Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Sep 27.
Published in final edited form as: Cogn Neuropsychol. 2013 Sep 27;30(3):10.1080/02643294.2013.835717. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2013.835717

Phonological facilitation of object naming in agrammatic and logopenic primary progressive aphasia (PPA)

Jennifer E Mack 1, Soojin Cho-Reyes 1, James D Kloet 1, Sandra Weintraub 2,3,4, M-Marsel Mesulam 2,3, Cynthia K Thompson 1,2,3
PMCID: PMC3832125  NIHMSID: NIHMS521595  PMID: 24070176

Abstract

Phonological processing deficits are characteristic of both the agrammatic and logopenic subtypes of primary progressive aphasia (PPA-G and PPA-L). However, it is an open question which substages of phonological processing (i.e., phonological word form retrieval, phonological encoding) are impaired in these subtypes of PPA, as well as how phonological processing deficits contribute to anomia. In the present study, participants with PPA-G (n=7), PPA-L (n=7), and unimpaired controls (n=17) named objects as interfering written words (phonologically related/unrelated) were presented at different stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) of 0, +100, +300, and +500 ms. Phonological facilitation (PF) effects (faster naming times with phonologically related interfering words) were found for the controls and PPA-L group only at SOA=0 and +100 ms. However, the PPA-G group exhibited protracted PF effects (PF at SOA=0, +100, and +300 ms). These results may reflect deficits in phonological encoding in PPA-G, but not in PPA-L, supporting the neuropsychological reality of this substage of phonological processing and the distinction between these two PPA subtypes.

Keywords: primary progressive aphasia, anomia, phonological processing, picture-word interference paradigm

Introduction

Primary progressive aphasia (PPA) presents a unique clinical syndrome among neurodegenerative diseases of the brain in that it selectively affects the language network in its early stages, preserving other cognitive capacities such as attention and memory (Mesulam, 1982, 2003). There are three major subtypes of PPA, each associated with different linguistic profiles and distinct patterns of neural atrophy. The agrammatic subtype of PPA (PPA-G) is characterized by effortful speech and impaired processing of morphosyntactic structure, with peak atrophy typically occurring in the left inferior frontal gyrus (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004, 2011; Mesulam et al., 2009, 2012). The logopenic subtype (PPA-L) has been linked to deficits in word retrieval and phonological working memory, with atrophy typically focused in the left posterior temporal lobe and temporo-parietal junction (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004, 2008, 2011; Mesulam et al., 2009, 2012; Rohrer et al., 2010). The semantic subtype (PPA-S) is characterized by difficulty processing lexical-semantic information (i.e., word meaning) in both production and comprehension, with associated neural atrophy in the left anterior temporal lobe (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004, 2011; Mesulam et al., 2009, 2012). Anomia is a feature common to all three subtypes and is influenced by phonological processing. However, relatively little is known about the contributions of phonological mechanisms to the anomia of PPA and whether these mechanisms are differentially impaired in patients presenting with different subtypes of PPA. In the present study, we test whether PPA-G and PPA-L are associated with impaired phonological processing during object naming.

Phonological processing is one of two major stages of word naming (e.g., Dell, Schwartz, Martin, Saffran, & Gagnon, 1997; Indefrey & Levelt, 2004; Levelt, 1992, 1999; Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999; Rapp & Goldrick, 2000). When a person is presented with an object to be named (e.g., a zebra), the image of the object is first processed by the visual system and transformed into a percept that can be linked to its multimodal verbal and non-verbal associations. The verbal association, a lexical progenitor or lemma, provides access to stored morphosyntactic and additional verbal information (e.g., that the percept is an ‘animal’ (generic stage of encoding), which is also a ‘zebra’ (specific stage of encoding); see Mesulam et al., 2013). This first stage, where the percept is transformed into a concept, is known as the semantic stage of linguistic processing. The second stage of linguistic processing is the phonological stage, which consists of phonological word form retrieval (i.e., retrieval of an abstract phonological representation, e.g., /zibra/) and phonological encoding (i.e., sublexical phonological processes including access to phonological segments and syllabification, e.g, /zi.bra/). This representation then undergoes phonetic encoding, i.e., generation of a motor representation for articulation. Disruption at either major level of processing may lead to semantic and/or phonological paraphasic production patterns, i.e., substitution of semantically related words (e.g., tiger for zebra) or production of responses that are phonologically related to the target (e.g., bebra for zebra), respectively. These errors are thought to reflect spreading activation both within and across levels of representation. That is, activation spreads to items that are semantically and/or phonologically related to the target (e.g., Caramazza & Hillis, 1990; Dell et al., 1997; Levelt 1999, Levelt et al., 1999; also see Goldrick & Rapp (2002) and Rapp & Goldrick (2000) for discussion). Whether or not the word production system is interactive (i.e., feedback from the phonological processing stage affects lemma selection; Damian & Martin, 1999; Starreveld, 2000; Starreveld & La Heij, 1995, 1996) or serial (i.e., no feedback of this sort; Schriefers, Meyer, & Levelt, 1990) is an unresolved issue.

All speakers occasionally produce paraphasias, but they are particularly common in aphasic speech. Previous research has shown that individuals with PPA-G and PPA-L tend to produce more phonological paraphasias than people with PPA-S, whereas the opposite pattern has been reported for semantic paraphasias (Clark, Charuvastra, Miller, Shapira, & Mendez, 2005; cf. Ash et al., 2010; Mesulam et al., 2012; Wilson et al., 2010). This suggests that phonological processing may be relatively prone to impairment in PPA-G and PPA-L. One recent study reported a higher rate of phonological paraphasias in PPA-L than in PPA-G (Croot, Ballard, Leyton, & Hodges, 2012), whereas other studies have found similar rates of phonological paraphasias across the two PPA subtypes (Mesulam et al., 2012; Wilson et al., 2010). Both PPA-G and PPA-L have also been associated with impairments in other aspects of phonological processing, including phonological working memory (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004, 2008; Mesulam et al., 2012; Rohrer et al., 2010) and pseudoword reading and spelling (Brambati et al., 2009; Shim, Hurley, Rogalski, & Mesulam, 2012). The substages of phonological processing (e.g., phonological word form retrieval, phonological encoding) that are selectively impaired during naming in PPA-G and PPA-L have not been characterized. Most previous studies have used off-line measures to examine phonological processing difficulty in PPA. However, on-line studies have the potential to reveal patterns of impairment that are not evident in off-line phonological measures. For instance, recent on-line studies indicate that even non-semantic variants of PPA (i.e., PPA-G and PPA-L) are associated with semantic processing impairments (Rogalski, Rademaker, Mesulam, & Weintraub, 2008; Thompson et al., 2012b; Vandenberghe et al., 2005).

Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that the brain regions supporting phonological processing during naming in healthy participants overlap with the regions that typically undergo atrophy in PPA-G and PPA-L. According to a meta-analysis of word production studies (Indefrey & Levelt, 2004), phonological word form retrieval recruits the left posterior middle and superior temporal gyri and the temporo-parietal junction (cf. Graves, Grabowski, Mehta, & Gordon, 2007; Graves, Grabowski, Mehta, & Gupta, 2008; Indefrey, 2011; Wilson, Isenberg, & Hickok, 2009). Indefrey and Levelt (2004) argue that phonological encoding, in contrast, is supported by the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), a region that may also support subsequent phonetic encoding and articulatory processes (see also Hickok & Poeppel, 2007; Patpoutsi et al., 2009). Given that PPA-L is characterized by atrophy in the left temporo-parietal junction, we might predict deficits in phonological word form retrieval for this subtype of PPA. Because PPA-G typically involves atrophy in the left IFG, deficits in phonological encoding are expected. If phonological word form retrieval and phonological encoding are indeed differentially impaired in these subtypes of PPA, this would support previous behavioral and neuroimaging evidence in favor of multiple substages of phonological processing (see Indefrey, 2011; Indefrey & Levelt, 2004 for reviews).

In the present study, we sought to test whether PPA-G and PPA-L are associated with deficits in different substages of phonological processing that are observable on-line as object naming unfolds. The picture-word interference paradigm (PWIP; Rosinski, Michnick-Golinkoff, & Kukish, 1975), which provides an online, automatic, time-constrained measure of the processes that support naming, was employed to study this effect. In the PWIP, adapted from the Stroop task (Stroop, 1935), participants are presented with a picture of an object to be named along with a visually- or auditorily-presented word, called an interfering stimulus (IS), which the participant is instructed to ignore. The dependent measure is naming latency. Generally, naming times are slower in the presence of an IS than they are for pictures in isolation (or for pictures labeled with their correct names), reflecting increased demands on processing resources (Lupker, 1982; Starreveld & La Heij, 1996). However, the interference effect depends on the specific properties of the interfering word as well as differences in time between picture presentation and word presentation, known as stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA). For example, interfering words that are semantically related to the target object (e.g. horse for target RABBIT) result in slower naming times relative to unrelated words (e.g., turnip for target RABBIT) when presented in close proximity to the to-be-named object (i.e., SOAs from 300 ms before picture presentation (−300 ms) to 100 ms after picture presentation (+100 ms)), an effect known as semantic interference (Glaser & Düngelhoff, 1984; Hashimoto & Thompson, 2010; Lupker, 1979; Rosinski et al., 1975; Schriefers et al., 1990; Starreveld & La Heij, 1995, 1996; Thompson et al., 2012b). When interfering stimuli are presented outside of this time window healthy speakers are no longer influenced by them. Interestingly, however, studies with PPA show semantic interference effects at long SOAs. Vandenberghe et al. (2005) found such effects at −750 ms in a group of PPA speakers and, in a recent study examining naming in PPA-G and PPA-L, Thompson et al. (2012b) found semantic interference effects at −1000 ms SOA in individuals with both types of PPA, indicating abnormal semantic processing.

In the present study, we focus on the phonological stages of object naming by manipulating the phonological form of the IS. Object naming can be speeded by words that are phonologically related to the target (e.g. radish for target RABBIT), an effect called phonological facilitation (PF) (Bi, Xu, & Caramazza, 2009; Hashimoto & Thompson, 2010; Lupker, 1982; Schriefers et al., 1990; Starreveld, 2000; Starreveld & La Heij, 1995, 1996). Two main explanations of this phenomenon have been proposed. On some accounts, the PF effect occurs because the target word and IS activate some of the same phonological segments, which raises the activation level for the segments in the target word and thus facilitates naming (Meyer & Schriefers, 1991; Roelofs, 1997; Schriefers et al., 1990). Alternatively, the PF effect may result from spreading activation between word forms, with the IS activating a cohort of phonologically-related word forms that includes the target (Levelt et al., 1999; Starreveld, 2000; Starreveld & La Heij, 1995, 1996). Spreading activation between orthographically-related representations likely also contributes significantly to this effect (Bi, Xu, & Caramazza, 2009; Lupker, 1982). The PF effect is sensitive to the SOA between the target picture and the IS, and has been observed at SOAs ranging from 300 ms prior to picture presentation (−300 ms) to 200 ms following picture presentation (+200 ms), with the precise time window varying based on properties of the experimental design (Bi, Xu, & Caramazza, 2009; Damian & Martin, 1999; Hashimoto & Thompson, 2010; Lupker, 1982; Meyer & Schriefers, 1991; Rayner & Springer, 1986; Schriefers et al., 1990; Starreveld, 2000; Starreveld & La Heij, 1995, 1996). To the best of our knowledge, the PF effect has not been observed in healthy speakers outside this range, although Starreveld (2000) found significant PF effects for healthy speakers at +300 ms with part word IS (e.g., pa), but not with full word IS.

In recent years, the PWIP also has emerged as a tool to study the processing mechanisms underlying naming difficulty in patients with anomia. Two recent studies have sought evidence of abnormal PF effects in aphasic speakers. One study of 11 patients with stroke-induced nonfluent aphasia found larger phonological facilitation effects at SOA=0 ms (i.e., simultaneous presentation of the picture and interfering word) for people with aphasia compared to age-matched controls (Hashimoto & Thompson, 2010). The authors interpret this heightened PF effect as evidence for a phonological processing impairment that led to greater reliance on phonological cues during naming. This hypothesis was supported by the aphasic participants’ impaired performance on language tests that targeted phonological processes. In addition, a case study of a patient with stroke-induced anomic aphasia reported a significant PF effect at SOA=0 ms, while the control group in the study demonstrated a trend towards PF that did not reach significance (Wilshire, Keall, Stuart, & O’Donnell, 2007).

To our knowledge, no previous studies have used the PWIP to investigate phonological processing in patients with PPA. Unlike stroke-induced aphasia, the syndrome of PPA is progressive and the neuroanatomy of disease is not dictated by vascular territories but rather by principles of neuronal connectivity patterns underlying large-scale networks (Mesulam, 1982, 2007; Seeley, Crawford, Zhou, Miller, & Grecius, 2009). Thus, PPA offers a unique opportunity to study language processing in a network undergoing gradual dissolution. In the present study, we used the PWIP paradigm to test the magnitude and time course of PF effects in people with PPA-G and PPA-L as well as healthy age-matched controls in four SOA conditions. In one condition target pictures were presented simultaneously with IS (i.e., SOA = 0 ms), which were either phonologically related or unrelated, and in the other three conditions the IS was presented after the target picture, either 100 ms (i.e., SOA = +100 ms), 300 ms (i.e., SOA = +300 ms) or 500 ms (i.e., SOA = +500 ms).

On the basis of previous findings indicating impaired phonological processing in PPA-G and PPA-L (e.g., Clark et al., 2005; Gorno-Tempini et al., 2008; Rohrer et al., 2010), we predicted that we would find evidence of abnormal phonological processing in both PPA variants. On the basis of previous studies on stroke-induced aphasia (Hashimoto & Thompson, 2010; Wilshire et al., 2007), we expected that phonological processing impairments would be reflected by larger PF effects in individuals with PPA relative to controls. On the basis of neurological evidence (i.e., typical regions of cortical atrophy), we predicted that participants with PPA-L would show abnormal (large) PF effects in earlier stages of naming (SOA 0 and/or +100 ms), reflecting impaired phonological word form retrieval, whereas participants with PPA-G would exhibit abnormal PF effects at later stages of naming (SOA +300 and/or +500 ms), reflecting impaired phonological encoding. Underlying these predictions is the assumption that PF effects may reflect spreading activation at either the lexical (Levelt et al., 1999; Starreveld, 2000; Starreveld & La Heij, 1995, 1996) or segmental (Meyer & Schriefers, 1991; Roelofs, 1997; Schriefers et al., 1990) level of representation.

Method

Participants

Participants in this experiment included two groups of patients with PPA, seven with agrammatic PPA (PPA-G) and seven with logopenic PPA (PPA-L), and a group of age- and education-matched healthy controls, consisting of 17 cognitively intact volunteers (see Table 1; age: χ2 (2, N = 31) = 4.045, p = .132; education: χ2(2, N = 31) = 1.923; p = .382, Kruskal-Wallis Test). Further, the two patient groups were matched for duration of symptoms (Z = −.971, p = .383, Mann-Whitney U Test), and reported symptom onsets ranging from 1.5 to 7 years prior to testing. All participants, both patients and healthy controls, were monolingual English speakers, who presented with no prior history of neurological, psychiatric, speech, language, or learning deficits. All passed a pure-tone hearing screening, had normal (or corrected-to-normal) vision, and were right-handed. All participants were recruited through the PPA Research and Clinical Program in the Cognitive Neurology and Alzheimer’s Disease Center (CNADC) at Northwestern University (Chicago, IL) and tested in the Aphasia and Neurolinguistics Research Laboratory at Northwestern University (Evanston, IL). They were paid for their participation and informed consent was obtained prior to the study. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Northwestern University. Nonparametric statistical tests (Kruskal-Wallis Test, Mann-Whitney U Test) were used to compare participant groups.

Table 1.

Summary of participant demographic data and scores on classification measures

Participant Age Gender Education Handedness Symptom Duration (years) PPVT (100%) NAVS SPPT
WAB-R Rep6 (100%)
C (100%) NC (100%)

PPA-G1 62 M 20 R 5 100.0 66.7 6.7 72.7
PPA-G2 59 M 12 R 3.1 94.4 0.0 0.0 66.7
PPA-G3 59 M 14 R 7 97.2 66.7 53.3 65.0
PPA-G4 52 F 18 R 1.5 97.2 80.0 26.7 63.6
PPA-G5 60 M 18 R 2 100.0 86.7 66.7 83.0
PPA-G6 61 F 18 R 5 100.0 100.0 13.3 81.8
PPA-G7 72 M 20 R 5 88.9 80.0 20.0 43.9

PPA-L1 69 M 15 R 2.5 97.2 100.0 100.0 85.0
PPA-L2 58 M 16 R 2 97.2 100.0 86.7 85.0
PPA-L3 65 F 13 R 5.3 83.3 86.7 100.0 81.8
PPA-L4 75 F 16 R 2.5 97.2 100.0 86.7 88.0
PPA-L5 76 F 16 R 2 97.2 100.0 100.0 61.0
PPA-L6 63 M 18 R 2.5 100.0 100.0 100.0 86.0
PPA-L7 64 F 16 R 2.8 N/A 100.0 93.3 84.8

Mean (SD)
PPA-G 60.71 (5.93) 17.14 (3.02) 4.09 (1.96) 96.83 (4.07) 68.57 C,L (32.37) 26.67C,L (24.65) 68.1C,L (13.21)
PPA-L 67.14 (6.57) 15.71 (1.50) 2.8 (1.14) 95.37 (6.0) 98.1 (5.04) 95.24 (6.34) 81.66C (9.29)

Control 62.76 (6.44) 15.94 (2.46) N/A 98.96 (1.72) 100.0 (0.0) 100.0 (0.0) 99.11 (2.09)

Note. PPVT = subset of Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; NAVS SPPT = Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences, Sentence Production Priming Test; C = Canonical sentences; NC = Noncanonical sentences; WAB-R, Rep6 = Western Aphasia Battery-Revised, subset of 6 most difficult items from Repetition subtest.

C

significantly impaired relative to control group,

L

significantly impaired relative to PPA-L group (p < .05, Mann-Whitney U Test).

The PPA participants presented with progressive language deficits with no evidence of other language or neurological deficits. Participants were diagnosed with PPA-G or PPA-L based on criteria presented by Mesulam et al. (2012), with individuals in both groups showing relatively intact single word comprehension and those with PPA-G, but not PPA-L, showing grammatical sentence production impairments (using a classification template with severity-based cutoffs; see Mesulam et al., 2012 for details). In addition, the classification criteria for PPA-L included impaired repetition. Single word comprehension was assessed using a 36-item subset of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT, i.e. moderately difficult items, 157–192; Dunn & Dunn, 2007). No differences between groups were observed (p > .05). Participants’ grammatical sentence production abilities were assessed with the Sentence Production Priming Test (SPPT) of the Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences (NAVS; Thompson, 2011; http://northwestern.flintbox.com). Production of noncanonical sentences was more difficult for the PPA-G than the PPA-L group (Z = −3.169, p < .01), and the PPA-G group performed more poorly than controls (Z = −4.592, p < .001) whereas the PPA-L group did not (Z = −2.74, p = .118). Repetition ability was assessed using a subset of items testing phrase/sentence repetition (10–15; Rep6) from the Repetition subtest of the Western Aphasia Battery-Revised (WAB-R; Kertesz, 2006). Both PPA groups showed impaired performance on this measure relative to controls (PPA-G vs. controls: Z = −4.220, p <.001; PPA-L vs. controls: Z = −4.221, p <.001), and phrase and sentence repetition was more impaired in the PPA-G group than in the PPA-L group (Z = −2.177, p = .026). See Table 1 for a summary of classification measures.

To examine working memory, visual perception, attention, executive function, and motor speech deficits (see Table 2 for a summary of scores) a battery of neuropsychological tests was administered, which included the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975), the Digit Span subtest (forward and backward spans) from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (Wechsler, 1997), the Facial Recognition Test (Benton, Hamsher, Varney, & Spreen, 1998), the Trail Making Tests (Reitan, 1958) and a motor speech screening (i.e., an oral apraxia screen and repetition of one, two, and three syllable words, with a maximum score of 10 for each; after Dabul, 2000, and Wertz, LaPointe & Rosenbek, 1984). Spontaneous speech samples also were collected (see below) and evaluated for motor speech ability. The PPA patients performed significantly more poorly than controls on the MMSE (PPA-G vs. control: Z = −2.713 p = .016; PPA-L vs. control: Z = −3.444, p = .001), likely reflecting the patients’ compromised language ability (see Golper, Rau, Erskine, Langhans, & Houlihan, 1987; Osher, Wicklund, Rademaker, Johnson, & Weintraub, 2008). In addition, both PPA groups showed impaired performance relative to controls on the Digit Span Forward and Backward tests (p’s < .05), which may reflect a deficit in phonological working memory (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2008). The PPA-G group also showed impairment relative to controls on the Trail Making Test (Z = −2.132, p = .034). On single-word repetition, the only significant group difference was that the PPA-G group had impaired performance relative to the controls and PPA-L group on three syllable words (PPA-G vs. control: Z = −2.94, p = .003; PPA-G vs. PPA-L: Z = −2.21, p = .027). These data indicated that the PPA participants showed at most mild motor speech deficits. On spontaneous speech samples all participants were judged to have good speech intelligibility.

Table 2.

Summary of neuropsychological and motor speech measures for PPA participants.

Participant MMSE (30) Motor Speech WMS-II FR (54) TM Test A
1 syl (10) 2syl (10) 3syl (10) DSF DSB
PPA-G1 28 10 10 9 6 2 48 44
PPA-G2 20 N/A N/A N/A 4 2 51 108
PPA-G3 24 10 10 8 3 3 39 87
PPA-G4 30 10 10 8 4 5 45 25
PPA-G5 28 10 10 10 6 6 50 35
PPA-G6 30 10 9 9 6 3 42 64
PPA-G7 28 10 10 7 3 6 52 34

PPA-L1 30 10 10 9 5 4 45 37
PPA-L2 23 10 10 10 5 3 44 49
PPA-L3 19 10 10 10 N/A N/A 41 N/A
PPA-L4 28 10 10 10 4 5 49 17
PPA-L5 24 10 10 10 4 5 47 25
PPA-L6 27 10 10 10 4 5 48 29
PPA-L7 26 10 10 10 6 4 46 28

Mean (SD)
PPA-G 26.86C (3.63) 10 (0) 9.83 (0.41) 8.57 C,L (1.04) 4.57C (1.40) 3.86C (1.77) 46.71 (4.89) 56.71C (30.97)
PPA-L 25.29C (3.64) 10 (0) 10 (0) 9.86 (0.38) 4.67C (0.82) 4.33C (.82) 45.71 (2.69) 30.83 (11.0)

Control 29.71 (0.59) 10 (0) 10 (0) 10 (0) 7.41 (1.06) 5.76 (1.35) 46.94 (3.89) 30.35 (8.87)

Note. MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination; Motor Speech = Single word repetition (1syl = 1 syllable; 2syl = 2 syllables; 3syl = 3 syllables); WMS-III = Wechsler Memory Scales-III; DSF = Digit Span Forward; DSB = Digit Span Backward; FR = Facial Recognition; TM = Trail Making Test. Maximum scores appear in parentheses. Numbers in Trail Making Test A indicate time to complete the test in seconds.

C

significantly impaired relative to control group;

L

significantly impaired relative to PPA-L group.

A number of additional tests also were administered to detail patient language deficit patterns (see Table 3), including the Western Aphasia Battery-Revised (WAB-R, Kertesz, 2006), which tests several aspects of language production and comprehension. Both PPA groups demonstrated impaired performance on all WAB subtests and Aphasia Quotients for the patients differed significantly from those of control participants (PPA-G vs. controls: Z = −3.85, p < .001; PPA-L vs. controls: Z = −3.85, p < .001). No significant differences were noted between PPA groups for naming ability as measured by the Boston Naming Test (BNT, Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1983; p’s > .05) and noun naming on the Confrontation Naming subtest of the Northwestern Naming Battery (NNB, Thompson & Weintraub, experimental version; p’s > .05), though both groups performed more poorly than controls (PPA-G vs. controls, BNT: Z = −2.98, p = .003, NNB: Z = −3.46, p = .002; PPA-L vs. controls: BNT: Z= −3.14, p = .001, NNB: Z = −3.04, p = .01). All participants demonstrated intact single-word reading, defined as scores of ≥7 on the Regular and Exception Word Reading Scales of the PALPA (Kay, Lesser, & Coltheart, 1992; see Table 3), though the PPA-G group performed more poorly than the controls (p’s < .05, Mann-Whitney U Test). Additionally, testing semantic knowledge revealed relatively preserved ability in both PPA participant groups. To evaluate semantic knowledge, both the picture and word versions of the Pyramids and Palm Trees Test (PPT; Howard & Patterson, 1992) were administered, with no significant differences between PPA groups for either measure (words: Z = −.735, p = .534; pictures: Z = −.464, p = .710).

Table 3.

Language testing results for PPA patients.

Participant WAB BNT NNB PALPA PPT Narrative measures
AQ (100) F (10) Comp (10) Rep (10) Nam (10) Noun W Rep (35) NW Rep (10) Reg (10) Exc (10) Words Pictures WPM MLU %GS %PE
PPA-G1 82.3 4 9.1 8 10 98.3 93.3 35 10 9 9 100.0 100.0 25.2 4.0 40.0 13.3
PPA-G2 79.9 4 8.85 7.8 9.3 81.7 95.0 35 8 8 10 90.4 94.2 55.4 5.0 66.7 2.8
PPA-G3 90.5 9 9.85 7.4 9 86.7 96.7 34 6 8 8 98.1 100.0 86.0 6.3 85.7 12.8
PPA-G4 78.8 5 8.5 7.6 9.3 76.7 93.3 35 8 10 8 98.1 98.1 110.3 7.6 57.5 4.6
PPA-G5 93.2 9 9.7 8.9 9 98.3 100.0 33 10 10 10 96.2 96.2 58.9 8.6 88.9 2.2
PPA-G6 75.3 4 8.25 8.8 8.6 88.3 98.3 33 7 9 8 88.5 96.2 36.0 5.4 66.7 17.9
PPA-G7 80.6 6 9 5.9 9.4 73.3 85.0 35 3 10 8 98.1 96.2 77.1 11.3 23.6 13.3

PPA-L1 92 9 9.2 9 9.8 98.3 96.7 35 10 10 10 98.1 98.1 104.5 9.5 92.0 5.9
PPA-L2 86.9 6 9.45 9 10 90.0 95.0 35 10 10 10 100.0 100.0 118.7 10.5 81.3 7.5
PPA-L3 78.6 6 7.4 8.8 8.1 83.3 N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A 78.8 94.2 N/A 5.4 84.6 3.6
PPA-L4 97.2 10 10 9 9.4 88.3 98.3 35 10 10 10 94.2 94.2 157.7 13.9 70.4 4.2
PPA-L5 88.8 8 9.6 7.2 9.6 83.3 98.3 35 10 10 10 100.0 98.1 94.1 10.7 81.3 7.4
PPA-L6 97.1 10 9.85 8.9 9.5 96.7 100.0 35 10 10 10 100.0 96.2 141.1 7.9 88.2 3.5
PPA-L7 93 9 9.2 9 9.3 90.0 83.3 35 10 10 9 N/A 96.2 98.1 7.8 94.7 6.4

Mean (SD)
PPA-G 82.94C (6.49) 5.86C (2.27) 9.04C (0.58) 7.77C,L (1.00) 9.23C (0.43) 86.19C (9.80) 94.52C (4.88) 34.29 (0.95) 7.43 C,L (2.44) 9.14C (0.9) 8.71C (0.95) 95.60 (4.40) 97.25 (2.18) 64.13C,L (29.41) 6.89C (2.51) 61.30C (23.46) 9.55C (6.24)
PPA-L 90.51C (6.51) 8.29C (1.70) 9.24C (0.87) 8.70C (0.67) 9.39C (0.61) 90.00C (5.86) 95.28C (6.09) 35 (0.0) 10.0 (0.0) 10.0 (0.0) 9.83 (0.41) 95.19 (8.32) 96.70C (2.14) 119.02 (25.5) 9.37 (2.71) 84.64C (8.12) 5.48C (1.73)

Control 99.69 (0.68) 10.0 (0.0) 9.97 (0.11) 9.94 (0.14) 9.95 (0.12) 98.24 (2.24) 99.58 (1.14) 35.0 (0.0) 10.0 (0.0) 9.94 (0.25) 9.94 (0.25) 98.08 (1.36) 98.87 (1.81) 131.85 (19.61) 11.15 (2.09) 93.58 (4.04) 0.83 (1.5)

Note. WAB = Western Aphasia Battery; BNT = Boston Naming Test; NNB = Northwestern Naming Battery; PALPA= Psycholinguistic Assessment of Language Processing in Aphasia; PPT = Pyramids and Palm Trees Test; AQ = Aphasia Quotient; F = Fluency; Comp = Auditory Comprehension; Rep = Repetition; Nam = Naming; Noun = Noun Naming; W Rep = Word Repetition; NW Rep = Nonword Repetition; Reg = Regular word reading; Exc = Exception word reading; WPM = words per minute; MLU = mean length of utterance; %GS = % grammatical sentences; %PE = % of words with phonological errors. Percent correct scores for BNT, NNB Noun naming, PPT, %GS, and % PE are shown.

C

significantly impaired relative to control group;

L

significantly impaired relative to PPA-L group.

Narrative language samples were obtained using a wordless picture book of the story of Cinderella using methods described by Thompson and colleagues (Thompson, Ballard, Tait, Weintraub, & Mesulam, 1997; Thompson et al., 2012a; Thompson, Shapiro, Li, & Schendel, 1995). The PPA-G group performed more poorly than controls on measures of fluency, specifically words per minute (WPM; Z = −3.38, p < .001) and mean length of utterance (MLU; Z = −2.70, p = .005), and also produced a smaller percentage of grammatically correct sentences than controls (Z = −3.47, p < .001). The PPA-L group did not differ from controls on measures of fluency but produced a smaller percentage of grammatically correct sentences (Z = −2.46, p = .013). Further, the PPA-G group, compared to the PPA-L group, produced less fluent speech, measured by WPM (Z = −2.571, p < .01) and also produced marginally fewer grammatically correct sentences (Z = −1.985, p =.053).

To assess phonological processing abilities, we obtained measures of word, nonword, and phrase/sentence repetition (Word Repetition and Nonword Repetition subtests of the NNB; Rep6 from the WAB, reported above). No group differences were observed on word repetition (p’s > .1), but the PPA-G group exhibited impaired performance relative to both controls and the PPA-L group on nonword repetition (PPA-G vs. Controls: Z =3.802, p = .005; PPA-G vs. PPA-L: Z = −2.448, p = .035), whereas the PPA-L group did not differ significantly from the controls. In addition, to obtain a measure of phonological processing in narrative speech, we calculated the percentage of nouns and verbs in the narrative sample that contained phonological errors, including phonological paraphasias as well as phonologically-related repair sequences (e.g., pr – prince). Both PPA groups produced significantly more phonological errors than the controls (PPA-G: Z = −3.43, p < .001; PPA-L: Z = −3.43, p < .001); the two PPA groups did not differ significantly.

Experimental Stimuli

Fifty nouns and corresponding pictures, all black-and-white line drawings, including 20 living things (fruits/vegetables, birds/mammals; 10 each), 20 nonliving things (tools, clothing; 10 each), and 10 filler items (from various categories) were selected (see Appendix). For each target item, a set of eight IS, consisting of written words (all nouns), were selected. Four were phonologically related words (matched for the onset and rhyme of the first syllable of the target word) and four were phonologically unrelated words. None of the IS were semantically related to their respective targets. For example, for the target item camel, the phonologically related IS were cannon, candor, cabbage, and canvas and the unrelated IS were fashion, lagoon, detour, and wallet. Phonologically related IS, phonologically unrelated IS, and target words were matched for length in syllables (1–3) and frequency (M=372.75, M=458.29, and M=416.85, respectively; F(2,357)=1.161, p =.314; data from the CELEX database (Baayen, Piepenbrock & van Rijn, 1993)). Phonologically related and unrelated IS did not differ with respect to imageability and both were significantly less imageable than target words (p’s < .05 Tukey HSD post-hoc test; F(2,227)=10.712, p<.001; M=533.19, M = 559.29, M=593.74, respectively; data from the MRC Psycholinguistic database).1 In addition, 10 healthy volunteers (age 25–46), who did not participate in the experiment, rated the phonological relatedness of word pairs, using a 7-point scale (1=no overlap, 7=high overlap). Word pairs were included in the phonologically related condition only if their mean relatedness rating was 5.7 or higher and in the phonologically unrelated condition only if their mean relatedness rating was 2.5 or lower.

For each of the four SOAs, one phonologically related and one phonologically unrelated IS were randomly selected and paired with each of the 50 stimulus pictures, for a total of 100 stimulus pairs per SOA (40 related target pairs, 40 unrelated target pairs, and 20 filler pairs (10 related and 10 unrelated)). Participants were tested on all four SOAs; thus, they were presented with 400 picture-word stimulus pairs in total. The stimulus pairs were pseudorandomly divided into 10 sets of 40 items each, with each set containing stimulus pairs from all four SOAs. Care was taken to ensure that each target item did not occur more than once per set. In addition, items from the same SOA condition were separated by at least three trials on each set. The picture and word stimulus pairs were entered into Superlab (version 4.0; Cedrus, Phoenix, AZ) for experimental presentation. Three versions of the experiment were created, using identical stimuli, but with different interstimulus intervals (ISIs): 3500 ms, 5000 ms and 7000 ms. For all healthy participants, the 3500 ms version was used. The version used for the PPA participants depended on their naming ability as observed during administration of the BNT, such that participants with more pronounced naming deficits were given versions with longer ISIs to allow adequate response time. For five participants (4 PPA-G and 1 PPA-L) the 5000 ms version was used, whereas two PPA-L participants were tested with the 7000 ms version. All others were tested with the 3500 ms version.

Procedure

Seated in front of an iMac computer monitor (20 inch, OSX 10.4.1), participants were instructed to name pictures as they appeared but to ignore the IS to the extent possible. On each experimental trial a cross was presented for 500 ms, followed by a target picture and IS in one of the four SOA conditions. The acoustic waveform of each response produced was recorded through the computer’s internal microphone using Praat 5.0 software (Boersma & Weenink, 2010). A sample trial is illustrated in Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Example stimulus item.

Before beginning the experimental trials, participants were pretested on all picture stimuli used in the experiment, in order to ascertain their naming abilities and to familiarize them with the stimuli. First, each picture was presented for naming and participants were given 5 seconds to respond with no feedback provided. Pictures were presented a second time when errors occurred. Picture naming performance was at least 69% correct for all participants included in the study. We also pretested participants’ ability to read the IS by presenting each for them to read aloud, with performance ranging from 92% to 100% correct. Finally, practice trials were presented, which required participants to name target pictures overlaid with written words. These trials used stimuli similar, but not identical, to the experimental items.

Data Analysis

Naming responses that matched the target pictures and occurred within the given response time were considered correct. Correct responses preceded by a filler word (e.g., uh, pencil) or a minimal grammatical context (e.g., it’s a pen) were accepted, with naming latency measured from the onset of the target word. Accuracy data were analyzed using mixed-effects logistic regression (e.g., Jaeger, 2008) using the languageR package in R (Baayen, 2010). The effects of group, SOA, and relatedness of the IS, and their interactions were evaluated in an additive stepwise procedure, with ANOVA tests used to compare models. Random intercepts for participant and item were included; the addition of random slopes did not improve model fit.

Errors were classified into the following types: phonological paraphasias (errors sharing at least 50% of phonemes with the target word, e.g., punger for plunger), semantic paraphasias (errors semantically related to the target word, e.g., giraffe for camel), neologistic errors (non-word errors sharing less than 50% of phonemes with the target word; e.g., azate for robot), non-related responses (real-word errors unrelated to the target word, e.g., volcano for raccoon), phonological attempts (phonologically-related attempts at producing the word followed by a correct production, e.g., rope-rose), other self-corrections (e.g., ka-elephant), non-responses (in which the participant failed to respond), and other responses (e.g., I don’t know). Nonparametric statistical tests (Kruskal-Wallis Test, Mann-Whitney U Test) were used to compare the frequency of each error type across groups.

Correct responses then were analyzed for reaction time (RT), measured from picture onset to production of the first phoneme of the target word marked in the acoustic waveform. Thirty percent of the data were rescored for both accuracy and RT by an independent coder for scoring reliability purposes; overall point-to-point agreement between the primary and secondary coders was 98%. Disagreements were resolved by discussions among the experimenters. Extreme outliers (RT’s less than 500 or greater than 5000 ms) were excluded (.06% of correct responses). Following model selection (see below), outlying data points with absolute standardized residuals greater than 2.5 standard deviations were eliminated (2.71% of correct responses), following the procedure in Baayen and Milin (2010).

The reaction time data were analyzed using mixed-effects linear regression, employing a stepwise additive procedure to evaluate the effects of group, SOA, relatedness, and their interactions; all models included random intercepts for participant and item, and random slopes for SOA and relatedness were included (for both participant and item) as they significantly improved model fit. Due to significant non-normality in the raw RT data, a log-transformation was applied prior to statistical analysis. These data were further transformed to z-scores [(participant trial-specific RT – participant mean RT)/participant SD RT] in order to scale the data to account for overall RT differences across groups (see e.g., Schuchard & Thompson, 2013; for a different method of data scaling, see Wilshire et al., 2007).

Results

Naming accuracy

Table 4 provides the mean percentage of correct responses for each participant group. Mean accuracy for the control group was near ceiling (98.3% and 98.6% for related and unrelated trials, respectively). Both patient groups also performed quite well, but accuracy was below that of the healthy controls (PPA-G: 89.7% and 88.1% for related and unrelated items, respectively; PPA-L: 91.5% and 88.2% for related and unrelated items, respectively). The best-fitting model of the data included predictors for group, SOA, and relatedness, and a group x relatedness interaction. While the PPA groups were less accurate than controls (PPA-G: z = −4.989, p < .001; PPA-L: z = −3.715, p < .001), accuracy for the two PPA groups did not differ (z = 1.054, p = .292). Accuracy across groups was higher at SOA +500 ms than at SOA 0 ms (z = 2.789, p= .005), but no differences were observed between SOA 0 and SOA’s +100 and +300 ms (z’s < +/− 1, p’s > .5). There was no significant main effect of relatedness (z =.778, p = .437). The group x relatedness interaction was driven by the PPA-L group, who were relatively less accurate on unrelated than on related trials (z = −2.234, p = .026).

Table 4.

Mean (SD) naming accuracy (% correct) for control, agrammatic PPA, and logopenic PPA groups at each SOA.

Group SOA Overall accuracy
0 ms +100 ms +300 ms +500 ms
Control Related 97.5 (2.4) 98.3 (1.9) 98.5 (2.0) 98.9 (1.9) 98.3 (1.5)
Unrelated 98.3 (2.2) 98.5 (2.2) 98.4 (3.3) 99.2 (1.5) 98.6 (1.4)

Agrammatic PPA Related 89.9 (6.9) 89.5 (8.1) 88.3 (6.6) 90.9 (5.5) 89.7 (5.3)
Unrelated 85.7 (9.2) 85.8 (11.1) 90.4 (7.8) 90.4 (7.8) 88.1 (7.9)

Logopenic PPA Related 92.6 (10.4) 89.7 (9.1) 89.3 (11.0) 94.5 (6.9) 91.5 (9.2)
Unrelated 88.3 (9.7) 86.0 (15.5) 88.8 (11.0) 89.7 (13.2) 88.2 (11.9)

Error Analysis

Table 5 summarizes the frequency of each error type (percent of all responses containing a given error type) across groups. Kruskal-Wallis tests with adjusted p-values (False Discovery Rate (FDR)), revealed significant group differences in the frequency of phonological paraphasias (χ2 (2, N = 31) = 18.237, adjusted p < .001), phonological attempts (χ2 (2, N = 31) = 18.652, p < .001), self-corrections (χ2 (2, N = 31) = 10.563, p < .010), and non-responses (χ2 (2, N = 31) = 12.418, p < .005). In trials with phonologically related IS, 16.5% of phonological errors (paraphasias and attempts) were considered perseverations of the interfering stimulus (17 perseverative errors out of 103 phonological errors). Follow-up pairwise tests (Mann-Whitney U, FDR-adjusted p-values) revealed that both PPA groups exhibited more phonological errors and phonological attempts than the control group (p’s < .05). In addition, the PPA-G group produced more self-corrections than the control group (p = .007), and the PPA-L group produced more non-responses than the controls (p = .004). There were no other significant group differences.

Table 5.

Mean (SD) percent of total responses containing errors of each type, by group.

  Error Type
Group Phonological Paraphasia Semantic Paraphasia Neologism Phonological Attempt Self- correction Unrelated Non- Response Other
Control 0.1 (0.3) 0.6 (0.8) 0.0 (0.1) 0.2 (0.4) 0.2 (0.5) 0.1 (0.2) 0.3 (0.7) 0.0 (0.0)
PPA-G 2.6 (1.7) 1.5 (1.8) 0.2 (0.3) 3.7 (3.9) 1.7 (1.4) 0.7 (1.0) 0.8 (1.2) 0.2 (0.5)
PPA-L 1.1 (1.4) 0.9 (1.2) 0.3 (0.4) 1.6 (1.5) 0.7 (0.9) 0.4 (0.6) 4.9 (7.1) 0.2 (0.5)

Reaction time analyses

Table 6 shows the mean RT for related and unrelated trials at each SOA for each participant group. The model comparison procedure resulted in a model with significant main effects of group, SOA, relatedness, and all two-way interactions between these factors. There was no significant three-way interaction, and thus this term was excluded from the model. Naming latencies were higher for both PPA groups than for controls (PPA-G: t = 3.09, p = .002; PPA-L: t = 4.5, p < .001), but there was no difference between PPA groups (t = 0.99, p = .323).2 Faster RT’s were observed at later SOA’s (+300 ms: t = −7.407, p < .001; +500 ms: t = −8.310, p < .001) than at SOA 0, whereas RT’s at SOA 0 and +100 ms did not differ (t = .233, p < .816). In addition, naming latencies were higher on trials with phonologically unrelated, compared to phonologically related, IS (t = 5.622, p < .001). The group x SOA interaction was driven by the PPA-G group responding relatively slowly at SOA +100, +300, and +500 ms compared to SOA 0 ms (t = 2.309, p =.021; t = 5.018, p < .001; t = 2.090, p = .037, respectively). The interaction between SOA and relatedness was driven by smaller PF effects at SOA +300 and +500 ms compared to SOA 0 ms (t = −6.403, p < .001; t = −5.606, p < .001; respectively); SOA 0 and +100 ms did not differ (t = −1.255, p = .21). Finally, the group x relatedness interaction was due to larger PF effects (i.e., RT differences between unrelated and related IS) in the PPA-G group compared to controls (t = 2.893; p = .004); PF effects in the PPA-L group did not differ from controls (t = 1.077; p = .281). To determine which SOA’s were associated with larger PF effects in the PPA-G group, we ran the model separately on the data from each SOA. A group x relatedness interaction, reflecting larger PF effects in PPA-G participants relative to controls, was found at SOA +300 ms (t = 2.352; p = .019) but not at any other SOA (t’s < 1.6; p’s > .1).

Table 6.

Mean (SD) RTs for phonologically related and unrelated trials for control, agrammatic PPA and logopenic PPA groups at each SOA.

Group SOA
0 ms +100 ms +300 ms +500 ms
Control
N=17
Related 1040.0 (117.6) 1041.3 (129.5) 967.4 (105.2) 939.4 (96.3)
Unrelated 1094.6 (125.0) 1085.0 (123.4) 953.7 (110.9) 939.2 (93.4)

PF 54.6** 43.7** −13.7 −0.2

PPA-G
N=7
Related 1257.5 (183.9) 1320.0 (156.2) 1201.1 (188.6) 1124.9 (181.0)
Unrelated 1347.0 (180.1) 1436.8 (210.6) 1268.5 (213.3) 1152.0 (194.0)

PF 89.5* 116.8* 67.4* 27.1

PPA-L
N=7
Related 1366.0 (279.9) 1347.0 (227.1) 1161.2 (210.4) 1119.2 (237.5)
Unrelated 1474.1 (216.2) 1431.7 (200.1) 1176.6 (217.0) 1127.8 (232.7)

PF 108.1 84.7* 15.38 8.6

Note:

**

p < .01,

*

p < .05 (FDR-corrected)

To determine which groups showed significant PF effects at the different SOA’s, we performed paired two-tailed t-tests on the related vs. unrelated participant mean log RT’s for each group, using FDR correction for multiple comparisons. The control group showed significant PF effects at SOA 0 and +100 ms (adjusted p’s < .001), but no PF effects at SOA +300 (p = .061, a trend towards phonological interference) and +500 ms (p = .99). The PPA-G group exhibited significant PF effects at SOA 0, +100, and +300 ms (p’s = .020, .022, .021, respectively) and no PF effect at SOA +500 ms (p = .259). The participants with PPA-L showed a marginally significant PF effect at SOA 0 (p = 0.064) and a significant PF effect as SOA +100 ms (p = .021) but no effects at SOA +300 and +500 ms (p’s > .5). Figure 2 summarizes the observed PF effects across groups and SOA’s; for clarity, PF effects are represented as the mean (raw) RT in the related condition subtracted from the unrelated condition.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Phonological facilitation effects at each SOA for the control, agrammatic (PPA-G) and logopenic (PPA-L) groups. ** = p < .01, * = p < .05 (FDR-corrected).

Finally, we tested for correlations between PF effects at SOA +300 ms (the SOA in which abnormal PF effects were found in the PPA-G group) and measures of phonological processing abilities: word and nonword repetition from the NNB, three-syllable word repetition from the motor speech screening, phrase and sentence repetition ability (Rep6; Items 10–15 on the Repetition subset of the WAB), and the rate of phonological errors in both the experimental task and the narrative speech sample. In addition, to determine whether online phonological deficits were related to grammatical impairments (particularly in the PPA-G group), we calculated correlations between PF effects at SOA +300 ms and performance on the noncanonical items on the Sentence Production Priming Task of the NAVS. PF effects were calculated by subtracting the mean (z-score of log-transformed) RT in the related condition from the unrelated condition. These correlations were performed separately for each PPA group using Pearson correlations with FDR correction. No statistically significant correlations were found.

Discussion

One of the central goals of research in primary progressive aphasia is to identify the mechanism of the pervasive anomia associated with this disorder. In particular, it is important to determine whether deficits in lexical-semantic processing, phonological processing, or both are at the root of naming impairments in each of the three variants of PPA. Previous on-line studies have shown that lexical-semantic processing may be impaired even in non-semantic variants of PPA, i.e., PPA-G and PPA-L (Rogalski et al., 2008; Thompson et al., 2012b; Vandenberghe et al., 2005). In the present study, we investigated the phonological processes that support naming in the agrammatic (PPA-G) and logopenic (PPA-L) variants of PPA. Previous studies have suggested that phonological processing is differentially impaired in these subtypes of PPA (Ash et al., 2010; Clark et al., 2005; Croot et al., 2012; Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004, 2008; Mesulam et al., 2012; Rohrer et al., 2010; Wilson et al., 2010). However, phonological processing has not been studied using on-line tasks in this patient population, and thus little is known about the nature of phonological processing deficits in PPA-G and PPA-L (i.e., whether phonological word form retrieval and/or phonological encoding is impaired), and how these deficits contribute to anomia. In the present study, we used the picture-word interference paradigm (PWIP) to investigate phonological processing in real time during naming in people with PPA-G and PPA-L as well as age-matched healthy controls. In doing so, we were able to compare the time course and magnitude of phonological facilitation (PF) effects in both PPA subgroups with that of controls, with the aim of gaining insight into the source of naming deficits in PPA.

Consistent with previous studies (e.g., Damian & Martin, 1999; Hashimoto & Thompson, 2010; Lupker, 1982; Meyer & Schriefers, 1991; Rayner & Springer, 1986; Schriefers et al., 1990; Starreveld, 2000; Starreveld & La Heij, 1995, 1996), the control group performed at ceiling on the task and showed significant PF effects at early SOAs (0 and +100 ms). No significant PF effects at later SOAs (+300 and +500 ms) were found, supporting previous studies using the PWIP. These findings indicate that normal naming involves rapid phonological processing, i.e., phonological word form retrieval and phonological encoding.

Participants with PPA-G and PPA-L performed with comparable speed and accuracy on the naming task, with both groups responding more slowly and less accurately than the control group, indicating slowed and impaired processes supporting naming. The two PPA groups also exhibited a similarly high rate of phonological errors, reflecting phonological processing impairments in both groups. However, these off-line markers of phonological processing deficits were accompanied by abnormal PF effects only in the PPA-G group. Relative to the control group, the PPA-G, but not the PPA-L, group showed protracted PF effects. Like the controls and the PPA-L group, the PPA-G group exhibited significant PF effects at SOA=0 ms and +100 ms; however, the PPA-G group also exhibited an abnormal PF effect at +300 ms. This abnormal PF effect, emerging at a late SOA in PPA-G, likely reflect deficits in phonological encoding. Consistent with this interpretation, Laganaro and colleagues (2009), in an event-related potential (ERP) study using a picture-naming task with participants with phonological encoding deficits resulting from stroke, found that the ERP signal in these participants began to deviate from that of unimpaired controls around 290 ms after picture onset. In addition, these results are consistent with neurological evidence indicating that the left inferior frontal gyrus, a typical site of cortical atrophy in PPA-G (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004, 2008, 2011; Mesulam et al., 2009, 2012; Rohrer et al., 2010), supports phonological encoding during naming (Indefrey, 2011; Indefrey & Levelt, 2004; Papoutsi et al., 2009). Interestingly, PF effects at SOA +300 ms were not correlated with grammatical ability (noncanonical sentence production), suggesting that phonological and grammatical processing deficits may be independent in PPA-G.

PF effects at SOA +300 ms also were not correlated with off-line measures of phonological processing ability, including phrase and sentence repetition and the rate of phonological errors in the experimental task and narrative speech samples, which were impaired to some extent in both PPA patient groups. This finding is not surprising in that performance on off-line measures reflects several components of phonological processing, including phonological working memory, word form retrieval, and encoding. Indeed, on-line measures using the word interference paradigm are more sensitive for identifying specific processing impairments than off-line measures. Specifically, PF effects in the present study likely reflect spreading activation between segments (Meyer & Schriefers, 1991; Roelofs, 1997; Schriefers et al., 1990), rather than between lexical items (Levelt et al., 1999; Starreveld, 2000; Starreveld & La Heij, 1995, 1996), suggesting that such effects reflect phonological encoding (a segmental process), but not phonological word form retrieval (a lexical process). It is plausible, and the present data suggest, that phonological deficits in PPA-L stem from phonological word form retrieval rather than phonological encoding, whereas individuals with PPA-G present with the opposite deficit pattern: impaired phonological encoding. This behavioral pattern coincides with cortical atrophy in PPA. PPA-L is associated with atrophy in the left temporo-parietal junction (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2004, 2008, 2011; Mesulam et al., 2009, 2012; Rohrer et al., 2010), which has been argued to support phonological word form retrieval (Graves, Grabowski, Mehta, & Gordon, 2007; Graves, Grabowski, Mehta, & Gupta, 2008; Indefrey, 2011; Indefrey & Levelt, 2004; Wilson, Isenberg, & Hickok, 2009), and PPA-G is associated with atrophy in the left IFG, argued to be engaged to support phonological encoding as well as subsequent phonetic encoding and articulatory processes (Indefrey & Levelt, 2004; see also Hickok & Poeppel, 2007; Patpoutsi et al., 2009). However, fMRI studies using the PWIP have reported decreased activation in left posterior temporal cortex associated with phonological facilitation (de Zubicaray & McMahon, 2009; de Zubicaray, McMahon, Eastburn, & Wilson, 2002, cf. Bles & Jansma, 2008; though see Abel et al., 2009; note that priming effects are typically associated with decreased activation in regions supporting stimulus processing; e.g., Lebreton, Desgranges, Landeo, Baron, & Eustache, 2001). Thus, further research is needed to determine whether atrophy in the temporo-parietal junction in PPA-L results in impaired phonological word form retrieval.

Conclusion

Previous studies have suggested that the agrammatic and logopenic variants of PPA are associated with phonological processing deficits. The present study used a picture-word interference paradigm to test whether people with PPA-G and PPA-L show evidence of impairments at different substages of phonological processing as naming unfolds. Compared to healthy control participants, participants with PPA-G exhibited protracted PF effects, which may reflect impaired phonological encoding. This processing deficit may be caused by atrophy within the left inferior frontal gyrus, which has been argued to support phonological encoding. Despite phonological deficits evident in off-line measures, the PPA-L group exhibited normal on-line PF effects. These findings indicate that deficits in phonological processing may contribute to anomia in both agrammatic and logopenic variants of PPA, but highlight important differences in the source of these deficits for the two patient types. Importantly, these differences may associate with unique patterns of naming (and other linguistic) declination as well as neural degeneration.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the following NIH grants: RO1DC01948-18 (C.K. Thompson), R01DC008551 (M. Mesulam), and AG13854 (Alzheimer’s Disease Core Center, Northwestern University).

Appendix. Target stimuli and interfering stimuli

A1. Living Targets
Related IS Unrelated IS
SOA 0 SOA +100 SOA +300 SOA +500 SOA 0 SOA +100 SOA +300 SOA +500
1 beetle being beacon beaker behalf towel guitar hormone sofa
2 broccoli broadcast bronchitis bronze brothel skeleton champion gravity pharmacy
3 cactus caddie candle camera campus magic fountain ribbon pizza
4 camel cannon candor cabbage canvas fashion lagoon detour wallet
5 cat cap cab cash can gas boot hoop jet
6 chicken chin chip chisel chimney whisper shower plastic glacier
7 elephant element elegant elderly eloquence umbrella adventure insurance oxygen
8 grape grade grace grain grate brick sleep plate shell
9 horse hoard horn horror horizon vase roof gift watch
10 lemon ledger lesson lecture leopard tower sewer cassette mansion
11 lion liar libel lighter lightning curtain magnet rocket tool
12 mushroom muffler mustard muscle mugger compass denim napkin congress
13 orange organ orbit order orphan iron album engine aspirin
14 pumpkin punch puppy public publisher tablet jelly harpoon mountain
15 rabbit racket rattle raft rally basket circle filter nickel
16 raccoon rapture rampart ransom rancher harbor motel lapel quarter
17 rose rope rogue roach robe card paste beach tire
18 sheep sheet sheen sheath sheaf phone chalk bridge wheel
19 spider spice spike spine spiral photo thermos planet prairie
20 tomato tobacco toboggan touch tongue galaxy jewellery volcano cathedral
A2. Nonliving Targets
Related IS Unrelated IS
SOA 0 SOA +100 SOA +300 SOA +500 SOA 0 SOA +100 SOA +300 SOA +500
1 anchor anger angle ancient ankle umpire infant elbow orchard
2 balloon bassoon banana barrage baton gender sausage pirate nutmeg
3 belt bet bell bend bench fig cook toe colt
4 bowl boat bone boa bolt run leaf jaw ranch
5 bucket bubble budget buddy butter cobra dentist tenant pony
6 crib crick cripple critic crimson drug fruit knight shrimp
7 desk depth deaf debt deck paint jazz fight beast
8 hammer habit hamster hanger hamper turnip diary waitress baboon
9 ladder lantern landscape laughter lather fossil devil hero riddle
10 lamp lab lamb latch lack yawn bomb hook dove
11 pants past pass patch path kiss mail hound tomb
12 pen pet pest pedestal peasant rain germ cell joke
13 pencil petal pebble pellet pedal turtle servant hockey lobster
14 pillow pigeon picnic pistol pickle wizard hippie fiber honey
15 plunger plug plum plus plunder shepherd grammar rhubarb knuckle
16 robot roller roman romance rodent zero bachelor warden tortoise
17 ruler ruin rumor ruby rubric coffee reptile lawyer poison
18 sock sob sod solve song king tea beak tooth
19 tank tab tack tap tag pork judge beard hug
20 tie tide title tile tights sand nurse ear hen
A3. Fillers
Related IS Unrelated IS
SOA 0 SOA +100 SOA +300 SOA +500 SOA 0 SOA +100 SOA +300 SOA +500
1 bench best beck belch bend noon gin valve disc
2 cannon cackle capsule captor caption kernel soldier vision supper
3 clock cloth clog clot closet glass bleach plant sleeve
4 donut dome donor domain docent valley digit neon cushion
5 leaf leak league lease leash toy wing golf dime
6 octopus octagon occupant octave octane accountant universe idealist editor
7 rake race rage rate rave gold waist deal mess
8 sun sum suck suds subject call dirt bunk vote
9 watch wad wand waft wasp booth tar hole deed
10 whistle whimper whimsy whip whisky princess grocer fragrance chapel

Footnotes

1

Imageability scores were unavailable for 130 stimulus items.

2

Overall group RT’s were compared using an otherwise identical model in which the dependent variable was the log-transformed RT (rather than the z-score of the log-transformed RT).

References

  1. Abel S, Dressel K, Bitzer R, Kümmerer D, Mader I, Weiller C, Huber W. The separation of processing stages in a lexical interference fMRI-paradigm. Neuroimage. 2009 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ash S, McMillan C, Gunawardena D, Avants B, Morgan B, Khan A, et al. Speech errors in progressive non-fluent aphasia. Brain and Language. 2010;113:13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baayen RH. LanguageR: Data sets and functions with “Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics”. 2010 Available from http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/languageR/index.html.
  4. Baayen RH, Milin P. Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research. 2010;3.2:12–28. [Google Scholar]
  5. Baayen RH, Piepenbrock R, van Rijn H. The CELEX Lexical Database (Release 1) Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  6. Benton A, Hamsher KS, Varney N, Spreen O. Contributions to neuropsychological assessment. 2. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bi Y, Xu Y, Caramazza A. Orthographic and phonological effects in the picture-word interference paradigm: Evidence from a logographic language. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2009;30:637–658. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bles M, Jansma BM. Phonological processing of ignored distractor pictures; an fMRI investigation. BMC Neuroscience. 2008;9:20. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Biran M, Friedmann N. From phonological paraphasias to the structure of the phonological output lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes. 2005;20:589–616. [Google Scholar]
  10. Boersma Paul, Weenink David. Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program] 2010 Version 5.0, retrieved 29 October 2010 from http://www.praat.org/
  11. Brambati SM, Ogar J, Neuhaus J, Miller BL, Gorno-Tempini ML. Reading disorders in primary progressive aphasia: A behavioral and neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:1893–1900. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Caramazza A, Hillis AE. Where do semantic errors come from? Cortex. 1990;26:95–122. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80077-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Clark DG, Charuvastra A, Miller BL, Shapira JS, Mendez MF. Fluent versus nonfluent primary progressive aphasia: A comparison of clinical and functional neuroimaging features. Brain and Language. 2005;94:54–60. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2004.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Croot K, Ballard K, Leyton CE, Hodges JR. Apraxia of speech and phonological errors in the diagnosis of nonfluent/agrammatic and logopenic variants of primary progressive aphasia. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 2012;55:1562–1572. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0323). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dabul BL. Apraxia Battery for Adults, Second Edition (ABA-2) Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  16. Damian MF, Martin RC. Semantic and phonological codes interact in single word production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1999;25:345–361. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.25.2.345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dell GS, Schwartz MF, Martin N, Saffran EM, Gagnon DA. Lexical access in aphasic and non-aphasic speakers. Psychological Review. 1997;104:801–838. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.104.4.801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fiebach CJ, Friederici AD, Müller K, von Cramon DY. fMRI evidence for dual routes to the mental lexicon in visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14:11–23. doi: 10.1162/089892902317205285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Glaser WR, Düngelhoff FJ. The time course of picture–word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1984;10:640–654. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.10.5.640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Goldrick M, Rapp B. A restricted interaction account (RIA) of spoken word production: The best of both worlds. Aphasiology. 2002;16:20–55. [Google Scholar]
  22. Golper LC, Rau MT, Erskine B, Langhans JJ, Houlihan J. Aphasic patients’ performance on a mental status examination. Clinical Aphasiology. 1987;17:124–135. [Google Scholar]
  23. Gorno-Tempini ML, Brambati SM, Ginex V, Ogar J, Dronkers NF, Marcone A, Perani D, Garibotto V, Cappa SF, Miller BL. The logopenic/phonological variant of primary progressive aphasia. Neurology. 2008;71:1227–1234. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000320506.79811.da. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gorno-Tempini ML, Dronkers NF, Rankin KP, Ogar JM, Phengrasamy L, Rosen HJ, Johnson JK, Weiner MW, Miller BL. Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Annals of Neurology. 2004;55:335–346. doi: 10.1002/ana.10825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gorno-Tempini ML, Hillis AE, Weintraub S, Kertesz A, Mendez M, Cappa SF, Ogar JM, Rohrer JD, Black S, Boeve BF, Manes F, Dronkers NF, Vandenberghe R, Rascovsky K, Patterson K, Miller BL, Knopman DS, Hodges JR, Mesulam MM, Grossman M. Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology. 2011;76:1006–1014. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821103e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Graves WW, Grabowski TJ, Mehta S, Gordon JK. A neural signature of phonological access: Distinguishing the effects of word frequency from familiarity and length in overt picture naming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2007;19:617–631. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.4.617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Graves WW, Grabowski TJ, Mehta S, Gupta P. The left posterior superior temporal gyrus participates specifically in accessing lexical phonology. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;20:1698–1710. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hashimoto N, Thompson CK. The use of picture-word interference to examine naming abilities in aphasic individuals. Aphasiology. 2010;24:580–611. doi: 10.1080/02687030902777567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Heim S, Alter K, Ischebeck AK, Amunts K, Eickhoff SB, Mohlberg H, Zilles K, von Cramon DY, Friederici AD. The role of left Brodmann’s areas 44 and 45 in reading words and pseudowords. Cognitive Brain Research. 2005;25:982–993. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hickok G, Poeppel D. The cortical organization of speech perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2007;8:393–402. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Howard D, Patterson K. The Pyramids and Palm Trees Test. San Antonio, TX: Pearson; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  32. Indefrey P. The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components: A critical update. Frontiers in Psychology. 2011;2:255. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Indefrey P, Levelt WJM. The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition. 2004;92:101–144. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2002.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jaeger TF. Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language. 2008;59:434–446. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Janse E. Lexical competition effects in aphasia: Deactivation of lexical candidates in spoken word processing. Brain and Language. 2006;97:1– 11. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  37. Kay J, Lesser R, Coltheart M. PALPA: Psycholinguistic Assessments of Language Processing in Aphasia. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  38. Kertesz A. Western Aphasia Battery-Revised. Austin-Tx: Pro-Ed; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  39. Laganaro M, Morand S, Schwitter V, Zimmerman C, Camen C, Schnider A. Electrophysiological correlates of different anomic patterns in comparison with normal word production. Cortex. 2009;45:697–707. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lebreton K, Desgranges B, Landeau B, Baron JC, Eustache F. Visual priming within and across symbolic format using a tachistoscopic picture identification task: A PET study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2001;13:670–686. doi: 10.1162/089892901750363226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Levelt WJM. Accessing words in speech production: Stages, processes, and representations. Cognition. 1992;42:1–22. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90038-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Levelt WJM. Models of word production. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 1999;3:223–232. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01319-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Levelt WJM, Roelofs A, Meyer AS. A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral & Brain Sciences. 1999;22:1–75. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99001776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Love T, Swinney D, Walenski W, Zurif E. How left inferior frontal cortex participates in syntactic processing: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language. 2008;107:203–219. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lupker SJ. The semantic nature of response competition in the picture–word interference task. Memory & Cognition. 1979;7:485–495. [Google Scholar]
  46. Lupker SJ. The role of phonetic and orthographic similarity in picture–word interference. Canadian Journal of Psychology. 1982;36:349–367. doi: 10.1037/h0080652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Mesulam MM. Slowly progressive aphasia without generalized dementia. Annals of Neurology. 1982;11:592–598. doi: 10.1002/ana.410110607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mesulam MM. Primary progressive aphasia: A language-based dementia. New England Journal of Medicine. 2003;348:1535–1542. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra022435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mesulam MM. Primary progressive aphasia: A 25-year retrospective. Alzheimer’s Disease and Associated Disorders. 2007;21:S8–S11. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31815bf7e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Mesulam MM, Wieneke C, Rogalski E, Cobia D, Thompson C, Weintraub S. Quantitative template for subtyping primary progressive aphasia. Archives of Neurology. 2009;66:1545–1551. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Mesulam M-M, Wieneke C, Thompson CK, Rogalski E, Weintraub S. Quantitative classification of primary progressive aphasia at early and mild impairment stages. Brain. 2012 doi: 10.1093/brain/aws080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mesulam MM, Wieneke C, Hurley R, Rademaker A, Thompson CK, Weintraub S, Rogalski EJ. Words and objects at the tip of the left temporal lobe in primary progressive aphasia. Brain. 2013;136:601–618. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Meyer AS, Schriefers H. Phonological facilitation in picture-word interference experiments: Effect of stimulus onset asynchrony and type of interfering stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition. 1991;17:1146–1160. [Google Scholar]
  54. Milberg W, Blumstein SE, Dworetzsky B. Phonological processing and lexical access in aphasia. Brain and Language. 1988;34:279–293. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(88)90139-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Osher JE, Wicklund AH, Rademaker A, Johnson N, Weintraub S. The mini-mental state examination in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and primary progressive aphasia. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and other Dementias. 2008;122:468–73. doi: 10.1177/1533317507307173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Papoutsi M, de Zwart JA, Jansma M, Pickering MJ, Bednar JA, Horwitz B. From phonemes to articulatory codes: An fMRI study of the role of Broca’s area in speech production. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19:2156–2165. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Prather PA, Zurif E, Love T, Brownell H. Speed of lexical activation in nonfluent Broca’s aphasia and fluent Wernicke’s aphasia. Brain and Language. 1997;59:391–411. doi: 10.1006/brln.1997.1751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Rapp B, Goldrick M. Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. Psychological Review. 2000;107:460–499. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.107.3.460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Rayner E, Springer CJ. Graphemic and semantic similarity effects in the picture–word interference task. British Journal of Psychology. 1986;77:207–222. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb01995.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Reitan RM. Validity of the Trail-Making Test as an indication of organic brain damage. Perceptual Motor Skills. 1958;8:271–276. [Google Scholar]
  61. Roelofs A. The WEAVER ++ model of word-form encoding in speech production. Cognition. 1997;64:249–284. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(97)00027-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Rogalski E, Rademaker A, Mesulam MM, Weintraub S. Covert processing of words and pictures in nonsemantic variants of primary progressive aphasia. Alzheimers Disease and Associated Disorders. 2008;22:343–351. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31816c92f7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Rohrer JD, Ridgway GR, Crutch SJ, Hailstone J, Goll JC, Clarkson MJ, Mead S, Beck J, Mummery C, Ourselin S, Warrington EK, Rossor MN, Warren JD. Progressive logopenic/phonological aphasia: Erosion of the language network. Neuroimage. 2010;49:984–993. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Rosinski RR, Michnick-Golinkoff R, Kukish KS. Automatic semantic processing in a picture-word interference task. Child Development. 1975;46:247–253. [Google Scholar]
  65. Schriefers H, Meyer AS, Levelt WJM. Exploring the time course of lexical access in language production: Picture-word interference studies. Journal of Memory and Langauge. 1990;21:686–698. [Google Scholar]
  66. Schuchard J, Thompson CK. Implicit and explicit learning in individuals with agrammatic aphasia. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 2013 doi: 10.1007/s10936-013-9248-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Seeley WW, Crawford RK, Zhou J, Miller BL, Grecius WD. Neurodegenerative diseases target large-scale human brain networks. Neuron. 2009;62:42–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Shim H, Hurley RS, Rogalski E, Mesulam MM. Anatomic, clinical, and neuropsychological correlates of spelling errors in primary progressive aphasia. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50:1929–1935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Starreveld PA. On the interpretation of onsets of auditory context effects in word production. Journal of Memory and Language. 2000;42:497–525. [Google Scholar]
  70. Starreveld PA, La Heij W. Semantic interference, orthographic facilitation, and their interaction in naming tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1995;21:686–698. [Google Scholar]
  71. Starreveld PA, La Heij W. Time-course analysis of semantic and orthographic context effects in picture naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1996;22:896–918. [Google Scholar]
  72. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1935;18:643–662. [Google Scholar]
  73. Thompson CK, Ballard KJ, Tait ME, Weintraub S, Mesulam MM. Patterns of language decline in non-fluent primary progressive aphasia. Aphasiology. 1997;11:297–321. [Google Scholar]
  74. Thompson CK, Cho S, Hsu CJ, Wieneke M, Rademaker A, Weitner BB, et al. Dissociations between fluency and agrammatism in primary progressive aphasia. Aphasiology. 2012a;26:20–43. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2011.584691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Thompson CK, Cho S, Price C, Wieneke C, Bonakdarpour B, Rogalski E, et al. Semantic interference during object naming in agrammatic and logopenic primary progressive aphasia (PPA) Brain and Language. 2012b;120:237–270. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Thompson CK, Shapiro LP, Li L, Schendel L. Analysis of verbs and verb-argument structure: A method for quantification of aphasic language production. Clinical Aphasiology. 1995;23:121–140. [Google Scholar]
  77. Vandenberghe R, Vandenbulcke M, Weintraub S, Johnson N, Porke K, Thompson CK, Mesulam MM. Paradoxical features of word finding difficulty in primary progressive aphasia. Annals of Neurology. 2005;57:204–209. doi: 10.1002/ana.20362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale®. 3. San Antonio, TX: Pearson; 1997. (WAIS®–III) [Google Scholar]
  79. Wertz RT, LaPointe LL, Rosenbeck JC. Apraxia of speech: The disorder and its management. New York: Grune and Statton; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  80. Wilshire CE, Keall LM, Stuart EJ, O’Donnell DJ. Exploring the dynamics of aphasic word production using the picture–word interference task: A case study. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:939–953. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.08.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Wilson SM, Isenberg AL, Hickok G. Neural correlates of word production stages delineated by parametric modulation of linguistic variables. Human Brain Mapping. 2009;30:3496–3608. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Wilson SM, Henry ML, Besbris M, Ogar JM, Dronkers NF, Jarrold W, et al. Connected speech production in three variants of primary progressive aphasia. Brain. 2010;133:2069–2088. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Yee E, Blumstein SE, Sedivy JC. Lexical-semantic activation in Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;20:592–612. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. de Zubicaray GI, McMahon KL. Auditory context effects in picture naming investigated with event-related fMRI. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009;9:260–269. doi: 10.3758/CABN.9.3.260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. de Zubicaray GI, McMahon KL, Eastburn MM, Wilson SJ. Orthographic/phonological facilitation of naming responses in the picture-word task: An event-related fMRI study using overt vocal responding. Neuroimage. 2002;16:1084–1093. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES