. [Google Scholar]
  37. Zuckerman M. Behavioral Expressions and Biosocial Bases of Sensation Seeking. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES