Abstract
Age is the only social category identifying subgroups that everyone may eventually join. Despite this, and despite the well-known growth of the older population, age-based prejudice remains an under-studied topic in social psychology. This paper systematically reviews the literature on ageism, highlighting extant research on its consequences and theoretical perspectives on its causes. We then identify a crucial gap in the literature: potential intergenerational tensions, speculating how a growing older population—and society’s efforts to accommodate it—might stoke intergenerational fires, particularly among the younger generation. Presenting both sides of this incipient issue, we review relevant empirical work that introduces reasons for both optimism and pessimism concerning intergenerational relations within an aging society. We conclude by suggesting future avenues for ageism research, emphasizing the importance of understanding forthcoming intergenerational dynamics for the benefit of the field and broader society.
Though age, gender, and race are the three primary dimensions of interpersonal categorization (e.g., Fiske, 1998; Kite, Deaux, & Miele, 1991; Kunda, 1999), only age encompasses categories that every living person potentially joins. Despite this universality, surprisingly scant research examines age-based prejudice, compared with racism and sexism (Nelson, 2004; 2005). Indeed, a quick PsycInfo search (February 2012) yields 8491 entries with the keyword “racism” and 2836 for “sexism,” but only 750 for “ageism.” Bugental and Hehman (2007) demonstrated the problem to be even more apparent when restricting the search to two of the premier social psychology journals; in the 20 years prior to their search, only one article on ageism appeared, compared with 50 and 33 respectively for racism and sexism. Some researchers have attempted to explain this lack of research focus by citing the socially-condoned nature of ageism, causing it to be overlooked altogether as a form of prejudice (Nelson, 2005; Palmore, 1999). Whatever the reason, age-based prejudice remains drastically under-investigated, despite the salience of age in interpersonal judgments.
Notwithstanding an overall lack of research on the subject, evidence does indicate age-prejudice to potentially complicate older people’s quality of life (Butler 1969; Nelson, 2004; Ng, 1998). As demonstrated by prejudice research concerning other social groups, stereotypes are not just static beliefs; rather, many have pernicious short- and long-term consequences. In the modern world, older people face reduced social and economic opportunities, damage to self-esteem, and exacerbated physical health problems, to name only a few consequences of ageist treatment. Adding more complexity, some note that along with negative elements of age-based stereotyping, “positive ageism” emerges via stereotypes of wisdom and happiness—in addition to practical benefits such special tax breaks, discounts and housing programs (Palmore, 1999). Regardless of one’s beliefs about the existence or nature of ageism, with average life expectancy increasing and the proportion of people over 65 in the United States estimated to double by the year 2030 (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2007a), improving elder well-being undoubtedly warrants urgent investigation.1
This paper represents an attempt to bring ageism into the scientific psychology mainstream through a socio-structural, intergenerational lens. We first systematically review the ageism literature, discussing (1) its complex consequences on older people and (2) theoretical perspectives that explain its causes. Although acknowledging the utility of these perspectives, we then identify an important gap in the literature: (3) intergenerational tension-based ageism, which will potentially—though not inevitably—intensify as the older population grows more prevalent and necessitates a redistribution of societal resources. Drawing from empirical literature on ageism, intergenerational perceptions, and other types of prejudice, we present both (4) empirical bases for pessimism and (5) empirical bases for optimism concerning how well generations might cope with shifting age dynamics. We conclude by suggesting (6) future avenues for research in this area, highlighting areas in which increased understanding of intergenerational perceptions seems particularly vital. Overall, this paper elucidates what is known about the complex domain of ageism, suggests ways in which the field can move forward in testing new theoretical (particularly intergenerational) ideas, and ultimately hopes to spur more researchers to investigate this increasingly important phenomenon.
1. Consequences of Ageism: Subtle and Complex
What research has been conducted on age-based prejudice and discrimination demonstrates it to be surprisingly pervasive, potentially infecting numerous societal facets. For instance, ageism appears in medicine, where medical schools under-emphasize geriatrics (Levenson, 1981) and older people often face less aggressive treatment for common ailments, which are dismissed as a natural part of aging (Bowling, 1999; 2007). In the workplace, despite considerable research indicating that job performance does not decrease with age (e.g., Cleveland & Landy, 1983; Liden, Stillwell, & Ferris, 1996; McEvoy & Cascio, 1989), evidence indicates that older job applicants are rated less positively than younger ones, even when they are similarly qualified (Avolio & Barrett, 1987). Many older people also face discrimination in the form of abuse and neglect, in nursing homes (e.g., Griffore, Barboza, Mastin, Oehmke, Schiamberg, & Post, 2009; Malmedal, Ingebrigsten, & Saveman, 2009) and even within their own families (e.g., Coyne, Reichman, & Berbig, 1993; Pillemer & Wolf, 1986; Ramsey-Klawsnik, 2004). Still more disturbing, this form of ageism is likely under-reported, due to caseworkers and doctors being less familiar with elder abuse than other forms of domestic violence (Nelson, 2005). Media representation of older people also tends to reflect age-based biases, as older people are traditionally underrepresented and typecast on television shows (Signorelli, 2001), precluded from lead roles in movies (Bildtgard, 2000), and stereotyped in magazine advertisements (Miller, Miller, McKibbin, & Pettys, 1999). Given the diverse societal facets of ageism, some argue that this form of prejudice is currently more prevalent than racism and sexism (Age Concern, 2008; Banaji, 1999; Rupp, Vodanovich, & Credé, 2005).
However, given ageism’s noted complexity, this potentially bleak picture warrants some qualification. For instance, the U.S. government does allocate a disproportionate amount of healthcare dollars toward the over-65 population; more than one-third of healthcare spending goes toward the older population, despite their forming a mere 12% of the total population (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2011). And whereas the family can be a source of abuse and neglect, it is far more often a source of positive elder interaction—to the point of helping foster positive attitudes toward older people in general (Harwood, Hewstone, Paolini, & Voci, 2005; Hewstone, Harwood, Voci, & Kenworthy, 2006). Moreover, although older people may not be commonly represented in mainstream media, evidence indicates that when they are featured, they are done so positively (e.g., happy, active, and strong; Dail, 1998; Roy & Harwood, 1997), are neglected in certain domains only (e.g., car marketing, but not financial service advertisements; Roy & Harwood, 1997), and experience disparate outcomes depending on gender (older men come across more favorably than women; Vernon, Williams, Phillips, & Wilson, 1990). Complicating things further, despite ageism’s apparent prevalence, emotional well-being tends to increase with age (Carstensen, 1995; Carstensen, Pasupathi, Mayr, & Nesselroade, 2000). All of this reinforces the multifaceted nature of ageism and its consequences on targets.
Ageism is complex from the perpetrator’s perspective as well. In fact, much of what would be considered ageist treatment is quite subtle in nature, and often has well-intentioned roots. For instance, just as people address small children, well-meaning people unwittingly speak to older people using benevolent-yet-patronizing “baby talk” and demeaning, exaggeratedly slow and loud over-accommodation (Giles, Fox, Harwood, & Williams, 1994; Nelson, 2005; Williams & Giles, 1998). (This occurs more in certain contexts—such as hospitals—than in others; Hummert, Shaner, Garstka, & Henry, 1998.) Distancing is another common, indirect form of ageist discrimination, and includes forms both physical (e.g., placing older people in retirement homes, avoiding places typically frequented by older people) and psychological (e.g., emphasizing differences in attitudes and traits between oneself and older people; Greenberg, Schimel, & Mertens, 2004). Sometimes the intentions that yield ageist consequences are even more explicitly positive, such as when discourse with anti-ageist intent (e.g. “youthful old age”) backfires by making poor health and frailty seem deviant (Coupland & Coupland, 1993). Similarly, governmental attempts to help the older population have been described as representing double-edged “compassionate ageism.” In this sense, economically assisting the “deserving” older population is apparently generous but nevertheless reinforces stereotypes of poverty, frailty, and dependence (Binstock, 2010). As another indication of ageism’s often well-intentioned roots, predominant perceptions of older people as worthy of pity foster the typical behavioral combination of well-intentioned active facilitation (desire to help), but deleterious passive harm (neglecting and demeaning; Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2007). Ageism’s indirectness factors heavily in workplace age discrimination, making it notoriously difficult to prove in court, despite age being a protected category under Title VII of the Civil Rights Act of 1964.
Notwithstanding perpetrators’ good intentions, stealth ageism and general societal beliefs about aging can take a toll on older people themselves. Negative stereotypes—such as forgetfulness—are disconcertingly difficult to overcome, persistent in the eyes of perceivers even in the face of targets’ stereotype-incongruent behavior (Cuddy, Norton, & Fiske, 2005). Older people may start to talk, think, and move more slowly as a result of receiving over-accommodated speech (Giles et al., 1994). Many internalize negative stereotypes, becoming more forgetful, sickly, and depressed, simply because they anticipate adopting such characteristics in their later life (Whitbourne & Sneed, 2004). Others may suffer lower self-esteem or less control as a result of ageist self-fulfilling prophecies (Rodin & Langer, 1980). With age, people risk internalizing negative age stereotypes; then, via a process of “self-stereotyping,” these internalizations present a host of deleterious consequences, including failing memory, cognitive confusion, physical frailty, and even cardiovascular responses to stress (Levy & Leifheit-Limson, 2009; Levy, Zonderman, Slade, & Ferrucci, 2009). Old-age stereotypes are powerful enough that in some cases young people will enact them if primed with elder stereotypes (Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996), despite implicitly holding positive attitudes toward older people (Cesario, Plaks, & Higgins, 2006).
Fortunately, research has begun to focus on ways to combat ageism’s negative effects. A particularly encouraging line of research has demonstrated the importance of older people’s positive self-perceptions—that is, their attitudes toward their own aging process. Holding positive self-perceptions of aging yields numerous benefits, including increased functional health and longevity, as well as resistance to cardiovascular problems and hearing loss (Levy, Slade, & Gill, 2006; Levy, Slade, & Kasl, 2002; Levy, Slade, Kunkel & Kasl, 2002; Levy et al., 2009). In a related vein, the subtle priming of positive aging stereotypes improves performance in related domains, particularly memory tasks (Levy, 1996). Moreover, notwithstanding Bargh et al.’s seminal finding, in many cases younger adults often do not assimilate to primed age stereotypes; in fact, a contrast effect can emerge if the old-age prime is clearly irrelevant to their age group identity (such as when an elder exemplar spurs younger people to actually walk faster; Dijksterhuis et al., 1998). All of these findings provide hope that older people can combat the effects of negative age stereotypes if they perceive such perceptions as off base. Still, this relatively new line of research needs more investigation.
Another positive development has been the recent rise of research on the benefits of getting older, rather than its handicaps. Numerous researchers have tested the notion that “older is wiser,” and a variety of paradigms have demonstrated the veracity of this statement. For instance, older people act more rationally (i.e., stably) than their juniors across various problem-solving tasks (Tentori, Osherson, Hasher, & May, 2001) and more wisely reason about social conflicts (Grossmann et al., 2010). And whereas people do face declines in fluid intelligence as they age (i.e., slower in cognitive speed and novel processing; e.g., Bugg, Zook, DeLosh, Davalos, & Davis, 2006), they do not show the same pattern for crystallized knowledge or experience (e.g., Horn & Cattell, 1967). A significantly positive relationship between age and cognitive complexity also emerges in the realm of language use (Pennebaker & Stone, 2003). And as noted, older people are generally more emotionally healthy, enjoying higher levels of socio-emotional regulation, conscientiousness, agreeableness, and duration of positive emotional experience (Blanchard-Fields, 2007; Carstensen & Mikels, 2005; Carstensen et al., 2000; Helson, Kwan, John, & Jones, 2002; Williams et al., 2006) as well as reduced neuroticism (Loehlin & Marin, 2001). Despite these boons, exaggerated beliefs about sad, frail, lonely elders persist, from the interpersonal to the societal level. To understand why this might be the case, we now turn to theoretical explanations that account for ageism.
2. Extant Social-Psychological Theories Used to Explain Ageism
Clearly the overall picture of ageism is complicated. Fortunately, theoretical perspectives on ageism—largely applied from existing, more general social-psychological theories—have provided a significant foundation for understanding the roots of age-based prejudice. Such theories provide a robust understanding, operating at several levels: individual, interpersonal, evolutionary, and socio-cultural. We highlight each in turn, but focus on the latter level as the most relevant to the current paper, given its focus on ageism’s potential broad, socio-structural, intergenerational roots.
Individual-Level Theories
At the individual level, theorists have described age stereotyping as serving an ego-protective function. For instance, one approach conceptualizes ageism in terms of terror management theory (TMT, e.g., Becker, 1973), positing that when confronted with the realization of their own mortality, people push away reminders of eventual death (i.e., older people) and identify more closely with similar others (i.e., younger or middle-aged people; Greenberg et al., 2004; Popham, Kennison, & Bradley, 2011).
Social Identity Theory (SIT), which highlights the relationship between personal identity and group identity, along with the need to feel positive about one’s group, may also account for ageism. According to SIT’s individual-level predictions, younger individuals should identify more strongly with their ingroup (other younger people) and consequently push away outgroup members (older people) in an effort to promote self-esteem (Kite & Wagner, 2004; Tajfel & Turner, 1979). (Older people may also do so, but as noted we focus here on prejudice against elders, rather than among them.)
A functional approach to age stereotyping (Snyder & Miene, 1994) likewise explains ageism at the individual level: Ageism may serve an ego-protective function that buffers the self from the threat of getting older (similar to TMT), or a social function that facilitates interaction with the young ingroup (similar to SIT). Buttressing all of these individual-level theories is the finding that negative attitudes toward death significantly predict devaluation of older adults among younger and middle-aged people (Collette-Pratt, 1976; Montepare & Zebrowitz, 2004).
Interpersonal Theories
Other theories step away from the individual and focus directly on face-to-face interactions. Many of these perspectives emphasize the role of older people’s physical appearance in fostering ageism (Palmore, 2003). One such premise is that ageism develops due to negative halo effects—that is, because older people are perceived as generally unattractive, they are also seen as having negative traits and abilities (Langlois et al., 2000). Another approach incorporates overgeneralization effects, whereby people believe that because older people’s appearance implies certain traits (e.g., lonesome, inferred from droopy eyes; sad, inferred from stooped posture), then they must be that way (Montepare & Zebrowitz, 2004). One other appearance-based theory centers on how older people’s facades generally lend clues about interaction potential. These clues, dubbed social affordances, may cause children, for instance, to learn that the wrinkles or slow gait in an older person signify someone who is not enthusiastic or outgoing (Palmore, 2003). Each of these theories highlights the idea that older people may be devalued simply from having more repellent bodily “blemishes” than the average person; these uncontrollable, highly visible characteristics undoubtedly devalue older people, paralleling other types of stigma as a marked status (Bugental & Hehman, 2007; Crocker, Major, & Steele, 1998; Jones et al., 1984).
Evolutionary Theories
Some ageism accounts suggest an evolutionary basis for the devaluation of older people. One study found that in hypothetical decisions to help, Darwinian cues for inclusive fitness lead to decisions with ageist implications—specifically, younger, fit people are favored over older, infirm people, and the healthy are favored over the sick (Burnstein, Crandall, & Kitayama, 1994). Another suggestion is that people have evolved to perceive changes in older people’s appearance and behavior as signs of weakness (Jensen & Oakley, 1980). Though not addressing ageism directly, one evolutionary approach to stigma (Kurzban & Leary, 2001) identifies people who indicate contagion (e.g., frequent illness) and those who lack the promise of ongoing resource-holding as prime targets for social exclusion; predominant perceptions of older people fit the bill on both counts. Others cite socio-developmental evidence that people are hard-wired to hold ageist beliefs. Illustrating this, children hold prejudicial feelings and stereotypic beliefs about older people as early as age three (Seefeldt, Jantz, Gapler, & Serock, 1977), which continues through adolescence (Doka, 1985–6), apparently rendering inevitable lifelong prejudices (Montepare & Zebrowitz, 2004).
A broader, socio-evolutionary, sociofunctional perspective of intragroup relations can also explain ageism (Cottrell & Neuberg, 2005; Cottrell, Neuberg, & Li, 2007). According to this theory, people have evolved to live in effective, interdependent groups to maximize group (and consequently individual) success. As a result, when certain group members threaten group returns, corresponding reactions ensue. Older people may be perceived as threats in that they allegedly cannot reciprocate benefits from other group members. This generally leads to paternalistic, prosocial helping behavior toward them as ingroup members, but in some cases their perceived inadequacy in assisting ingroup success can also foster anger and resentment. Given its broad applicability to different social groups living in a society (particularly older people), we will return to the sociofunctional approach in our socio-structural discussion of ageism.
Socio-cultural Theories
Socio-historical accounts of ageism cite major events that caused society as a whole to evolve in ageist ways. Nelson (2005) emphasizes two such occasions: the first, the advent of the printing press, naturally improved record-keeping of important events; this in turn replaced the traditional storytelling, wisdom-sharing role of older people. The second turning point was the industrial revolution, which necessitated greater mobility in the family (to re-locate for available jobs) and placed increased value on workers who could quickly adapt to and perform difficult manual labor tasks, with less value on experienced employees per se (Butler, 2009; Nelson, 2005). Some theorists have suggested other causes as a result of modernization. For instance, improved education has created a literate majority of young people and thus reduced the role of older people as primary sources of knowledge. Another consequence of modernization is improved medical care, which has created a disproportionately large (and ever-increasing) older population that society has not traditionally accommodated (Cuddy & Fiske, 2004).
In a related vein, a social role perspective on ageism might link perceptions of older people with the societal roles they are seen as playing—a theory used to explain beliefs about other social groups (predominantly women; Eagly, 1987). For instance, because many older adults are retired, they might be correspondingly perceived as less agentic (Kite & Wagner, 2004). These hypotheses may explain why older people are less valued than in the past, and indeed studies show that an unfortunate but common attitude toward older people is that they are dispensable and useless members of society (Levy & Banaji, 2004).
Other theories focus on modern social structure in fostering prejudice, including ageism. The Stereotype Content Model (SCM; Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick, 2007; Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002) theorizes that the dimensions of warmth (“How friendly and trustworthy are this person’s intentions?”) and competence (“How well can this person enact those intentions?”) are fundamental in people’s perceptions of others. As established by convenience samples of older and younger adults and by a U.S. national random-sample survey, warmth and competence combinations drive perceivers’ stereotypes, emotional prejudices, and behavioral reactions toward members of specific groups. From this standpoint, older people by default compose a pitied social group—that is, stereotypically warm but incompetent. This pattern is in contrast to pride-inducing societal reference groups (stereotypically high in both warmth and competence), envied groups (competent but not warm), and contemptible, disgusting groups (low on both).
Reflecting ageism’s complexity, mainstream society quite reliably stereotypes older people in a simultaneously positive and negative light—that is, as both warm and incompetent (Cuddy & Fiske, 2004; Cuddy, Norton, & Fiske, 2005; Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002; Glick & Fiske, 2001). The high-warmth, low-competence perception of older people is not only pervasive—spanning Eastern cultures traditionally believed to hold their elders in higher esteem—but also persistent, even in the face of counter-stereotypic behavior (Cuddy et al., 2009; Cuddy, Norton, & Fiske, 2005). Importantly, warmth and competence stereotypes drive (and derive from) socio-structural perceptions of cooperativeness and status, respectively. Given their high-warmth, low-competence stereotype, in the default case older people are correspondingly viewed as non-competitive and low-status (e.g., Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002). Low-competence elder stereotypes resemble the sociofunctional threat of non-reciprocation, as well as social role theory’s described lack of agency.
Overall, these theories might seem pessimistic, portraying older people as irrelevant in a modernized society. However, because an ever-growing older population will inevitably change the social structure of age, and because older people will become increasingly difficult to ignore, so too might social structure-based perceptions of older people shift. But resembling ageism’s overall complexity, this shift can occur in a potentially positive or negative direction. In light of the socio-cultural perspectives, which best capture broad societal perceptions of older people changing for the better or for the worse, we discuss these possibilities in the coming sections.
3. An Important Gap: Potential Socio-structural, Intergenerational Tensions
A crux underlying virtually all of the reviewed ageism theories is that older people are largely absent from the mainstream—out of societal sight and mind. Individually, people are motivated to push them away (e.g., TMT, SIT). They are allegedly unattractive and representative of undesirable traits from which people dissociate themselves (interpersonal). Ostensibly representing illness and contagion, they are to be avoided (evolutionary). Broader socio-cultural theories portray older people as peripheral members of society: low in status and competitiveness (SCM), lacking in agency (social role theory), and allegedly non-reciprocating (sociofunctional) and useless in an industrialized, modernized world (socio-historical). All of these notions jibe with common and all-too-accurate depictions of older people as “invisible,” existing far away from conventional media, marketing and culture (Robinson & Skill, 1995; Thompson, 2001; Thompson & Thompson, 2009).
Nevertheless, socio-structural age dynamics are quickly changing in the United States. As noted, the older population is expected to double in the next twenty years. By 2050, the number of older people should swell to almost one-fourth of the population, outnumbering the number of children for the first time in history (Gale, 2010; United Nations, 2002, 2009). According to experts, growth trends portend half of all children born since 2000 living past their 100th birthday (Tugend, 2011). In other words, the people society now considers older and irrelevant are about to become far more common and visible—perhaps more so than ever in modern society.
A more noticeable older population presents the very real chance of antiquating invisibility-driven stereotypes. Nevertheless, the way in which such a redefinition occurs could yield either positive or negative consequences. That is, the new older population will be bigger and potentially more influential than ever, but also potentially more depletive, putting an unprecedented burden of care on younger generations. Either way, prevailing beliefs about older age eventually making way for young may not continue to function as they traditionally have.
Various media outlets have taken note of these possibilities. For instance, The New York Times now contains a regular online section on “The New Old Age” (http://newoldage.blogs.nytimes.com). A plethora of bestselling books have also emerged in this arena, both relatively optimistic (e.g., Ken Dychtwald’s [1999] Age Wave: How the 21st Century Will Be Ruled by the New Old) and relatively pessimistic (e.g., Susan Jacoby’s [2011] Never Say Die: The Myth and Marketing of the New Old Age). Posturing on budgetary issues concerning Medicare and Social Security, political leaders have also recognized the delicate, hot-button issue of the newly-prominent older population and its implications for intergenerational resource distribution. In all cases, there is potential for resentment against the older population as policymakers strive to maintain generational balance.
Unfortunately, psychological research has lagged behind mainstream pundits in pondering the intergenerational consequences of an aging population. In fact, social scientists have seldom cited generational differences as a mechanism in age prejudice at all (for a notable exception, see Hagestad & Uhlenberg, 2005). Braithwaite (2004) does note that “Stepping back to take a broader view of our institutional structures for dealing with all types of ‘isms’ may be a necessary first step for making progress on addressing ageism,” (p. 332). But despite this, and in spite of other social psychologists lamenting social psychology’s lack of focus on broad socio-structural forces (Oishi, Kesebir, & Snyder, 2009), intergenerational dynamics are nevertheless largely missing from the ageism literature.
Part of the reason for psychology’s lack of intergenerational focus might be that research has been largely uncertain about whether younger people particularly hold ageist attitudes toward their elders (Kite & Wagner, 2004). In identifying ageist perpetrators—at least as measured by general attitudes toward aging and the aged—studies have found equal evidence for younger and older ageists (Bailey, 1991; Chasteen, Schwarz, & Park, 2002; Nosek, Banaji, & Greenwald). Some research even suggests that older people themselves are the greater culprits (Hellbusch, Corbin, Thorson, & Stacy, 1995; Kite, Stockdale, Whitley, & Johnson, 2005), corroborated by recent evidence that younger people hold greater positive attitudes toward older people than previously thought (as measured by the Fraboni Scale of Ageism; Fraboni, Saltstone, & Hughes, 1990; Lin, Bryant, & Boldero, 2011). But other studies implicate younger people as the greatest endorsers of negative old-age stereotypes (Finkelstein, Burke, & Raju, 1995; Kalavar, 2001; Sanders, Montgomery, Pittman, & Balkwell, 1984; Rupp et al., 2005). Although clearly inconclusive, this overall body of research suggests that people of all ages are prone to reacting negatively to the concepts of aging and the aged, presumably due to universally associated negative characteristics of both (e.g., reminders of mortality and unattractiveness).
Still, a socio-structural analysis might yield different results, in that different generations might feel differently about generational deservingness. In the modern era, age groups typically take their turn at reaping different levels of societal resources. Younger people start out with very few resources; for instance, many are dependent on caregivers for support, and society curbs various freedoms (e.g., driving and voting restrictions; Garstka, Schmitt, Branscombe, & Hummert, 2004; Westman, 1991). But as people grow into middle age, they reap many more resources—including maximal prestige, influence, income, wealth, employment, mainstream media coverage, and societal leadership positions (Garstka, Schmitt, Branscombe, & Hummert, 2004; Gerbner, 1998; Rodríguez, Díaz-Giménez, Quadrini, & Ríos-Rull, 2002; Szafran 2002; Todaro, 2003; U.S. Census Bureau, 2009). Eventually at some point in older age people begin to tail off, largely stepping down from prominent positions and ceding a degree of resources (and, as discussed, less mainstream exposure). Though this pattern may not hold universally within particular societal sectors (e.g., athletics may favor the younger, and certain political offices may favor the older), broader society tends to follow default age progression—spurring pity for older people but also relegating them to a low-status, irrelevant position.
But the rapid growth of the older population, along with the potentially more visible “new old age” alters the system, adding a layer of ambiguity to the traditional pattern and changing the dynamics of intergenerational interdependence. It is not so clear whether younger generations will appreciate an old-age revolution if they themselves are negatively affected by it (as underscored by Social Security concerns). In other words, in contrast to global, negative attitudes toward aging—which, as noted, potentially implicates people of all ages—socio-structural, resource-based research approaches might yield more age-differentiated results, with people tending to favor their own generation. This is not to say that intergenerational conflict is inevitable, nor that pure harmony is entirely feasible; each potential outcome presents a wealth of empirical and social questions to be investigated. For now, we can draw upon the empirical literature on ageism, age relations, and general intergroup prejudice to infer reasons for both optimism and pessimism in forthcoming intergenerational relations and perceptions.
4. Empirical Bases for Intergenerational Pessimism
Several lines of evidence suggest grounds for pessimism based on intergenerational interdependence: resource threat, “benevolent” prejudices, prescriptive stereotypes, and age progressions.
Resource Threat
As noted, age prejudice lacks theory on potential intergenerational competition over resources. Nonetheless, social psychological theories on other forms of intergroup prejudice do often stem from a finite-resource perspective (Allport, 1954)—a concept that dates back to at least the early 20th century (Sumner, 1906). For instance, classic Realistic Group Conflict Theory (RCGT; Sherif, Harvey, White, Hood, & Sherif, 1961) states that competition over scarce resources drives prejudice between groups. Other theories, such as the SCM and the sociofunctional approach, also view perceived conflict as a source of prejudice. Given an aging population, scarce resources, and the social structure of age almost certainly changing from the status quo in some form, forthcoming circumstances present risk for intergenerational prejudice and tension—particularly from younger generations who might bear the brunt of taking care of an enlarged older population.
From the standpoint of age specifically, it is possible that older people who increasingly delay retirement or receive an increasingly disproportionate amount of government funding will face backlash from a younger generation eager to take their turn at such societal rewards. Though empirical work in this area is sorely lacking, various social scientists have at least speculated about impending “age wars” between younger and older generations over scarce resources (Binstock, 2005; Dychtwald, 1999; Hamil-Luker, 2001; Kingson, Hirshorn, & Cornman, 1986; Longman, 1986; Minkler, 2006). In fact, some have argued that the very concept of a generation stems from inter-age conflict over cultural resources (Turner, 1998).
Although work on age-based resource tension is lacking, broader work on prejudice indicates that favorable or mixed views of a stigmatized outgroup can quickly turn into purely antagonistic ones if the outgroup becomes a direct threat (Dear & Gleeson, 1991; Lee, Farrell, & Link, 2004). Likewise, when an outgroup’s goals conflict with those of the ingroup, the ingroup ascribes negative traits toward the outgroup (e.g., untrustworthy, ill-intentioned) and experiences negative feelings toward them (Fiske & Ruscher, 1993). If the younger generation starts perceiving the older one as inhibiting their own success, these reactions are likely to be particularly strong. Sociofunctionally speaking, “people are most attuned to threats to ingroup success when there are tangible outcomes at stake” (Cottrell & Neuberg, 2005, pp. 772–773).
Benevolent Prejudices
Extant prejudice research also depicts subtle, benevolent forms of prejudice (which ageism is, as delineated) turning overt and hostile due to interdependence-based tensions. For instance, the combination of male dominance in society and male-female interdependence in intimate spheres forms benevolent sexism; this combines paternalistic, benevolent prejudice (e.g., chivalry) toward women if they “know their place” but backlash and resentment if they act in threateningly nontraditional manners (as is the case with activist and agentic women; Glick & Fiske, 1996, 2001; Rudman & Glick, 2001). Likewise, older forms of race relations spurred Whites to be motivated to view African Americans as benignly subservient—stemming from the combination of White superiority beliefs and the need for Black labor (Baron, 2000)—but risked hostility if Blacks acted in ways that were too assertive or “uppity” (Jackman, 1996). (Such a reaction is also reflected in people’s willingness to scapegoat minority groups when they become successful; Glick, 2005.) Naturally, age groups are similarly independent, as they tend to live in the same society (not to mention often in the same family), and draw from the same resource pool. Thus, the risk for generational tension will be particularly ripe if younger generations view older ones as increasingly overstepping their boundaries.
Prescriptive Stereotypes
Potential beliefs about what older people deserve in relation to the young reflect “should”-based, prescriptive stereotypes, which attempt to maintain a certain social status quo and control what other social groups are to do (in contrast with descriptive, “are”-based stereotypes; Burgess & Borgida, 1999; Fiske & Stevens, 1993; Prentice & Carranza, 2002; Terborg, 1977). Again, researchers have applied this concept to forms of intergroup prejudice other than age, such as sexism (Rudman & Glick, 2001). However, as noted, forthcoming age trends might signal a broad violation of the traditional progression of age—whereby older people do not transition away from mainstream society as much as they traditionally have, thereby (possibly) infringing upon the traditional territory of younger generations.
Age Progressions
Despite ageism’s apparent similarities with other forms of prejudice, some elements are likely to differentiate it, given the unique, inevitable, universal progression of age. One approach proposes three distinct, age-driven domains, within which younger people might be particularly motivated to hold prescriptive stereotypes aimed at curbing elder control of resources: ensuring older people’s active succession of enviable resources, minimizing the older population’s passive consumption of shared resources, and prevention of elder identity infringement on symbolic resources. Though these domains yield various empirical questions to be tested, recent work does already implicate the young as the strongest endorsers of such stereotypes (North & Fiske, 2010, 2011). In their focus on a turn-based queue, these domains conceptualize a natural age-driven progression—thereby differentiating age (and ageism) from other social categories (and prejudices).
For instance, age groups take turns in enjoying prime resources; as noted, at some point older people are expected to step aside and make way for younger generations. In this vein, Succession suggests younger people’s desire to limit older people’s active control of envied resources, such as wealth, seniority, political clout, and more recently, employment. Some pundits argue that elder perceptions will shift increasingly from pity to envy, based on the predicted upsurge in healthy, pensioned older people with greater disposable incomes than many young workers (Friis, 1991). Others argue that the prevailing stereotype of American elders already constitutes a wealthy and powerful voting bloc (Binstock, 1985; Minkler, 2006). The swelling of the older population might only exacerbate tensions made apparent by the recent recession, which has made all too salient the limited number of available jobs. A poor economy sparks debates about mandatory retirement within diverse professional fields, ranging from neurosurgery and gastroenterology, to aviation and the judiciary, and even to academia (Day, 2009; La Corte, 2009; Scarrow, Linskey, Asher, Anderson, & Selden, 2009; Thomson, Bernstein, & Leddin, 2008; Wilber, 2007). As real-world evidence for the delicacy of succession issues, older-worker layoffs and age discrimination claims have already reached record numbers (Elmer, 2009). From the standpoint of the younger generation, the outcry for older people to “just retire already” has intensified in recent years (e.g., Quindlen, 2009), and may only increase as the population’s age imbalance grows.
Additionally, age groups take turns at reaping shared resources, such as when younger and middle-aged people pay into Social Security with the promise of enjoying the benefits in later life. Thus, in contrast to Succession’s active withholding of desirable resources and positions, a second potential area of intergenerational tension revolves around passive Consumption, or depletion of shared, allotted resources. From the standpoint of the younger generation, a swollen older population might necessitate a redistribution of resources that will favor the old. Already, despite the fact that there are twice as many children as older people, governmental spending ratios range from four-to-one to three-to-one in favor of the older population (Howard, 2008). Additionally, despite currently representing less than a quarter of the total population, older people consume 51 percent of government expenditures for social services (Minkler, 2006). The predicted increase of the older population intensifies fears that older people will bankrupt the economy and pillage natural resources by their sheer existence (Schulz & Binstock, 2006). With real concerns over Medicare and Social Security running out of funds (e.g. Wolf, 2011), younger people might resent elder depletion of social programs that the former may never enjoy. Complicating the issue, empirical experiments utilizing trolley-problem paradigms (where participants must choose to sacrifice one person to save a number of others or vice-versa) indicate that people generally sacrifice older and other low-status people to spare other types of people (Cikara, Farnsworth, Harris, & Fiske, 2010). This finding suggests that policies catered toward the older population might face significant backlash.
Age groups take turns enjoying symbolic resources as well; for instance, what is considered “cool” among younger people tends to differ drastically from what is valued by other age groups. From this, a third, more figurative domain of intergenerational tension, Identity, revolves around activities and roles usually reserved for the young. This dimension offers caution to those who envision a smooth reinvention of older age, with older people increasingly venturing into traditionally young territory. For example, research indicates that older people who attempt to cross ingroup boundaries, such as those who try to look younger, are no longer pitied but often resisted (at least as measured by vignettes about targets in their fifties; Schoemann & Branscome, 2010; Walz, 2002). In light of extant group identity-based theories, younger people might have particular motivation to maintain generational boundaries, for two main reasons. The first centers around maintaining self and group-level esteem; for instance, Social Identity Theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) would predict that younger people push away and demean older outgroup members as a means of protecting group-level self worth, as does a functional approach to ageist stereotyping (Snyder & Miene, 1994). Likewise, the creation of a strong, exclusive youth culture may be a way of asserting autonomy and esteem among younger people (Bytheway, 1995; Hagestad & Uhlenberg, 2005; Sardiello, 1998), satisfying higher-order needs for power and identity (Carroll, Howard, Vetere, Peck, & Murphy, 2002). Research supports these identity-based theoretical applications, in that younger people are more likely to mimic and look more favorably upon ingroup members who express stereotypic statements toward old-agers, rather than stereotype-inconsistent ones (Castelli, Pavan, Ferrari, & Kashima, 2009).
A second potential purpose of maintaining generational identity boundaries is the ego-protective maneuver of precluding identity threat. As noted, because older people are reminders of eventual mortality, younger people might emphasize a difference in attitudes and personality traits to maintain psychological distance (Greenberg et al., 2004). Snyder and Miene’s functional approach also encompasses such motivations, explaining how barring older people from the young ingroup buffers the self from its future negative aspects, such as declines in daily functioning and physical appearance. From a scarce-resource (pessimistic) perspective, diminishing the identity of older people causes them to be viewed as repellent and societally useless; younger people may use this to create social inequalities, mirroring other forms of stigma-based prejudice (Crocker, Major, & Steele, 1998).
Though more empirical investigation is warranted, from a theoretical standpoint, these domains (succession, consumption, identity) build upon the socio-structural theories of prejudice already described, in that each represents a potential elder departure from default perceptions of pity. In SCM terms, older people’s succession-based denial of desired resources might be associated with envy—that is, a forfeiture of traditional high-warmth attributions, but perhaps a (begrudging) gain of increased competence. By contrast, the consumption-based portrayal of older people as passive societal freeloaders might comprise the contemptuous combination of low warmth (i.e., selfishness) along with default low competence (i.e., dependency)—similar to that of other perceived “parasitic” social groups, such as homeless people. Additionally, identity motivations reinforce the ingroup pride cluster, in the sense that youth-based, ingroup boundaries exclude people (particularly older people) who are not “us.” A sociofunctional perspective would corroborate all of these notions: Succession violations might arouse anger or envy among younger people, and a consequent attempt to obtain desired resources (Cottrell & Neuberg, 2005), over-Consumption represents a threat to ingroup economic resources, property, and reciprocity relations—which arouses anger and disgust—and crossing Identity boundaries might be a “contamination by an unpalatable object or idea” that yields active avoidance or rejection (Cottrell & Neuberg, 2005, p. 772). Even social role theory would depict a newly “obstructive” older population as deviating from its traditional position of non-agency. In all cases, the growing older population and related violations of these intergenerational dimensions potentially give rise to novel, more hostile elder subtypes in the eyes of the young, as discussed later in this paper.
5. Empirical Bases for Intergenerational Optimism
Several lines of evidence suggest a more optimistic outlook: age-specific interests, improved elder images, increased contact, less cognitive impairment, and elder altruism.
Age-Specific Interests
In spite of the potentially distressing picture depicted in the prior section, intergenerational tensions are not inevitable. Some question the inherent structural conflict between younger and older age groups. For instance, in contrast to Turner’s (1998) noted views on generational conflict, Irwin (1996, 1998) counters that little empirical evidence supports different age groups’ sharing homogenous interests; by extension, there is therefore little motivation to pit their interests against those of other generations, as doomsayers claim. Similarly, Higgs and Gilleard (2010) argue that the situation is more complex than mere generational tensions over resources. In particular, they argue that social policies (such as Social Security) do not have nearly the impact on generational relative well-being that more powerful, volatile market forces do. A similar argument avers that the aging of the population is a fairly minor factor in the rising cost of health care, as compared to the alternative explanation of health-related costs simply outpacing the general rate of inflation (Binstock, 2010; Reinhardt, 2003).
Improved Elder Images
Moreover, though a larger, older population possibly enhances perceptions of intergenerational competition over resources, such a development can also improve long-held perceptions of older people. Research has already demonstrated that younger people, when given the opportunity, can subtype older people in meaningful and often positive ways. For example, Brewer, Dull, and Lui (1981) first showed that younger people reliably differentiate the nurturing “grandmother,” and distinguished “elder statesman” from the lonely “senior citizen.” Follow-up work by Schmidt and Boland (1986)—and later Hummert (1990)—has suggested that younger people’s favorable representations of older people can get even more specific, including the “John Wayne conservative,” “perfect grandparent,” and “sage.” A greater, more diverse pool of older people might mean even more opportunities for positive elder subtypes to emerge in the eyes of the young, particularly if the predicted redefinition of older age occurs.
Increased Contact
Additionally, shifting age dynamics will likely necessitate greater intergenerational interaction. Fortunately, a wealth of social psychological research suggests that intergroup contact can effectively reduce prejudice, under the right circumstances (Amir, 1969; Dovidio & Gaertner, 1999; Gaertner & Dovidio, 2000, Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). Specific to intergenerational interaction, high levels of contact with older people predict low levels of ageist stereotypes, regardless of perceiver age (Hale, 1998). Existing evidence already indicates that high-quality intergenerational interaction can be a successful buffer for elder stereotype threat, allowing older targets to overcome beliefs about cognitive impairment (Abrams, Eller, & Bryant, 2006). Other research suggests that arenas in which generations already tend to interact homogeneously–such as religious communities—are effectively free of ageist sentiment (Evans, 2011; Grefe, 2011). These findings indicate that more frequent intergenerational contact might yield significantly positive social outcomes.
Less Cognitive Impairment
When contact between members of different age groups has not worked, it has often been because older people confirm prior stereotypes of cognitive impairment (Griff, Lambert, Dellman-Jenkins, & Fruit, 1996; Seefeldt, 1987). But in line with the idea of older age’s reinvention, increasingly more research is starting to show that age-related cognitive decline is largely overblown (Verhaeghen, 2011). For example, the link between age-based neuroanatomical changes and age-related cognitive decline is less clear than traditionally believed (Salthouse, 2011). Naturally this is a contentious issue, and one that has already become a prime research topic, in large part due to the continual fight against Alzheimer’s disease. Nevertheless, much as increased contact might beget positive intergenerational perceptions, so too should increased awareness of the exaggerations of age-related cognitive decline.
Elder Altruism
More directly related to intergenerational competition, empirical evidence suggests that older people who are demonstrably altruistic can escape perceptions of stinginess and self-interest. For example, research indicates that many older people would be willing to give up their place in line for cardiac services and believe that this is the right thing to do (Mariotto et al., 1999). Likewise, despite stereotypes that they merely care about their own self-interests and unanimously endorse taxing the young for their own benefit, research shows that older people are actually much more altruistic and support generational equity much more often than most believe (Logan & Spitze, 1995). Thus, much as greater elder exposure should help disconfirm stereotypes of universal senility, increased public view of older people should also help alleviate exaggerations of self-interest.
That older people can garner favorable views by assisting younger ambitions also fits with the noted socio-cultural theories of ageism. From an SCM point of view, being perceived as appropriately assisting younger generations may suggest a degree of competence to go along with the traditional high-warmth stereotype of older people. In this more generous context, older people are likely to be perceived as trustworthy allies to younger success; as a result, reactions involving admiration (high warmth and high competence) might ensue. Similarly, from a sociofunctional perspective, the ability of older people to share (or give up) resources to benefit the young entails cooperative “ultrasociality,” which enhances perceptions of trustworthiness. Because these traits are considered extremely important among interdependent groups, they are likely to predict increased prosocial and affiliatory behaviors (Cottrell, Neuberg, & Li, 2007). Social role theory once again might conceptualize these newly-active older people as atypical of their traditional unagentic role, but in a more positive light. Still, how perceptions of a more visible, consuming older population develop comprises numerous empirical questions—largely dependent on how the “new old age” ultimately defines its societal place. We suggest particularly important directions for future research in the following section.
6. Future Research Avenues for Intergenerational Investigation
Proverbial wisdom suggests that it behooves younger people to be sympathetic toward their elders. Unfortunately, the anticipated graying of the population and a recessional societal resource pool may only increase generational competition, fostering resentment instead of respect. Nevertheless, the optimist might counter that a larger old population will increase opportunities for intergenerational contact and understanding, which has the potential to improve relations, increase understanding, and debunk intergroup misconceptions. Stronger, consistent relationships between old and young may form mutually respectful relationships that enhance the portrayal of older people as helpful societal allies. With this aim in mind, we suggest various future research directions.
Framing of the “Graying” Problem: Can Perceptions of Intergenerational Competition be Changed?
There is a certain “inconvenient truth” inherent in shifting age dynamics; a greater number of older people does inevitably increase societal influence and resource consumption, and a healthier older population might increasingly participate in traditionally young activities. Nevertheless, journalists, pundits, and other media merchants of doom may exaggerate competition between generations, and unflatteringly depict older people as wealthy yet costly burdens (Schulz & Binstock, 2006). Likewise, implicating Social Security and Medicare for society’s economic woes inflames canes-versus-kids battles, which may deflect attention from more central potential causes of economic crisis (e.g., income inequality; Minkler, 2006; Pollack, 1986). Inter-age hostility is likely to result from proclamations of a zero-sum intergenerational game.
Increased awareness of realities (rather than exaggerations) underlying these issues will become all the more important as the older population grows. Researchers should test interventions that de-emphasize the notion of older people as responsible for societal age inequalities or problems, and see if this increases how favorably younger people perceive them. Taking a cue from Binstock (2010), reframing “old-age entitlements” as part of a broader social contract that benefits all generations might go a long way to maintaining positive images of older people in an aging society.
Ensuring Successful Intergenerational Contact: What Works and What Doesn’t?
Another key research track should focus on the impact of intergenerational programs in undermining perceptions of intergenerational exploitation. Some research does evaluate such programs, which attempt to foster contact, cooperation and understanding between generations (e.g., Newman et al., 1997). Encouragingly, the benefits of intergenerational contact have already been demonstrated within the family: Frequent contact with grandparents predicts changing attitudes toward older people (Harwood et al., 2005), and higher levels of self-disclosure with grandparents leads to more positive explicit attitudes toward older people in general (Hewstone et al., 2006). As noted, many religious communities have demonstrated that contact is an effective mechanism for reducing ageism under certain circumstances.
However, to date it remains to be seen whether beneficial intergenerational contact extends beyond a few select domains. Indeed, the overall evidence of intergenerational programs consistently altering younger attitudes toward older people appears mixed at best (Montepare & Zebrowitz, 2004). Extending the benefits of intergenerational contact outside the family is not out of the realm of possibility, but a major obstacle is pervasive, socialized age segregation (Hagestad & Uhlenberg, 2005). This may be a primary reason that people generally hold ageist attitudes toward older people in general, despite holding mostly positive views toward specific older people that they know intimately (Kite & Johnson, 1988). As the burden of caring for an enlarged older population falls more heavily on the young, it is imperative that research continue to establish what works and what does not in intergenerational contact.
Established Elder Subtypes: Reintroducing the Young-Old Versus Old-Old Distinction
Despite lay beliefs that older people are relatively uniform in appearance, attitudes, and health, the older population is actually one of the most diverse. A particularly critical distinction lies between the “young-old”—the age group roughly 65 to 75, who are relatively healthy, active, and socially involved—and the less active “old-old” (McGinnis & Zelinsky, 2003; Neugarten, 1974; Yasuda et al., 1997). In terms of research on allocation of resources between generations, this division is crucial. For instance, policymakers may have an easier time making a case for healthcare resources being spent on more-active young-old versus the old-old. By contrast, debates about mandatory retirement might disproportionately target the young-old (who are more often still employed, and thus more at risk for accusations of delaying retirement). Again, these are empirical questions, but research findings concerning these potential differences will have important policy implications. In certain contexts, society’s traditional over-65 definition of “senior” may be more usefully broken down into these more meaningful subcategories. Notably, aging researchers have further revised these original subtypes to encompass an additional “oldest-old” category (e.g., Suzman, Willis, & Manton, 1992), suggesting that many are beginning to realize the importance of subtyping what has vaguely been conceptualized as a generalized older age.
Emergent Elder Subtypes: Hostile Ageism and Elder Admiration
Backlash from the young against older people who violate their alleged societal place represents a hostile form of ageism rarely considered in the ageism literature. In fact, a search for “hostile ageism” on PsycInfo currently yields exactly zero results. (Bugental & Hehman [2007] do speculate briefly that a hostile form of ageism might occur if older people appear to seek resources at the expense of the young, as the current paper has argued.) To date, research has indicated that, when ageism is expressed in a basically negative manner, it occurs more often via implicit measures than explicit ones (Bugental & Hehman, 2007; Cesario et al., 2006). However, younger people might have little reason to hide their resentment if they feel that their natural path to social resources is blocked by the older generation. This is especially so given that explicit expressions of common old-age stereotypes are rewarded by greater affiliation with young ingroup members (Castelli et al., 2009).
On the other hand, as noted in the “optimistic” section, older people can potentially break away from their predominant low-status perception in a positive way. One of the running themes of Ken Dychtwald’s (1999) Age Power: How the 21st Century Will Be Ruled By the New Old is that the next older generation will be unlike any other: larger, healthier, more active, and, with hope, more societally productive than merely consuming. This latter aspect may be the most important in changing mainstream perceptions of decrepitness, or from a socio-structural perspective, older people who refuse to stay in a low-status, unobtrusive place. New, societally-friendly social policies—such as the creation of an elder corps—could go a long way in not only accommodating an enlarged older population, but changing these negative perceptions. Nevertheless, empirical evidence on the potential impact of such policies is lacking, and thus this is a direction ripe for future research.
Cross-cultural Explorations
Cross-cultural research has been a major crux of social psychology for the past 30-plus years, but surprisingly little research has examined directly whether ageist beliefs differ between cultures. Prevailing wisdom is that ageist beliefs may not be as prevalent in Eastern cultures, which traditionally hold their elders in higher esteem (e.g. Nelson, 2009). One important factor in this belief is the historical Eastern tradition of filial piety, or xiao—the Confucian ideal that places responsibility on younger people to respect, obey, and care for their elders (Ng, 1998, 2000). A more general line of support for this hypothesis is that Eastern cultures tend to be more interdependent and holistic. Thus, people within them should allegedly more often seek to maximize collectivist, relational success—in contrast to more independent, individual agency-focused Western cultures (Nisbett, 2003). Given greater cultural focus on the relational, as well as a tradition of filial piety, Easterners theoretically should place greater importance on maintaining effective relationships with and taking care of elders.
However, empirical evidence that such beliefs result in a cultural reduction in ageism is surprisingly sparse. One notable exception is Levy and Langer’s (1994) study with older Chinese and American participants. They found that (a) views toward aging and memory performance were positively correlated, and (b) the Chinese elders exceeded the older Americans on a memory task. From this they concluded that cultural beliefs about aging shape older people’s degree of memory loss, which implied that Chinese cultural beliefs about aging were more positive. However, even this particular instance provides only indirect support, not aiming to directly demonstrate ageism being less common in Chinese culture.
Even more rare are comparisons based on intergenerational resource perspectives. Speculatively, beliefs about age-based allocation of societal resources may not be so different across cultures that are historically distinct but similarly industrialized in the modern world. As previously indicated, one plausible explanation for why older people may have come to be less valued than in the past is that they do not offer as much value in modernized societies. From this standpoint, even traditionally interdependent-focused cultures may come to perceive their elders as consuming but not contributing. As recent evidence, Eastern elders face many of the same forms of discrimination as Western ones do, such as outright abandonment in Japan (Fackler, 2010). Moreover, the warm-but-incompetent older-age stereotype pervades Eastern societies as well as Western ones (Cuddy et al., 2009), and even includes rural Eastern cultures (Chen, n.d.). More research is needed to make definitive cross-cultural conclusions, but a similarly aging population in the East implies that the risk for intergenerational ageist sentiment may prevail across borders.
Gender and Race Explorations
As the default conception of older people—like other social groups—likely comprises White, male targets, this presents an obvious bias in itself against women and minorities (admittedly, we ourselves have not speculated about demographic differences in this paper). But little empirical investigation focuses on the compounding factors of gender and race in prejudice toward older people. Concerning the former, some research indicates that older women, compared with older men, may be viewed more positively by younger people (Narayan, 2008), but other work suggests that older women might suffer the “double-whammy” of sexism in addition to ageism. For instance, attributions of dependency, ineffectiveness, and passivity might render their situation more dire than that of older men (Block, Davidson, & Grambs, 1981; Duncan & Loretto, 2004; Nuessel, 1982).
Concerning racial and ethnic differences, surprisingly little research investigates attitudes among African American or Latino populations toward older people. Preliminary findings might suggest that minority groups hold lower levels of ageism. For instance, African Americans report respect for older African Americans (Fiske, Bergsieker, Russell, & Williams, 2009). It is also possible that the Hispanic emphasis on familism—loyalty and solidarity among family members, including the extended family (Sabogal, Marin, Otero-Sabogal, Marin, & Perez-Stable, 1987)—and higher incidence of co-residence with older family members (Burr & Mutchler, 1999) may indicate lower levels of ageism among younger Latinos. In any case, these potential demographic differences are another domain that promises importance, considering that half of the older population is female, and that the current older population is fast becoming more ethnically diverse than any in U.S. history (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2007b).
Ageism Against the Young
Though its focus usually connotes prejudice toward older people, the word ageism naturally includes people discriminated against at any age. As under-researched as ageism generally is, even more scant is the sub-field of ageism against the young. Indeed, if intergenerational contentions exist, then they might exist in both directions. For instance, an older generation may view younger people as uncultured, and immature (as evidenced by Mark Bauerlein’s [2008] book, The Dumbest Generation: How the Digital Age Stupefies Young Americans and Jeopardizes Our Future). And while the current paper has outlined how younger people might prescriptively stereotype older people to stay out of the way, it remains to be seen whether older people hold corresponding resentments toward a younger generation that might be forced to support them. Such a development is not likely to transpire harmoniously if perceptions of inadequate, illiterate, and intransigent youngsters continue to stew.
Though research in this area is scarce, a small empirical foundation does suggest younger people’s being the target of prejudice from adults (if not older ones specifically). The workplace is a prime arena, as large-scale surveys find at least 25% of younger workers indicate the experience of some form of age discrimination (Loretto, Duncan, & White, 2000). Specifically, younger workers often feel persecuted in the form of negative (demeaning) attitudes, denial of promotions, and disproportionately lower pay (Duncan & Loretto, 2004). This latter belief is corroborated by statistical evidence that the relative wages and employments rates of young workers have fallen in recent decades (Blanchflower & Freeman, 1996). Moreover, outside the workplace, younger people face comparable forms of prejudice to their elders (as noted), such as patronizing speech (Giles & Williams, 1994) and restricted freedoms (Westman, 1991). As psychological research moves further into age-related territory, it behooves researchers to simultaneously consider ways in which all age groups endure age-based bias.
Conclusion: As the Field Moves Forward
A recurring theme throughout this paper is that an ever-growing older population might stoke issues of generational injustice. From this standpoint, despite society’s treatment of age-bias as a mere second-class civil rights issue (Cohen, 2009), ageism should become a prime research topic across multiple social sciences. Organizations such as the National Institute on Aging and the Alliance on Aging Research have been instrumental in nudging the social sciences into this important territory. Still, the need for more work—and broader, forward-thinking structural approaches—is evident, given changing age dynamics.
Fortunately, social psychologists already have the tools to increase understanding of this stealth phenomenon. By characterizing younger and older people as distinct social groups, social psychology can draw upon one of its strengths—a wealth of literature on intergroup and interpersonal biases—in order to shed light on intergenerational ageist resentments. Moreover, structural, intergenerational approaches present the advantage of taking into account both traditional psychological foundations of prejudice at the interpersonal level and the broader sociological contexts in which they occur. Such a multi-level perspective exemplifies what psychologists have recommended more generally (Oishi et al., 2009). Ageism research should certainly be no exception, especially within an increasingly complex, rapidly aging society.
Acknowledgments
We acknowledge the Princeton Joint Degree Program in Psychology and Social Policy for providing support to the first author. We also thank Jack Dovidio for helpful feedback on a prior version of this manuscript, and are grateful for the constructive criticism of four anonymous reviewers.
Footnotes
It is for this reason that the current paper focuses on elders rather than youth, though we acknowledge that ageism goes both ways (and in fact targets people of any age, given the right context). Moreover, much as racism and sexism presuppose certain targets of prejudice, ageism tends to target older people most saliently—rendering them the logical starting point in a discussion on the subject.
References
- Abrams D, Eller A, Bryant J. An age apart: The effects of intergenerational contact and stereotype threat on performance and intergroup bias. Psychology and Aging. 2006;21(4):691–702. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.4.691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Age Concern England. Ageism in Britain 2006: An age concern research briefing. London: Age Concern England; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Allport GW. The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books; 1954. [Google Scholar]
- Amir Y. Contact hypothesis in ethnic relations. Psychological Bulletin. 1969;71(5):319–342. doi: 10.1037/h0027352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avolio BJ, Barrett GV. Effects of age stereotyping in a simulated interview. Psychology and Aging. 1987;2(1):56–63. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.2.1.56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey WT. Knowledge, attitude, and psychosocial development of young and old adults. Educational Gerontology. 1991;17(3):269–274. [Google Scholar]
- Banaji MR. Unconscious isms: Examples from racism, sexism, and ageism. Paper presented at the Way Women Lean Conference; New Haven, CT. 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Bargh JA, Chen M, Burrows L. Automacity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;71(2):230–244. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.2.230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron HM. The demand for Black labor: Historical notes on the political economy of racism. In: Hayes FW III, editor. A turbulent voyage: Readings in African American studies. 3. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield; 2000. pp. 435–467. [Google Scholar]
- Bauerlein M. The Dumbest Generation: How the digital age stupefies young Americans and jeopardizes our future. New York: Tarcher/Penguin Group; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Becker E. The denial of death. New York: Free Press; 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Bildtgard T. The sexuality of elderly people on film-visual limitations. Journal of Aging and Identity. 2000;5(3) [Google Scholar]
- Binstock RH. The oldest old: A fresh perspective or compassionate ageism revisited? Milbank Memorial Fund Quarterly. 1985;63:420–451. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Binstock RH. Old-age policies, politics, and ageism. Generataions. 2005;29(3):73–78. [Google Scholar]
- Binstock RH. From compassionate ageism to intergenerational conflict? The Gerontologist. 2010;50(5):574–585. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnq056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blanchard-Fields F. Everyday problem solving and emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16(1):26–31. [Google Scholar]
- Blanchflower DG, Freeman RB. Centre for Economic Performance Discussion Papers, London School of Economics. 1996. Growing into work. [Google Scholar]
- Block MR, Davidson JL, Grambs JD. Women over forty: Visions and realities. New York: Springer; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Bowling A. Ageism in cardiology. British Medical Journal. 1999;319:1353–1355. doi: 10.1136/bmj.319.7221.1353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowling A. Honour your father and mother: Ageism in medicine. British Journal of General Practice. 2007;57(538):347–348. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braithwaite V. Reducing ageism. In: Nelson TD, editor. Ageism: Stereotyping against older persons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2004. pp. 311–337. [Google Scholar]
- Brewer MB, Dull V, Lui L. Perceptions of the elderly: Stereotypes as Prototypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1981;41(4):656–670. [Google Scholar]
- Bugental DB, Hehman JA. Ageism: A review of research and policy implications. Social Issues and Policy Review. 2007;1(1):173–216. [Google Scholar]
- Bugg JM, Zook NA, DeLosh EL, Davalos DB, Davis HP. Age differences in fluid intelligence: Contributions of general slowing and frontal decline. Brain and Cognition. 2006;62(1):9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgess D, Borgida E. Who women are, who women should be: Descriptive and prescriptive gender stereotyping in sex discrimination. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 1999;5(3):665–692. [Google Scholar]
- Burnstein E, Crandall C, Kitayama S. Some neo-Darwinian decision rules for altruism: Weighting cuees for inclusive fitness as a function of the biological importance of the decision. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;67(5):773–789. [Google Scholar]
- Burr JA, Mutchler JE. Race and ethnic variation in norms of filial responsibility among older persons. Journal of Marriage and Family. 1999;61(3):674–687. [Google Scholar]
- Butler RN. Age-ism: Another form of bigotry. The Gerontologist. 1969;9:243–246. doi: 10.1093/geront/9.4_part_1.243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler RN. Combating ageism. International Psychogeriatrics. 2009;21(2):211. doi: 10.1017/S104161020800731X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bytheway W. Ageism. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Carroll J, Howard S, Vetere F, Peck J, Murphy J. Just what do the youth of today want? Technology appropriation by young people. Proceedings of the 35th Annual Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences. 2002;5:131b. [Google Scholar]
- Carstensen LL. Evidence for a life-span theory of socioemotional selectivity. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1995;4:151–155. doi: 10.1177/09637214211011468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carstensen LL, Mikels JA. At the intersection of emotion and cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2005;14(3):117–121. [Google Scholar]
- Carstensen LL, Pasupathi M, Mayr U, Nesselroade J. Emotion experience in everyday life across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;79:644–655. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castelli L, Pavan G, Ferrari E, Kashima Y. The stereotyper and the chameleon: The effects of stereotype use on perceivers’ mimicry. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2009;45:835–839. [Google Scholar]
- Cesario J, Plaks JE, Higgins ET. Automatic social behavior as motivated preparation to interact. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;90(6):893–910. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.6.893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chasteen AL, Schwarz N, Park DC. The activation of aging stereotypes in younger and older adults. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2002;57B:P540–P547. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.6.p540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen Z. Stereotype content of urban and rural groups. n.d. Unpublished data. [Google Scholar]
- Cikara M, Farnsworth RA, Harris LT, Fiske ST. On the wrong side of the trolley track: Neural correlates of relative social valuation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2010;5:404–413. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cleveland JN, Landy FJ. The effects of person and job stereotypes on two personnel decisions. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1983;68(4):609–619. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen A. New York Times. 2009. Nov 7, Age bias gets second-class treatment; p. A22. [Google Scholar]
- Collette-Pratt C. Attitudinal predictors of devaluation of old age in a multigenerational sample. Journal of Gerontology. 1976;31:193–197. doi: 10.1093/geronj/31.2.193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cottrell CA, Neuberg SL. Different emotional reactions to different groups: A sociofunctional threat-based approach to “prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005;88(5):770–789. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cottrell CA, Neuberg SL, Li NP. What do people desire in others? A sociofunctional perspective on the importance of different valued characteristics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2007;92(2):208–231. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coyne AC, Reichman WE, Berbig LJ. The relationship between dementia and elder abuse. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1993;150:643–646. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.4.643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crocker J, Major B, Steele C. Social stigma. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, Lindzey G, editors. The handbook of social psychology. 4. Vol. 2. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1998. pp. 504–553. [Google Scholar]
- Cuddy AJC, Fiske ST. Doddering but dear: Process, content, and function in stereotyping of older persons. In: Nelson TD, editor. Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 2004. pp. 3–26. [Google Scholar]
- Cuddy AJC, Norton MI, Fiske ST. This old stereotype: The pervasiveness and persistence of the elderly stereotype. Journal of Social Issues. 2005;61(2):267–285. [Google Scholar]
- Cuddy AJC, Fiske ST, Glick P. The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2007;92(4):631–648. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuddy AJC, Fiske ST, Kwan VSY, Glick P, Demoulin S, Leyens JP, et al. Stereotype content model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some differences. British Journal of Social Psychology. 2009;48(1):1–33. doi: 10.1348/014466608X314935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dail PW. Prime-time television portrayals of older adults in the context of family life. The Gerontologist. 1988;28(5):700–106. doi: 10.1093/geront/28.5.700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Day J. Former UPEI faculty members fight mandatory retirment. The Guardian; 2009. Oct 14, Retrieved from http://www.theguardian.pe.ca. [Google Scholar]
- Dear M, Gleeson B. Community attitudes toward the homeless. Urban Geography. 1991;12:155–76. [Google Scholar]
- Dijksterhuis A, Spears R, Postmes T, Stapel DA, Koomen W, van Knippenberg A, Scheepers D. Seeing one thing and doing another: Contrast effects in automatic behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;75(4):862–871. [Google Scholar]
- Doka KJ. Adolescent attitudes and beliefs toward aging and the elderly. International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 1985–1986;22:173–187. doi: 10.2190/mkyy-7vgg-j1el-mkq4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dovidio JF, Gaertner SL. Reducing prejudice: Combating intergroup biases. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1999;8(4):101–104. [Google Scholar]
- Duncan C, Loretto W. Never the right age? Gender and age-based discrimination in employment. Gender, Work and Organization. 2004;11(1):95–115. [Google Scholar]
- Dychtwald K. Age power: How the 21st century will be ruled by the new old. New York: Tarcher/Putnam; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Eagly AH. Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role interpetation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Elmer V. Age discrimination claims by workers reach record high. AARP Bulletin Today. 2009 Mar 29; Accessed from http://www.aarp.org.
- Evans AR. Now is the time: Seminaries responding to the graying of our congregations. Journal of Religion, Spirituality and Aging. 2011;23(1–2):77–91. [Google Scholar]
- Fackler M. New York Times. 2010 Aug 14; Japan, checking on its oldest, finds many gone. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com.
- Finkelstein LM, Burke MJ, Raju MS. Age discrimination in simulated employment contexts: An integrative analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1995;80(6):652–663. [Google Scholar]
- Fiske ST, Ruscher JB. Negative interdependence and prejudice: Whence the affect? In: Mackie DM, Hamilton DL, editors. Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception. Academic Press; 1993. pp. 239–268. [Google Scholar]
- Fiske ST, Stevens LE. What’s so special about sex? Gender stereotyping and discrimination. In: Oskamp S, Costanzo M, editors. Gender issues in contemporary society: Applied social psychology annual. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1993. pp. 173–196. [Google Scholar]
- Fiske ST. Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, Lindzey G, editors. The handbook of social psychology. 4. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Fiske ST, Bergsieker H, Russell AM, Williams L. Images of Black Americans: Then, “them” and now, “Obama!”. DuBois Review: Social Science Research on Race. 2009;6:83–101. doi: 10.1017/S1742058X0909002X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiske ST, Cuddy AJ, Glick P, Xu J. A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002;82:878–902. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiske ST, Cuddy AJC, Glick P. Universal dimensions of social perception: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2007;11:77–83. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fraboni M, Saltstone R, Hughes S. The Fraboni Scale of Ageism (FSA): An attempt at a more precise measure of ageism. Canadian Journal on Aging. 1990;9(1):56–66. [Google Scholar]
- Friis H. The changing elderly in a changing Danish society. Ageing International. 1991;18(1):25–32. [Google Scholar]
- Gaertner SL, Dovidio JF. Reducing intergroup bias: The common ingroup identity model. Philadelphia: Psychology Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Gale J. Elderly to outnumber children for first time in 2045, UN says. Bloomberg News. 2010 Jan 11; Retrieved from http://www.bloomberg.com.
- Garstka TA, Schmitt MT, Branscombe NR, Hummert ML. How young and older adults differ in their responses to perceived age discrimination. Psychology and Aging. 2004;19(2):326–335. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaugler JE, Leach CR, Anderson KA. Family. In: Palmore EB, Branch LG, Harris DK, editors. Encyclopedia of ageism. Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press; 2005. pp. 147–151. [Google Scholar]
- Gerbner G. 1998 Screen Actors Guild Report: Casting the American Scene. 1998. Dec, Casting the American scene: A look at the characters on prime time and daytime television. [Google Scholar]
- Giles H, Fox S, Harwood J, Williams A. Talking age and aging talk: Communicating through the life span. In: Hummert M, Wiemann J, Nussbaum J, editors. Interpersonal communication in older adulthood: Interdisciplinary theory and research. Manchester, UK: Manchester University Press; 1994. pp. 192–212. [Google Scholar]
- Giles H, Williams A. Patronizing the young: Forms and evaluations. International Journal of Aging & Human Development. 1994;39:33–53. doi: 10.2190/0LUC-NWMA-K5LX-NUVW. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glick P. Choice of scapegoats. In: Dovidio JF, Glick P, Rudman LA, editors. On the nature of prejudice: Fifty years after Allport. Malden, MA: Blackwell; 2005. pp. 244–261. [Google Scholar]
- Glick P, Fiske ST. The Ambivalent Sexism Inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;70:491–512. [Google Scholar]
- Glick P, Fiske ST. Ambivalent stereotypes as legitimizing ideologies: Differentiating paternalistic and envious prejudice. In: Jost JT, Major B, editors. The psychology of legitimacy: Emerging perspectives on ideology, justice, and intergroup relations. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2001. pp. 278–306. [Google Scholar]
- Greenberg J, Schimel J, Mertens A. Ageism: Denying the face of the future. In: Nelson TD, editor. Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 2004. pp. 27–48. [Google Scholar]
- Grefe D. Combating ageism with narrative and intergroup contact: Possibilities of intergenerational connections. Pastoral Psychology. 2011;60(1):99–105. [Google Scholar]
- Griff M, Lambert D, Dellmann-Jenkins M, Fruit D. Intergenerational activity analysis with three groups of older adults: Frail, community-living, and Alzheimer’s. Educational Gerontology. 1996;22(6):601–12. [Google Scholar]
- Griffore RJ, Barboza GE, Mastin T, Oehmke J, Schiamberg LB, Post LA. Family members’ reports of abuse in Michigan nursing homes. Journal of Elder Abuse and Neglect. 2009;21(2):105–114. doi: 10.1080/08946560902779910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossmann I, Na J, Varnum MEW, Park DC, Kitayama S, Nisbett RE. Reasoning about social conflicts improves into old age. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107(16):7246–7250. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001715107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagestad GO, Uhlenberg P. The social separation of old and young: A root of ageism. Journal of Social Issues. 2005;61(2):343–360. [Google Scholar]
- Hale NM. Effects of age and interpersonal contact on stereotyping of the elderly. Current Psychology. 1998;17(1):28–38. [Google Scholar]
- Hamil-Luker J. The prospects of age war: Inequality between (and within) age groups. Social Science Research. 2001;30(3):386–400. [Google Scholar]
- Harwood J, Hewstone M, Paolini S, Voci A. Grandparent-grandchild contact and attitudes toward older adults: Moderator and mediator effects. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2005;31(3):393–406. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hellbusch JS, Corbin DE, Thorson JA, Stacy RD. Physicians’ attitudes towards aging. Gerontology and Geriatrics Education. 1995;15(2):55–65. [Google Scholar]
- Helson R, Kwan VSY, John OP, Jones C. The growing evidence for personality change in adulthood: Findings from research with personality inventories. Journal of Research in Personality. 2002;36:287–306. [Google Scholar]
- Hewstone M, Harwood J, Voci A, Kenworthy J. Intergroup contact and grandparent-grandchild communication: The effects of self-disclosure on implicit and explicit biases against older people. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 2006;9(3):413–429. [Google Scholar]
- Higgs P, Gilleard C. Generational confict, consumption, and the ageing welfare state in the United Kingdom. Ageing and Society. 2010;30:1439–1451. [Google Scholar]
- Horn JL, Cattell RB. Age differences in fluid and crystallized intelligence. Acta Psychologica. 1967;26(2):107–129. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(67)90011-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howard C. Giving the people what they want? Age, class, and distribution in the United States. In: Shapiro I, Swenson PA, Donno D, editors. Divide and deal: the politics of distribution in democracies. New York: New York University Press; 2008. pp. 221–242. [Google Scholar]
- Hummert ML. Multiple stereotypes of elderly and young adults: A comparison of structure and evaluations. Psychology and Aging. 1990;5(2):182–193. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.5.2.182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hummert ML, Shaner JL, Garstka TA, Henry C. Communication with older adults: The influence of age stereotypes, context, and communicator age. Human Communication Research. 1998;25(1):124–151. [Google Scholar]
- Irwin S. Age related distributive justice and claims on resources. British Journal of Sociology. 1996;47(1):68–92. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irwin S. Age, generation and inequality: A reply to the reply. British Journal of Sociology. 1998;49(2):305–310. [Google Scholar]
- Jackman MR. The velvet glove: Paternalism and conflict in gender, class, and race relations. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Jacoby S. Never Say Die: The Myth and Marketing of the New Old Age. New York: Pantheon Books; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Jensen GD, Oakley FB. Aged appearance and behavior: An evolutionary and ethological perspective. The Gerontologist. 1980;20(5 part 1):595–597. doi: 10.1093/geront/20.5_part_1.595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones EE, Farina A, Hastorf AH, Markus H, Miller DT, Scout RA, French R, editors. Social stigma: The psychology of marked relationships. New York: Plenum; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Kalavar JM. Examining ageism: Do male and female college students differ? Educational Gerontology. 2001;27(6):507–513. [Google Scholar]
- Kingson ER, Hirshorn BA, Cornman JM. Ties that bind: The interdependence of generations in an aging society. Washington, D.C: Seven Locks Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Kite M, Deaux K, Miele M. Stereotypes of young and old: Does age outweigh gender? Psychology and Aging. 1991;6:19–27. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.6.1.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kite ME, Johnson BT. Attitudes toward older and younger adults: A meta analysis. Psychology and Aging. 1988;3:233–244. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.3.3.233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kite ME, Wagner LS. Attitudes toward older adults. In: Nelson TD, editor. Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. Cambridge: The MIT Press; 2004. pp. 129–161. [Google Scholar]
- Kite ME, Stockdale GD, Whitley BE, Jr, Johnson BT. Attitudes toward younger and older adults: An updated meta-analytics review. Journal of Social Issues. 2005;61(2):241–266. [Google Scholar]
- Kunda Z. Social Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Kurzban R, Leary MR. Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: The functions of social exclusion. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:187–208. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- La Corte R. Most state judges in the U.S. must retire in 70s. Seattle Post- Intelligencer. 2009 Oct 24; Retrieved from http://www.seattlepi.com.
- Langlois JH, Kalakanis L, Rubenstein AJ, Larson A, Hallam M, Smoot M. Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytics and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126:390–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee BA, Farrell CR, Link BG. Revisiting the contact hypothesis: The case of public exposure to homelessness. American Sociological Review. 2004;69(1):40–63. [Google Scholar]
- Levenson AJ. Ageism: A major deterrent to the introduction of curricula in aging. Gerontology and Geriatrics Education. 1981;1:161–162. doi: 10.1300/J021v01n03_01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy BR, Slade MD, Gill TM. Hearing decline predicted by elders’ stereotypes. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2006;61(2):82–87. doi: 10.1093/geronb/61.2.p82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy BR, Slade MD, Kasl SV. Longitudinal benefit of positive self-perceptions of aging on functioning health. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2002;57(5):409–417. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.5.p409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy BR, Slade MD, Kunkel SR, Kasl SV. Longevity increased by positive self-perceptions of aging. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002;83(2):261–270. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.83.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy B. Improving memory in old age through implicit self-stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;71:1092–1107. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.6.1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy BR, Banaji MR. Implicit ageism. In: Nelson TD, editor. Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. Cambridge: The MIT Press; 2004. pp. 49–75. [Google Scholar]
- Levy B, Langer E. Aging free from negative stereotypes: Successful memory in China among the American deaf. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;66(6):989–997. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.66.6.989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy BR, Leifheit-Limson E. The stereotype-matching effect: Greater influence on functioning when age stereotypes correspond to outcomes. Psychology and Aging. 2009;24:230–233. doi: 10.1037/a0014563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levy BR, Zonderman AB, Slade MD, Ferrucci L. Age stereotypes held earlier in life predict cardiovascular events in later life. Psychological Science. 2009;20:296–298. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02298.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liden RC, Stilwell D, Ferris GR. The effects of supervisor and subordinate age on objective performance and subjective performance ratings. Human Relations. 1996;49(3):327–347. [Google Scholar]
- Lin X, Bryant C, Boldero K. Measures for assessing student attitudes toward older people. Educational Gerontology. 2011;37(1):12–26. [Google Scholar]
- Loehlin JC, Martin NG. Age changes in personality traits and their heritabilities during the adult years: Evidence from the Australian twin registry samples. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30:1147–1160. [Google Scholar]
- Logan JR, Spitze GD. Self-interest and altruism in intergenerational relations. Demography. 1995;32(3):353–364. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Longman P. Age wars: The coming battle between young and old. The futurist. 1986;20:8–11. [Google Scholar]
- Loretto W, Duncan C, White PJ. Ageism and employment: controversies, ambiguities, and younger people’s perceptions. Ageing and Society. 2000;20:279–302. [Google Scholar]
- Malmedal W, Ingebrigsten O, Saveman B. Inadequate care in Norwegian nursing homes-as reported by nursing staff. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences. 2009;23(2):231–242. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6712.2008.00611.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mariotto A, De Leo D, Dello Buono M, Favaretti C, Ausin P, Naylor CD. Will elderly patients stand aside in the queue for cardiac services? The Lancet. 1999;354(9177):467–470. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(98)12192-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEvoy GM, Cascio WF. Cumulative evidence of the relationship between employee age and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1989;74(1):11–17. [Google Scholar]
- McGinnis D, Zelinski EM. Understanding unfamiliar words in young, young-old, and old-old adults: Inferential processing and the abstraction-deficit hypothesis. Psychology and Aging. 2003;18(3):493–509. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.3.497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller PN, Miller DW, McKibbin EM, Pettys GL. Stereotypes of the elderly in magazine advertisements, 1956–1999. International Journal of Aging and Human Development. 1999;49(4):319–37. doi: 10.2190/A078-FRJE-NQ1P-C3KY. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minkler M. Generational equity and the new victim blaming. In: Moody HR, editor. Aging: Concepts and Controversies. Newbury Park, CA: Pine Forge Press; 2006. pp. 181–190. (Re-printed from Critical Perspectives on Aging, 67–79, by M.Minkler and C. Estes, Eds., 1991, Amityville, NY: Baywood.) [Google Scholar]
- Montepare JM, Zebrowitz LA. A social-developmental view of ageism. In: Nelson TD, editor. Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. Cambridge: The MIT Press; 2004. pp. 129–161. [Google Scholar]
- Narayan C. Is there a double standard of aging? Older men and women and ageism. Educational Gerontology. 2008;34(9):782–787. [Google Scholar]
- Nelson TD, editor. Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Nelson TD. Ageism: Prejudice against our feared future self. Journal of Social Issues. 2005;61(2):207–221. [Google Scholar]
- Nelson TD. Ageism. In: Nelson TD, editor. Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination. New York: Psychology Press; 2009. pp. 431–440. [Google Scholar]
- Neugarten BL. Age groups in American society and the rise of the young-old. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. 1974;415(1):187–198. [Google Scholar]
- Newman S, Ward C, Smith T, Wilson J, McCrea J, Calhoun G, Kingson E. Intergenerational programs: Past, present, and future. Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Ng SH. Social psychology in an ageing world: Ageism and intergenerational relations. Asian Journal of Social Psychology. 1998;1(1):99–116. [Google Scholar]
- Ng SH. Will families support their elders? Answers from across cultures. In: Nelson TD, editor. Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2002. pp. 295–309. [Google Scholar]
- Nisbett RE. The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently…and why. New York: The Free Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- North MS, Fiske ST. Succession, Identity, and Consumption: Dimensions of control in age-based prejudice. Poster presentation at the 11th Annual Society for Personality and Social Psychology Conferences; Las Vegas, NV. Jan, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- North MS, Fiske ST. A prescriptive, intergenerational scale of ageism: Succession, Identity, & Consumption. Poster presentation at the 12th Annual Society for Personality and Social Psychology Conference; San Antonio, TX. Jan, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Nosek BA, Banaji MR, Greenwald AG. Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice. 2002;6(1):101–115. [Google Scholar]
- Nuessel FH. The language of ageism. The Gerontologist. 1982;22:273–276. doi: 10.1093/geront/22.3.273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oishi S, Kesebir S, Snyder BH. Sociology: A lost connection in social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2009;13(4):334–353. doi: 10.1177/1088868309347835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmore E. Ageism: Negative and positive. 2. New York: Springer; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Palmore E. Ageism comes of age. The Gerontologist. 2003;43(3):418–420. [Google Scholar]
- Pennebaker JW, Stone LD. Words of wisdom: Language use over life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;85(2):291–301. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pettigrew TF, Tropp LR. A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;90(5):751–783. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pillemer KA, Wolf RS. Elder abuse: Conflict in the family. Dover, MA: Auburn House; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Pollack RF. Generational equity: The current debate. Presentation before the 32nd Annual Meeting of the American Society on Aging; San Francisco, CA. 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Popham LE, Kennison SM, Bradley KI. Ageism, sensation-seeking, and risk-taking behavior in young adults. Current Psychology: A Journal for Diverse Perspectives on Diverse Psychological Issues. 2011;30(2):184–193. [Google Scholar]
- Prentice DA, Carranza E. What women and men should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2002;26(4):269–281. [Google Scholar]
- Quindlen A. Stepping aside. Newsweek. 2009 May 18; Retrieved from http://www.newsweek.com.
- Ramsey-Klawsnik H. Elder sexual abuse within the family. Journal of Elder Abuse & Neglect. 2004;15(1):43–58. [Google Scholar]
- Reinhardt UE. Does the aging of the population really drive the demand for health care? Health Affairs. 2003;22(6):27–39. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.22.6.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson JD, Skill T. The invisible generation: Portrayals of the elderly on prime-time television. Communication Reports. 1995;8(2):111–119. [Google Scholar]
- Rodin J, Langer E. Aging labels: The decline of control and the fall of self-esteem. Journal of Social Issues. 1980;36(2):12–29. [Google Scholar]
- Rodríguez SB, Díaz-Giménez J, Quadrini V, Ríos-Rull JV. Updated facts on the U.S. distribution of earnings, income, and wealth. Federal Reserve Bank of Minneapolis Quarterly Review. 2002;26(3):2–35. [Google Scholar]
- Roy A, Harwood J. Underrepresented, positively portrayed: Older adults in television commercials. Journal of Applied Communication Research. 1997;25:39–56. [Google Scholar]
- Rudman LA, Glick P. Prescriptive gender stereotypes and backlash toward agentic women. Journal of Social Issues. 2001;57(4):743–762. [Google Scholar]
- Rupp DE, Vodanovich SJ, Credé M. The multidimensional nature of ageism: Construct validity and group differences. The Journal of Social Psychology. 2005;145(3):335–362. doi: 10.3200/SOCP.145.3.335-362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sabogal F, Marin G, Otero-Sabogal R, Marin BV, Perez-Stable EJ. Hispanic familism and acculturation: What changes and what doesn’t? Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences. 1987;9(4):397–412. [Google Scholar]
- Salthouse TA. Neuroanatomical substrates of age-related cognitive decline. Psychological Bulletin. 2011;137(5):753–784. doi: 10.1037/a0023262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanders GF, Montgomery JE, Pittman JF, Jr, Balkwell C. Youth’s attitudes toward the elderly. Journal of Applied Gerontology. 1984;3(1):59–70. [Google Scholar]
- Sardiello R. Identity and status stratification in deadhead subculture. In: Epstein JS, editor. Youth culture: identity in a postmodern world. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell; 1998. pp. 118–147. [Google Scholar]
- Scarrow AM, Linskey M, Asher AL, Anderson VC, Selden NR. Neurosurgeon transition to retirement: Results of the 2007 Congress of Neurological Surgeons Consensus Conference. Neurosurgery. 2009;65(2):231–236. doi: 10.1227/01.NEU.0000349932.37540.02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt DF, Boland SM. Structure of perceptions of older adults: Evidence for multiple stereotypes. Psychology and Aging. 1986;1:255–260. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.1.3.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoemann AM, Branscombe NR. Looking young for your age: Perceptions of anti-aging actions. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2010;41:86–95. [Google Scholar]
- Schultz JH, Binstock RH. Aging nation: The economics and politics of growing older in America. Westport, CT: Praeger/Greenwood; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Seefeldt C, Jantz RK, Gapler A, Serock K. Using pictures to explore children’s attitudes toward the elderly. Gerontologist. 1977;17:506–512. doi: 10.1093/geront/17.6.506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seefeldt C. The effect of preschoolers’ visits to a nursing home. Gerontologist. 1987;27(2):228–232. doi: 10.1093/geront/27.2.228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherif M, Harvey OJ, White BJ, Hood WR, Sherif CW. The Robbers Cave experiment: Intergroup conflict and cooperation. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press; 1961/1988. [Google Scholar]
- Signorelli N. Aging in television: The picture in the Nineties. Generations. 2001;25(3):34–38. [Google Scholar]
- Snyder M, Miene PK. Stereotyping of the elderly: A functional approach. British Journal of Social Psychology. 1994;33:63–82. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1994.tb01011.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sumner WG. Folkaways. New York: Ginn; 1906. [Google Scholar]
- Suzman RM, Willis DP, Manton KG, editors. The Oldest Old. New York: Oxford University Press; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Szafran RF. Age-adjusted labor force participation rates, 1960–2045. U.S. Department of Labor Statistics. Monthly Labor Review. 2002:25–38. Retrieved from http://www.bls.gov/opub/mlr/2002/09/art3full.pdf.
- Tajfel H, Turner J. An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In: Austin WG, Worchel S, editors. The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole; 1979. pp. 94–109. [Google Scholar]
- Tentori K, Osherson DN, Hasher L, May C. Wisdom and aging: Irrational preferences in college students but not older adults. Cognition. 2001;81(3):B87–B96. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(01)00137-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terborg JR. Women in management: A research review. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1977;62:647–664. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson EH. In: Understanding inequality: The intersection of race/ethnicity, class, and gender. Arrighi BA, editor. Lanham, Maryland: Rowman & Littlefield; 2001. pp. 261–268. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson NJ, Thompson KE. Can marketing practice keep up with Europe’s agein population? European Journal of Marketing. 2009;43(11/12):1281–1288. [Google Scholar]
- Thomson ABR, Bernstein CN, Leddin D. Mandatory retirement for gastroenterologists: A Viewpoint. Canadian Journal of Gastroenterology. 2008;22(2):125–127. doi: 10.1155/2008/213840. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todaro W. Want to be a CEO? Stay put. Forbes 500s. 2003 Mar 31; Retrieved from http://www.forbes.com.
- Tugend A. Fears, and opportunities, on the road to retirement. New York Times. 2011 Jun 3; Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com.
- Turner BS. Ageing and generational conflicts: A reply to Sarah Irwin. British Journal of Sociology. 1998;49(2):299–304. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- United Nations. World population ageing: 1950–2050. United Nations Population Division; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- United Nations. World population ageing 2009. Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Census Bureau. Current population survey, 2009 annual social and economic supplement. 2009 Available at http://www.census.gov/hhes/www/cpstables/032009/perinc/new01_001.htm.
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Centers for Disease Control and Prevention and The Merck Company Foundation. The state of aging and health in America 2007. 2007 Available at: http://www.cdc.gov/aging/saha.htm.
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Keeping the aging population healthy: Legislator policy brief. 2007 Aug; Retrieved from http://www.healthystates.csg.org/NR/rdonlyres/B810343C-5D88-4210-B999-1A1A42711F62/0/agingLPB.pdf.
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Medicaire and Medicaid Services. National health expenditure data, NHE fact sheet. 2011 Retrieved from https://www.cms.gov/NationalHealthExpendData/25_NHE_Fact_sheet.asp.
- Verhaeghen P. Aging and executive control: Reports of a demise greatly exaggerated. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2011;20(3):174–180. doi: 10.1177/0963721411408772. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vernon JA, Williams JA, Phillips T, Wilson J. Media stereotyping: A comparison of the way elderly women and men are portrayed on prime-time television. Journal of Women & Aging. 1990;2(4):55–68. [Google Scholar]
- Walz T. Crones, dirty old men, sexy seniors: Representations of the sexuality of older persons. Journal of Aging and Identity. 2002;7(2):99–112. [Google Scholar]
- Westman JC. Juvenile ageism: Unrecognized prejudice and discrimination against the young. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 1991;21(4):237–256. doi: 10.1007/BF00705929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitbourne SK, Sneed JR. The paradox of well-being, identity processes, and stereotype threat: Ageism and its potential relationships to the self in later life. In: Nelson TD, editor. Ageism. Cambridge: The MIT Press; 2004. pp. 242–273. [Google Scholar]
- Wilber DQ. FAA to raise retirement age for pilots. Washington Post. 2007 Jan 31; Retrieved from http://www.washingtonpost.com.
- Williams A, Giles H. Communication of ageism. In: Hecht ML, Michael L, editors. Communicating prejudice. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1998. pp. 136–160. [Google Scholar]
- Williams LM, Brown KJ, Palmer D, Liddell BJ, Kemp AH, Olivieri G, Peduto A, Gordon E. The mellow years? Neural basis of improving emotional stability with age. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:6422–6430. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0022-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf R. Medicare, Social Security running out of money faster. USA Today. 2011 May 13; Retrieved from http://www.usatoday.com.
- Yesuda N, Zimmerman SI, Hawkes W, Fredman L, Hebel JR, Magaziner J. Relation of social network characteristics to 5-year morality among young-old versus old-old white women in an urban community. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1997;145(6):516–523. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]