Abstract
Social categorization is known to be an important part of social cognition. The categorizations we use, despite their multitude, frequently take the form of the general ingroup/outgroup distinction. A meta-analysis of 33 fMRI studies, reporting selective activations to various social groups, was used to identify common neural structures responsible for relational representation of social structure. Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE) analysis revealed areas in bilateral amygdala, cingulate gyrus, fusiform gyrus, right TPJ and right insula as implementing various aspects of social categorization. Activation of amygdala can be associated with modulation of behavioral response to subjectively significant stimuli. A more ventral part of anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) can be associated with self-referential reasoning about ingroup members while a more dorsal part of ACC is involved in the regulation of emotions toward outgroup members. Right insula can be engaged in the modulation of outgroup avoidance behavior. Fusiform gyrus (FG) appears to be directly involved in social categorization process via top-down modulation of social perception. Yet it is difficult to associate any of the revealed clusters with the relational ingroup/outgroup structure.
Keywords: social categorization, in-group, out-group, social structure, social identity
INTRODUCTION
Social categorization is considered an important part of social cognition (Macrae and Bodenhausen, 2000). Differentiating social world into more or less stable categories helps coping with it by making it graspable and predictable. While psychological interest in social categorization is typically restricted to individual behavior and intergroup relations (Brown and Gaertner, 2003), other fields such as sociology treat it as part of a more complex process of societies’ functioning, when the objective social structure, defined by unequal distribution of resources, the division of labor and other macro-level processes, interacts with micro-level representations of social structure and individual choice of strategies to produce and transform this social world (Bourdieau, 1980; Giddens, 1984). Understanding the mechanisms of social categorization is thus important for understanding societies, as well as individuals and groups.
The basic ingroup/outgroup opposition is the most typical way to represent group differences. Since the pioneering works of Lèvi-Strauss (1967), binary opposition is supposed to be the simplest and the basic form of human social cognition underlying many complex social phenomena. The hypothesis of ingroup/outgroup opposition, as the basic relational egocentric form of representing social structure, is fully compatible with this anthropological view. The social differentiations we use, despite their multitude, frequently (although not necessarily) take the form of ‘us and them’. This distinction is universally applicable to different social categories and has been intensively studied within social psychology (e.g. Taifel et al., 1971; Brewer, 2007; Bernstein et al., 2007; Krueger and DiDonato, 2008; Dovidio et al., 2009; Miller et al., 2010). These studies reveal the common features of the ingroup/outgroup opposition, such as ingroup favoritism, biases or better memory for various types of social categories: race, age, political affiliation. These effects took place even for contextually defined groups within the so-called ‘minimal group paradigm’ (Bernstein et al., 2007; Van Bavel and Cunningham, 2009). The existence of such a general form of categorization is plausible from evolutionary perspective as well. An ability to make, maintain and recognize alliances is crucial to survival of collectively living kinds. Categorization is a cognitive basis for dealing with alliances. As a multitude of categorizations are possible, they can use the same mechanism for performing the same function. This hypothesis was partially developed by Kurzban et al. (2001), who treated race as the by-product of a more general system evolved for the fast detection of alliances (see also Cosmides et al., 2003). Although this coalition–detection mechanism seems to be less applicable to categorizations based on different biological functions (age, sex), even more general and simple systems can encompass them as well.
This study is an attempt to identify brain structures responsible for this form of social categorization—a relational egocentric binary structure differentiating social world into two universally applicable categories, traditionally labeled ‘ingroup’ and ‘outgroup’.
METHOD
Data selection
Data search was performed in Google Scholar and PubMed using search term ‘fMRI’ with one of the following terms: ‘social categorization’, ‘social status’, ‘social group’, ‘ingroup’, ‘outgroup’. Additional texts were found through citation analysis. Inclusion criteria for the data set (314 foci from 33 experiments) were the following:
a study should use fMRI technique;
subjects are healthy adults;
experimentation design should include implicit or explicit social categorization (differentiation of targets into social groups or categories) that could be distinguished as ingroup/outgroup; and
coordinates of activations for the contrasts of interest should be reported in MNI or Talairach and Tournoux space.
Direct comparisons of both ingroup and outgroup members (e.g. Black vs White faces), and correlations between brain activations and behavioral measures (e.g. IAT) were included. Studies exploring the differences in social stratification (high vs low status) were intentionally excluded to avoid messing of two distinct mechanisms (Fiske, 1992). Studies in which a particular social category was contrasted with the non-social baseline, without further comparisons, were excluded as well.
Coordinates from both whole-brain and ROI analyses were included. Several studies investigating predetermined regions-of-interests were included when corresponding coordinates were available. The rationale for inclusion of ROI-based activations is that, although this approach may result in the loss of relevant data, it should not add to noise data. If a ROI-based study reports effects of interest, it produces relevant input data that can be included in the meta-analysis. When both ROI-based and whole-brain analyses were available, only the latter were included.
Coordinates reported in MNI space were transformed to Talairach space using Lancaster transform implemented in GingerALE 2.1 (UTHSCSA, www.brainmap.org). For every study, information in respect of the following elements was extracted: coordinates of peaks, corresponding relational status of target (ingroup or outgroup), social category involved in experimentation situation (race, age, political affiliation, etc), experimentation task and stimuli (see Appendix Table A1 for details).
Table A1.
Studies included in the meta-analysis
Study | Social category | Task | Stimuli | Main contrast as defined by authors | Activations | Notes |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Adams et al., 2010 | Ethnic/cultural | Mental state recognition (reading mind in the eyes task) | Visual | (Same culture RME > baseline) > (other culture RME > baseline) | 5 | |
Van Bavel et al., 2011 | Minimal | Group categorization | Visual | Ingroup > outgroup and novel faces | 2 | Bilateral activations were identified as an average of the reported ROI coordinates |
Van Bavel et al., 2008 | Minimal | Group or race categorization | Visual | Ingroup > outgroup | 6 | |
Race | 5 | |||||
Bruneau and Saxe, 2010 | Ethnic | Evaluation of statements about ingroup or outgroup | Semantic | Emotional > nonemotional judgments | 2 | Pro-outgroup judgments are supposed to trigger strong emotional reaction |
Cheon et al., 2011 | Cultural/ethnic | Rating of empathy toward Korean and Caucasian American targets | Visual | (Ingroup pain > ingroup neutral) > (outgroup pain > outgroup neutral) | 12 | |
Chiao et al., 2008b | Ethnic/cultural | Emotion recognition (fear faces) | Visual | Own culture > other culture | 8 | |
Cunningham et al., 2004 | Race | Face perception | Visual | Black (outgroup) > White | 15 | |
Falk et al., 2012 | Political | Taking the perspective of one’s own and the opposing politician | Visual/aural | Own candidate > opponent | 7 | |
Fischer et al., 2004 | Gender | Face perception | Visual | Female (outgroup) > male | 2 | |
Freeman et al., 2010b | Race | Superficial and individuated judgments about others | Semantic | Individuated > superficial judgments (Covaried with race) | 1 | |
Golby et al., 2001 | Race | Attention to stimuli | Visual | Same race > other race (Covaried with memory scores) | 3 | |
Harada et al., 2010 | Kin | Implicit self-recognition task, individualistic and collectivistic priming | Semantic | Father-relevant information > information about strangers | 24 | It can be hypothesized that father-relevant information not simply enables familiarity processing but serves as a kin-detection mechanism. Those who are not in the kinship ingroup are an outgroup |
Hart et al., 2000 | Race | Gender categorization of faces | Visual | Outgroup > ingroup | 11 | |
Hein et al., 2010 | Social | Empathy toward members of one’s own and rival football teams | Semantic | (High > low ingroup pain) > (high > low outgroup pain) | 10 | |
Hoehl et al., 2010 | Age | Face perception, response to an ape face presentation | Visual | (Adults > fixation) > (children > fixation), Happy faces; (adults > fixation) > (children > fixation), Angry faces | 30 | |
Kaplan et al., 2007 | Political | Viewing pictures of politicians | Visual | Opposing candidate > own candidate | 18 | |
Krill and Platek, 2009 | Race | Reaction to social exclusion | Visual | Same race > other race | 6 | |
Leibenluft et al., 2004 | Age | Repetition detection task | Visual | Unfamiliar children > adults | 14 | |
Losin et al., 2012 | Race | Imitation of gestures of three racial group members | Visual | European (ingroup) > Chinese; African (outgroup) > European | 27 | Authors link these differences to particular content of each of three groups, see discussion |
Mitchell et al., 2006 | Political | Judgments about (politically) similar and dissimilar others | Semantic | Similar > dissimilar other | 7 | |
Phelps et al., 2000 | Race | Face recognition | Visual | Black (outgroup) > White male faces (Correlated with behavioral measures of evaluation) | 6 | |
Richeson et al., 2003 | Race | Face perception | Visual | Black (outgroup) > White faces | 10 | |
Richeson et al., 2008. | Race | Face perception | Visual | Black > White | 1 | |
Rilling et al., 2008 | Minimal | Decision to cooperate in prisoner’s dilemma game | Semantic | Interaction with ingroup > with outgroup (covaried with behavioral outcome and functional connectivity with dmPFC) | 15 | |
Ronquillo 2010 | Race | Race reversal learning task | Visual | Black (outgroup) > White | 8 | |
Rule et al., 2010 | Political | Voting decisions | Visual | Voted > not-voted politicians | 4 | |
Ethnic/cultural | Visual | Voted > not-voted, (covaried with targets’ culture) | 2 | |||
Taylor et al., 2009 | Kin | Observation of personally familiar and unknown faces, attention to unusual 'ghost' pictures | Visual | Parents > unknown faces | 3 | |
Volz et al., 2009 | Minimal | Money distribution among others | Semantic | Ingroup trials > outgroup trials, ingroup bias > fair behavior mixed group trials > ingroup trials, (covaried with subjective identification with the ingroup) | 12 | |
Vrtička et al., 2008 | Minimal | Virtual game with plus or minus feedback accompanied with smiling or angry faces from two teams | Visual | Smiling > Angry faces in lost/won feedback condition | 18 | Smiling faces in ‘won’ condition and angry faces in ‘lost’ condition designate ‘friends’ (coalitional ingroup), and the reversed combination are ‘enemies’ (outgroup) |
Vrtička et al., 2009 | Minimal | Recognition of faces from previously learned in- and out-groups | Visual | Old > new faces (covaried with target’s social status, see information on previous study) | 5 | |
Westen et al., 2006 | Political | Judgments about contradictory statements of Democrat and Republican leaders | Semantic | Same/other party > neutral target | 10 | |
Wheeler and Fiske, 2005 | Race | Individuation judgments | Visual | Black (outgroup) > White faces | 1 | |
Age categorization | 1 | |||||
Xu et al., 2009 | Race/ethnic | Empathy (estimation of Other’s pain) | Visual | (Pain > No pain) and (Chinese > Caucasian) | 2 | |
Total | 314 |
The main interest of this meta-analysis is to understand how the brain processes representation of relational social differences. This raises the important methodological question of classifying activations as ‘ingroup-sensitive’ or ‘outgroup-sensitive’. If a study reports differential neural responses for the faces of Black and White targets, it does not point to social categorization as such. Social categorization implies that objects in classes are expected to be different in their mental states and behaviors. Such categorization process is not a priory given in any fMRI experimental setting. Differential response to various types of targets with one of which participants can be reasonably associated is, however, a good reason to hypothesize categorization process. This may explain the logic of data inclusion.
Take for example a study reporting selective neural response to children and adults in adult participants. It might seem strange to treat ‘children’ targets as ‘outgroup’ (as if they were ‘enemies’!). In this meta-analysis, however, the In/Out-group distinction is used in a broader sense. If children are expected to be socially different from adults and to have different interests, goals and behaviors, then they can be treated as an outgroup.
Not all possible social oppositions were however included. In studies dealing with gender differences, those that investigated the effects of attractiveness (e.g. Aharon et al., 2001; O’Doherty et al., 2003) were excluded to decrease the amount of noise data generated from certain aspects of sexual behavior. For the same reasons, I excluded contrasts in which mothers differentially responded to their own and unfamiliar children (Leibenluft et al., 2004; Nitschke et al., 2004). Although viewing one’s own relatives is expected to trigger kinship-based ingroup conception, it can be blurred by reward processing and other emotionally laden processes irrelevant to the goal of this study. I thus left only those kinship-related studies which did not include maternal behavior. Effects of exclusion of these kinship-related contrasts were also investigated.
Data analysis
The meta-analysis was based on activation likelihood estimation (ALE) method (Laird et al., 2005). A revised algorithm developed by Eickhoff et al. (2009) and implemented in GingerALE 2.1 software was used to find clusters most typically and robustly involved in the representation of social differences. This method’s great advantage is that it produces information about each voxel in the brain and can control for sample size and the number of activations reported in each particular study. False discovery rate threshold was set to q = 0.01. Brain images were produced in Mango 2.5 (Research Imaging Center, UTHSCSA).
RESULTS
ALE-based analysis revealed several clusters most commonly involved in social categorization: bilateral amygdala and adjacent regions, cingulate gyrus, including anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), left fusiform gyrus and right TPJ (see Table 1 and Supplementary data for figures and clusters found with a more liberal threshold). Ingroup and outgroup activations were also analyzed separately. For the ingroup condition, the largest clusters include ACC, left amygdaloid region, including globus pallidus and putamen, and the posterior part of cingulate gyrus. The outgroup condition revealed left amygdala and right insula as the largest clusters. Smaller clusters were also found in medial frontal gyrus and occipital lobe/precuneus in the ingroup condition.
Table 1.
Extrema values and coordinates for ALE-based clusters, q = 0.01
Cluster No | weighted center |
size, mm3 | Extrema value | Extrema coordinates |
Label | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | x | y | z | ||||
Ingroup + Outgroup | |||||||||
1 | −20 | −5 | −13 | 2976 | 0.04 | −20 | −6 | −12 | L Amygdala |
2 | 0 | 14 | 33 | 472 | 0.02 | −2 | 12 | 34 | Cingulate Gyrus, BA 24 |
3 | 3 | 37 | 16 | 424 | 0.02 | 2 | 36 | 16 | Anterior Cingulate, BA 32 |
4 | 15 | −7 | −8 | 408 | 0.02 | 16 | −6 | −8 | Medial Globus Pallidus |
5 | −45 | −59 | −16 | 248 | 0.02 | −46 | −58 | −16 | L Fusiform Gyrus, BA 37 |
6 | 50 | −54 | 33 | 208 | 0.02 | 54 | −54 | 32 | R Supramarginal Gyrus, BA 40 |
0.02 | 48 | −54 | 32 | R Superior Temporal Gyrus, BA 39 | |||||
7 | 39 | 8 | 9 | 192 | 0.02 | 40 | 8 | 10 | R Insula, BA 13 |
8 | −4 | −27 | 34 | 152 | 0.02 | −4 | −28 | 34 | Cingulate Gyrus, BA 31 |
9 | −12 | 43 | 1 | 128 | 0.02 | −12 | 44 | 2 | Anterior Cingulate, BA 32 |
10 | −20 | 14 | −8 | 112 | 0.02 | −20 | 14 | −8 | L Putamen |
11 | 33 | −72 | 20 | 104 | 0.02 | 34 | −72 | 20 | R Occipital Gyrus/Precuneus |
Ingroup | |||||||||
1 | 3 | 37 | 16 | 592 | 0.02 | 2 | 36 | 16 | Anterior Cingulate, BA 32 |
2 | −21 | −5 | −8 | 520 | 0.02 | −20 | −4 | −8 | L Globus Pallidus |
3 | −3 | −27 | 34 | 328 | 0.02 | −4 | −28 | 34 | Cingulate Gyrus, BA 31 |
4 | −20 | 14 | −8 | 208 | 0.02 | −20 | 14 | −8 | L Putamen |
5 | −1 | 26 | 43 | 168 | 0.01 | −2 | 28 | 42 | Medial Frontal Gyrus, BA 8 |
6 | −11 | 43 | 0 | 160 | 0.01 | −12 | 42 | 0 | Anterior Cingulate |
7 | 28 | −28 | −17 | 128 | 0.01 | 28 | −28 | −16 | R Parahippocampal Gyrus, BA 36 |
Outgroup | |||||||||
1 | −19 | −4 | −14 | 1816 | 0.03 | −20 | −4 | −14 | L Amygdala |
2 | 39 | 8 | 10 | 504 | 0.02 | 40 | 8 | 10 | R Insula, BA 13 |
3 | −3 | 10 | 35 | 160 | 0.01 | −2 | 10 | 34 | Cingulate Gyrus, BA 24 |
4 | 55 | −55 | 32 | 152 | 0.01 | 56 | −54 | 32 | R Supramarginal Gyrus, BA 40 |
5 | −43 | −37 | −8 | 136 | 0.01 | −42 | −38 | −8 | L Parahippocampal Gyrus, BA 36 |
6 | 15 | −7 | −8 | 112 | 0.01 | 16 | −6 | −8 | Medial Globus Pallidus |
To test the possible effect of ROI-based activations, they were excluded from the data set. This resulted in only minor changes: clusters in the left FG, right TPJ (ingroup + outgroup condition), as well as three smaller clusters in different conditions, were found when a slightly more liberal statistical threshold q = 0.02 was adopted.
To test the possible effect of personal relationship and familiarity with the kinship-based ingroup, activations corresponding to the contrasts in which one’s relatives were among targets, were also excluded. This resulted in decreased significance of activations in the cingulate gyrus and ACC in the combined and ingroup conditions. Partially, the main cluster in cingulate gyrus (0, 14, 33) in the main data set, as well as the corresponding cluster in the ingroup condition, was found when statistical threshold was set to q = 0.02; the size of this cluster was significantly smaller. A cluster in rostral ACC with coordinates (−12, 43, 1) from the combined condition of the main data set was found when the threshold was set to q = 0.05 in the combined condition, and to q = 0.02 in the ingroup condition. No changes in the outgroup conditions were found.
Exclusion of ‘children vs adults’ and the opposite contrasts did not significantly change the results.
DISCUSSION
Many aspects of how people represent social relations and structure their social world fit well into one simple and general form of ‘us and them’ distinction. This opposition and the related effects, such as group preferences and differing evaluations, look very similar for different social categorizations and contexts. This universality leads to a hypothesis of underlying cognitive structure or the set of common mechanisms. It seems plausible, evolutionarily, cognitively and sociologically, that a universal relational structure operating with a general form of ingroup/outgroup distinction exists in human mind/brain.
Although it is difficult to associate any of the revealed clusters with such a universal relational structure, several conclusions can be drawn in this regard to their possible functions in social categorization.
Amygdala
Activations in the left, and less significantly right amygdala and adjacent regions, are the most typically involved in selective response to various social targets. Amygdala has long been considered crucial to implicit processing and fast evaluation of socially relevant information (Lieberman, 2007; Stanley et al., 2008; Adolphs, 2009). Activation of the amygdala is more frequent, but not exclusive, in the outgroup condition. The question, however, is whether the amygdala discriminates by itself between targets or receives categorization input from other systems. Cunningham et al. (2004) report that amygdala is selective for racial outgroup in the subliminal presentation of faces. This might indicate that amygdala performs categorization function, at least for racial category, but the mechanism of this intellectual operation is not clear. How can it determine to what stimuli one must respond to? Fast response is possible via relatively simple and strict learned associations between particular cues and emotional reaction. This, however, is at best a content-based categorization, not a relational one.
The fact that the amygdala may be inhibited or habituated to particular types of targets (e.g. Hart et al., 2000; Cunningham et al., 2004; Wheeler and Fiske, 2005; Westen et al., 2006) indicates that other brain regions step in deliberately to take decisions about appropriate alertness level and regulate the amygdala activity (Heatherton, 2011).
The idea that the amygdala is especially important for fast processing of visual stimuli, as compared to cortical regions, has been questioned by Pessoa and Adolphs (2010) who proposed its major role is coordination and integration of biologically significant information and modulation of response (see also Sander et al., 2003). This function is too broad to implement social categorization as such but is compatible with the fact that amygdala activation is found both in ingroup and outgroup conditions, for various tasks and stimuli. Amygdala seems to be an important mediating structure but hardly implementing social categorization. Still, it can be partially responsible for content-based categorization via selective response to particular social targets strongly associated with ‘salience markers’, along with other routs for social categorization.
ACC/cingulate gyrus
Several clusters in ACC and other parts of cingulate gyrus were found for various conditions. ACC has often been considered as composed of two functionally distinct areas, dorsal and rostral/ventral, implementing either automatic and controlled processes or, in an alternative view, appraisal and regulative functions (Lieberman, 2007; Etkin et al., 2011). Among the revealed clusters, a more dorsal one centered at (0, 14, 33) is generally compatible with this distinction. This area is activated primarily in the outgroup condition. Several studies reporting activations in this region treat it as performing self-regulatory functions, including monitoring the need for control and suppression of negative emotions toward outgroup members (Richeson et al., 2003; Cunningham et al., 2004; Kaplan et al., 2007; Knutson et al., 2007). The same region, however, was reported as responding to familiar faces, including kinship ingroup and was linked to personal knowledge (Taylor et al., 2009). Besides, the exclusion of kinship-related activations from the data set resulted in its diminished significance in the combined condition, but has not changed the situation in the outgroup condition. Considering that other studies, investigating the effects of familiarity, have not pointed to this particular region of cingulate gyrus (Gobbini et al., 2004; Leibenluft et al., 2004; Harada et al., 2010), its monitoring/regulatory function seems more stable. Probably, viewing familiar faces activated this region owing to necessity of emotion regulation in response to emotionally salient stimuli.
Other, more ventral portions of ACC extending into ventromedial PFC are activated in the ingroup condition. Activity in these areas were associated with positive emotions during social comparison (Volz et al., 2009; see also Zink et al., 2008), the application of stereotypes (Knutson et al., 2007), mentalizing similar others (Gobbini et al., 2004; Freeman et al., 2010b) and representation of self-relevant information (Northoff et al., 2006; Harada et al., 2010; Ng et al., 2010; Heatherton, 2011). Further, activations in all these clusters, almost exclusively, appear in tasks using semantic stimuli (Rilling et al., 2008; Freeman et al., 2010b; Harada et al., 2010; Ng et al., 2010; Contreras et al., 2012). Taken together, these facts point to a possible mechanism of the ingroup/outgroup distinction processing, based on the similarity assessment and self-referential thinking.
It would be plausible to suppose that the ingroup/outgroup distinction is based on similarity assessment in such a way that similar others are treated as more predictable and understandable, thus enabling them to be included in the ‘ingroup’ category. This inclusion may result in the application of knowledge about self to ingroup members, up to including them in the self-concept (Volz et al., 2009; Ng et al., 2010) and, consequently, in self-preferential evaluations and reasoning thus implementing ingroup favoritism strongly associated with rACC in the study of Volz et al. (2009).
In this interpretation, the causal relations are however not clear. Does the perceived similarity lead to identification of a target as an ingroup member? Or is it a categorization process that turns on reasoning ‘by analogy’? Ingroup members may then be considered more similar and mentalizable by reference to one’s own position, as is probably the case in the study of Rilling et al. (2008). This however does not explain why some targets are represented as ingroups and others as outgroups.
Finally, a cluster in the posterior part of the cingulate gyrus was activated primarily in the ingroup condition. It is reported for various types of targets and tasks (Harada et al., 2010; Hoehl et al., 2010; Falk et al., 2012). A nearby cluster in cingulate gyrus was associated with the application of stereotypes (Mitchell et al., 2008) and with autobiographical memory (Spreng et al., 2008), but the rules controlling activation of this area in social categorization remain unclear.
Fusiform gyrus
Fusiform gyrus is typically associated with visual expertise and, partially, with remarkable sensitivity to human faces (Kanwisher and Yovel, 2006). That it is often activated in social cognition experiments is no surprise considering that human faces are the most typical type of stimuli in the imaging studies of social categorization. But why should FG appear in this meta-analysis, in which every activation included is especially sensitive to various types of social targets all of which are represented as faces? Although more familiar ingroup faces can lead to increased activation of the face-sensitive area (Golby et al., 2001), for the minimal ingroup members, who were not more familiar than outgroup members, the same effect was revealed (Van Bavel et al., 2011). Authors of the latter study considered that this region is more ingroup sensitive owing to motivational factors influencing activity in this region. The results of this meta-analysis support the idea of top-down modulation of the cluster revealed in the FG/inferior temporal gyrus (ITG), but not the idea of its ingroup bias. Indeed, several studies report outgroup sensitivity of this area as well (e.g. Ronquillo, 2010; Vrtička et al., 2009). Moreover, several studies relating to investigating social cognition report selective activation in this area of the FG/ITG or nearby region in the cerebellum to socially meaningful, but non-facial stimuli (Rilling et al., 2008; Marsh et al., 2009; Hein et al., 2010). Almost exactly the same region was associated with visual word perception (McCandliss et al., 2003), and an impressive meta-analysis of Binder et al. (2009) reveals a nearby cluster of FG/ITG that is constantly engaged in semantic processing.
A possible explanation can be given if experimental situations in which the FG/ITG is activated are to be compared. In the studies of social categorization, the cluster revealed in FG and adjacent regions in the left, and sometimes in the right hemisphere, is often activated when participant’s attention is explicitly directed toward social differences or the changing social context. First, it was often activated in the social categorization tasks, i.e. experimental settings in which participants had to discriminate between social targets: males and females, Black and White, etc. (O’Doherty et al., 2003; Singer et al., 2004; Van Bavel et al., 2008, 2011; Marsh et al., 2009; Farrow et al., 2011). In several other studies, differential activation of FG was found in tasks in which participants had to dynamically update their own and/or the targets’ social position (Rilling et al., 2008; Zink et al., 2008; Ronquillo, 2010). In other studies, participants had to explicitly rate the pain experienced by one’s own and rivals’ football team fans (Hein et al., 2010), explicitly (but not implicilly!) rated attractiveness of faces (Iaria et al., 2008), or explicitly evaluated and classified social targets (Krendl et al., 2006).
Although such tasks can be orthogonal to the process of interest, the fact that FG area is repeatedly involved in explicit social categorization may point to its role in the top-down modulation of social perception, rather than specific visual sensitivity. The preferential engagement of the revealed cluster, centered in the FG in explicit social categorization and in response to the changing social context, may reflect its role in the initial predisposition to social discrimination and indexing of socially significant perceptual input according to pre-existing expectancies and attention to particular aspects of social milieu. This function is compatible with the results of Bollinger et al. (2010), who reported that activation in the visual association cortex, including fusiform face area, is dependent on stimuli expectancies. Expected stimulus would affect activity and functional connectivity of these regions before the presentation of the stimulus (see also Righart et al., 2010). The particular sensitivity of this area to some types of social targets may reflect the interaction between the expectancies and the actual stimulus presentation. It is worth noting that, in experimental studies of social cognition, participants perform tens and hundreds of trials, so that they can easily produce their own ‘theory of an experiment’ and their own expectancies and predispositions, probably contributing to the amount of effort invested in social indexing.
The top-down modulation function of this area is also compatible with a recent study of Contreras et al. (2012) in which non-social semantic knowledge was compared with that of social categories. The study shows that social categorical information is processed in a different way in comparison with the seemingly equivalent non-social categorical information. This study reports, inter alia, activations in the left FG. Further research may focus on this area as being involved in interactive and bidirectional processing of social stimuli (Amodio and Ratner, in press): a more linear and monotonous bottom-up perception of visual input (Freeman et al., 2010a; Ramon et al., 2010), and more categorical top-down, ‘theory-laden’ perception and interpretation of social stimuli.
Right temporoparietal junction
This region is known to be crucial to several high- and low-level cognitive processes, including attention modulation and Theory of Mind construction (Decety and Lamm, 2007; Van Overwalle, 2009). No surprise that activation of this region was found in experiments in which mentalizing was necessary or plausible (Rilling et al., 2008; Adams et al., 2010; Cheon et al., 2011; Falk et al., 2012). More interesting is the fact that mentalizing functions are recruited differentially for ingroup and outgroup members in various social contexts. Given that differential mentalizing abilities for ingroup and outgroup members are associated with two radically distinct areas in the ACC/medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and right temporoparietal junction, the former being probably based on self-referential reasoning and the latter on simulation; it would be interesting to compare their roles in social inferences (Adolphs, 2009; Van Overwalle, 2009). However, there are no reasons to suppose that the right TPJ plays a role in social categorization as such or that it can discriminate between social targets.
Right insula
Right insula is the only region that demonstrates strong outgroup bias. Several studies dealing with various social categories and tasks report activation of this region in response to racial and political outgroups (Phelps et al., 2000; Richeson et al., 2003, 2008; Kaplan et al., 2007; Falk et al., 2012), unfamiliar faces (Gobbini et al., 2004) and dominant faces (Chiao et al., 2008a), and correlation with discriminating behavior toward minimal outgroup (Rilling et al., 2008). Given that insula damage is associated with difficulties in risky decisions (Weller et al., 2009), and the left insula is associated with response to disgusting stimuli (Harris and Fiske, 2006), it is plausible to suppose the role of this region in modulating avoidance behavior in social context with uncertainty or danger.
Turning back to the main goal of this meta-analysis, namely searching for the brain systems that implement putative relational structure which differentiates social targets into two opposite social categories, it should be mentioned that no union mechanism was found.
In the changing social context found by several studies, the fusiform gyrus is a promising line of further research, especially in the light of its involvement in re-categorization process. It is, however, difficult to say that this structure functions as simple binary relational mechanism of social differentiation and that it associates targets with specific relational social category. Instead, its role is more important for understanding the content-based categorization.
There are several reasons to explain why the neural basis of social categorization remains still unidentified. One is that the studies under question reflect brain activations pointing to different cognitive and affective effects associated with social categorization, but not the categorization itself. In neuroscience experiments, the researcher typically introduces categorization from without, leaving aside mechanisms of categorization as such. Social meaning of group differentiation is in its related evaluations, behaviors and expectations that can be very specific to various contexts. Although many studies declare their findings as dealing with social categorization, reporting differential activations to targets from different social groups or categories, their experimental designs do not often allow distinguishing the representation of social differences from related effects, such as ‘preferential choice to reward ingroup member’ or ‘increasing caution toward other-race person’. While most clusters revealed by ALE analysis are important for various categorization-related effects, such as stereotype suppression, modulating appraisal or avoidance behavior, etc., it is only FG cluster that seems to deal with categorization process as such. The possible role of self-referential region in the rACC/vmPFC, in the production of ingroup/outgroup distinction, is also possible and can be investigated in further research. It would be extremely useful to perform fMRI experiments for studying one sample in various categorization-related tasks and comparing different social classification systems. Till now, to my knowledge, only one fMRI study performed a direct comparison of different social categories, race and gender (Knutson et al., 2007).
Another side of the problem is the interaction of abstract binary classifications with content-based social categorizations. Most social categories we use deal with specific contents associated with them, from the skin color to the expected behaviors. The ingroup/outgroup distinction is, on the contrary, content-free because it is applicable to different social categories. At the same time, being egocentric, it is expected to produce specific effects such as ingroup favoritism or outgroup avoidance. In real life, these relational and substantial, content-based aspects of categorization are interwoven. Relative failure of the search for the ingroup/outgroup distinction in this meta-analysis can partially be explained by the involvement of content-based categorizations.
Several fMRI studies, indeed, found activations that were sensitive to a particular social group (category) in substantial rather than relational manner. This effect was found in the study of Lieberman et al. (2005) in which both African-Americans and Caucasian-Americans showed specific brain response to only African-American faces; an even stronger effect was found in a study of Losin et al. (2012), in which European-Americans imitated gestures of three racial groups (European, African and Chinese). Two of these outgroups have been processed differently, probably because of the difference in their content.
It is thus possible that a combination of different cognitive mechanisms, or modules, is involved in the processing of information on different social categories (Cosmides et al., 2003; Gil-White, 2006). It is also possible that relational forms, so obviously engaged in the minimal group experiments, interact in real life with other, content-based categorizations.
CONCLUSION
Simple rules and operations can produce complex effects. In the field of societies’ functioning such rules can include cognitive mechanisms representing social structure. One such simple mechanism is representation of social differences via ingroup/outgroup opposition. The results of this meta-analysis point to amygdala, ACC, fusiform gyrus and right insula as being most typically involved in differential response to social targets. Most of these brain areas, however, reflect various categorization-related effects, rather than categorization as such. Clusters in the ventral part of ACC appear to be sensitive to the ingroup members, probably indicating dispositional preference toward the ingroup and involvement of self-referential thinking about similar others. A more dorsal part of ACC can be involved in monitoring and/or regulation of emotional tensions raised by observing outgroup members. Right insula is strongly associated with outgroup targets probably modulating avoidance behavior. The fusiform gyrus is sensitive to social categorization tasks and to changing social context urging for re-classification of social objects probably reflecting expectancy-driven bias in category application to visual social stimuli. Further research could focus on direct comparison of neural systems involved in the processing of various social classification systems.
SUPPLEMENTARY DATA
Supplementary data are available at SCAN online.
Acknowledgments
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.
REFERENCES
- Adams RB, Jr., Rule NO, Franklin RG, Jr., et al. Cross-cultural reading the mind in the eyes: an fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2010;22:97–108. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adolphs R. The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology. 2009;60:693–716. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aharon I, Etcoff N, Ariely D, Chabris CF, O’Connor E, Breiter HC. Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron. 2001;32:537–51. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00491-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amodio DM, Ratner KG. The neuroscience of social cognition. In: Carlston D, editor. Handbook of Social Cognition. NewYork, NY: Oxford University Press; in press. http://www.psych.nyu.edu/amodiolab/Publications_files/Amodio_Ratner_chapter_inpress.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein MJ, Young SG, Hugenberg K. The cross-category effect: mere social categorization is sufficient to elicit an own-group bias in face recognition. Psychological Science. 2007;18:706–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01964.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19:2767–96. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bollinger J, Rubens MT, Zanto TP, Gazzaley A. Expectation-driven changes in cortical functional connectivity influence working memory and long-term memory performance. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:14399–410. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1547-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourdieau P. Le Sens Pratique. Paris: Les Editions des Minuit; 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Brewer MB. The importance of being We: human nature and intergroup relations. American Psychologist. 2007;62:728–38. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.62.8.728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown R, Gaertner SL, editors. Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes. Malden: Blackwell; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Bruneau EG, Saxe R. Attitudes towards the outgroup are predicted by activity in the precuneus in Arabs and Israelis. NeuroImage. 2010;52:1704–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheon BK, Im DM, Harada T, et al. Cultural influences on neural basis of intergroup empathy. NeuroImage. 2011;57:642–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiao JY, Adams RB, Tse PU, Lowenthal L, Richeson JA, Ambady N. Knowing who’s boss: fMRI and ERP investigations of social dominance perception. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 2008a;11:201–14. doi: 10.1177/1368430207088038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiao JY, Iidaka T, Gordon HL, et al. Cultural specificity in amygdala response to fear faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008b;20:2167–74. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Contreras JM, Banaji MR, Mitchell JP. Dissociable neural correlates of stereotypes and other forms of semantic knowledge. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2012;7(7):764–70. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cosmides L, Tooby L, Kurzban R. Perceptions of race. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2003;7:173–9. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00057-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham WA, Johnson MK, Raye CL, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Banaji MR. Separable neural components in the processing of Black and White faces. Psychological Science. 2004;15:806–13. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00760.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Decety J, Lamm C. The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: how low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. Neuroscientist. 2007;13:580–93. doi: 10.1177/1073858407304654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Doherty J, Winston J, Critchley H, Perrett D, Burt DM, Dolan RJ. Beauty in a smile: the role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:147–55. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00145-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dovidio JF, Gaertner SL, Saguy T. Commonality and the complexity of "We": Social attitudes and social change. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2009;13:3–20. doi: 10.1177/1088868308326751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Grefkes C, Wang LE, Zilles K, Fox PT. Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30:2907–26. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20718. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Falk EB, Spunt RP, Lieberman MD. Ascribing beliefs to ingroup and outgroup political candidates: neural correlates of perspective-taking, issue importance and days until the election. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 2012;367:731–743. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farrow TFD, Jones SC, Kaylor-Hughes CJ, et al. Higher or lower? The functional anatomy of perceived allocentric social hierarchies. NeuroImage. 2011;57:1552–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer H, Sandblom J, Herlitz A, Fransson P, Wright CI, Bäckman L. Sex-differential brain activation during exposure to female and male faces. NeuroReport. 2004;15:235–8. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200402090-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiske AP. The four elementary forms of sociality: framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review. 1992;99:689–723. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.99.4.689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman JB, Rule NO, Adams RB, Ambady N. The neural basis of categorical face perception: graded representations of face gender in fusiform and orbitofrontal cortices. Cerebral Cortex. 2010a;20:1314–22. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman JB, Schiller D, Rule NO, Ambady N. The neural origins of superficial and individuated judgments about ingroup and outgroup members. Human Brain Mapping. 2010b;31:150–9. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giddens A. The Constitution of Society: Outline of The Theory of Structuration. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Gil-White FJ. The study of ethnicity and nationalism needs better categories. Journal of Bioeconomics. 2006;7:239–70. [Google Scholar]
- Gobbini MI, Leibenluft E, Santiago N, Haxby JV. Social and emotional attachment in the neural representation of faces. NeuroImage. 2004;22:1628–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golby AJ, Gabrieli JDE, Chiao JY, Eberhardt JL. Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other-race faces. Nature Neuroscience. 2001;4:845–50. doi: 10.1038/90565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harada T, Li Z, Chiao JY. Differential dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal representations of the implicit self modulated by individualism and collectivism: an fMRI study. Social Neuroscience. 2010;5:257–71. doi: 10.1080/17470910903374895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris LT, Fiske ST. Dehumanizing the lowest of the low. Psychological Science. 2006;17:847–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01793.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart AJ, Whalen PJ, Shin LM, McInerney SC, Fisher H, Rauch SL. Differential response in the human amygdala to racial outgroup vs ingroup face stimuli. NeuroReport. 2000;11:2351–5. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200008030-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heatherton TF. Neuroscience of self and self-regulation. Annual Review of Psychology. 2011;62:363–90. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hein G, Silani G, Preuschoff K, Batson CD, Singer T. Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members’ suffering predict individual differences in costly helping. Neuron. 2010;68:149–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoehl S, Brauer J, Brasse G, Striano T, Friederici AD. Children's processing of emotions expressed by peers and adults: An fMRI study. Social Neuroscience. 2010;5:543–59. doi: 10.1080/17470911003708206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iaria G, Fox CJ, Waite CT, Aharon I, Barton JJS. The contribution of the fusiform gyrus and superior temporal sulcus in processing facial attractiveness: neuropsychological and neuroimaging evidence. Neuroscience. 2008;155:409–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.05.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaplan JT, Freedman J, Iacobony M. Us versus them: Political attitudes and party affiliation influence neural response to faces of presidential candidates. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanwisher N, Yovel G. The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 2006;361:2109–28. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson KM, Mah L, Manly CF, Grafman J. Neural correlates of automatic beliefs about gender and race. Human Brain Mapping. 2007;28:915–30. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krendl AC, Macrae CN, Kelley WM, Fugelsang JA, Heatherton TF. The good, the bad, and the ugly: an fMRI investigation of the functional anatomic correlates of stigma. Social Neuroscience. 2006;1:5–15. doi: 10.1080/17470910600670579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krill A, Platek SM. In-group and out-group membership mediates anterior cingulate activation to social exclusion. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience. 2009;1 doi: 10.3389/neuro.18.001.2009. Article 1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger JI, DiDonato TE. Social categorization and the perception of groups and group differences. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2008;2:733–50. [Google Scholar]
- Kurzban R, Tooby J, Cosmides L. Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization. PNAS. 2001;98:15387–92. doi: 10.1073/pnas.251541498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird AR, Fox PM, Price CJ, et al. ALE meta-analysis: controlling the false discovery rate and performing statistical contrasts. Human Brain Mapping. 2005;25:155–64. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leibenluft E, Gobbini MI, Harrison T, Haxby JV. Mothers’ neural activation in response to pictures of their children and other children. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;56:225–32. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lèvi-Strauss C. Structural Anthropology. Garden City, NY: Anchor; 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Lieberman MD. Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:259–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lieberman MD, Hariri A, Jarcho JM, Eisenberger NI, Bookheimer SY. An fMRI investigation of race-related amygdala activity in African American and Caucasian American individuals. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:720–22. doi: 10.1038/nn1465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Losin EAR, Iacobony M, Martin A, Cross KA, Dapretto M. Race modulates neural activity during imitation. NeuroImage. 2012;59:3594–603. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macrae CN, Bodenhausen GV. Social cognition: thinking categorically about others. Annual Review of Psychology. 2000;51:93–120. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsh AA, Blair KS, Jones MM, Soliman N, Blair RJR. Dominance and submission: the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and responses to status cues. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21:713–24. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCandliss BD, Cohen L, Dehaene S. The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7:293–9. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00134-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller SL, Maner JK, Becker DV. Self-protective biases in group categorization: threat cues shape the psychological boundary between "us" and "them". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2010;99:62–77. doi: 10.1037/a0018086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell GP, Macrae CN, Banaji MR. Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron. 2006;50:655–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell GP, Ames DL, Jenkins AC, Banaji MR. Neural correlates of stereotype application. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;21:594–604. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ng SH, Han S, Mao L, Lai JCL. Dynamic bicultural brains: fMRI study of their flexible neural representation of self and significant others in response to culture primes. Asian Journal of Social Psychology. 2010;13:83–91. [Google Scholar]
- Nitschke JB, Nelson EE, Rusch BD, Fox AS, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ. Orbitofrontal cortex tracks positive mood in mothers viewing pictures of their newborn infants. NeuroImage. 2004;21:583–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Northoff G, Heinzel A, de Greck M, Bermpohl F, Dobrowolny H, Panksepp J. Self-referential processing in our brain - A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage. 2006;31:440–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pessoa L, Adolphs R. Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a ‘low road’ to ‘many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nature Review Neuroscience. 2010;11:773–83. doi: 10.1038/nrn2920. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phelps EA, O'Connor KJ, Cunningham WA, et al. Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2000;12:729–38. doi: 10.1162/089892900562552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramon M, Dricot L, Rossion B. Personally familiar faces are perceived categorically in face-selective regions other than the fusiform face area. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;32:1587–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07405.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richeson JA, Baird AA, Gordon HL, et al. An fMRI investigation of the impact of interracial contact on executive function. Nature Neuroscience. 2003;6:1323–8. doi: 10.1038/nn1156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richeson JA, Todd AR, Trawalter S. Eye-gaze direction modulates race-related amygdala activity. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 2008;11:233–46. [Google Scholar]
- Righart R, Andersson F, Schwartz S, Mayer E, Vuilleumier P. Top-down activation of fusiform cortex without seeing faces in prosopagnosia. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20:1978–890. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rilling JK, Dagenais JE, Goldsmith DR, Glenn AL, Pagnoni G. Social cognitive neural networks during in-group and out-group interactions. NeuroImage. 2008;41:1447–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ronquillo JM. Neural Correlates of Learning and Reversal of Approach Versus Withdraw Responses to In- and Out-group Individuals. A PhD Dissertation, University of Southern California: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Rule NO, Freeman JB, Moran JM, Gabrieli JDE, Adams RB, Jr, Ambady N. Voting behavior is reflected in amygdala response across cultures. SCAN. 2010;5:349–55. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sander D, Grafman J, Zalla T. The human amygdala: an evolved system for relevance detection. Reviews in Neuroscience. 2003;14:303–16. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2003.14.4.303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer T, Kiebel. SJ, Winston JS, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. Brain responses to the acquired moral status of faces. Neuron. 2004;41:653–62. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00014-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim ASN. The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;21:489–510. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanley D, Phelps E, Bahaji M. The neural basis of implicit attitudes. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2008;17:164–70. [Google Scholar]
- Taifel H, Billig MG, Bundy RP, Flament C. Social categorization and intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology. 1971;1:149–78. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor MJ, Arsalidou M, Bayless SJ, Morris D, Evans JW, Barbeau EJ. Neural correlates of personally familiar faces: parents, partner and own faces. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30:2008–20. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Bavel JJ, Cunningham WA. Self-categorization with a novel mixed-race group moderates automatic social and racial biases. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2009;35:321–35. doi: 10.1177/0146167208327743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Bavel JJ, Packer DJ, Cunningham WA. Modulation of the fusiform face area following minimal exposure to motivationally relevant faces: evidence of in-group enhancement (not out-group disregard) Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2011;23:3343–54. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Bavel JJ, Packer DJ, Cunningham WA. The neural substrates of in-group bias: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Psychological Science. 2008;19:1130–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02214.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Overwalle F. Social cognition and the brain: a meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30:829–58. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20547. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volz KG, Kessler T, von Gramon YD. In-group as part of the self: in-group favoritism is mediated by medial prefrontal cortex activation. Social Neuroscience. 2009;4:244–60. doi: 10.1080/17470910802553565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vrtička P, Andersson F, Grandjean D, Sander D, Vuilleumier P. Individual attachment style modulates human amygdala and striatum activation during social appraisal. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e2868. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vrtička P, Andersson F, Sander D, Vuilleumier P. Memory for friends or foes: The social context of past encounters with faces modulates their subsequent neural traces in the brain. Social Neuroscience. 2009;4:384–401. doi: 10.1080/17470910902941793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu X, Zuo X, Wang X, Han S. Do you feel my pain? Racial group membership modulates empathic neural responses. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:8525–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2418-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weller JA, Levin IP, Shiv B, Bechara A. The effects of insula damage on decision-making for risky gains and losses. Social Neuroscience. 2009;4:347–58. doi: 10.1080/17470910902934400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westen D, Blagov PS, Harenski K, Kilts C, Hamann S. Neural bases of motivated reasoning: an fMRI study of emotional constraints on partisan political judgment in the 2004 U.S. presidential election. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2006;18:1947–58. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.11.1947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wheeler ME, Fiske ST. Controlling racial prejudice: social-cognitive goals affect amygdala and stereotype activation. Psychological Science. 2005;16:56–63. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00780.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zink CF, Tong Y, Chen Q, Bassett DS, Stein LL, Meyer-Lindenberg Know your place: Neural processing of social hierarchy in humans. Neuron. 2008;58:273–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]