Abstract
Decades of research have established a model that includes the medial temporal lobe, and particularly the hippocampus, as a critical node for episodic memory. Neuroimaging and clinical studies have shown the involvement of additional cortical and subcortical regions. Among these areas, the thalamus, the retrosplenial cortex, and the prefrontal cortices have been consistently related to episodic memory performance. This article provides evidences that these areas are in different forms and degrees critical for human memory function rather than playing only an ancillary role. First we briefly summarize the functional architecture of the medial temporal lobe with respect to recognition memory and recall. We then focus on the clinical and neuroimaging evidence available on thalamo-prefrontal and thalamo-retrosplenial networks. The role of these networks in episodic memory has been considered secondary, partly because disruption of these areas does not always lead to severe impairments; to account for this evidence, we discuss methodological issues related to the investigation of these regions. We propose that these networks contribute differently to recognition memory and recall, and also that the memory stage of their contribution shows specificity to encoding or retrieval in recall tasks. We note that the same mechanisms may be in force when humans perform non-episodic tasks, e.g., semantic retrieval and mental time travel. Functional disturbance of these networks is related to cognitive impairments not only in neurological disorders, but also in psychiatric medical conditions, such as schizophrenia. Finally we discuss possible mechanisms for the contribution of these areas to memory, including regulation of oscillatory rhythms and long-term potentiation. We conclude that integrity of the thalamo-frontal and the thalamo-retrosplenial networks is necessary for the manifold features of episodic memory.
Keywords: recognition memory, recollection, familiarity, recall, thalamus, retrosplenial cortex, prefrontal cortex, schizophrenia
The only proof of there being retention is that recall actually takes place
(James, 1890).
Memory is a fascinating puzzle for neuroscientists: at first glance it seems straightforward to grasp the unity of this cognitive skill and its adaptive meaning; one is prompted to search for the storage room in the brain, like it happened in the beginning of memory research. Today there is consensus that many memory systems rely on dissociable neural substrates (Squire and Kandel, 2000). Within cognitive neuroscience, different research fields kept searching for a main character of the “memory play.” The best candidate for episodic memory, as defined by Tulving (1987, 2002) and Tulving and Markowitsch (1997, 1998), has been the hippocampus (HC1; Scoville and Milner, 1957).
Destruction of the HC is sufficient to wipe out novel episodic learning in humans and non-human primates (Mishkin, 1982; Zola-Morgan et al., 1982; reviewed by Aggleton and Brown, 1999). A less straightforward question is whether the loss of hippocampal function is necessary for episodic memory impairments. Damage to other regions, including the cortices of the parahippocampal gyrus, the prefrontal cortex (PFC), the thalamus, and the retrosplenial and posterior cingulate cortex (RSC), may also result in memory deficits (reviewed by Aggleton and Brown, 1999; Kopelman, 2002; Van der Werf et al., 2003a; Eichenbaum et al., 2007; Mitchell and Johnson, 2009; Vann et al., 2009a; Brown et al., 2010). All of these regions presumably perform operations that differ at least to some extent, so what we call “episodic memory” is the result of a number of sub-functions underlay by many brain regions.
A logical consequence is that damage to different brain areas leads to qualitatively different episodic memory impairments; this argument also applies to brain activations detected by means of neuroimaging techniques, which will overlap to a large extent, but not completely, depending on subtle task differences. One strategy to attack this complexity is to “average” the evidence and single out brain regions which regularly contribute to episodic memory. This way of proceeding is robust with respect to the inferences drawn, e.g., we can predict that surgical ablation of the HC will instantiate amnesia. However, this procedure is insufficient to study more fine-grained mechanisms underlying episodic memory, for example as a function of stimulus material or memory stage. This article reviews evidence on the involvement of other brain structures, aside from the HC, in what is the hallmark of HC-dependent memory: recall.
We will discuss clinical and neuroimaging literature about brain networks supporting different aspects of recall, particularly a thalamic-PFC and a thalamic-RSC network. A main tenet of this work is that interpretation of the results strictly depends on the tasks used to assess memory function. We will therefore argue that empirical work needs to study recall directly and to assess the neurophysiological correlates of recall subprocesses, in order to identify the mechanisms by which different brain regions contribute to recall. Damage to the thalamo-PFC and to the thalamo-RSC networks also impairs other cognitive functions beyond episodic memory, and hypotheses on the mechanisms of contribution of these networks to cognition are discussed in the final part of the review.
The Main Actor – A Brief Reappraisal on the Role of the Medial Temporal Lobe in Recognition Memory and Recall
Since Mandler’s (1980) proposal to distinguish a form of recognition accompanied by retrieval of contextual and associative information (recollection) and one more implicit-like, simply consisting of the feeling that something is “old” or “new” (familiarity), the distinction between familiarity and recollection has been investigated widely in cognitive neuroscience. Increasing evidence has emerged in recent years supporting this “dual process model” of recognition memory (Yonelinas, 2002; Eichenbaum et al., 2007; Suchan et al., 2008; Ranganath, 2010; Voss and Paller, 2010). The dual process model assumes that the two processes are qualitatively different. Familiarity is graded and not well suited for associative memory; recollection accomplishes lively and detailed retrieval and is thought to be a threshold process. The main alternative view, the “single process account,” assumes a quantitative difference between recollection and familiarity, i.e., stronger memory traces elicit a feeling of recollection (Squire et al., 2007; Slotnick, 2013). Proponents of this view acknowledge that different behavioral outcomes indicate different neural processing within the MTL (Wixted et al., 2010). They stress, however, that tests commonly used introduce confounds in the form of different strength of the memory trace. In general, the techniques used to separately assess recollection and familiarity are debated; for a more complete picture of this controversy it is best to consult more focused reviews (Eichenbaum et al., 2007; Squire et al., 2007; Aggleton et al., 2010; Brown et al., 2010, 2012; Montaldi and Mayes, 2010; Wixted et al., 2010; Rugg et al., 2012; Slotnick, 2013).
The MTL includes the HC and the parahippocampal cortices. The neocortical areas that send inputs into and receive outputs from the MTL include all higher order “association” areas and no primary sensory (except for the olfactory) or motor cortices. This connectivity pattern is illustrated in Figure 1. The HC is on top of this information flow (Lavenex and Amaral, 2000; Witter et al., 2000; Eichenbaum and Lipton, 2008). The outputs of hippocampal processing are directed back down this information path in reversed order.
It is debated whether the distinct processes subserved by the HC and the parahippocampal cortices correspond to recollection and familiarity (Yonelinas et al., 1998; Holdstock et al., 2002, 2005; Mayes et al., 2002; Yonelinas, 2002; Davachi et al., 2003; Bastin et al., 2004; Ranganath et al., 2004; Aggleton et al., 2005; Uncapher and Rugg, 2005a,b; Montaldi et al., 2006; Uncapher et al., 2006). There is agreement, however, that associative memory encoding tasks recruit the HC, relative to tasks without requirement (or success) of associative encoding (Henke et al., 1997, 1999; Sperling et al., 2003; Achim and Lepage, 2005; Chua et al., 2007). The neural substrates of recognition memory also depend on more subtle differences related to stimuli presentation (Henderson et al., 2003; Cipolotti et al., 2006; Bird et al., 2007; Peters et al., 2007a,b; Awipi and Davachi, 2008). Of interest to the empirical setting of novel investigations, there is consensus that recall and recognition are dissociable cognitive skills, and that the HC is necessary for recall, but likely insufficient. Yonelinas et al. (2010), for instance, highlighted the role of the PFC in recognition memory, concluding that the extant evidence favors a prefrontal contribution to recollection, in particular during encoding.
Thus, the constituents of the MTL play different roles in recognition memory and recall, with a major role of the HC in recall. The contribution of other areas of the MTL to recognition memory is intensely debated. Beyond the MTL, there is evidence on the involvement of other brain areas in recall. This will be the subject of Section “A Crowded Stage – Evidence for Other Recognition and Recall Networks.”
Intricated Plots – How to Assess the Contribution of Thalamo-Cortical Networks to Memory
Since this review focuses on clinical and neuroimaging evidence, we will briefly discuss the impact of testing procedures on results derived by these techniques.
Recognition memory tasks fall into two major categories: subjective and objective (Eichenbaum et al., 2007). Subjective tasks rely on self-assessment of memory traces, and include for instance the remember/know paradigm (Tulving, 1987) and the receiving operating curves based on confidence levels (Yonelinas, 2002). Objective paradigms, instead, test directly for memory of associations related to the recognition cue; source memory and recall tests are examples of this kind of experimental procedure.
In remember/know paradigms subjects are instructed to assess whether their memory is more based on conscious associations or on “feelings” of familiarity, entailing the sensation that one “knows” an item but does not “remember” anything about it. These paradigms have been criticized because they may rather tap subjective awareness of one’s memory than “true” features of the memory trace (Gardiner, 1988; Newell and Dunn, 2008; Geraci et al., 2009; McCabe and Geraci, 2009). Moreover, memory strength might confound remember/know results (Slotnick, 2013).
Receiving operating curves, on the other hand, cannot discriminate between “recollected” and “recognized” stimuli, but provide a global estimation of familiarity and recollection in one condition, individual, or group. The estimates are based on the notion that recollection is a threshold process (Yonelinas et al., 2010), an assumption that did not fail to trigger criticism (Wixted et al., 2010; Slotnick, 2013), although recent fMRI evidence appears to support it (Pustina et al., 2012). On the other hand, the idea that lower confidence involves greater familiarity is still prone to the alternative interpretation that recollection simply reflects greater memory strength (Wixted, 2007).
Montaldi et al. (2006) and Kafkas and Montaldi (2012) developed a subjective task that, like Remember/Know, entails training subjects to distinguish recollection from familiarity. Participants focus on familiarity and assess their confidence (from one to three), while recollection should be avoided. If a participant detects that he/she has been using recollection, he/she reports this. This task effectively matches memory strength, with the caveat that subjects are focusing on familiarity: in objective tasks they are usually actively engaged in recall. This novel subjective task appears to be a promising tool to study familiarity free of the “memory strength” confound. Results obtained on “recollection” trials by using this task, however, share the general limitations of subjective tasks and introduce the additional feature that recollection is undesired. Participants’ orientation at encoding and retrieval may be different from what is typical in objective tasks.
Objective paradigms are more powerful than subjective paradigms in indicating recollection. In particular, objective paradigms allow trial-by-trial discrimination of recollection by asking subjects to report features associated with the recognition cue, which may be perceptual (e.g., color; Cycowicz et al., 2001), contextual (e.g., place where the cue was previously shown; Cansino et al., 2002), semantic (e.g., match or mismatch with the category of other items; Pergola et al., 2013b), and episodic (e.g., decision taken during previous exposure or imagination of the items; Vilberg and Rugg, 2012). There is general agreement that, when memory strength and confidence are equated, recollection is more likely to be involved in recognition memory followed by recall, as compared to familiarity (Brown et al., 2010; Wixted et al., 2010).
Nevertheless, objective paradigms present a certain degree of variability, and subtle details can change the pattern of activations in a neuroimaging study, as well as the pattern of deficits displayed by clinical samples. For example, it has been shown that some forced choice tasks also entail neural correlates typical of familiarity (Quamme et al., 2007; Diana et al., 2008). Mayes et al. (2007) proposed that this might happen because of lower-level relational operations performed in the parahippocampal and perirhinal cortices. Items belonging to the same context (e.g., steer and brakes in a car) might be encoded already at the level of the perirhinal cortex: overlearned associations require less integration. This argument extends to material learned through “unitized” representation (e.g., the word association sun-set compared to sun-toy). Moreover, remembering the information associated with a recognition cue when choosing between two and three possibilities is prone to guessing influence. Recall tasks which require retrieval of a unique association, instead, are robust with respect to guesses and to familiarity (Montaldi and Mayes, 2010), a strategy recently used in both clinical and neuroimaging setting (Pergola et al., 2012, 2013b,d). The trials in which correct recall occurs are most likely recollection trials, although the converse is not true: it is possible that a participant recognizes the cue based on recall, but not on recall of the information tested, e.g., an associated picture or context (non-criterial recollection: see Yonelinas et al., 2010).
In our opinion, investigations of the recollection/familiarity dichotomy are made difficult by the pragmatic definitions of recollection and familiarity. Both are thought to support recognition, and additionally recollection is thought to support recall. Hence evidence of recall is commonly used to infer recollection; lack of recall is used to infer familiarity (see for example Slotnick, 2013). However, in order to show the involvement of a brain structure in familiarity processing, it is necessary to find exclusive correlates of familiarity – something that recollection cannot support. It is our impression that consensus on such specialized features of familiarity has not yet been reached. Putative correlates of familiarity that have been disputed include reaction times, responses given under time pressure, differential modulation of responses induced by perceptual manipulation, and electrophysiological components (Eichenbaum et al., 2007; Squire et al., 2007; Brown et al., 2010, 2012; Montaldi and Mayes, 2010; Wixted et al., 2010; Paller et al., 2012; Rugg et al., 2012). The paradigms that seem most successful in detecting familiarity are subjective tasks (Yonelinas, 2002; Montaldi et al., 2006), within the limitations discussed. Hypotheses about the involvement of select brain structures in familiarity appear empirically ill-posed, if they must rely on lack of evidence of recollection.
Following this framework, in the following we will differentiate recall (i.e., recognition memory followed by recall of contextual or associative details) and recognition without recall (i.e., correct judgment of previous occurrence not followed by successful retrieval of contextual and associative information). These behavioral outcomes are easier to probe empirically than the putative underlying processes (recollection and familiarity). It is important for the following of the article to keep in mind this distinction between behavior and underlying processes.
A Crowded Stage – Evidence for Other Recognition and Recall Networks
The earliest observations on non-hippocampal based amnesia relate to the “Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome,” a degenerative disease caused by depletion of thiamine (often secondary to alcohol abuse; for reviews see Kopelman, 2002; Kopelman et al., 2009). Amnesic symptoms in Korsakoff patients have been related to damage in the mammillary bodies and the anterior nuclei of the thalamus (abbreviated in the following as AT; Victor et al., 1989; Harding et al., 2000). Another clinical condition leading to amnesia is thalamic stroke (reviewed by Schmahmann, 2003; Carlesimo et al., 2011). Behavioral symptoms include anterograde amnesia, executive deficits, and rarely retrograde memory loss. Implicit memory is mostly preserved, much like in hippocampal amnesia (Daum and Ackermann, 1994; but see Exner et al., 2001).
Aggleton and Brown (1999) challenged the distinction between medial temporal and subcortical amnesias by proposing that the AT are functionally linked to the HC (i.e., critical for recollection), while the mediodorsal nucleus (MD) contributes more to familiarity based on its connections to the perirhinal cortex. This model has been recently revised (see Aggleton et al., 2011, for an update). We will now evaluate the extant evidence on the role of the MD and the AT in recognition memory in the framework of the thalamo-PFC and the thalamo-RSC networks.
The thalamo-prefrontal network
The conspicuous evidence delineating the anatomical basis of the thalamic-PFC network has already been discussed elsewhere (Taber et al., 2004; Byne et al., 2009; Klein et al., 2010; Barbas et al., 2012). Figure 2 illustrates the main patterns of anatomical connection of this network.
At least three important circuits involved in episodic memory relate the thalamus and the PFC. The first is the reciprocal MD-PFC connection, which shows corresponding thalamic medio-lateral and prefrontal ventromedial-dorsolateral topographical gradients (Russchen et al., 1987; Barbas et al., 1991; Ray and Price, 1993). In other words, the MD-PFC connectivity is not homogeneous within the nucleus. In humans, the MD2 is comprised of a magnocellular portion (MDmc), covering the medial third of it, and a parvocellular portion (MDpc), larger and lateral to the MDmc. The connectivity patterns of the MDmc and the MDpc differ (for discussion see Barbas et al., 2012; Pergola et al., 2012; Mitchell and Chakraborty, 2013). The MDmc is reciprocally connected to the ventromedial PFC and also receives afferents from the MTL (Aggleton, 2012). The MDpc, instead, is reciprocally connected to the dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) and this is its major source of input, although it receives further input from other prefrontal areas (Mitchell and Chakraborty, 2013). There is no evidence of input from the MTL to the MDpc (Mitchell and Chakraborty, 2013). Byne et al. (2009) observed that, similar to the MDmc, also the medial pulvinar is connected to the PFC as well as with temporal and parietal cortices, and may be involved in declarative memory (see also Nadeau and Crosson, 1997). This suggestion has been supported by more recent neuroimaging findings on pulvinar activations during associative memory encoding (Pergola et al., 2013b).
The second pathway includes the diffuse projections from the intralaminar nuclei (ILN)3 to the PFC. While the ILN send specific projections to the basal ganglia (Preuss and Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Barbas et al., 1991; Sadikot et al., 1992), projections to the PFC are more sparse and widespread. The functional role of these connections is unclear. The ILN are considered part of a cerebellar-striato-frontal network that has been proposed to be essential for language production and control (Nadeau and Crosson, 1997). It has also been proposed that the activity of these nuclei may rapidly recruit large cortical portions and entrain synchronization of cortical activity (reviewed by Jones, 2007), hence contributing to allocate attentional resources (Van der Werf et al., 2002).
A third pathway, proposed to especially contribute to attention control, includes the reticular thalamic nucleus (RTN), the main source of GABAergic input to the thalamus. The thalamo-cortical cells of the AT, MD, and other nuclei are mostly glutamatergic and excite cortical neurons; local GABAergic interneurons, however, constitute up to 25–30% of cells, a peculiarity of the primate thalamus, compared to the rodent thalamus (reviewed by Jones, 2007). Beyond this “intrinsic” inhibition, thalamic nuclei are regulated by the RTN, which receives collaterals from both thalamo-cortical and cortico-thalamic fibers, but only inhibits thalamic cells (Avanzini et al., 1996). Therefore this nucleus is in the place to switch between patterns of thalamic electrophysiological activity, by selectively inhibiting the thalamo-cortical projections (Crick, 1984). Notably, the left and right RTN are connected directly, unlike most nuclei of the dorsal thalamus. The topographical order of the connections between the RTN and the nuclei of the dorsal thalamus forms “sectors” of the RTN that selectively control specific circuits and functions, thus affecting specific cortical areas (Barbas et al., 2012). On the other hand, the activity of an RTN subregion can quickly recruit the whole nucleus, and thereby the whole thalamo-cortical network, through gap junctions (Wang and Rinzel, 1993). Intriguingly, the MD interacts in a specific way with the RTN. All other nuclei project to a specific sector of the RTN, whereas the MD projects to all RTN subregions (Barbas et al., 2012). The same holds for the PFC, which regulates RTN activity as a whole (Zikopoulos and Barbas, 2006). This anatomical evidence suggests that the MD-PFC-RTN circuitry is critical for allocating cognitive resources and that the MD-PFC interactions are able to effectively modulate the activity in other thalamic areas through the interaction with the RTN. In sum, anatomical evidence differentiates three integrated components of the thalamo-PFC network: the MD-PFC connections, further composed of two pathways (magnocellular and parvocellular) which project to distinct cortical areas; the ILN-PFC connections; and the RTN-PFC connection, which also interacts with the MD.
Lesion studies in animals highlighted the importance of this MD-PFC system for episodic memory (see Mitchell and Chakraborty, 2013 for review). However, it has been suggested that deficits found in rewarded recognition tasks may reflect the effects of MD damage on aspects of task performance other than recognition, particularly on reward association learning (Corbit et al., 2003; Cross et al., 2012; see Baxter, 2013 for a discussion based on evidence from non-human primates). There might be differences between rodents and primates as concerns the role of the MD-PFC network in recognition memory, reflecting a greater influence of PFC-dependant processing in object recognition in primates (Aggleton et al., 2011; Cross et al., 2012). This notion can help reconcile seemingly contrasting findings in animals and humans. The MD nuclei of rodents and primates differ in the relative dimensions of their subunits, in the expression of intrinsic GABAergic neurons (reviewed by Jones, 2007), in the connectivity to the RTN (Zikopoulos and Barbas, 2012), in the expression of dopaminergic receptors (Garcia-Cabezas et al., 2007, 2009) and of transcripts related to dopaminergic transmission (Hurd and Fagergren, 2000). So many differences entangle inferences on the functions of the human thalamo-PFC network based on work with rodents.
The evidence available based on studies with non-human primates confirms the role of this network in learning and memory, especially with respect to an involvement of the MDmc in encoding (reviewed by Baxter, 2013). Even though the concerns about mixed influences of episodic memory and reward processing still apply, work with animal models highlights multiple interactions between the thalamus and the PFC, with the MD being a key hub of the network.
Clinical evidence
Patients with frontal lobe lesions are impaired in recognition and recall, with disproportionate impairment on the latter (Shimamura, 1995; Wheeler et al., 1995). These patients have difficulties with strategic aspects of recall, i.e., effectively generating and using cues to build/retrieve memory traces. The PFC is ubiquitously activated in recognition memory fMRI experiments (Cansino et al., 2002; Dobbins and Wagner, 2005; see Mitchell and Johnson, 2009 for a review), yet its exact contribution is far from clear. In event-related potentials (ERP) studies, frontal activity is found during episodic memory encoding (Neufang et al., 2006; Blumenfeld and Ranganath, 2007; Kim et al., 2009; Pergola et al., 2013d), as well as retrieval (Allan and Rugg, 1998; Duzel et al., 1999; Ranganath et al., 2000; Badgaiyan et al., 2002; Dobbins et al., 2002; Rugg and Curran, 2007; Pergola et al., 2013d). Most likely, the pattern of activations found in recognition memory studies at frontal sites actually depends on the activity of several PFC subregions processing novelty detection, relational encoding, maintenance, weighing, and selection of concurrent responses (Thompson-Schill et al., 1997; Dobbins and Han, 2006; Blumenfeld and Ranganath, 2007; Burgess et al., 2007; Bergstrom et al., 2013). For example, the ventrolateral PFC seems involved in memory formation irrespective of its associative nature (Blumenfeld and Ranganath, 2007; Mitchell and Johnson, 2009), while the DLPFC specifically contributes to successful associative encoding (Dolan and Fletcher, 1997; Staresina and Davachi, 2006; Murray and Ranganath, 2007; Mitchell and Johnson, 2009; Blumenfeld et al., 2011; Huijbers et al., 2013).
As regards the thalamus, deficits of recall and associative memory have been documented following ischemic lesion in the territory of the MD (Zoppelt et al., 2003; Edelstyn et al., 2006, 2012a; Soei et al., 2008), although those studies could not rule out a role of damage to the mammillothalamic tract (MTT) in the deficit pattern (discussed by Carlesimo et al., 2011; the MTT is considered part of the thalamo-RSC network). We performed a systematic review4 of all case reports of thalamic stroke with damage in the territory of the MD and without apparent damage in the territory of the AT and the MTT. Only reports including neuropsychological assessment of memory skills were included. Results are shown in Table 1.
Table 1.
I. Article | II. Laterality | III. Phase of lesion at test | IV. MTT involvement | V. Medial MD lesion | VI. Lateral MD lesion | VII. Recall | VIII. Recognition | IX. Encoding deficits | X. Retrieval deficits | XI. Comments |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Speedie and Heilman (1982) | L | Acute, sub-acute | Perhaps | Unclear | Unclear | Impaired | NA | – | Apparent deficit only after delay | CT scan suggests that the MTT could be involved. Psychiatric history |
Winocur et al. (1984) | B | Chronic (<1 Y) | Perhaps | Yes | Unclear | Impaired | Impaired | Greater deficit after short stimuli presentation | Deficit more evident after delay | CT scan suggests that the MTT could be involved. Deficit interpreted as encoding-dependent |
von Cramon et al. (1985)a | L (P6); B (P5) | Chronic (>1 Y) | NO | Minor | Yes, not associated with amnesia | Spared | Spared | – | – | Overlap analysis associated amnesia with MTT damage |
Bogousslavsky et al. (1986) | L | Acute | NO | Yes | Yes | Impaired | NA | Short-term memory deficits | – | Post-mortem confirmation. ILN and extrathalamic damage |
Kritchevsky et al. (1987) | R (P1); B (P2) | Sub-acute | NO | Yes | Only in P2 | Spared | Spared | – | – | <15% of MD volume involved |
Reilly et al. (1992) | B | Acute, chronic (<1 Y) | Unclear | Unclear | Unclear | Amnesia (two cases) | NA | Short-term memory deficits | – | Limited neuropsychological examination |
Calabrese et al. (1993) | B | Sub-acute | NO | Yes | Minor | Impaired | Impaired | Immediate recall deficits | Deficit more evident after delay | |
Shuren et al. (1997) | R | Acute, chronic (<1 Y) | NO | Yes | Yes | Spared but source memory deficit | Spared | – | ILN damaged. Source memory for temporal order was selectively impaired | |
Isaac et al. (1998) | B | Chronic (>1 Y) | NO | Yes | Yes | Impaired | Impaired | – | Deficit more evident after delay | Atrophy of the mammillary bodies |
Fukutake et al. (2002) | L | Sub-acute | NO | Yes | Minor | Spared | NA | – | – | Test under pharmacological treatment. Limited neuropsychological examination |
Van der Werf et al. (2003b)b | L | Chronic (>1 Y) | NO | NO | Yes | Mildly impaired | Mildly impaired | – | Increased forgetting rate | Impaired visual memory. P13 showed impaired verbal memory and extrathalamic lesions |
Zoppelt et al. (2003) | L (five cases), R (four cases) | Chronic (mostly >1 Y) | Perhaps | Two cases, familiarity and recollection deficit | Three cases, recollection deficit | Impaired | Impaired in some cases | Backward span impaired, short-term memory spared | Increased forgetting rate | Damage to the MTT was likely in several cases |
Tanji et al. (2003) | L | Chronic (<1 Y) | NO | Yes | Minor | Impaired | Mildly impaired | Immediate recall deficits | Deficit more evident after delay | Extrathalamic damage |
Soei et al. (2008) | L (six cases); R (three cases); B (one case) | Chronic (mostly >1 Y) | Perhaps | Non-associative memory impaired | Milder deficit | Impaired | Impaired in some cases | – | Apparent deficit only after delay | Damage to the MTT was likely in several cases |
Hampstead and Koffler (2009) | B | Sub-acute | Perhaps | Yes | Yes | Impaired | Spared | Immediate recall deficits | Deficit more evident after delay | Profound recall deficit. History of substance abuse. Extrathalamic damage |
Ioannidis et al. (2012) | B | Chronic (>1 Y) | Unclear | Yes | Yes | Impaired | NA | Immediate recall deficits | – | Profound recall deficit. Neuroimaging data suggest that the MTT might have been affected |
Pergola et al. (2012)c | L (two cases); R (four cases); B (two cases) | Chronic (>1 Y) | NO | Yes | Yes | Impairment proportional to MDpc damage | Impaired in some cases | Immediate recall deficits | Deficit more evident after delay | Relatively mild deficits, with patients mostly between 0 and 3 standard deviations below controls |
Only studies in which authors did not find clear evidence of damage to the anterior nuclei (including the laterodorsal nucleus) and to the mammillothalamic tract were included. Shaded rows highlight studies in which confounding factors should be considered when evaluating results. Confounding factors are written in italic font. Acute phase: ≤1 week after lesion onset; sub-acute phase: 1 week < lesion-test interval ≤2 months; chronic phase: >2 months after lesion onset.
B, bilateral lesion; ILN, intralaminar thalamic nuclei; L, left-sided lesion; MD, mediodorsal thalamic nucleus; MTT, mammillothalamic tract; NA, not assessed; P, patient; R, right-sided lesion; Y, year.
a Two cases without AT/MTT damage considered (Patients 5 and 6).
b Two cases without AT/MTT damage considered (Patients 1 and 13).
c Eight cases without AT/MTT damage were considered (Patients 2, 3, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 16). All patients except for P7, P8, and P9 were free of psychoactive medication.
We considered 17 studies, for a total of 44 cases. A first look at Table 1 reveals how heterogeneous the cases were with respect to laterality, lesion-test interval, and lesion assessment. The paucity of studies meeting the requirements we set and their heterogeneity aligns with the current lack of agreement on the function of the MD.
This analysis reveals that no single report documents impairments of recognition without recall deficits (Table 1, columns VII and VIII), a fact also acknowledged by other researchers (Aggleton et al., 2011; Carlesimo et al., 2011; Mitchell and Chakraborty, 2013). The general pattern of deficits is consistent with the idea that a primary impairment on recall entails a deficit in recognition memory because of disrupted recollection (Pergola et al., 2012).
The picture becomes more complicated when the evidence is evaluated more strictly. In several studies (Table 2, gray background) lesion to the MTT or to extrathalamic regions cannot be excluded; in others, pharmacological treatment or history of psychiatric disorders and/or substance abuse limit the clarity of the results. When studies with these potential confounds are excluded, only 13 cases remain (von Cramon et al., 1985; Kritchevsky et al., 1987; Calabrese et al., 1993; Shuren et al., 1997; Van der Werf et al., 2003b; Pergola et al., 2012). Two observations can be made on these studies: first, these most informative reports document less severe deficits; second, it seems that more recent reports found greater impairments compared to the earlier ones. We suggest that more recent studies employed more sensitive and/or extensive testing, changing the framework from the study of “amnesia” to the study of specific memory deficits.
Table 2.
I. Article | II. Laterality | III. Phase of lesion at test | IV. MTT involvement | V. Recall | VI. Recognition | VII. Encoding deficits | VIII. Retrieval deficits | IX. Comments |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hankey and Stewart-Wynne (1988) | L | Acute | YES | Impaired | NA | Immediate recall deficits | – | Post-mortem confirmation of lesion localization |
Ott and Saver (1993)a | L | Acute and chronic (<1 Y) | Perhaps | Impaired | NA | – | Apparent deficit only after delay | Follow-up after 5 months from onset revealed an additional lesion in the caudate nucleus. MD could have been affected |
Clarke et al. (1994) | L | Acute and chronic (<1 Y) | YES | Impaired in the verbal domain | Impaired in the verbal domain | – | Short-term memory was spared | Sensitivity to interference. ILN clearly involved. Posterior cingulate cortex showed hypometabolism |
Hanley et al. (1994, 2001)b | L | Chronic (>1 Y) | YES | Impaired | Spared | – | Deficit interpreted as retrieval-dependent | Involvement of the fornix and the caudate nucleus |
Ghika-Schmid and Bogousslavsky (2000) | L (eight cases), R (four cases) | Acute, sub-acute, and chronic (<1 Y) | YES | Impaired | Mildly impaired | Immediate recall deficits in the acute phase | Deficit more evident after delay. Repeated exposure did not improve performance. Confabulations, intrusions, false memory during the acute and sub-acute phases | Palipsychism in the acute phase, sensitivity to interference, word finding difficulties |
Only studies in which no clear evidence of damage to the mediodorsal nucleus was found were included. Shaded rows highlight studies in which confounding factors should be considered when evaluating results. Confounding factors are written in italic font. Acute phase: ≤1 week after lesion onset; sub-acute phase: 1 week < lesion-test interval ≤2 months; chronic phase: >2 months after lesion onset.
ILN, intralaminar thalamic nuclei; L, left-sided lesion; MD, mediodorsal thalamic nucleus; MTT, mammillothalamic tract; NA, not assessed; P, patient; R, right-sided lesion; Y, year.
a One case with anterior thalamic lesion was considered.
b The lesion was consequent to rupture of an aneurysm.
Pergola et al. (2012), for instance, found a decline in recall performance in patients with focal medial thalamic stroke, who were not impaired in recognition without recall. The task involved single-item recognition and cued recall of uniquely paired associates. Quantitative assessment of the lesions revealed that patients’ recall deficits were proportional to damage to the MDpc (but not MDmc/midline or ILN). Even though some of the patients showed evidence of lesion in the MTT, those with no evidence of such damage showed a deficit pattern similar to that of the whole sample. Overlap/subtraction analysis confirmed that in these patients memory deficits were associated to lesion in the MDpc.
This evidence suggests that the MDmc and the MDpc might contribute differently to recognition memory. Zoppelt et al. (2003) proposed that the MDmc could be related to familiarity. Conversely, the recall deficits observed by Pergola et al. (2012) selectively involved the MDpc in recall performance. It can thus be hypothesized that the MDpc is required for recall, while the MDmc is required for recognition without recall. Clinical evidence appears inconclusive in this respect (Table 1, columns V and VI), and data from animal studies are problematic for such a proposal (Mitchell and Chakraborty, 2013). Likewise, clinical evidence is of little avail with respect to the contribution of the MD to the encoding or retrieval phase of memory processing (Table 1, columns IX and X; Winocur et al., 1984; Mitchell and Chakraborty, 2013).
Neuroimaging evidence
Neuroimaging evidence specifically addressing the role of the thalamus in cognition is sparse (Metzger et al., 2013). To our knowledge, evidence on the differential contribution of the two portions of the MD to episodic memory is limited to a single fMRI study (Pergola et al., 2013b). The study employed a single-item recognition and associative cued recall task and included an anatomical parcellation of the functional clusters activated during task performance. Robust thalamic activation characterized recognition accompanied by recall, compared to recognition not followed by recall, consistent with Achim and Lepage (2005), who found higher thalamic activation during associative than single-item recognition. The MDpc was activated during both encoding and retrieval, similarly to the DLPFC; critically, the MDpc was more activated during recall than during recognition not followed by recall, matching the clinical results and supporting the hypothesis that an MDpc-DLPFC network subserves recall. Activation in the MDmc was only found during retrieval. No thalamic voxel displayed greater activation during recognition without recall compared with recognition followed by recall.
These findings leave unexplained whether the MDmc preferentially supports recognition without recall rather than recall. Perhaps support to this hypothesis comes from two studies performed by Montaldi et al. (2006) and Kafkas and Montaldi (2012), using the task previously described (see Intricated Plots – How to Assess the Contribution of Thalamo-Cortical Networks to Memory). Montaldi et al. (2006) found that activity in the dorsomedian thalamus correlated with familiarity self-reported confidence, although the same region was equally activated during familiarity based and recollection-based judgments. Kafkas and Montaldi (2012) reported greater dorsomedian thalamic activity during high-confidence familiarity trials than during unwanted recollection trials. The same pattern was observed in the orbitofrontal cortex, while the DLPFC and more lateral thalamic clusters were significantly more activated during recollection than familiarity. Intriguingly, the thalamic cluster activated by familiarity was remarkably medial, with the peak located in a position consistent with the putative MDmc or midline territory (coordinates in MNI space: x = −1; y = −15; z = 6). The FMRIB connectivity atlas (Behrens et al., 2003; Johansen-Berg et al., 2005; http://www2.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/connect/) reports the following projection probabilities at these coordinates: sensory cortex 0.02; Occipital cortex 0.10; PFC 0.02; temporal cortex 0.24. This connectivity pattern is compatible with localization of this peak in the MDmc, the midline nuclei, or the AT, because of the relatively high probability of connection with the temporal lobe, but very unlikely in the MDpc. Kafkas and Montaldi (2012) stressed that the dorsomedian thalamic cluster did discriminate hits from misses also in the recollection condition and that, in general, clusters activated during familiarity included subsets of voxels of clusters activated during recollection.
The findings by Kafkas and Montaldi (2012) and Pergola et al. (2013b) agree to some extent. Both studies observed higher thalamic and PFC activation during recall than recognition without recall and an involvement of the MD and the DLPFC in recollection-based recognition, in accord with other fMRI studies (Mottaghy et al., 1999; de Rover et al., 2008; Blumenfeld et al., 2011). On the other hand, the results by Montaldi’s group support a role of the MD in familiarity-based recognition, whereas our results suggest that the MDpc is specifically activated by recognition followed by recall. The implications of this discrepancy are discussed in the next section.
Models on the functional architecture of the thalamo-PFC network
Clinical results fail to support the hypothesis of selective familiarity deficits following lesion to the MD. This lack of evidence led Aggleton et al. (2011) to revise their 1999 model. In their multi-effect multi-nuclei (MEMN) model of thalamic contribution to recognition memory, they proposed that the MD, the ILN, and midline nuclei, as well as the pulvinar, may contribute in a graded way to both recollection and familiarity. It was proposed that the MD contributes more to familiarity than to recollection. In light of the data reviewed above, support for the proposal that the MD contributes to familiarity remains shaky, possibly limited to the fMRI results obtained by Montaldi et al. (2006) and Kafkas and Montaldi (2012).
In our opinion there are two possibilities to reconcile the seemingly conflicting neuroimaging findings obtained by Montaldi et al. (2006) and Pergola et al. (2013b). Firstly, the MDpc may support recall, while the MDmc may support familiarity. The distinction between the MDmc and MDpc connectivity patterns has also been advocated by Aggleton (2012) as a discriminant in their functional role; in this perspective, the finding by Kafkas and Montaldi (2012) that a region in the orbitofrontal cortex responded more strongly to familiarity than recollection avails the dissociation between the MDmc and the MDpc, because the MDmc is more strongly connected to the orbitofrontal cortex compared to the MDpc. The dichotomy between the magnocellular and the parvocellular MD may encompass a wider ground than the recollection/familiarity distinction, since the MDmc has also been implicated in reward-based learning. Lesions to the MDmc impair reward-based learning in rodents (Mitchell and Dalrymple-Alford, 2005), and also in monkeys, in particular during acquisition (i.e., initial learning; Mitchell and Gaffan, 2008). This impairment is also seen after disconnection from the ventromedial PFC (Mitchell et al., 2007), which suggests that it does not depend on the MTL afferents. Removal of cortical neurons produces a greater impairment in memory retrieval than in new learning, whereas subcortical damage produces a greater impairment in new learning than in memory retrieval (Mitchell et al., 2008). Although intriguing, a stark cognitive dissociation between subdivisions of the MD is weakly supported by lesion evidence in animals overall (Mitchell and Chakraborty, 2013). The parcellation of the MD needs further investigation in humans, and should in our opinion be taken into account in future clinical and neuroimaging studies. It is especially important to quantify the extent of the lesions and activations detected to bridge the gap with evidence based on animal studies. Even though the deficits found by Pergola et al. (2012) were relatively mild, quantitative assessments of the lesions revealed that the maximal volume loss in the MDpc was <30% and in most cases unilateral. This percentage is very far from the complete removal of select nuclei that is accomplished with the use of animal models, and it is possible that the role of the MD in memory is underestimated because of the limited extent of the lesions available for study.
A second possibility is that the difference between the studies lies in the tasks used. In particular, the instructions of the task employed by Montaldi et al. (2006) and Kafkas and Montaldi (2012) induced participants to focus their attention on the detection of familiarity during the retrieval phase. This focus on familiarity during retrieval could modulate the cognitive orientation of participants during encoding. The instructions of the task employed by Pergola et al. (2013b), instead, focused attention on associative recall. What kind of memory system is one that changes behavior depending on the conditions set up by the experimenter? The answer is, perhaps, that the MD-PFC network responds to cognitive orientation by setting its function depending on the behavioral goal (Monchi et al., 2001). This hypothesis is consistent with current models of the functional role of the DLPFC (Dobbins and Han, 2006). Accordingly, activation of the thalamo-PFC network is observed not only during retrieval, but also during encoding (Blumenfeld et al., 2011; Pergola et al., 2013b). Following this interpretation, damage to the MD (perhaps the MDpc in particular) would be expected to disrupt goal-directed memory processing more than goal-unrelated memory. This hypothesis remains to be tested.
The functions of the thalamo-PFC network seemingly encompass a wider cognitive domain than episodic memory (see Metzger et al., 2013, for a review). Patients with ischemia in the medial thalamus manifest a spectrum of symptoms including distractibility, aphasia, irritability, disinhibiting, disorganization of perception and thoughts, and executive deficits (Nadeau and Crosson, 1997; Schmahmann, 2003; Van der Werf et al., 2003a; Carrera and Bogousslavsky, 2006; Peterburs et al., 2011; Edelstyn et al., 2012b; Biesbroek et al., 2013). Ischemic lesions in the left medial thalamus, affecting the MD and the ILN, can result in semantic memory deficits in non-aphasic patients (Pergola et al., 2013a). Patients show deficits on a semantic retrieval task requiring activation of a third object from a pair of cues (Kraut et al., 2002b, 2006, 2007; see also Kraut et al., 2002a; Segal et al., 2003; Assaf et al., 2006). As it can be expected, the PFC also plays a role in semantic memory, together with the lateral temporal lobe, the left inferior frontal gyrus, and the occipito-temporo-parietal cortex (Martin and Chao, 2001; Patterson et al., 2007; Hayama and Rugg, 2009; Greenberg and Verfaellie, 2010; Binder and Desai, 2011).
Perhaps even more intriguingly, the thalamo-frontal network has been involved in future thinking. Clinical evidence in this respect is slim, yet Weiler et al. (2010c) documented two cases of patients with mediodorsal ischemic lesions, who showed deficits on a future thinking task. The task required subjects to provide a detailed account of future events in response to a verbal cue. Interestingly, one of the patients appeared free of recognition memory deficits. Weiler et al. (2010a,b) also provided fMRI evidence on the activation of the thalamus, the DLPFC, and the HC during future thinking.
In conclusion, there is strong anatomical evidence supporting a thalamic-PFC network centered in the MD. This network can exert its influence on cognition directly or through the interaction with other brain regions mediated by the PFC and the RTN. Evidence from clinical and neuroimaging studies highlights the importance of this network in episodic memory, particularly with respect to recall. The subunits of the MD show different connectivity patterns and also different activation patterns; however, clinical evidence in this regard is still very limited. Future studies need also to address more systematically whether the deficits are related to the encoding or retrieval phase of memory. It is crucial, in our view, to provide quantitative evidence on the lesions and the activations documented in the MD, and also to consider other components of the thalamo-PFC network (ILN, midline nuclei, RTN) when interpreting the data. Finally, the contribution of the thalamo-PFC system to episodic memory likely depends on task requirements. In light of the seemingly wide function of the thalamo-PFC network in cognition, we suggest that specifically the relevance to the behavioral goal is a variable to take into account in future experimental designs.
The thalamo-retrosplenial network
In this review we refer by this name to the network that has been introduced by Aggleton and Brown (1999) and extensively characterized from anatomical and functional viewpoints over the last years (for reviews, see: Aggleton et al., 2000; Aggleton and Pearce, 2001; Aggleton, 2008, 2010, 2012; Vann et al., 2009a; Aggleton et al., 2010, 2011). We focused on the thalamus and the RSC for our definition to highlight that in the hypothesized information flow from the HC to the cortex these nodes seem to play a different role, compared to “pre-thalamic” regions. This stance is meant to highlight specializations within the functional unity of the network. In general, however, we use the term “thalamo-RSC” network to indicate the whole connectivity pattern including the HC, the fornix, the mammillary bodies, the MTT, the AT (including the laterodorsal nucleus), the thalamo-RSC connections, and the RSC (including the posterior cingulate cortex).
To briefly summarize the anatomy of the network, also schematized in Figure 3, hippocampal efferents run from the subiculum through the fornix to reach the mammillary bodies and the AT (Vann and Aggleton, 2004; Vann, 2010), even though some MTL fibers reach the anterior midline of the thalamus through the inferior thalamic peduncle (Aggleton, 2012). The mammillary bodies also project to the AT (Vann et al., 2007). The AT, in turn, sends direct projections to the HC through the fornix and also through the cingulum bundle (Aggleton et al., 2010), giving off collaterals in the cingulate cortex. An important intermediate station in the cingulate cortex is the RSC (Morris et al., 1999), which communicates reciprocally with the AT and the HC.
Although the network has been analyzed in greater detail in animal models, results from fMRI studies yielded consistent evidence for RSC activation during recognition memory, together with the lateral parietal cortex (Wagner et al., 2005; Pustina et al., 2012). These regions are thought to belong to the so-called “default mode network” (DMN), which also includes the HC and the ventromedial PFC, and is probably involved in associative processing (Raichle et al., 2001; Vincent et al., 2006; Bar, 2007; Bar et al., 2007; Mason et al., 2007). The existence of a default mode or resting state network was first discussed by Gusnard and Raichle (2001). The specific features of this network have been studied extensively since this time, the most consistent finding being that the DMN is more active during rest than during task performance. Bar et al. (2007) suggested that the DMN is also involved in processing contextual associations, and has thus been named “context network.” It has been shown that the DMN is recruited when subjects are using an associative strategy to encode items (Cavanna and Trimble, 2006; Peters et al., 2009). More in general, DMN regions have been reported to support stimulus-independent processing (McGuire et al., 1996; Christoff et al., 2004) and mind-wandering (Mason et al., 2007), although recent results more tightly link its activity to declarative memory (Shapira-Lichter et al., 2013). The thalamo-RSC network discussed in this review appears to be a subcomponent of the DMN network that is most convincingly related to episodic memory processing.
As far as recall is concerned, there is general agreement that integrity of all nodes and tracts of this pathway is critical (Park et al., 2007; Vann et al., 2007, 2009a,b; Vann and Albasser, 2009; Aggleton et al., 2010; Carlesimo et al., 2011). In spite of this consensus, the individual contributions of its components are still under investigation. The AT seem to exert its influence especially over the cingulate cortex and the RSC in particular. Garden et al. (2009) demonstrated decrease of synaptic plasticity in the RSC following AT lesions in rodents. Interestingly, no overt modulation of the electrophysiological response of single receptors occurred, suggesting indirect physiological effects. Evidence from studies targeting genetic expression shows that lesions to the AT result in under regulation of select genes’ expression in the RSC (reviewed by Vann and Albasser, 2009; Aggleton, 2010). The tight link between the AT and the RSC is supported by evidence that, across species, the degree of differentiation and the size of the AT correlates with the degree of differentiation in the RSC (reviewed by Jones, 2007). Since Aggleton and Brown (1999) proposal, the AT has been assumed to extend the hippocampal function. This tenet is certainly warranted and well-grounded on solid evidence. However, we will argue that the AT also shows a contribution to memory that is different from that operated by the HC, and perhaps more linked to the modulation they exert on the RSC.
Clinical evidence
The findings mentioned from animal models appear to extend to humans. It has been shown that RSC activity decreases after lesion in the AT (Fazio et al., 1992; Reed et al., 2003; Clarke et al., 1994). Amnesia following damage to the RSC is a well-known phenomenon (reviewed by Vann et al., 2009a; Aggleton, 2010). Damage to the AT also induces amnesia. Harding et al. (2000), in a post-mortem study, found that in alcoholics diagnosed with Wernicke–Korsakoff syndrome cell loss in the AT was the best predictor for amnesia. Disruption of the MTT and the fornix similarly causes recall deficits (Carlesimo et al., 2007; Cipolotti et al., 2008; Tsivilis et al., 2008; Rudebeck et al., 2009). Hence Aggleton and Brown (1999) and Aggleton et al. (2011) proposed that the thalamo-RSC network acts as a unitary recall system, and that damage to any node of the network will cause amnesia.
There is general agreement that lesion to the AT causes recall deficits that resemble the consequences of HC lesion, yet these deficits are surprisingly poorly characterized in neuropsychological literature. We systematically reviewed all stroke reports of memory deficits following lesions to the AT with spared MD and sufficient neuropsychological assessment of memory. Results are shown in Table 2 (see footnote 4, for the criteria used).
We were able to find only six studies that met our criteria, for a total of 16 patients. Data are so scarce because usually AT damage follows an infarct of the tuberothalamic artery; however, the same artery supplies the rostro-lateral part of the MD, especially the MDpc (Schmahmann, 2003). Damage to the MD is assumed to occur in particular following paramedian infarcts, but an analysis including 19 patients with thalamic stroke found no significant difference in the volume lost in the MD following paramedian and tuberothalamic stroke (Pergola et al., 2013c). This may help explain why memory deficits are similar between patients with different etiology (Pergola et al., 2012).
The cases available support the notion that lesion to the AT disproportionately impairs recall over recognition memory (Table 2, columns V and VI). Although the MD-PFC connections could have been involved because of damage to the anterior thalamic radiation, there was no evidence of direct damage to the MD in the cases reviewed.
Next, we asked whether these reports supported the assumption that damage to the AT disrupts the function of the HC. Based on the HC-AT connectivity, Van der Werf et al. (2003a) proposed that impairment after AT lesions derives from defective encoding. The RSC, instead, is mostly activated at retrieval (Wiggs et al., 1999; Huijbers et al., 2012, 2013), so we used neuropsychological clues of disrupted encoding or retrieval to inform our analysis.
Although the sample size is very small, 15 out of 16 cases showed evidence of retrieval deficits. Some cases showed encoding deficits only in the acute phase of the disease (i.e., the first week after lesion onset). Hence we found no published evidence of stable memory deficits that could be attributed to defective encoding following selective lesions to the AT. For 13 cases (Hanley et al., 1994; Ghika-Schmid and Bogousslavsky, 2000) the deficits were explicitly interpreted as retrieval-dependent. This conclusion, however, remains a working hypothesis in light of the confounds that also some of these studies present (Table 2, gray background) and because of the few cases available. We suggest that future clinical studies should employ tests that allow discrimination of encoding and retrieval deficits and/or use neuroimaging techniques to study the functional consequences of selective lesion to the AT.
Neuroimaging evidence
The phase of activation of the DMN regions, i.e., encoding or retrieval, is a currently debated controversy in the neuroimaging community. There is large consensus on the paramount role of the HC in episodic encoding; evidence on the involvement of the HC in retrieval is more conflicting, since only some of the numerous neuroimaging studies on the role of the HC in episodic memory did find significant activations during retrieval. When activation of the HC during retrieval was observed, an interference of incidental encoding processes during retrieval could not be ultimately ruled out (Stark and Okado, 2003; Huijbers et al., 2009).
Reas and Brewer (2013) performed a parametric analysis of BOLD activation during retrieval, using response times as a proxy for the duration of retrieval search. The authors found that activation of the HC was negatively correlated with response times. The same pattern applied to the medial PFC, posterior cingulate, and inferior parietal cortex. The same regions increased their reciprocal connectivity during successful incidental encoding. The authors interpreted this pattern as a deactivation induced by effortful retrieval, in the view of competition for cognitive resources between encoding and retrieval (Huijbers et al., 2009). Interestingly, however, the thalamus (anterior and medial, as can be judged based on Figure 4 by Reas and Brewer, 2013; no coordinates were reported) showed a positive correlation with response times, hence it was involved in effortful retrieval according to authors’ interpretation. This evidence suggests that in the thalamo-RSC network there is a parcellation of labor, with the thalamic node performing somewhat different operations compared to the HC and RSC.
Within the thalamus, different regions are activated in different phases of the memory process. The parcellation of the thalamic activation performed by Pergola et al. (2013b) revealed that the AT was selectively active during retrieval. The contrast used compared recognition cues characterized by post-scanning successful recall of the unique associations studied with cues characterized by no subsequent recall. Therefore the findings are in line with the role of the AT in recall. The activation appeared confined to the retrieval phase, and no voxels were activated at group level during encoding. The connectivity pattern of the activated voxels, assessed using established connectivity atlases, appeared consistent with the localization of the clusters in the AT. Finally and most importantly, activation in the putative region of the AT (defined on the basis of an anatomical atlas) correlated with individual recall scores during retrieval, and not during encoding. This evidence does not imply that the AT are inactive during encoding: it is possible that the AT were equally active during successful and unsuccessful encoding, hence preventing detection of significant clusters. However, the finding that inter-individual variability in the activation of the AT during retrieval correlates with recall performance suggests that the AT play an autonomous role during this phase of memory processing.
As we also argued for the thalamo-PFC network in Section “The Thalamo-Prefrontal Network,” the thalamo-RSC network has been implicated in wider cognitive functions than recall. It is debated whether the involvement of the DMN is exclusive of memory, or extends to endogenous processing more in general. Tasks without overt demand for memory processing appear to recruit the DMN, including for instance theory of mind (Buckner and Carroll, 2007) and self-referential processing (Johnson et al., 2002). The neural network that supports semantic knowledge related to the self includes the anterior and posterior cingulate cortices as well as the RSC (Gobbini et al., 2004; Donix et al., 2010). However, the same areas are activated even to a greater extent, together with the anteromedial PFC, by episodic autobiographical memory (Levine, 2004; Levine et al., 2004). A recent meta-analysis on neural correlates of autobiographical memory highlighted the common participation of the thalamus, the RSC, the anterior cingulate, and the medial PFC in both episodic and semantic aspects of self-referenced memory (Martinelli et al., 2012). By comparing directly mnemonic and non-mnemonic tasks, Shapira-Lichter et al. (2013) could show that the RSC was more activated during mnemonic processing, both episodic and semantic.
These pieces of evidence suggest that the thalamo-RSC network could constitute a possible trait d’union between episodic and semantic memory; on the other hand, they also point to differences in the contribution of the individual nodes of the network to memory. We suggest that the interaction between the AT and the RSC in particular deserves more investigation in humans, and we put forward working hypotheses on the functional specialization of these regions in the next section.
Models on the functional architecture of the thalamo-RSC network
Functional dissociations between the nodes of the thalamo-RSC network have been examined by Huijbers et al. (2012). Authors highlighted that the posterior cingulate and retrosplenial regions include a number of different areas that also show different connectivity patterns, and for this reason several models fail to account exhaustively for RSC activation in fMRI studies. Given that also the AT present heterogeneous connectivity patterns with the hippocampal subfields (reviewed by Aggleton, 2012), it is possible that the thalamo-RSC network consists of multiple pathways partially segregated with respect to their connectivity and function.
In general, the thalamo-RSC network, as part of the DMN, likely subserves associative memory and particularly recall, as documented by a large body of evidence. Work with animal models suggested that the commonalities between impairments observed following HC and AT damage may depend on the particular task used to assess memory (Sziklas and Petrides, 1999, 2004); it is also possible that the RSC constitutes a non-fornical pathway for bilateral communication between the HC and the AT (Henry et al., 2004; Dumont et al., 2010). Evidence from animal studies therefore presents commonalities and differences with respect to the HC, AT, and RSC contribution to memory. Evidence from clinical and neuroimaging studies suggests that the phase of activation – i.e., retrieval – of the AT and RSC is different from what is typically found for the HC, i.e., encoding.
It has thus been suggested that the AT and the RSC are involved in activating and maintaining stored perceptual representations during retrieval (von Zerssen et al., 2001). This proposal would explain why cued recall is required to elicit this activity: recognition by itself does not require activation and maintenance of perceptual representations. This hypothesis assumes a role of the AT during retrieval that is consistent with the evidence reviewed. Vilberg and Rugg (2012) studied the different activation patterns between transient and sustained activation as a function of the duration of maintenance of the representations (analysis focused on the retrieval phase). They did not find activations in the thalamus (although this may partly depend on scanning parameters), but they found that the HC and the RSC were only transiently activated during retrieval. Their findings therefore do not lend direct support to the idea that the HC-RSC network contributes to representation maintenance.
Another intriguing possibility is provided by the Multiple memory Trace Theory (Moscovitch et al., 2005), which predicts that memory retrieval entails the cumulative generation of multiple memory traces that somewhat differ from the original memory trace. In particular, new memory traces are more schematic and more dependent on the RSC (Hirshhorn et al., 2012), while they become progressively independent from the HC. This theoretical framework requires the existence of brain regions intermediate between the HC and the neocortex that transform the memory traces and establish the information originally encoded by the HC into neocortical areas. Also on the basis of neurophysiological findings reviewed below, we propose that the features of the AT (in particular, the regulation of RSC plasticity) match the requirements to subserve this function. In this view, the AT-RSC connections would underlie the generation of multiple memory traces, a role that includes retrieval processes (of the original memory trace) and re-encoding (generation of novel memory traces); on the cognitive side, this information flow could be a relevant path for the conversion of episodic into semantic memory traces. Testing this working hypothesis will require a deeper understanding of the temporal dynamics of network activations, in line with the approach followed by Vilberg and Rugg (2012). Rather than focusing on the magnitude of activations in the nodes of the network, it is necessary to collect more information on the duration of the activations – an approach that would match the focus on long-term potentiation (LTP) found in neurophysiology.
Consequences of Disruption of the Thalamo-PFC and Thalamo-RSC Networks in Psychiatric Disorders
Thalamic neuropathology, especially of ischemic and degenerative etiology, has been thoroughly studied in connection with memory deficits (reviewed by Kopelman, 2002; Schmahmann, 2003; Carrera and Bogousslavsky, 2006; Carlesimo et al., 2011). More recently, evidence is accumulating that also psychiatric conditions might include thalamo-cortical dysfunction as a salient feature of their neuropathological picture.
Patients with schizophrenia, in particular, show structural and functional abnormalities in the HC, PFC, and the thalamus. The cognitive profile that accompanies the structural and functional peculiarities of schizophrenia is characterized, among other symptoms, by episodic memory deficits that constitute one of the most impaired aspects of the cognitive profile (Saykin et al., 1991; Mitropoulou et al., 2002; D’Argembeau et al., 2008; Minzenberg et al., 2009). The episodic memory impairment is more evident on recall than on recognition (Pelletier et al., 2005; Thoma et al., 2006; Libby et al., 2013). The weak dependence of the deficits on the duration of the study-test delay led to the hypothesis that the impairments relate to encoding rather than retrieval (Aleman et al., 1999; Gold et al., 2000; Dickinson et al., 2007).
The deficits displayed by patients with schizophrenia are thus suggestive of a dysfunctional thalamo-PFC network (Kuperberg, 2008; Mitchell and Johnson, 2009; Blumenfeld et al., 2011; Libby et al., 2013). Beside the amount of data involving the PFC in the pathology (reviewed by Bertolino and Blasi, 2009), post-mortem studies found specific cell loss in the MD of patients with schizophrenia (Young et al., 2000; Byne et al., 2002), and especially of its parvocellular portion (Popken et al., 2000; reviewed by Byne et al., 2009). Other reports highlighted altered metabolism in the MD and ILN of patients with schizophrenia (Hazlett et al., 2004). Drugs targeting the D2 dopaminergic receptor improve the symptomatology, and the MD presents a high density of D2 receptors (Rieck et al., 2004; Vogt et al., 2008). This feature characterizes primates compared to rodents (Garcia-Cabezas et al., 2009). In healthy subjects, low density of D2 receptors in the thalamus has been related to high creativity in a positron emission tomography study (de Manzano et al., 2010). Dysfunction and decreased connectivity of the MDmc and the orbitofrontal PFC to which it projects have been related to the psychotic symptoms of schizophrenia (Popken et al., 2000; Kubota et al., 2013). The involvement of the MDpc, on the other hand, has been claimed to relate to the cognitive impairments shown by patients with schizophrenia (reviewed by Alelú-Paz and Giménez-Amaya, 2008; Pakkenberg et al., 2009).
Interestingly, the critical alterations in the brain of patients with schizophrenia that affect the networks described in this review, and the thalamo-PFC in particular, are not related to acute damage in a single node of the network. Schizophrenia has been characterized as a neurodevelopmental disorder. In this framework, symptoms of schizophrenia can be thought of as a model of progressive thalamo-cortical dysfunction, with a strong genetic component (Bertolino and Blasi, 2009; Blasi et al., 2013). Accordingly, the neuroimaging literature is rich of evidence on dysfunctional activation and connectivity of the thalamo-PFC network in patients with schizophrenia and also in healthy controls with genetic risk for schizophrenia (Hariri et al., 2003; Bertolino et al., 2008; Di Giorgio et al., 2012; Anticevic et al., 2013). Functional connectivity studies have established altered coupling between the HC and the lateral PFC as a correlate of genetic risk for schizophrenia (Bertolino et al., 2006; Tunbridge et al., 2013).
Altered coupling of the HC and the DLPFC is a notable finding, because the HC is more strongly connected to the ventromedial and orbitofrontal PFC, than to the DLPFC. Based on the thalamo-cortical networks outlined in this review, it is plausible that a transthalamic route involving the MDpc, the RTN, and the MDmc/midline nuclei mediates the interaction between the two brain regions. Cholvin et al. (2013) found evidence of this functional circuitry in rodents; Klingner et al. (2013) obtained results consistent with this suggestion using resting state fMRI. The MDpc-DLPFC connectivity could modulate activity in the RTN, which is able to regulate the midline thalamic nuclei directly projecting to the HC. Matching this hypothesis, Dauvermann et al. (2013) showed decreased MD-PFC connectivity during a verbal fluency task in healthy participants with high genetic risk for schizophrenia, as assessed through non-linear Dynamic Causal Modeling. The effect was greatest for patients also showing symptoms of psychosis.
In summary, the study of the pathophysiology of schizophrenia offers the opportunity of further insight into the physiological and molecular properties of the thalamo-PFC and the thalamo-RSC networks. The next section will go more in detail in the putative mechanisms that might underlie their role in episodic memory.
Possible Mechanisms of the Thalamo-Cortical Contribution to Memory and Related Pathologies
The thalamus is considered to serve as an interface between subcortical structures and the cortex and basal ganglia. It has been described as a “searchlight” (Crick, 1984; Smythies, 1997), an “enhancer” (LaBerge, 1997), and a “focuser” (Van der Werf et al., 2003a). Research has partially discounted the unitary views in recent years, placing emphasis on the diversity of the thalamic nuclei (Sherman and Guillery, 2002).
In this respect it is noteworthy that some thalamic nuclei receive their driving inputs from non-cortical regions (first-order nuclei; e.g., the AT), while other nuclei, such as the MD and the pulvinar, receive their driving input from the cortex, particularly from the V cortical layer. Since these driver inputs do not synapse in the RTN, but they do synapse on subcortical effector nuclei, it has been proposed that these cortico-thalamic connections bear information on planned actions, that is then relayed by the thalamus to other cortical regions (Guillery and Sherman, 2011; Sherman and Guillery, 2011). This information route is called “transthalamic” and is hypothesized to supply upstream cortical regions with information about the signals they are about to receive from downstream cortical regions, particularly with respect to action execution (Byne et al., 2009). Given the size and conductance of cortico-thalamic and thalamo-cortical fibers it is even possible that the transthalamic route is faster than direct cortico-cortical communication (Salami et al., 2003). The next paragraphs will briefly sketch possible mechanisms of the thalamo-cortical contribution to memory, with mention of the most recent evidence on the topic.
Pacemaker
The connectivity pattern of the MD enables it to mediate transthalamic communication between temporal and prefrontal regions, as well as between lateral prefrontal subregions. The role of thalamic nuclei in entraining cortical oscillations is well-recognized (Steriade, 2006; Sherman, 2007), and more recent evidence supports the involvement of the MD in particular in regulating PFC oscillations during memory processes. By using a procedure to reversibly disconnect the MD and the PFC in mice, Parnaudeau et al. (2013) demonstrated memory deficits in a delayed non-matching to sample task at long delays (see Aggleton and Brown, 1999, for a discussion about the use of this task to assess recognition memory in rodents). Performance was impaired in trained animals, suggesting that the deficit was not limited to the encoding phase. The critical range of frequencies for the MD-PFC connectivity was the beta and gamma range.
The dynamics of the MD-prefrontal interplay in humans have been recently reported by Staudigl et al. (2012), who studied a patient with epilepsy by means of intracranial recordings. The authors demonstrated a link between thalamic activity in the putative MD territory during retrieval and scalp frontal beta-frequency modulation in the time window 300–500 ms post-stimulus onset. This time window is commonly associated to a frontal old/new effect in the ERP literature (reviewed by Paller et al., 2007; Rugg and Curran, 2007). Strikingly, Staudigl et al. (2012) could show that the direction of the signal was thalamo-cortical. Additionally, the authors reported cross-frequency coupling relating the power in the beta range with power in the gamma frequency-range (Staudigl et al., 2012). Unfortunately, the study did not report activity during encoding and concluded that the MD is more involved in retrieval compared to the AT, a finding to our knowledge not supported by any evidence based on studies with humans (as discussed, see The Thalamo-Retrosplenial Network). Notwithstanding the difficulties of locating electrodes in the thalamus of the epileptic patient, the AT are first-order nuclei projecting to deep brain regions, so several synapses could be needed before the signal reaches the outer cortex, which is the main source of scalp potentials. Fitzgerald et al. (2013) extended these findings by showing a more complex pattern of functional connectivity between the thalamus and the PFC. The interactions included phase-amplitude and amplitude-amplitude coupling and were established based on data from three patients receiving deep brain stimulation.
This evidence suggests that the contribution of the thalamo-PFC network to episodic memory can be mediated by modulation of cortical oscillations induced by thalamic activity. Disruption of thalamo-PFC connectivity also characterizes schizophrenia: in the study by Anticevic et al. (2013), the most prominent locus of thalamic dysconnectivity was centered on the MD.
Plasticity device
Aggleton et al. (2011) proposed that the AT may be part of a triangular circuitry including also the HC and the cingulate and prefrontal cortices. Triangular connections enable the setup of coincidence detection systems (Jones, 2007) based on LTP. In rodents, LTP can be induced in the cingulate and PFC by co-activation of convergent hippocampal and thalamic afferents (Gigg et al., 1992, 1994). Further evidence from animal studies suggests that the AT modulates plasticity in the RSC (Garden et al., 2009). As previously mentioned, it can be hypothesized that the cingulate (especially RSC) and medial prefrontal LTP could foster transfer of memory traces from the HC to the neocortex during retrieval and concurrent re-encoding (Moscovitch et al., 2005; Hirshhorn et al., 2012). A proof of concept for this working hypothesis comes from a study with rats in which modulation of the MD-PFC connectivity was shown to affect prefrontal LTP, resulting in strengthening or decreasing thalamo-cortical connectivity (Bueno-Junior et al., 2012).
Long-term potentiation could therefore be one physiological mechanism underlying the role of thalamo-cortical networks, and especially the thalamo-RSC network, in episodic memory. It is established that LTP is a critical feature of memory formation, and evidence is accumulating in favor of a “plasticity account” for diseases such as schizophrenia (reviewed by Weinberger and Harrison, 2011).
A critical feature of a plasticity regulator is selectivity: enhancing or decreasing signal in general would not be sufficient to afford a selective increase of specific patterns of neural activity. The thalamus, as a complex, is particularly suitable for such a role. The thalamus sends selective as well as widespread projections to the cortex, ending in specific cortical layers (Byne et al., 2009). The convergence of specific (e.g., MD to PFC) and aspecific connections (e.g., ILN to PFC) would provide the basic circuitry necessary for a coincidence detection system (see the core/matrix hypothesis, Jones, 2007) that would be able to modulate plasticity in specific cortical populations. The potential importance of such processes for memory has been shown by Logothetis et al. (2012), who found that increased activation in the HC was accompanied by decreased thalamic activation. The authors proposed that decreased thalamic activity may serve to shield the cortex from interference during memory consolidation. This proposal is particularly intriguing in light of the findings that in the acute phase of ischemia involving the AT patients are especially sensitive to interference (see Table 2).
In summary, the effect of the thalamo-cortical interaction could operate at a greater time scale than previously thought, determining long-term changes in connectivity, a suitable mechanism for memory formation and transformation.
Summary
In this article we summarized evidence that beside the medial temporal lobe, two other brain networks are involved in memory processing. These structures are not exclusively bound to memory processes, but also contribute to executive functions and future thinking. The evidence reviewed shows that memory processing at different stages draws on different and also shared resources depending on the required processes.
The thalamo-PFC network has often been mentioned as fundamental for declarative, and especially episodic, memory. Until now, however, it is debated whether it rather plays only an ancillary role with respect to the MTL. We argued that by using tests that specifically tap recall it is possible to reveal the contribution of the thalamo-PFC network to episodic memory. Differently from previous views, we argued that the MD is critical for recall. The field has not sufficiently investigated whether subunits of the MD are functionally segregated, and this remains a task for future experimental scrutiny. Our working hypothesis is that this network is especially involved in goal-directed, as opposed to incidental, memory acquisition. This hypothesis can be tested by comparing incidental against goal-directed encoding, and also by comparing results obtained through tasks that induce different cognitive orientations. Regulating medial temporal and prefrontal oscillations and connectivity are possible mechanisms by which the network could act; these mechanisms have been shown to be dysfunctional in schizophrenia, which in many aspects shows a neuropathophysiology consistent with disruption of the thalamo-PFC network.
The importance of the thalamo-RSC network in recall has met wide consensus. We reviewed evidence that the understanding of the functional unity of this network that dominated the recent literature is changing, following recent discoveries on functional specializations of its nodes. The recruitment of different nodes of the network in different memory stages (encoding and retrieval) is a consistent finding. In particular, clinical and neuroimaging evidence indicates an involvement of the AT and the RSC in the retrieval phase of memory processing. The functional parcellation of the network is particularly interesting in light of models of memory, such as the Multiple Memory Trace theory, that posit the existence of regions governing the information flow from the HC to the neocortex. The thalamo-RSC network appears to be especially fit for this function, possibly mediated by regulation of neocortical plasticity. This hypothesis can be tested by focusing on the duration of activations and on the effects of repeated exposure to stimuli, whose representation passes from episodic- to semantic-like.
We suggest that advancing the field will require a more extensive use of quantitative procedures in the assessment of thalamic lesions and activations, and we share the impression of Metzger et al. (2013) that improving the spatial resolution of imaging methods will greatly enhance our understanding of the function of non-MTL regions in memory. Moreover, we suggest that focusing research on different aspects of recall and their neural determinants rather than on recall/recognition dichotomies will be an effective way to move forward. We are still bound to the statement by James reported at the beginning of this review about empirical design of memory studies: recall seems to be the chief task to study episodic memory.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This work was funded by a grant (SFB 874) from the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, DFG) awarded to Boris Suchan (Project B8). Giulio Pergola was partly supported by the FOODcast grant awarded to Prof. Dr. Raffaella Rumiati and partly supported by the grant “Sviluppo del Capitale Umano ad Alta Qualificazione" awarded by the foundation “Fondazione Con il Sud” to Prof. Dr. Alessandro Bertolino. The authors are grateful to Dr. Giuseppe Blasi for insightful comments on the manuscript.
Footnotes
1For the sake of simplicity the term “hippocampus,” abbreviated with HC, refers throughout the article to the hippocampal formation, including the dentate gyrus, CA fields, subiculum, presubiculum, and parasubiculum. Aggleton (2012) goes more in detail with respect to the connectivity of HC subregions and should be consulted for further reference.
2Throughout the review we will endorse the viewpoint expressed by Jones (2007) that other partitions of the MD (densocellular, paralamellar, or multiformis) rather belong to the centrolateral nucleus, which is considered part of the ILN.
3As specified in the previous footnote, this group of thalamic nuclei includes the densocellular and paralamellar (or multiformis) partition of the MD in the current review.
4Articles were considered based on previous reviews and on the PubMed search: (thalam*[title/abstract]) AND (stroke[title/abstract] OR infarct[title/abstract] OR ischemia[title/abstract] OR ischaemia[title/abstract] OR ischemic[title/abstract]) AND (memory[title/abstract] OR learning[title/abstract] OR recollection[title/abstract] OR recognition[title/abstract] OR familiarity[title/abstract] OR amnesia[title/abstract] OR amnesic[title/abstract]) AND english[language].
References
- Achim A. M., Lepage M. (2005). Neural correlates of memory for items and for associations: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 652–667 10.1162/0898929053467578 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton J. P. (2008). EPS Mid-Career Award 2006. Understanding anterograde amnesia: disconnections and hidden lesions. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (Hove) 61, 1441–1471 10.1080/17470210802215335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton J. P. (2010). Understanding retrosplenial amnesia: insights from animal studies. Neuropsychologia 48, 2328–2338 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton J. P. (2012). Multiple anatomical systems embedded within the primate medial temporal lobe: implications for hippocampal function. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1579–1596 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton J. P., Brown M. W. (1999). Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal-anterior thalamic axis. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 425–444 10.1017/S0140525X99002034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton J. P., Dumont J. R., Warburton E. C. (2011). Unraveling the contributions of the diencephalon to recognition memory: a review. Learn. Mem. 18, 384–400 10.1101/lm.1884611 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton J. P., O’Mara S. M., Vann S. D., Wright N. F., Tsanov M., Erichsen J. T. (2010). Hippocampal-anterior thalamic pathways for memory: uncovering a network of direct and indirect actions. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 2292–2307 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07251.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton J. P., Pearce J. M. (2001). Neural systems underlying episodic memory: insights from animal research. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 356, 1467–1482 10.1098/rstb.2001.0946 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton J. P., Vann S. D., Denby C., Dix S., Mayes A. R., Roberts N., et al. (2005). Sparing of the familiarity component of recognition memory in a patient with hippocampal pathology. Neuropsychologia 43, 1810–1823 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aggleton J. P., Vann S. D., Oswald C. J., Good M. (2000). Identifying cortical inputs to the rat hippocampus that subserve allocentric spatial processes: a simple problem with a complex answer. Hippocampus 10, 466–474 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alelú-Paz R., Giménez-Amaya J. M. (2008). The mediodorsal thalamic nucleus and schizophrenia. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 33, 489–498 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aleman A., Hijman R., de Haan E. H., Kahn R. S. (1999). Memory impairment in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 1358–1366 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allan K., Rugg M. D. (1998). Neural correlates of cued recall with and without retrieval of source memory. Neuroreport 9, 3463–3466 10.1097/00001756-199810260-00023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anticevic A., Cole M. W., Repovs G., Murray J. D., Brumbaugh M. S., Winkler A. M., et al. (2013). Characterizing thalamo-cortical disturbances in schizophrenia and bipolar illness. Cereb. Cortex. 10.1093/cercor/bht165 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Assaf M., Calhoun V. D., Kuzu C. H., Kraut M. A., Rivkin P. R., Hart J., Jr., et al. (2006). Neural correlates of the object-recall process in semantic memory. Psychiatry Res. 147, 115–126 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avanzini G., de Curtis M., Franceschetti S., Sancini G., Spreafico R. (1996). Cortical versus thalamic mechanisms underlying spike and wave discharges in GAERS. Epilepsy Res. 26, 37–44 10.1016/S0920-1211(96)00037-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Awipi T., Davachi L. (2008). Content-specific source encoding in the human medial temporal lobe. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 34, 769–779 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.769 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Badgaiyan R. D., Schacter D. L., Alpert N. M. (2002). Retrieval of relational information: a role for the left inferior prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 17, 393–400 10.1006/nimg.2002.1219 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bar M. (2007). The proactive brain: using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 280–289 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bar M., Aminoff E., Mason M., Fenske M. (2007). The units of thought. Hippocampus 17, 420–428 10.1002/hipo.20287 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbas H., Garcia-Cabezas M. A., Zikopoulos B. (2012). Frontal-thalamic circuits associated with language. Brain Lang. 126, 49–61 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.10.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbas H., Henion T. H., Dermon C. R. (1991). Diverse thalamic projections to the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 313, 65–94 10.1002/cne.903130106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastin C., Linden M., Charnallet A., Denby C., Montaldi D., Roberts N., et al. (2004). Dissociation between recall and recognition memory performance in an amnesic patient with hippocampal damage following carbon monoxide poisoning. Neurocase 10, 330–344 10.1080/13554790490507650 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baxter M. G. (2013). Mediodorsal thalamus and cognition in non-human primates. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:38. 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behrens T. E., Johansen-Berg H., Woolrich M. W., Smith S. M., Wheeler-Kingshott C. A., Boulby P. A., et al. (2003). Non-invasive mapping of connections between human thalamus and cortex using diffusion imaging. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 750–757 10.1038/nn1075 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergstrom Z. M., Henson R. N., Taylor J. R., Simons J. S. (2013). Multimodal imaging reveals the spatiotemporal dynamics of recollection. Neuroimage 68, 141–153 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertolino A., Blasi G. (2009). The genetics of schizophrenia. Neuroscience 164, 288–299 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.04.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertolino A., Di Giorgio A., Blasi G., Sambataro F., Caforio G., Sinibaldi L., et al. (2008). Epistasis between dopamine regulating genes identifies a nonlinear response of the human hippocampus during memory tasks. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 226–234 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bertolino A., Rubino V., Sambataro F., Blasi G., Latorre V., Fazio L., et al. (2006). Prefrontal-hippocampal coupling during memory processing is modulated by COMT val158met genotype. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 1250–1258 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biesbroek J. M., Kuijf H. J., van der Graaf Y., Vincken K. L., Postma A., Mali W. P., et al. (2013). Association between subcortical vascular lesion location and cognition: a Voxel-Based and Tract-Based Lesion-Symptom Mapping Study. The SMART-MR Study. PLoS ONE 8:e60541. 10.1371/journal.pone.0060541 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Binder J. R., Desai R. H. (2011). The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 527–536 10.1016/j.tics.2011.10.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bird C. M., Shallice T., Cipolotti L. (2007). Fractionation of memory in medial temporal lobe amnesia. Neuropsychologia 45, 1160–1171 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blasi G., Napolitano F., Ursini G., Di Giorgio A., Caforio G., Taurisano P., et al. (2013). Association of GSK-3beta genetic variation with GSK-3beta expression, prefrontal cortical thickness, prefrontal physiology, and schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 170, 868–876 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12070908 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumenfeld R. S., Parks C. M., Yonelinas A. P., Ranganath C. (2011). Putting the pieces together: the role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in relational memory encoding. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 257–265 10.1162/jocn.2010.21459 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumenfeld R. S., Ranganath C. (2007). Prefrontal cortex and long-term memory encoding: an integrative review of findings from neuropsychology and neuroimaging. Neuroscientist. 13, 280–291 10.1177/1073858407299290 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bogousslavsky J., Miklossy J., Deruaz J. P., Regli F., Assal G. (1986). Unilateral left paramedian infarction of thalamus and midbrain: a clinico-pathological study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 49, 686–694 10.1136/jnnp.49.6.686 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown M. W., Barker G. R., Aggleton J. P., Warburton E. C. (2012). What pharmacological interventions indicate concerning the role of the perirhinal cortex in recognition memory. Neuropsychologia 50, 3122–3140 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown M. W., Warburton E. C., Aggleton J. P. (2010). Recognition memory: material, processes, and substrates. Hippocampus 20, 1228–1244 10.1002/hipo.20858 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner R. L., Carroll D. C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 49–57 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bueno-Junior L. S., Lopes-Aguiar C., Ruggiero R. N., Romcy-Pereira R. N., Leite J. P. (2012). Muscarinic and nicotinic modulation of thalamo-prefrontal cortex synaptic plasticity [corrected] in vivo. PLoS ONE 7:e47484. 10.1371/journal.pone.0047484 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess P. W., Gilbert S. J., Dumontheil I. (2007). Function and localization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10). Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 362, 887–899 10.1098/rstb.2007.2095 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byne W., Buchsbaum M. S., Mattiace L. A., Hazlett E. A., Kemether E., Elhakem S. L., et al. (2002). Postmortem assessment of thalamic nuclear volumes in subjects with schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 159, 59–65 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.1.59 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byne W., Hazlett E. A., Buchsbaum M. S., Kemether E. (2009). The thalamus and schizophrenia: current status of research. Acta Neuropathol. 117, 347–368 10.1007/s00401-008-0404-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calabrese P., Haupts M., Markowitsch H. J., Gehlen W. (1993). The cognitive-mnestic performance profile of a patient with bilateral asymmetrical thalamic infarction. Int. J. Neurosci. 71, 101–106 10.3109/00207459309000597 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cansino S., Maquet P., Dolan R. J., Rugg M. D. (2002). Brain activity underlying encoding and retrieval of source memory. Cereb. Cortex 12, 1048–1056 10.1093/cercor/12.10.1048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlesimo G. A., Lombardi M. G., Caltagirone C. (2011). Vascular thalamic amnesia: a reappraisal. Neuropsychologia 49, 777–789 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlesimo G. A., Serra L., Fadda L., Cherubini A., Bozzali M., Caltagirone C. (2007). Bilateral damage to the mammillo-thalamic tract impairs recollection but not familiarity in the recognition process: a single case investigation. Neuropsychologia 45, 2467–2479 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrera E., Bogousslavsky J. (2006). The thalamus and behavior: effects of anatomically distinct strokes. Neurology 66, 1817–1823 10.1212/01.wnl.0000219679.95223.4c [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavanna A. E., Trimble M. R. (2006). The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129, 564–583 10.1093/brain/awl004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cholvin T., Loureiro M., Cassel R., Cosquer B., Geiger K., De Sa Nogueira D., et al. (2013). The ventral midline thalamus contributes to strategy shifting in a memory task requiring both prefrontal cortical and hippocampal functions. J. Neurosci. 33, 8772–8783 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0771-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christoff K., Ream J. M., Gabrieli J. D. (2004). Neural basis of spontaneous thought processes. Cortex 40, 623–630 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70158-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chua E. F., Schacter D. L., Rand-Giovannetti E., Sperling R. A. (2007). Evidence for a specific role of the anterior hippocampal region in successful associative encoding. Hippocampus 17, 1071–1080 10.1002/hipo.20340 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cipolotti L., Bird C., Good T., Macmanus D., Rudge P., Shallice T. (2006). Recollection and familiarity in dense hippocampal amnesia: a case study. Neuropsychologia 44, 489–506 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cipolotti L., Husain M., Crinion J., Bird C. M., Khan S. S., Losseff N., et al. (2008). The role of the thalamus in amnesia: a tractography, high-resolution MRI and neuropsychological study. Neuropsychologia 46, 2745–2758 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.05.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke S., Assal G., Bogousslavsky J., Regli F., Townsend D. W., Leenders K. L., et al. (1994). Pure amnesia after unilateral left polar thalamic infarct: topographic and sequential neuropsychological and metabolic (PET) correlations. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 57, 27–34 10.1136/jnnp.57.1.27 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit L. H., Muir J. L., Balleine B. W. (2003). Lesions of mediodorsal thalamus and anterior thalamic nuclei produce dissociable effects on instrumental conditioning in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, 1286–1294 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02833.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crick F. (1984). Function of the thalamic reticular complex: the searchlight hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 81, 4586–4590 10.1073/pnas.81.14.4586 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cross L., Brown M. W., Aggleton J. P., Warburton E. C. (2012). The medial dorsal thalamic nucleus and the medial prefrontal cortex of the rat function together to support associative recognition and recency but not item recognition. Learn. Mem. 20, 41–50 10.1101/lm.028266.112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cycowicz Y. M., Friedman D., Snodgrass J. G. (2001). Remembering the color of objects: an ERP investigation of source memory. Cereb. Cortex 11, 322–334 10.1093/cercor/11.4.322 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Argembeau A., Raffard S., Van der Linden M. (2008). Remembering the past and imagining the future in schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 117, 247–251 10.1037/0021-843X.117.1.247 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daum I., Ackermann H. (1994). Dissociation of declarative and nondeclarative memory after bilateral thalamic lesions: a case report. Int. J. Neurosci. 75, 153–165 10.3109/00207459408986299 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dauvermann M. R., Whalley H. C., Romaniuk L., Valton V., Owens D. G., Johnstone E. C., et al. (2013). The application of nonlinear dynamic causal modelling for fMRI in subjects at high genetic risk of schizophrenia. Neuroimage 73, 16–29 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davachi L., Mitchell J. P., Wagner A. D. (2003). Multiple routes to memory: distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 2157–2162 10.1073/pnas.0337195100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Manzano O., Cervenka S., Karabanov A., Farde L., Ullen F. (2010). Thinking outside a less intact box: thalamic dopamine D2 receptor densities are negatively related to psychometric creativity in healthy individuals. PLoS ONE 5:e10670. 10.1371/journal.pone.0010670 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Rover M., Petersson K. M., van der Werf S. P., Cools A. R., Berger H. J., Fernandez G. (2008). Neural correlates of strategic memory retrieval: differentiating between spatial-associative and temporal-associative strategies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 1068–1079 10.1002/hbm.20445 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Giorgio A., Gelao B., Caforio G., Romano R., Andriola I., D’Ambrosio E., et al. (2012). Evidence that hippocampal-parahippocampal dysfunction is related to genetic risk for schizophrenia. Psychol. Med. 43, 1661–1671 10.1017/S0033291712002413 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diana R. A., Yonelinas A. P., Ranganath C. (2008). The effects of unitization on familiarity-based source memory: testing a behavioral prediction derived from neuroimaging data. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 34, 730–740 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.730 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson D., Ramsey M. E., Gold J. M. (2007). Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64, 532–542 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.532 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dobbins I. G., Foley H., Schacter D. L., Wagner A. D. (2002). Executive control during episodic retrieval: multiple prefrontal processes subserve source memory. Neuron 35, 989–996 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00858-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dobbins I. G., Han S. (2006). Isolating rule- versus evidence-based prefrontal activity during episodic and lexical discrimination: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of detection theory distinctions. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1614–1622 10.1093/cercor/bhj098 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dobbins I. G., Wagner A. D. (2005). Domain-general and domain-sensitive prefrontal mechanisms for recollecting events and detecting novelty. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1768–1778 10.1093/cercor/bhi054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dolan R. J., Fletcher P. C. (1997). Dissociating prefrontal and hippocampal function in episodic memory encoding. Nature 388, 582–585 10.1038/41561 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donix M., Petrowski K., Jurjanz L., Huebner T., Herold U., Baeumler D., et al. (2010). Age and the neural network of personal familiarity. PLoS ONE 5:e15790. 10.1371/journal.pone.0015790 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumont J. R., Petrides M., Sziklas V. (2010). Fornix and retrosplenial contribution to a hippocampo-thalamic circuit underlying conditional learning. Behav. Brain Res. 209, 13–20 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duzel E., Cabeza R., Picton T. W., Yonelinas A. P., Scheich H., Heinze H. J., et al. (1999). Task-related and item-related brain processes of memory retrieval. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 1794–1799 10.1073/pnas.96.4.1794 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edelstyn N. M., Hunter B., Ellis S. J. (2006). Bilateral dorsolateral thalamic lesions disrupts conscious recollection. Neuropsychologia 44, 931–938 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.08.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edelstyn N. M., Mayes A. R., Denby C., Ellis S. J. (2012a). Impairment in material-specific long-term memory following unilateral mediodorsal thalamic damage and presumed partial disconnection of the mammillo-thalamic tract. J. Neuropsychol. 6, 119–140 10.1111/j.1748-6653.2011.02019.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edelstyn N. M., Mayes A. R., Ellis S. J. (2012b). Damage to the dorsomedial thalamic nucleus, central lateral intralaminar thalamic nucleus, and midline thalamic nuclei on the right-side impair executive function and attention under conditions of high demand but not low demand. Neurocase. 10.1080/13554794.2012.713497 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum H., Lipton P. A. (2008). Towards a functional organization of the medial temporal lobe memory system: role of the parahippocampal and medial entorhinal cortical areas. Hippocampus 18, 1314–1324 10.1002/hipo.20500 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eichenbaum H., Yonelinas A. P., Ranganath C. (2007). The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 123–152 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Exner C., Weniger G., Irle E. (2001). Implicit and explicit memory after focal thalamic lesions. Neurology 57, 2054–2063 10.1212/WNL.57.11.2054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fazio F., Perani D., Gilardi M. C., Colombo F., Cappa S. F., Vallar G., et al. (1992). Metabolic impairment in human amnesia: a PET study of memory networks. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 12, 353–358 10.1038/jcbfm.1992.52 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgerald T. H., Valentin A., Selway R., Richardson M. P. (2013). Cross-frequency coupling within and between the human thalamus and neocortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:84. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00084 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fukutake T., Akada K., Ito S., Okuda T., Ueki Y. (2002). Severe personality changes after unilateral left paramedian thalamic infarct. Eur. Neurol. 47, 156–160 10.1159/000047974 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia-Cabezas M. A., Martinez-Sanchez P., Sanchez-Gonzalez M. A., Garzon M., Cavada C. (2009). Dopamine innervation in the thalamus: monkey versus rat. Cereb. Cortex 19, 424–434 10.1093/cercor/bhn093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia-Cabezas M. A., Rico B., Sanchez-Gonzalez M. A., Cavada C. (2007). Distribution of the dopamine innervation in the macaque and human thalamus. Neuroimage 34, 965–984 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garden D. L., Massey P. V., Caruana D. A., Johnson B., Warburton E. C., Aggleton J. P., et al. (2009). Anterior thalamic lesions stop synaptic plasticity in retrosplenial cortex slices: expanding the pathology of diencephalic amnesia. Brain 132, 1847–1857 10.1093/brain/awp090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardiner J. M. (1988). Functional aspects of recollective experience. Mem. Cognit. 16, 309–313 10.3758/BF03197041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geraci L., McCabe D. P., Guillory J. J. (2009). On interpreting the relationship between remember-know judgments and confidence: the role of instructions. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 701–709 10.1016/j.concog.2009.04.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghika-Schmid F., Bogousslavsky J. (2000). The acute behavioral syndrome of anterior thalamic infarction: a prospective study of 12 cases. Ann. Neurol. 48, 220–227 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gigg J., Tan A. M., Finch D. M. (1992). Glutamatergic excitatory responses of anterior cingulate neurons to stimulation of the mediodorsal thalamus and their regulation by GABA: an in vivo iontophoretic study. Cereb. Cortex 2, 477–484 10.1093/cercor/2.6.477 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gigg J., Tan A. M., Finch D. M. (1994). Glutamatergic hippocampal formation projections to prefrontal cortex in the rat are regulated by GABAergic inhibition and show convergence with glutamatergic projections from the limbic thalamus. Hippocampus 4, 189–198 10.1002/hipo.450040209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gobbini M. I., Leibenluft E., Santiago N., Haxby J. V. (2004). Social and emotional attachment in the neural representation of faces. Neuroimage 22, 1628–1635 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold J. M., Rehkemper G., Binks S. W., III, Carpenter C. J., Fleming K., Goldberg T. E., et al. (2000). Learning and forgetting in schizophrenia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 109, 534–538 10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.534 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg D. L., Verfaellie M. (2010). Interdependence of episodic and semantic memory: evidence from neuropsychology. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16, 748–753 10.1017/S1355617710000676 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guillery R. W., Sherman S. M. (2011). Branched thalamic afferents: what are the messages that they relay to the cortex? Brain Res. Rev. 66, 205–219 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.08.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gusnard D. A., Raichle M. E. (2001). Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 685–694 10.1038/35094500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hampstead B. M., Koffler S. P. (2009). Thalamic contributions to anterograde, retrograde, and implicit memory: a case study. Clin. Neuropsychol. 23, 1232–1249 10.1080/13854040902936679 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankey G. J., Stewart-Wynne E. G. (1988). Amnesia following thalamic hemorrhage. Another stroke syndrome. Stroke 19, 776–778 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanley J. R., Davies A. D. M., Downes J. J., Mayes A. R. (1994). Impaired recall of verbal material following rupture and repair of anterior communicating artery aneurysm. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 11, 543–578 10.1080/0264329940825198511516451 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hanley J. R., Davies A. D. M., Downes J. J., Roberts J. N., Gong Q. Y., Mayes A. R. (2001). Impaired recall of verbal material following rupture and repair of anterior communicating artery aneurysm. Neuropsychologia 39, 1003–1010 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00020-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harding A., Halliday G., Caine D., Kril J. (2000). Degeneration of anterior thalamic nuclei differentiates alcoholics with amnesia. Brain 123, 141–154 10.1093/brain/123.1.141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hariri A. R., Goldberg T. E., Mattay V. S., Kolachana B. S., Callicott J. H., Egan M. F., et al. (2003). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor val66met polymorphism affects human memory-related hippocampal activity and predicts memory performance. J. Neurosci. 23, 6690–6694 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayama H. R., Rugg M. D. (2009). Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is engaged during post-retrieval processing of both episodic and semantic information. Neuropsychologia 47, 2409–2416 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hazlett E. A., Buchsbaum M. S., Kemether E., Bloom R., Platholi J., Brickman A. M., et al. (2004). Abnormal glucose metabolism in the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 161, 305–314 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henderson J. M., Williams C. C., Castelhano M. S., Falk R. J. (2003). Eye movements and picture processing during recognition. Percept. Psychophys. 65, 725–734 10.3758/BF03194809 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henke K., Buck A., Weber B., Wieser H. G. (1997). Human hippocampus establishes associations in memory. Hippocampus 7, 249–256 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henke K., Weber B., Kneifel S., Wieser H. G., Buck A. (1999). Human hippocampus associates information in memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 5884–5889 10.1073/pnas.96.10.5884 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry J., Petrides M., St-Laurent M., Sziklas V. (2004). Spatial conditional associative learning: effects of thalamo-hippocampal disconnection in rats. Neuroreport 15, 2427–2431 10.1097/00001756-200410250-00025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirshhorn M., Grady C., Rosenbaum R. S., Winocur G., Moscovitch M. (2012). The hippocampus is involved in mental navigation for a recently learned, but not a highly familiar environment: a longitudinal fMRI study. Hippocampus 22, 842–852 10.1002/hipo.20944 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holdstock J. S., Mayes A. R., Gong Q. Y., Roberts N., Kapur N. (2005). Item recognition is less impaired than recall and associative recognition in a patient with selective hippocampal damage. Hippocampus 15, 203–215 10.1002/hipo.20046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holdstock J. S., Mayes A. R., Roberts N., Cezayirli E., Isaac C. L., O’Reilly R. C., et al. (2002). Under what conditions is recognition spared relative to recall after selective hippocampal damage in humans? Hippocampus 12, 341–351 10.1002/hipo.10011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huijbers W., Pennartz C. M., Cabeza R., Daselaar S. M. (2009). When learning and remembering compete: a functional MRI study. PLoS Biol. 7:e11. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huijbers W., Schultz A. P., Vannini P., McLaren D. G., Wigman S. E., Ward A. M., et al. (2013). The encoding/retrieval flip: interactions between memory performance and memory stage and relationship to intrinsic cortical networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 1163–1179 10.1162/jocn_a_00366 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huijbers W., Vannini P., Sperling R. A., Pennartz C.M., Cabeza R., Daselaar S. M. (2012). Explaining the encoding/retrieval flip: memory-related deactivations and activations in the posteromedial cortex. Neuropsychologia 50, 3764–3774 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.08.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hurd Y. L., Fagergren P. (2000). Human cocaine- and amphetamine-regulated transcript (CART) mRNA is highly expressed in limbic- and sensory-related brain regions. J. Comp. Neurol. 425, 583–598 10.1002/1096-9861(20001002)425:43.0.CO;2-# [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ioannidis A. E., Kimiskidis V. K., Loukopoulou E., Geroukis T., Vlaikidis N., Kosmidis M. H. (2012). Apathy, cognitive dysfunction and impaired social cognition in a patient with bilateral thalamic infarction. Neurocase 19, 513–520 10.1080/13554794.2012.701645 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isaac C. L., Holdstock J. S., Cezayirli E., Roberts J. N., Holmes C. J., Mayes A. R. (1998). Amnesia in a patient with lesions limited to the dorsomedial thalamic nucleus. Neurocase 4, 497–508 10.1093/neucas/4.6.497-a [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- James W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology, with Introduction by Miller George A. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 654 p. [Google Scholar]
- Johansen-Berg H., Behrens T. E., Sillery E., Ciccarelli O., Thompson A. J., Smith S. M., et al. (2005). Functional-anatomical validation and individual variation of diffusion tractography-based segmentation of the human thalamus. Cereb. Cortex 15, 31–39 10.1093/cercor/bhh105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson S. C., Baxter L. C., Wilder L. S., Pipe J. G., Heiserman J. E., Prigatano G. P. (2002). Neural correlates of self-reflection. Brain 125, 1808–1814 10.1093/brain/awf181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones E. G. (2007). The Thalamus. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press [Google Scholar]
- Kafkas A., Montaldi D. (2012). Familiarity and recollection produce distinct eye movement, pupil and medial temporal lobe responses when memory strength is matched. Neuropsychologia 50, 3080–3093 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim A. S., Vallesi A., Picton T. W., Tulving E. (2009). Cognitive association formation in episodic memory: evidence from event-related potentials. Neuropsychologia 47, 3162–3173 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein J. C., Rushworth M. F., Behrens T. E., Mackay C. E., de Crespigny A. J., D’Arceuil H., et al. (2010). Topography of connections between human prefrontal cortex and mediodorsal thalamus studied with diffusion tractography. Neuroimage 51, 555–564 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klingner C. M., Langbein K., Dietzek M., Smesny S., Witte O. W., Sauer H., et al. (2013). Thalamocortical connectivity during resting state in schizophrenia. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 10.1007/s00406-013-0417-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kopelman M. D. (2002). Disorders of memory. Brain 125, 2152–2190 10.1093/brain/awf229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kopelman M. D., Thomson A. D., Guerrini I., Marshall E. J. (2009). The Korsakoff syndrome: clinical aspects, psychology and treatment. Alcohol Alcohol. 44, 148–154 10.1093/alcalc/agn118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraut M. A., Cherry B., Pitcock J. A., Anand R., Li J., Vestal L., et al. (2007). The Semantic Object Retrieval Test (SORT) in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 20, 62–67 10.1097/WNN.0b013e3180335f7d [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraut M. A., Cherry B., Pitcock J. A., Vestal L., Henderson V. W., Hart J., Jr. (2006). The Semantic Object Retrieval Test (SORT) in normal aging and Alzheimer disease. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 19, 177–184 10.1097/01.wnn.0000213922.41008.22 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraut M. A., Kremen S., Moo L. R., Segal J. B., Calhoun V., Hart J., Jr. (2002a). Object activation in semantic memory from visual multimodal feature input. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 37–47 10.1162/089892902317205302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraut M. A., Kremen S., Segal J. B., Calhoun V., Moo L. R., Hart J., Jr. (2002b). Object activation from features in the semantic system. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 24–36 10.1162/089892902317205302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kritchevsky M., Graff-Radford N. R., Damasio A. R. (1987). Normal memory after damage to medial thalamus. Arch. Neurol. 44, 959–962 10.1001/archneur.1987.00520210055019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kubota M., Miyata J., Sasamoto A., Sugihara G., Yoshida H., Kawada R., et al. (2013). Thalamocortical disconnection in the orbitofrontal region associated with cortical thinning in schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 12–21 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.1023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuperberg G. R. (2008). Building meaning in schizophrenia. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 39, 99–102 10.1177/155005940803900216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBerge D. (1997). Attention, awareness, and the triangular circuit. Conscious. Cogn. 6, 149–181 10.1006/ccog.1997.0305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavenex P., Amaral D. G. (2000). Hippocampal-neocortical interaction: a hierarchy of associativity. Hippocampus 10, 420–430 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine B. (2004). Autobiographical memory and the self in time: brain lesion effects, functional neuroanatomy, and lifespan development. Brain Cogn. 55, 54–68 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00280-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine B., Turner G. R., Tisserand D., Hevenor S. J., Graham S. J., McIntosh A. R. (2004). The functional neuroanatomy of episodic and semantic autobiographical remembering: a prospective functional MRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1633–1646 10.1162/0898929042568587 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Libby L. A., Yonelinas A. P., Ranganath C., Ragland J. D. (2013). Recollection and familiarity in schizophrenia: a quantitative review. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 944–950 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.10.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logothetis N. K., Eschenko O., Murayama Y., Augath M., Steudel T., Evrard H. C., et al. (2012). Hippocampal-cortical interaction during periods of subcortical silence. Nature 491, 547–553 10.1038/nature11618 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mandler G. (1980). Recognizing: the judgment of previous occurence. Psych. Rev. 87, 252–271 10.1037/0033-295X.87.3.252 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Martin A., Chao L. L. (2001). Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 194–201 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00196-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinelli P., Sperduti M., Piolino P. (2012). Neural substrates of the self-memory system: new insights from a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1515–1529 10.1002/hbm.22008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason M. F., Norton M. I., Van Horn J. D., Wegner D. M., Grafton S. T., Macrae C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315, 393–395 10.1126/science.1131295 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayes A., Montaldi D., Migo E. (2007). Associative memory and the medial temporal lobes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 126–135 10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayes A. R., Holdstock J. S., Isaac C. L., Hunkin N. M., Roberts N. (2002). Relative sparing of item recognition memory in a patient with adult-onset damage limited to the hippocampus. Hippocampus 12, 325–340 10.1002/hipo.1111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCabe D. P., Geraci L. D. (2009). The influence of instructions and terminology on the accuracy of remember-know judgments. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 401–413 10.1016/j.concog.2009.02.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuire P. K., Paulesu E., Frackowiak R. S., Frith C. D. (1996). Brain activity during stimulus independent thought. Neuroreport 7, 2095–2099 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Metzger C. D., Van der Werf Y. D., Walter M. (2013). Functional mapping of thalamic nuclei and their integration into cortico-striatal-thalamo-cortical loops via ultra-high resolution imaging-from animal anatomy to in vivo imaging in humans. Front. Neurosci. 7:24. 10.3389/fnins.2013.00024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minzenberg M. J., Laird A. R., Thelen S., Carter C. S., Glahn D. C. (2009). Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 811–822 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.91 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mishkin M. (1982). A memory system in the monkey. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 298, 83–95 10.1098/rstb.1982.0074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell A. S., Baxter M. G., Gaffan D. (2007). Dissociable performance on scene learning and strategy implementation after lesions to magnocellular mediodorsal thalamic nucleus. J. Neurosci. 27, 11888–11895 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1835-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell A. S., Browning P. G., Wilson C. R., Baxter M. G., Gaffan D. (2008). Dissociable roles for cortical and subcortical structures in memory retrieval and acquisition. J. Neurosci. 28, 8387–8396 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1924-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell A. S., Chakraborty S. (2013). What does the mediodorsal thalamus do? Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:37. 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell A. S., Dalrymple-Alford J. C. (2005). Dissociable memory effects after medial thalamus lesions in the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 973–985 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04421.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell A. S., Gaffan D. (2008). The magnocellular mediodorsal thalamus is necessary for memory acquisition, but not retrieval. J. Neurosci. 28, 258–263 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4922-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell K. J., Johnson M. K. (2009). Source monitoring 15 years later: what have we learned from fMRI about the neural mechanisms of source memory? Psychol. Bull. 135, 638–677 10.1037/a0015849 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitropoulou V., Harvey P. D., Maldari L. A., Moriarty P. J., New A. S., Silverman J. M., et al. (2002). Neuropsychological performance in schizotypal personality disorder: evidence regarding diagnostic specificity. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 1175–1182 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01426-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monchi O., Petrides M., Petre V., Worsley K., Dagher A. (2001). Wisconsin card sorting revisited: distinct neural circuits participating in different stages of the task identified by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 21, 7733–7741 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montaldi D., Mayes A. R. (2010). The role of recollection and familiarity in the functional differentiation of the medial temporal lobes. Hippocampus 20, 1291–1314 10.1002/hipo.20853 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montaldi D., Spencer T. J., Roberts N., Mayes A. R. (2006). The neural system that mediates familiarity memory. Hippocampus 16, 504–520 10.1002/hipo.20178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morel A. (2007). Stereotactic Atlas of the Human Thalamus and Basal Ganglia. New York: Informa Healthcare USA, Inc [Google Scholar]
- Morris R., Petrides M., Pandya D. N. (1999). Architecture and connections of retrosplenial area 30 in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta). Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 2506–2518 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00672.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moscovitch M., Rosenbaum R. S., Gilboa A., Addis D. R., Westmacott R., Grady C., et al. (2005). Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: a unified account based on multiple trace theory. J. Anat. 207, 35–66 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2005.00421.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mottaghy F. M., Shah N. J., Krause B. J., Schmidt D., Halsband U., Jancke L., et al. (1999). Neuronal correlates of encoding and retrieval in episodic memory during a paired-word association learning task: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Exp. Brain Res. 128, 332–342 10.1007/s002210050853 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray L. J., Ranganath C. (2007). The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex contributes to successful relational memory encoding. J. Neurosci. 27, 5515–5522 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0406-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadeau S. E., Crosson B. (1997). Subcortical aphasia. Brain Lang. 58, 355–402 10.1006/brln.1997.1810 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neufang M., Heinze H. J., Duzel E. (2006). Electromagnetic correlates of recognition memory processes. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 37, 300–308 10.1177/155005940603700407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newell B. R., Dunn J. C. (2008). Dimensions in data: testing psychological models using state-trace analysis. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 285–290 10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ott B. R., Saver J. L. (1993). Unilateral amnesic stroke. Six new cases and a review of the literature. Stroke 24, 1033–1042 10.1161/01.STR.24.7.1033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pakkenberg B., Scheel-Kruger J., Kristiansen L. V. (2009). Schizophrenia; from structure to function with special focus on the mediodorsal thalamic prefrontal loop. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 120, 345–354 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01447.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paller K. A., Lucas H. D., Voss J. L. (2012). Assuming too much from ‘familiar’ brain potentials. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 313–315 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paller K. A., Voss J. L., Boehm S. G. (2007). Validating neural correlates of familiarity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 243–250 10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park K.-C., Yoon S.-S., Chang D. I., Chung K.-C., Ahn T.-B., Ku B. D., et al. (2007). Amnesic syndrome in a mammillothalmic tract infarction. J. Korean Med. Sci. 22, 1094–1097 10.3346/jkms.2007.22.6.1094 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parnaudeau S., O’Neill P. K., Bolkan S. S., Ward R. D., Abbas A. I., Roth B. L., et al. (2013). Inhibition of mediodorsal thalamus disrupts thalamofrontal connectivity and cognition. Neuron 77, 1151–1162 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.01.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patterson K., Nestor P. J., Rogers T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 976–987 10.1038/nrn2277 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelletier M., Achim A. M., Montoya A., Lal S., Lepage M. (2005). Cognitive and clinical moderators of recognition memory in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 74, 233–252 10.1016/j.schres.2004.08.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pergola G., Bellebaum C., Gehlhaar B., Koch B., Schwarz M., Daum I., et al. (2013a). The involvement of the thalamus in semantic retrieval: a clinical group study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 872–886 10.1162/jocn_a_00364 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pergola G., Ranft A., Mathias K., Suchan B. (2013b). The role of the thalamic nuclei in recognition memory accompanied by recall during encoding and retrieval: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 74, 195–208 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pergola G., Suchan B., Koch B., Schwarz M., Daum I., Gunturkun O. (2013c). Quantitative assessment of chronic thalamic stroke. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 34, E51–E55 10.3174/ajnr.A2897 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pergola G., Trotta M., Suchan B. (2013d). Asymmetric hemispheric contribution to ERPs in associative memory indexes goal relevance and quantity of information. Behav. Brain Res. 241, 7–16 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.11.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pergola G., Gunturkun O., Koch B., Schwarz M., Daum I., Suchan B. (2012). Recall deficits in stroke patients with thalamic lesions covary with damage to the parvocellular mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus. Neuropsychologia 50, 2477–2491 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.06.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterburs J., Pergola G., Koch B., Schwarz M., Hoffmann K. P., Daum I., et al. (2011). Altered error processing following vascular thalamic damage: evidence from an antisaccade task. PLoS. ONE 6:e21517. 10.1371/journal.pone.0021517 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters J., Daum I., Gizewski E., Forsting M., Suchan B. (2009). Associations evoked during memory encoding recruit the context-network. Hippocampus 19, 141–151 10.1002/hipo.20490 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters J., Koch B., Schwarz M., Daum I. (2007a). Domain-specific impairment of source memory following a right posterior medial temporal lobe lesion. Hippocampus 17, 505–509 10.1002/hipo.20297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters J., Suchan B., Koster O., Daum I. (2007b). Domain-specific retrieval of source information in the medial temporal lobe. Eur. J. Neurosci. 26, 1333–1343 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05752.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popken G. J., Bunney W. E., Jr., Potkin S. G., Jones E. G. (2000). Subnucleus-specific loss of neurons in medial thalamus of schizophrenics. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 9276–9280 10.1073/pnas.150243397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preuss T. M., Goldman-Rakic P. S. (1987). Crossed corticothalamic and thalamocortical connections of macaque prefrontal cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 257, 269–281 10.1002/cne.902570211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pustina D., Gizewski E., Forsting M., Daum I., Suchan B. (2012). Human memory manipulated: dissociating factors contributing to MTL activity, an fMRI study. Behav. Brain Res. 229, 57–67 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.12.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quamme J. R., Yonelinas A. P., Norman K. A. (2007). Effect of unitization on associative recognition in amnesia. Hippocampus 17, 192–200 10.1002/hipo.20257 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle M. E., Macleod A. M., Snyder A. Z., Powers W. J., Gusnard D. A., Shulman G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 676–682 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ranganath C. (2010). A unified framework for the functional organization of the medial temporal lobes and the phenomenology of episodic memory. Hippocampus 20, 1263–1290 10.1002/hipo.20852 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ranganath C., Johnson M. K., D’Esposito M. (2000). Left anterior prefrontal activation increases with demands to recall specific perceptual information. J. Neurosci. 20, RC108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ranganath C., Yonelinas A. P., Cohen M. X., Dy C. J., Tom S. M., D’Esposito M. (2004). Dissociable correlates of recollection and familiarity within the medial temporal lobes. Neuropsychologia 42, 2–13 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ray J. P., Price J. L. (1993). The organization of projections from the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus to orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 337, 1–31 10.1002/cne.903370102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reas E. T., Brewer J. B. (2013). Retrieval search and strength evoke dissociable brain activity during episodic memory recall. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 219–233 10.1162/jocn_a_00335 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed L. J., Lasserson D., Marsden P., Stanhope N., Stevens T., Bello F., et al. (2003). FDG-PET findings in the Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome. Cortex 39, 1027–1045 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70876-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reilly M., Connolly S., Stack J., Martin E. A., Hutchinson M. (1992). Bilateral paramedian thalamic infarction: a distinct but poorly recognized stroke syndrome. Q. J. Med. 82, 63–70 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rieck R. W., Ansari M. S., Whetsell W. O., Jr., Deutch A. Y., Kessler R. M. (2004). Distribution of dopamine D2-like receptors in the human thalamus: autoradiographic and PET studies. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 362–372 10.1038/sj.npp.1300336 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rudebeck S. R., Scholz J., Millington R., Rohenkohl G., Johansen-Berg H., Lee A. C. (2009). Fornix microstructure correlates with recollection but not familiarity memory. J. Neurosci. 29, 14987–14992 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4707-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rugg M. D., Curran T. (2007). Event-related potentials and recognition memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 251–257 10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rugg M. D., Vilberg K. L., Mattson J. T., Yu S. S., Johnson J. D., Suzuki M. (2012). Item memory, context memory and the hippocampus: fMRI evidence. Neuropsychologia 50, 3070–3079 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.06.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russchen F. T., Amaral D. G., Price J. L. (1987). The afferent input to the magnocellular division of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the monkey, Macaca fascicularis. J. Comp. Neurol. 256, 175–210 10.1002/cne.902560202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadikot A. F., Parent A., Francois C. (1992). Efferent connections of the centromedian and parafascicular thalamic nuclei in the squirrel monkey: a PHA-L study of subcortical projections. J. Comp. Neurol. 315, 137–159 10.1002/cne.903150203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salami M., Itami C., Tsumoto T., Kimura F. (2003). Change of conduction velocity by regional myelination yields constant latency irrespective of distance between thalamus and cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 6174–6179 10.1073/pnas.0937380100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saykin A. J., Gur R. C., Gur R. E., Mozley P. D., Mozley L. H., Resnick S. M., et al. (1991). Neuropsychological function in schizophrenia. Selective impairment in memory and learning. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 48, 618–624 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810310036007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann J. D. (2003). Vascular syndromes of the thalamus. Stroke 34, 2264–2278 10.1161/01.STR.0000087786.38997.9E [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scoville W. B., Milner B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 20, 11–21 10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Segal J. B., Williams R., Kraut M. A., Hart J., Jr. (2003). Semantic memory deficit with a left thalamic infarct. Neurology 61, 252–254 10.1212/01.WNL.0000073145.08816.E2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shapira-Lichter I., Oren N., Jacob Y., Gruberger M., Hendler T. (2013). Portraying the unique contribution of the default mode network to internally driven mnemonic processes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 4950–4955 10.1073/pnas.1209888110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherman S. M. (2007). The thalamus is more than just a relay. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 17, 417–422 10.1016/j.conb.2007.07.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherman S. M., Guillery R. W. (2002). The role of the thalamus in the flow of information to the cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 357, 1695–1708 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherman S. M., Guillery R. W. (2011). Distinct functions for direct and transthalamic corticocortical connections. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1068–1077 10.1152/jn.00429.2011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shimamura A. P. (1995). Memory and the prefrontal cortex. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 769, 151–159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shuren J. E., Jacobs D. H., Heilman K. M. (1997). Diencephalic temporal order amnesia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 62, 163–168 10.1136/jnnp.62.2.163 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slotnick S. D. (2013). The nature of recollection in behavior and the brain. Neuroreport 24, 663–670 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328362e47e [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smythies J. (1997). The functional neuroanatomy of awareness: with a focus on the role of various anatomical systems in the control of intermodal attention. Conscious. Cogn. 6, 455–481 10.1006/ccog.1997.0315 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soei E., Koch B., Schwarz M., Daum I. (2008). Involvement of the human thalamus in relational and non-relational memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 28, 2533–2541 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06536.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Speedie L. J., Heilman K. M. (1982). Amnestic disturbance following infarction of the left dorsomedial nucleus of the thalamus. Neuropsychologia 20, 597–604 10.1016/0028-3932(82)90033-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sperling R. A., Bates J. F., Chua E. F., Cocchiarella A. J., Rentz D. M., Rosen B. R., et al. (2003). fMRI studies of associative encoding in young and elderly controls and mild Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 74, 44–50 10.1136/jnnp.74.1.44 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire L. R., Kandel E. R. (2000). Memory: From Mind to Molecules. Henry: Holt and Company [Google Scholar]
- Squire L. R., Wixted J. T., Clark R. E. (2007). Recognition memory and the medial temporal lobe: a new perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 872–883 10.1038/nrn2154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staresina B. P., Davachi L. (2006). Differential encoding mechanisms for subsequent associative recognition and free recall. J. Neurosci. 26, 9162–9172 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2877-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stark C. E., Okado Y. (2003). Making memories without trying: medial temporal lobe activity associated with incidental memory formation during recognition. J. Neurosci. 23, 6748–6753 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staudigl T., Zaehle T., Voges J., Hanslmayr S., Esslinger C., Hinrichs H., et al. (2012). Memory signals from the thalamus: early thalamocortical phase synchronization entrains gamma oscillations during long-term memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia 50, 3519–3527 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.08.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steriade M. (2006). Grouping of brain rhythms in corticothalamic systems. Neuroscience 137, 1087–1106 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.10.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suchan B., Gayk A. E., Schmid G., Koster O., Daum I. (2008). Hippocampal involvement in recollection but not familiarity across time: a prospective study. Hippocampus 18, 92–98 10.1002/hipo.20371 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sziklas V., Petrides M. (1999). The effects of lesions to the anterior thalamic nuclei on object-place associations in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 559–566 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00448.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sziklas V., Petrides M. (2004). Egocentric conditional associative learning: effects of restricted lesions to the hippocampo-mammillo-thalamic pathway. Hippocampus 14, 931–934 10.1002/hipo.20044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taber K. H., Wen C., Khan A., Hurley R. A. (2004). The limbic thalamus. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 16, 127–132 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16.2.127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanji K., Suzuki K., Fujii T., Higano S., Yamadori A. (2003). A case of frontal network amnesia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 74, 106–109 10.1136/jnnp.74.1.106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thoma P., Zoppelt D., Wiebel B., Daum I. (2006). Recollection and familiarity in negative schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia 44, 430–435 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson-Schill S. L., D’Esposito M., Aguirre G. K., Farah M. J. (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94, 14792–14797 10.1073/pnas.94.26.14792 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsivilis D., Vann S. D., Denby C., Roberts N., Mayes A. R., Montaldi D., et al. (2008). A disproportionate role for the fornix and mammillary bodies in recall versus recognition memory. Nat. Neurosci. 11, 834–842 10.1038/nn.2149 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E. (1987). Multiple memory systems and consciousness. Hum. Neurobiol. 6, 67–80 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E. (2002). Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 1–25 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E., Markowitsch H. J. (1997). Memory beyond the hippocampus. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 7, 209–216 10.1016/S0959-4388(97)80009-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E., Markowitsch H. J. (1998). Episodic and declarative memory: role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 8, 198–204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tunbridge E. M., Farrell S. M., Harrison P. J., Mackay C. E. (2013). Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) influences the connectivity of the prefrontal cortex at rest. Neuroimage 68, 49–54 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uncapher M. R., Otten L. J., Rugg M. D. (2006). Episodic encoding is more than the sum of its parts: an fMRI investigation of multifeatural contextual encoding. Neuron 52, 547–556 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uncapher M. R., Rugg M. D. (2005a). Effects of divided attention on fMRI correlates of memory encoding. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 1923–1935 10.1162/089892905775008616 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uncapher M. R., Rugg M. D. (2005b). Encoding and the durability of episodic memory: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 25, 7260–7267 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1641-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Werf Y. D., Jolles J., Witter M. P., Uylings H. B. (2003a). Contributions of thalamic nuclei to declarative memory functioning. Cortex 39, 1047–1062 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70877-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Werf Y. D., Scheltens P., Lindeboom J., Witter M. P., Uylings H. B., Jolles J. (2003b). Deficits of memory, executive functioning and attention following infarction in the thalamus; a study of 22 cases with localised lesions. Neuropsychologia 41, 1330–1344 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00059-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van der Werf Y. D., Witter M. P., Groenewegen H. J. (2002). The intralaminar and midline nuclei of the thalamus. Anatomical and functional evidence for participation in processes of arousal and awareness. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 39, 107–140 10.1016/S0165-0173(02)00181-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann S. D. (2010). Re-evaluating the role of the mammillary bodies in memory. Neuropsychologia 48, 2316–2327 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann S. D., Aggleton J. P. (2004). The mammillary bodies: two memory systems in one? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 35–44 10.1038/nrn1299 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann S. D., Aggleton J. P., Maguire E. A. (2009a). What does the retrosplenial cortex do? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 792–802 10.1038/nrn2733 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann S. D., Tsivilis D., Denby C. E., Quamme J. R., Yonelinas A. P., Aggleton J. P., et al. (2009b). Impaired recollection but spared familiarity in patients with extended hippocampal system damage revealed by 3 convergent methods. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 5442–5447 10.1073/pnas.0812097106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann S. D., Albasser M. M. (2009). Hippocampal, retrosplenial, and prefrontal hypoactivity in a model of diencephalic amnesia: evidence towards an interdependent subcortical-cortical memory network. Hippocampus 19, 1090–1102 10.1002/hipo.20574 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann S. D., Saunders R. C., Aggleton J. P. (2007). Distinct, parallel pathways link the medial mammillary bodies to the anterior thalamus in macaque monkeys. Eur. J. Neurosci. 26, 1575–1586 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05773.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Victor M. A., Adams R. D., Collins G. H. (1989). The Wernicke-Korsakoff Syndrome and Related Neurologic Disorder Due to Alcoholism and Malnutrition. Philadelphia, PA: F.A. Davis [Google Scholar]
- Vilberg K. L., Rugg M. D. (2012). The neural correlates of recollection: transient versus sustained FMRI effects. J. Neurosci. 32, 15679–15687 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3065-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vincent J. L., Snyder A. Z., Fox M. D., Shannon B. J., Andrews J. R., Raichle M. E., et al. (2006). Coherent spontaneous activity identifies a hippocampal-parietal memory network. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 3517–3531 10.1152/jn.00048.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogt B. A., Hof P. R., Friedman D. P., Sikes R. W., Vogt L. J. (2008). Norepinephrinergic afferents and cytology of the macaque monkey midline, mediodorsal, and intralaminar thalamic nuclei. Brain Struct. Funct. 212, 465–479 10.1007/s00429-008-0178-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Cramon D. Y., Hebel N., Schuri U. (1985). A contribution to the anatomical basis of thalamic amnesia. Brain 108(Pt 4), 993–1008 10.1093/brain/108.4.993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Zerssen G. C., Mecklinger A., Opitz B., von Cramon D. Y. (2001). Conscious recollection and illusory recognition: an event-related fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 2148–2156 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01589.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voss J. L., Paller K. A. (2010). Bridging divergent neural models of recognition memory: introduction to the special issue and commentary on key issues. Hippocampus 20, 1171–1177 10.1002/hipo.20851 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner A. D., Shannon B. J., Kahn I., Buckner R. L. (2005). Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 445–453 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang X. J., Rinzel J. (1993). Spindle rhythmicity in the reticularis thalami nucleus: synchronization among mutually inhibitory neurons. Neuroscience 53, 899–904 10.1016/0306-4522(93)90474-T [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiler J. A., Suchan B., Daum I. (2010a). Foreseeing the future: occurrence probability of imagined future events modulates hippocampal activation. Hippocampus 20, 685–690 10.1002/hipo.20695 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiler J. A., Suchan B., Daum I. (2010b). When the future becomes the past: differences in brain activation patterns for episodic memory and episodic future thinking. Behav. Brain Res. 212, 196–203 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiler J. A., Suchan B., Koch B., Schwartz M. F., Daum I. (2010c). Differential impairment of remembering the past and imagining novel events after thalamic lesions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3037–3051 10.1162/jocn.2011.21633 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinberger D. R., Harrison P. J. (2011). Schizophrenia. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell [Google Scholar]
- Wheeler M. A., Stuss D. T., Tulving E. (1995). Frontal lobe damage produces episodic memory impairment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1, 525–536 10.1017/S1355617700000655 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiggs C. L., Weisberg J., Martin A. (1999). Neural correlates of semantic and episodic memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia 37, 103–118 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00044-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winocur G., Oxbury S., Roberts R., Agnetti V., Davis C. (1984). Amnesia in a patient with bilateral lesions to the thalamus. Neuropsychologia 22, 123–143 10.1016/0028-3932(84)90056-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witter M. P., Wouterlood F. G., Naber P. A., Van Haeften T. (2000). Anatomical organization of the parahippocampal-hippocampal network. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 911, 1–24 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb06716.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wixted J. T. (2007). Dual-process theory and signal-detection theory of recognition memory. Psychol. Rev. 114, 152–176 10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wixted J. T., Mickes L., Squire L. R. (2010). Measuring recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe. Hippocampus 20, 1195–1205 10.1002/hipo.20854 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yonelinas A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: a review of 30 years of research. J. Mem. Lang. 46, 441–517 10.1006/jmla.2002.286416899208 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yonelinas A. P., Aly M., Wang W. C., Koen J. D. (2010). Recollection and familiarity: examining controversial assumptions and new directions. Hippocampus 20, 1178–1194 10.1002/hipo.20864 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yonelinas A. P., Kroll N. E., Dobbins I., Lazzara M., Knight R. T. (1998). Recollection and familiarity deficits in amnesia: convergence of remember-know, process dissociation, and receiver operating characteristic data. Neuropsychology 12, 323–339 10.1037/0894-4105.12.3.323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young K. A., Manaye K. F., Liang C., Hicks P. B., German D. C. (2000). Reduced number of mediodorsal and anterior thalamic neurons in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 47, 944–953 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00826-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zikopoulos B., Barbas H. (2006). Prefrontal projections to the thalamic reticular nucleus form a unique circuit for attentional mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 26, 7348–7361 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5511-05.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zikopoulos B., Barbas H. (2012). Pathways for emotions and attention converge on the thalamic reticular nucleus in primates. J. Neurosci. 32, 5338–5350 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4793-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zola-Morgan S., Squire L. R., Mishkin M. (1982). The neuroanatomy of amnesia: amygdala-hippocampus versus temporal stem. Science 218, 1337–1339 10.1126/science.6890713 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zoppelt D., Koch B., Schwarz M., Daum I. (2003). Involvement of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in mediating recollection and familiarity. Neuropsychologia 41, 1160–1170 10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00019-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]