Abstract
Background
Stroke often results in chronic disability and the need for long-term assistance, which is provided in large part by spouses. Stroke caregivers experience poorer health and well-being compared with non-caregivers, but less is known about the specific toll that caregiving may exact on cognitive functioning.
Objective
To investigate whether persons caring for a spouse who experienced a recent stroke may be at risk for poor cognitive functioning compared with non-caregivers.
Methods
Existing data from the United States’ Health and Retirement Study (HRS) were used to identify 146 caregivers from among couples in which 1 individual reported surviving a recent stroke and experiencing functional limitations. This cross-sectional population-based analysis compared the stroke caregivers with 3,416 non-caregivers in time orientation, working memory, semantic memory, learning, and episodic memory.
Results
Overall, the caregiver group was considerably more disadvantaged than the non-caregiver group in terms of background characteristics, socioeconomic status, health, and well-being. Results of weighted Poisson regression models indicated that stroke caregivers were at risk for poorer performance than non-caregivers in working memory, semantic memory, learning, and episodic memory. The gap between stroke caregivers and non-caregivers in episodic memory remained after adjusting for systematic differences between the 2 groups across an array of risk factors.
Conclusions
Spousal caregivers of stroke survivors may be at risk for poor cognitive functioning. More work is needed to identify the processes that may contribute to the diminished cognitive capacity among these adults so that interventions may be developed to reduce caregiver burden and promote cognitive health.
Keywords: cognitive function, family caregiving, Health and Retirement Study, memory
Stroke is one of the most prevalent health problems in the United States, and stroke-related complications among family members, as well as survivors, are a major public health concern among the aging American population. Approximately 75% of stroke survivors are discharged home after acute and subacute rehabilitation care,1 returning to lives shared with their spouses and partners. With the increasing awareness of the growing problem of caregiver burden,2 the American Stroke Association has devoted an entire Web site (Life after Stroke) to caregiver support, and its magazine (Stroke Connection) often publishes personal stories and professional advice for families.3,4 This outreach is informed by decades of research demonstrating that spousal caregivers of stroke survivors are more likely to experience poor physical5 and mental health,6 financial strain,7 social isolation,8 biographical disruption,9 and poor quality of life.10 Comparatively little research has focused on cognitive functioning as an aspect of caregiver health and well-being that is negatively affected by the strain of caring for a stroke survivor.
Research conducted among caregivers for persons with other chronic conditions suggests that cognitive functioning suffers after a protracted illness. In a longitudinal study of spousal caregivers of people with Alzheimer's disease, Vitaliano and colleagues reported that the depressed mood of caregivers mediated decline in caregivers’ cognitive processing speed11 and that chronic stress was associated with decline in caregivers’ verbal intelligence.12 Based on these findings, Vitaliano et al13 concluded that caregivers are at risk for cognitive decline independent of the aging process. Cross-sectional studies have consistently shown that in comparison with healthy non-caregivers, spousal caregivers of people with chronic diseases or disabilities had poorer processing speed, verbal learning, and memory, which may be mediated by caregivers’ chronic stress,14 distress,15 or self-perceived incompetence.16 However, very few studies have focused on cognitive functioning in spousal caregivers of stroke survivors. A notable exception is the work of Lee and colleagues17 based on the Nurses’ Health Study. After controlling for psychological stress and mental health, which may confound the effect of caregiving on cognitive performance, the authors found that members of this relatively homogeneous sample of nursing professionals were at risk for poorer cognitive functioning if they provided care to their spouses who had survived a stroke in comparison with their non-caregiving peers.17
Other evidence suggests that impaired caregiver functioning may impede the recovery of stroke survivors18 as well as compromise caregivers’ cognition and health. In the general population, most spousal caregivers have limited health care training or inadequate knowledge of long-term consequences after a stroke19 and thus are often ill prepared to manage and adapt to the new life shared with their spouses who have had a stroke.20 This study was designed to explore the relationship between caregiving and cognitive functioning using data from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) to advance the understanding of the health risks posed by caring for a stroke survivor in the United States. We compared stroke caregivers with non-caregivers across 5 areas of cognitive functioning (ie, time orientation, working memory, semantic memory, learning, and episodic memory) and adjusted for a range of demographic, socioeconomic, and health-related covariates. A particular strength of this analysis was the use of a large, nationally representative dataset of older adults to assess these associations.
Methods
Database
This study used publicly available data from the HRS, a nationally representative, longitudinal study of noninstitutionalized adults born between 1931 and 1941. The HRS used a multistage area probability design to select a sample of US households with supplemental oversamples of African Americans, Hispanics, and residents of the state of Florida.21 Household respondents included the age-eligible individuals (a member of the 1931-1941 birth year cohort) and their spouses. This sample design resulted in a large number of healthy, community-dwelling couples, which was well suited to the analytic objectives of this study. HRS was initiated in 1992, and participants were re-interviewed at subsequent 2-year intervals. This study is based primarily on data from the 1996 interview when several areas of cognitive functioning were fully assessed for the first time. Use of the 1996 data, rather than more current data, eliminated the practice effect, which may result from multiple administrations of the same cognitive tests.22 A total of 10,964 individuals completed the 1996 interview, yielding a response rate of 86.9%. Approval to conduct this analysis was obtained from the authors’ local institutional review board.
Analytic sample derivation
The panel design of the HRS enabled us to derive caregiving status among persons whose spouse experienced a stroke. We began by selecting couples in the database. A respondent was dropped from the sample if he or she was not a member of a couple household (n = 2,658) and if he or she had more than 1 spouse who was an HRS participant (n = 410). A respondent was categorized as a stroke caregiver if he or she met 2 criteria: (1) his or her spouse was healthy (ie, reported no major health conditions or activity limitations) in 1992 and subsequently reported experiencing a stroke in either 1994 or 1996, and (2) the stroke survivor reported limitations in performing everyday activities. Respondents were dropped from among the dyads if both spouses reported a stroke between 1992 and 1996 (n = 12). A comparison group of non-caregivers was selected from HRS couples who reported no history of stroke between 1992 and 1996. Among the nonstroke dyads, a respondent was dropped if his or her spouse reported limitations in performing everyday activities (n = 788), implying that spousal assistance might be required for another, nonstroke-related condition. One respondent per non-caregiving couple was randomly dropped to eliminate nesting in the dyads (n = 3,076). Of the remaining 3,710 non-caregivers and 155 stroke caregivers, 327 cases were dropped as a result of either proxy reports or missing data on the dependent variable. This resulted in a final analytic sample of 146 stroke caregivers and 3,416 noncaregivers (N = 3,562).
Measures of cognitive functioning
Objective measures of 5 areas of cognitive functioning used by the HRS study were selected for analysis. Time orientation was evaluated by asking the interviewees the year, month, date, and day of week. Working memory was assessed with the serial 7s test, in which the examinees were asked to subtract 7 from 100 for 5 times. Semantic memory was assessed with 4 questions (What do people usually use to cut paper? What do you call the kind of prickly plant that grows in the desert? Who is the President of the United States right now? Who is the Vice President?). Learning was defined by the number of accurately recalled words immediately after the examiner read a list of 10 words (ie, immediate recall). Episodic memory was measured by the number of accurately recalled words approximately 30 minutes later (ie, delayed recall). All these tests are commonly used in neuropsychologic assessment batteries.23 Each test is scored in the counts of correct answers, and the higher counts in a given test reflect the better functioning in its corresponding cognitive area.
Covariates
Several predictors of the effects of stroke caregiving on cognitive functioning were assessed for the analysis. Demographic background included categorical measures of gender, race/ethnicity, and age group. Socioeconomic status (SES) was assessed by a categorical measure of years of education completed (did not complete high school, high school graduate, and completed some college or more) and household income in 1996. Because of the skewed distribution of the income variable, a logarithmic transformation was used in all analyses to satisfy normality assumptions and improve the accuracy and interpretability of this predictor.24
Health and behavioral risk factors included depressive symptoms, self-rated heath, and stroke-related risk factors assessed in 1996. Depressive symptoms were based on a count of 8 items from the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale.25 Using a timeframe of the past week, the respondent provided yes or no answers to statements that they felt depressed, felt that everything was an effort, had restless sleep, could not get going, felt lonely, enjoyed life, felt sad, and felt happy. Positive items were reverse scored, and all items were then summed to recreate a symptom total where higher scores indicated more depressive symptoms. The self-rated general health status, which was measured on a 5-point scale, was dichotomized into poor health (ie, rated poor or fair health) versus good health (ie, rated good, very good, or excellent health). The binary measures of stroke-related risk factors included physically inactive (vs physically active 3 or more times a week), obese (versus nonobese based on body mass index BMI < 30), current smoking (vs nonsmoker or former smoker), current heavy drinking (vs no or moderate drinking [ie consumption of fewer than 3 drinks daily]) as well as self-reported history of diagnosed hypertension, diabetes, and heart problems.
Analysis
The goal of this analysis was to assess whether there may be an association between caregiver status and cognitive functioning after stroke among spousal caregivers. Multivariate models were estimated by using Poisson regression, which is appropriate for analysis of count data. Tests of overdispersion and comparisons of the deviance statistic across models were used to assess the appropriateness of the Poisson model and model fit, respectively. Following a sequential strategy for each area of cognitive functioning, a model first estimated the overall difference in the score between stroke caregivers and non-caregivers. Subsequent models adjusted for the demographic, socioeconomic, and health and behavioral risk factors. A final “full” model adjusted for all of these factors simultaneously. All multivariate analyses, conducted in Stata/SE version 12 (StataCorp LP, College Station, TX),26 applied normalized grand sample weights as recommended for use of the HRS data and generalizations to the general population.
Results
Table 1 provides the descriptive statistics of the analytic sample. Males were slightly underrepresented relative to females, and the majority of the sample was non-Hispanic White. With nearly 70% of the individuals between age 55 and 64, the sample was relatively homogeneous in terms of age, which is largely attributable to the HRS sampling design. Overall, the majority of the sample was well educated, and the average household income of $66,000 was only slightly higher than the national average in 1996.27 The sample was characterized by low levels of depressive symptomatology in 1996, with approximately 1 in 5 individuals evaluating their health as “poor.” However, obesity and hypertension characterized a large portion of the sample. Smoking, heavy drinking, and a history of diabetes or heart problems were comparatively less prevalent.
Table 1.
Characteristics | Total (N=3,562) | Non-caregivers (n=3,416) | Stroke caregivers (n=146) |
---|---|---|---|
Demographic background | |||
Female | 53.2 | 52.5 | 68.5*** |
Race/ethnicity | |||
Non-Hispanic White | 78.5 | 79.2 | 62.3 |
African American | 11.2 | 10.7 | 24.7 |
Hispanic | 8.5 | 8.3 | 12.3 |
Asian/Pacific Islander/Other | 1.8 | 1.8 | 0.7*** |
Age group, years | |||
Less than 45 | 1.66 | 1.6 | 2.1 |
45-54 | 17.7 | 17.9 | 12.3 |
55-64 | 66.3 | 66.1 | 69.2 |
65 or older | 14.4 | 14.3 | 16.4 |
Socioeconomic characteristics | |||
Education level | |||
Less than 12 years | 25.0 | 24.1 | 44.5 |
High school graduate/GED | 35.9 | 35.9 | 34.9 |
Some college or more | 39.1 | 40.0 | 20.6*** |
Income (in 1,000 dollars) | 65.5 (74.5) | 67.0(75.4) | 33.1 (34.2)*** |
Health & behavioral risk factors | |||
Depressive symptoms (0-8) | 1.12 (1.74) | 1.07 (1.70) | 2.05 (2.25)*** |
Poor vs good health | 20.2 | 19.6 | 33.6*** |
Physically inactive vs active | 52.0 | 52.5 | 40.4** |
Obese | 41.5 | 41.8 | 33.6* |
Current smoker vs nonsmoker | 18.2 | 17.7 | 28.1** |
Drinks 3+ drinks/day | 9.3 | 9.4 | 7.5 |
History of hypertension | 36.0 | 35.6 | 45.2* |
History of diabetes | 10.9 | 10.8 | 14.4 |
History of heart problems | 12.7 | 12.6 | 15.1 |
Cognitive function | |||
Working memory (0-5) | 3.70 (1.57) | 3.72 (1.55) | 3.12 (1.85)*** |
Semantic memory (0-4) | 3.65 (0.63) | 3.66 (0.62) | 3.42 (0.78)*** |
Time orientation (0-4) | 3.81 (0.50) | 3.81 (0.50) | 3.78 (0.48) |
Learning (0-10) | 6.05 (1.67) | 6.07 (1.67) | 5.60 (1.72)*** |
Episodic memory (0-10) | 5.03 (2.04) | 5.06 (2.03) | 4.23 (2.13)*** |
Note: Data are presented as a percentage or mean (SD). Between-group comparisons are bivariate chi-square tests of significance or t tests.
P < .05.
P < .01.
P < .001.
Caregiver and non-caregiver differences
Although the analytic sample was fairly representative of older adults in the United States, we found substantial differences between the stroke caregiver and non-caregiver groups (reported in Table 1). The caregivers were significantly more likely to be female, to be from a minority group, and to have low SES; this reflects, in part, the risk profile of a spouse who had a stroke in this sample. As expected, the stroke caregivers were more likely to report poor health and a higher average number of depressive symptoms than non-caregivers. However, the pattern of group differences for the stroke-related risk factors was mixed. Noncaregivers were significantly more likely to be physically inactive and obese, and smoking and hypertension were common among the stroke caregivers. There were no differences observed between the 2 groups for drinking and history of diabetes and heart problems.
Caregiver differences in cognitive functioning
The results from the multivariate models are presented in Table 2. As shown in Model I for each sequence, stroke caregivers had a significantly lower rate of correct scores compared with non-caregivers across 4 of the 5 areas (working memory, semantic memory, episodic memory, and learning). However, these differences were largely accounted for by group differences in demographic background, SES, and health characteristics (Models II-IV).
Table 2.
Relative risk | 95% CI | P value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Working memory | ||||
I | Caregiver only | 0.89 | 0.81-0.96 | .005 |
II | Adjusted for demographic background | 0.94 | 0.87-1.02 | .134 |
III | Adjusted for SES | 0.97 | 0.90-1.04 | .363 |
IV | Adjusted for health & behavioral risk factors | 0.94 | 0.88-1.02 | .178 |
V | Fully adjusted model | 1.02 | 0.95-1.10 | .554 |
Semantic memory | ||||
I | Caregiver only | 0.95 | 0.92-0.98 | .003 |
II | Adjusted for demographic background | 0.97 | 0.95-1.00 | .064 |
III | Adjusted for SES | 0.98 | 0.94-1.00 | .189 |
IV | Adjusted for health & behavioral risk factors | 0.97 | 0.94-1.00 | .067 |
V | Fully adjusted model | 0.99 | 0.97-1.02 | .692 |
Time orientation | ||||
I | Caregiver only | 0.99 | 0.97-1.01 | .422 |
II | Adjusted for demographic background | 0.99 | 0.97-1.01 | .399 |
III | Adjusted for SES | 1.00 | 0.98-1.02 | .827 |
IV | Adjusted for health & behavioral risk factors | 1.00 | 0.98-1.02 | .987 |
V | Fully adjusted model | 1.00 | 0.98-1.02 | .789 |
Learning (immediate recall) | ||||
I | Caregiver only | 0.94 | 0.90-0.99 | .030 |
II | Adjusted for demographic background | 0.95 | 0.91-0.99 | .029 |
III | Adjusted for SES | 0.99 | 0.94-1.04 | .791 |
IV | Adjusted for health & behavioral risk factors | 0.97 | 0.93-1.02 | .295 |
V | Fully adjusted model | 0.99 | 0.95-1.04 | .718 |
Episodic memory (delayed recall) | ||||
I | Caregiver only | 0.86 | 0.78-0.93 | .000 |
II | Adjusted for demographic background | 0.86 | 0.78-0.93 | .000 |
III | Adjusted for SES | 0.91 | 0.84-0.99 | .028 |
IV | Adjusted for health & behavioral risk factors | 0.89 | 0.82-0.97 | .006 |
V | Fully adjusted model | 0.91 | 0.84-0.98 | .018 |
Note: SES = socioeconomic status.
The gap between stroke caregivers and non-caregivers in episodic memory (assessed with a 10-word delayed recall task) remained after adjusting for systematic differences between the 2 groups across an array of risk factors. Table 3 presents the details of the modeling analyses on episodic memory. The experience of caregiving for a spouse who has had a stroke decreases the expected number of correct items measuring episodic memory by 14% (Model I), a gap that is not diminished after adjusting for differences in age, gender, and race between caregivers and non-caregivers (Model II). The caregiver gap is mitigated slightly by income and education level (relative risk [RR] = 0.91; 95% CI, 0.84-0.99), suggesting that the difference in episodic memory can be explained in part by differences in SES (Model III). In other words, higher income and education may be somewhat protective of cognitive functioning in the face of caregiving-related stress and burden. A similar pattern emerged after adjusting for the health and behavioral risk factors, depicted in Model IV (RR = 0.89; 95% CI, 0.82-0.97), suggesting that poorer episodic memory among caregivers is partially related to their overall poorer health status relative to non-caregivers. Despite systematic differences across all of these factors, a small effect for caregiving on episodic memory persists (Model V) after controlling for a full array of the covariates (Table 3).
Table 3.
Model I | Model II | Model III | Model IV | Model V | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Stroke caregivera | 0.86 (0.04) [0.78-0.93] | 0.86 (0.04) [0.79-0.93] | 0.91 (0.04) [0.84-0.99] | 0.89 (0.04) [0.82-0.97] | 0.91 (0.04) [0.84-0.98] | |
Demographic characteristics | ||||||
Femaleb | 1.17 (0.02) [1.13-1.20] | 1.18 (0.02) [1.15-1.22] | ||||
Age, yearsc | < 45 | 1.14 (0.06) [1.03-1.27] | 1.08 (0.06) [0.97-1.19] | |||
45-54 | 1.18 (0.03) [1.12-1.24] | 1.12 (0.03) [1.07-1.18] | ||||
55-64 | 1.12 (0.03) [1.07-1.17] | 1.09 (0.02) [1.07-1.17] | ||||
Race/ethnic groupd | African American | 0.82 (0.02) [0.78-0.86] | 0.82 (0.02) [0.78-0.86] | |||
Hispanic | 0.83 (0.02) [0.78-0.87] | 0.83 (0.02) [0.78-0.87] | ||||
Other | 0.93 (0.05) [0.83-1.04] | 0.93 (0.05) [0.83-1.04] | ||||
Socioeconomic characteristics | ||||||
Income (logged) | 1.04 (0.00) [1.03-1.06] | 1.04 (0.00) [1.03-1.06] | ||||
Educatione | High school | 1.17 (0.02) [1.12-1.22] | 1.17 (0.02) [1.12-1.22] | |||
Some college | 1.25 (0.03) [1.20-1.31] | 1.25 (0.03) [1.20-1.31] | ||||
Health and behavioral risk factors | ||||||
Depressive symptoms | 0.98 (0.01) [0.97-0.99] | 0.98 (0.01) [0.96-1.00] | ||||
Perceived poor healthf | 0.89 (0.02) [0.85-0.93] | 0.95 (0.02) [0.91-1.00] | ||||
Hypertensiong | 0.96 (0.01) [0.93-0.99] | 0.98 (0.01) [0.96-1.01] | ||||
Diabetesh | 0.93 (0.02) [0.89-0.98] | 0.97 (0.02) [0.92-1.02] | ||||
Heart problemsi | 0.96 (0.02) [0.91-1.00] | 0.98 (0.02) [0.94-1.02] | ||||
Sedentaryj | 1.00 (0.01) [0.98-1.03] | 0.99(0.01) [0.97-1.02] | ||||
Obesek | 0.99 (0.01) [0.97-1.01] | 1.03 (0.01) [1.00-1.05] | ||||
Smokerl | 0.99 (0.02) [0.95-1.02] | 1.01 (0.02) [0.97-1.04] | ||||
Heavy drinkerm | 0.96 (0.02) [0.92-1.00] | 1.00 (0.02) [0.96-1.05] | ||||
Intercept | 5.14 (0.04) [5.07-5.21] | 4.32 (0.09) [4.14-4.52] | 2.76 (0.23) [2.34-3.24] | 5.49 (0.07) [5.36-5.63] | 3.01 (0.02) [2.57-3.53] | |
Deviance statistic | 2720.05 | 2518.42 | 2555.72 | 2632.23 | 2376.71 | |
Degrees of freedom | 3499 | 3492 | 3496 | 3486 | 3476 |
Note: Data are expressed as incident rate ratios [95% confidence interval] unless noted otherwise. Entries are bolded if P < .05. Omitted reference categories:
non-caregiver
male
age > 65 years
non-Hispanic white
less than high school
good health
no hypertension
no diabetes
no heart problems
physically active
non-obese (body mass index < 30)
non-smoker or former smoker
non-heavy drinker or nondrinker.
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is 1 of 2 population-based studies17 focused on cognitive functioning in spousal caregivers of stroke survivors. These caregivers, as suggested by the current findings, may be at risk for poor cognitive functioning. In particular, being a stroke caregiver was associated with a small but persistent increase in the risk for poor episodic memory. Episodic memory is a fundamental neurocognitive function in daily life as one constantly encounters and needs to remember new events, names, faces, objects, and locations. Although we initially also observed significant differences in working memory, semantic memory, and learning between stroke caregivers and non-caregivers, the results of the multivariate analysis indicated that poorer functioning in these areas is largely attributable to the greater vulnerability observed among the stroke caregivers in terms of demographic, SES, and several major dimensions of health and well-being.
Stroke changes the lives not only of the patients but also of their spousal caregivers. Mental health18,28-31 and quality of life8,10,32 in these caregivers have been extensively studied, and various interventions focused on depression, stroke care knowledge, or social support have been developed to improve their well-being (see review by Visser-Meily et al33). However, cognitive functioning also plays a crucial role in well-being but is underrecognized in the literature. For example, depression may lead to poor cognitive functioning, especially memory.34 Many studies have suggested that depression in caregivers mediates poor cognitive functioning.11,16 However, similar to the findings of Lee et al,17 we found that stroke caregivers recalled fewer words than non-caregivers in a delayed recall task, despite differences between these groups in depressive symptoms.
The focus of the present investigation was to establish the association between stroke caregiving and cognitive functioning. The finding demonstrates that stroke caregivers’ poor performance in episodic memory cannot be fully accounted for by demographic, SES, and several major dimensions of health and well-being, suggesting that being a stroke caregiver is a significant predictor of cognitive functioning.
The next direction for the current research in the field of stroke rehabilitation is to identify mediators of the specific poor cognitive functioning in stroke caregivers so that interventions may be tailored to alleviate these negative effects of caregiving. In particular, caregiver strain has been identified elsewhere in the literature as a mediator of cognitive functioning. In studies on caregivers of spouses with dementia, it was shown that caregivers were more likely to exhibit cognitive decline during the study period if they had higher suspicion that others intend harm to them12 or if they had low self-competence.16 Furthermore, prior work suggests that gender may moderate the complex interplay among caregiving, strain, and cognition.35 Also, episodic memory is vulnerable to stress14 and aging,36 because the related brain deficits commonly occur in the prefrontal and medial temporal cortices or the connecting white-matter tracts that are critical for episodic memory.37-39 The age-related decline in episodic memory may be accelerated in people who became stroke caregivers of their spouses at an older age.
The limitations of this study include the lack of objective measures of caregiver status (ie, they were not explicitly identified as caregivers but as spouses living with stroke survivors), caregivers’ cognitive functioning before their spouses’ stroke, stroke severity (ie, the degree of dependence), poststroke disorders (physical, mental, or cognitive), caregiver burden (eg, hours of caregiving per day), and nonverbal-mediated neurocognitive functioning (eg, visuospatial cognition). The analysis was cross-sectional with the use of an existing database, thus limiting our ability to draw causal inferences about the relationship between caregiver status and cognition. The main drawback of secondary data is that the data were not collected to examine the question of the current interest. For example, the spousal caregivers were not explicitly identified in this study. However, we based our procedure on the strength of other analyses that had identified caregivers retrospectively for exploratory analytic purposes.35 Also, measures of the caregivers’ cognitive function before their spouses’ stroke onset were not controlled; these measures were unavailable, because we used the survey wave when the HRS first incorporated cognitive measures. We selected this specific survey wave, rather than a more current survey wave, to eliminate the practice effect that may result from multiple administration of the same cognitive tests.22 In addition, the cognitive measures selected were all verbally mediated for the convenience of telephone surveys. A complete face-to-face examination of the global and detailed cognitive functions, including visuospatial cognitive skills, would be ideal for obtaining more specific information on cognitive consequences related to providing care for a stroke survivor.
Although the use of secondary data limits this analysis to existing measures, the HRS dataset is an unparalleled source of information on many issues related to aging in the general population. One of the advantages of using this exceptionally rich dataset was the large sample and wide array of measures that allowed us to build a fully elaborated model. In particular, the HRS data provide the opportunity to analyze the multiple dimensions of SES – an important social determinant of health that changes significantly at the end of the life course. Based on our inclusion of education and income as important predictors in the model, we have demonstrated that stroke caregivers are considerably disadvantaged relative to noncaregivers, which has further implications for the relationship between caregiving and cognition. Further analysis is needed to assess how the effects of a disadvantaged SES compound over time. Moreover, as the risk for stroke increases with age, we expect that a greater number of individuals from other social strata will become caregivers. Therefore, it will be important to assess how the effect of caregiving and cognition may be buffered by SES resources and other support.
Another potential limitation is the use of the data collected in 1996 to extrapolate to the problems faced by stroke caregivers today. Professional stroke care has changed significantly compared with care provided more than 15 years ago, thanks to the advances of medical and rehabilitative research. Such change has improved the stroke survival rates and consequently increased the number of stroke survivors returning home and receiving care from their families, especially spouses.40 Thus, over the years, more people have become caregivers for their relatives. However, there is little evidence that informal stroke care has changed. Spousal caregivers nowadays may or may not be experiencing less stress or less burden, or in turn be having fewer cognitive consequences, than those in 1996. Because there is not sufficient evidence of an effective intervention program or systematic training widely accessible for family caregivers then or now,33 our finding may well be applied to the present day. The nationally representative HRS sample was a considerable strength of this study, albeit the generalizations of these findings are limited to community-dwelling older adults. Our finding of the gap between stroke caregivers and non-caregivers may in fact be underestimated, because individuals who were institutionalized or were very impaired were either not included or dropped out over time from the HRS study.
Demonstration of an association between caregiver status and episodic memory in this study provides a basis for future analyses to investigate the implications of caregiving over time and to identify the processes related to accelerated cognitive decline. The results of this study add support to the body of evidence documenting the risk that caregiving may pose to health and well-being in the older population. Spousal caregivers of stroke survivors may be at risk for poor cognitive functioning. More work is needed to identify the processes that negatively contribute to the diminished cognitive capacity among these adults, so that interventions may be developed to alleviate the negative effects of caregiving on cognitive health.
Acknowledgments
Financial support/disclosures: This work was supported by the Kessler Foundation.
Additional contributions: The authors thank Giuseppe Amore for assistance with the literature search.
REFERENCES
- 1.Wilson DB, Houle DM, Keith RA. Stroke rehabilitation: A model predicting return home. West J Med. 1991;154(5):587–590. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.McCullagh E, Brigstocke G, Donaldson N, Kalra L. Determinants of caregiving burden and quality of life in caregivers of stroke patients. Stroke. 2005;36(10):2181–2186. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000181755.23914.53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Caswell J. A full measure of devotion. Stroke Connection. 2011 Mar-Apr; http://www. nxtbook.com/nxtbooks/aha/strokeconnection_20110304/#/10.
- 4.Caswell J. Classes for caregivers. Stroke Connection. 2010 Nov-Dec; [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hinojosa MS, Rittman M. Association between health education needs and stroke caregiver injury. J Aging Health. 2009;21(7):1040–1058. doi: 10.1177/0898264309344321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tang WK, Lau CG, Mok V, Ungvari GS, Wong KS. Burden of Chinese stroke family caregivers: The Hong Kong experience. Arch Phys Med. Rehabil. 2011;92(9):1462–1467. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2011.03.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kusano CT, Bouldin ED, Anderson LA, et al. Adult informal caregivers reporting financial burden in Hawaii, Kansas, and Washington: Results from the 2007 Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System. Disabil Health J. 2011;4(4):229–237. doi: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2011.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bulley C, Shiels J, Wilkie K, Salisbury L. Carer experiences of life after stroke—a qualitative analysis. Disabil Rehabil. 2010;32(17):1406–1413. doi: 10.3109/09638280903531238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Greenwood N, Mackenzie A. Informal caring for stroke survivors: Meta-ethnographic review of qualitative literature. Maturitas. 2010;66(3):268–276. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2010.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Rombough RE, Howse EL, Bagg SD, Bartfay WJ. A comparison of studies on the quality of life of primary caregivers of stroke survivors: A systematic review of the literature. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2007;14(3):69–79. doi: 10.1310/tsr1403-69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Vitaliano PP, Zhang JP, Young HM, Caswell LW, Scanlan JM, Echeverria D. Depressed mood mediates decline in cognitive processing speed in caregivers. Gerontologist. 2009;49(1):12–22. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnp004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Vitaliano PP, Echeverria D, Yi J, Phillips PEM, Young H, Siegler IC. Psychophysiological mediators of caregiver stress and differential cognitive decline. Psychol Aging. 2005;20(3):402–411. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Vitaliano PP, Murphy M, Young HM, Echeverria D, Borson S. Does caring for a spouse with dementia promote cognitive decline? A hypothesis and proposed mechanisms. J. Am Geriatr Soc. 2011;59(5):900–908. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03368.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Caswell LW, Vitaliano PP, Croyle KL, Scanlan JM, Zhang JP, Daruwala A. Negative associations of chronic stress and cognitive performance in older adult spouse caregivers. Exp Aging Res. 2003;29(3):303–318. doi: 10.1080/03610730303721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mackenzie CS, Wiprzycka UJ, Hasher L, Goldstein D. Associations between psychological distress, learning, and memory in spouse caregivers of older adults. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64B(6):742–746. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.de Vugt ME, Jolles J, van Osch L, et al. Cognitive functioning in spousal caregivers of dementia patients: Findings from the prospective MAASBED study. Age Ageing. 2006;35(2):160–166. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afj044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lee S, Kawachi I, Grodstein F. Does caregiving stress affect cognitive function in older women? J Nerv Ment Dis. 2004;192(1):51–57. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000106000.02232.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Perrin PB, Heesacker M, Hinojosa MS, Uthe CE, Rittman MR. Identifying at-risk, ethnically diverse stroke caregivers for counseling: A longitudinal study of mental health. Rehabil Psychol. 2009;54(2):138–149. doi: 10.1037/a0015964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wellwood I, Dennis MS, Warlow CP. Perceptions and knowledge of stroke among surviving patients with stroke and their carers. Age Ageing. 1994;23(4):293–298. doi: 10.1093/ageing/23.4.293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.King RB, Hartke RJ, Houle TT. Patterns of relationships between background characteristics, coping, and stroke caregiver outcomes. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2010;17(4):308–317. doi: 10.1310/tsr1704-308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Heeringa SG, Connor JH. Technical Description of the Health and Retirement Survey Sample Design. Ann Arbor, MI: 1995. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ofstedal MB, Fisher GG, Herzog AR. Documentation of Cognitive Functioning Measures in the Health and Retirement Study. University of Michigan; Ann Arbor: 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration Norms and Commentary. 3rd ed. Oxford University Press; New York: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Keene ON. The log transformation is special. Stat Med. 1995;14(8):811–819. doi: 10.1002/sim.4780140810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1(3):385–401. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Stata Statistical Software: Release 12 [computer program] StataCorp LP; College Station, TX: 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 27.US Department of Commerce [February 22, 2012];United States Census Bureau. Historical Income Tables: Households by total money income, race, and Hispanic origin of householder 2011. http://www.census.gov/hhes/www/income/data/historical/household/.
- 28.Saban KL, Sherwood PR, DeVon HA, Hynes DM. Measures of psychological stress and physical health in family caregivers of stroke survivors: A literature review. J Neurosci Nurs. 2010;42(3):128–138. doi: 10.1097/jnn.0b013e3181d4a3ee. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Han B, Haley WE. Family caregiving for patients with stroke—review and analysis. Stroke. 1999;30(7):1478–1485. doi: 10.1161/01.str.30.7.1478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Anderson CS, Linto J, Stewartwynne EG. A population-based assessment of the impact and burden of caregiving for long-term stroke survivors. Stroke. 1995;26(5):843–849. doi: 10.1161/01.str.26.5.843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Chumbler NR, Rittman MR, Wu SS. Associations in sense of coherence and depression in caregivers of stroke survivors across 2 years. J Behav Health Serv Res. 2008;35(2):226–234. doi: 10.1007/s11414-007-9083-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Chen YK, Lu JY, Wong KS, Mok VCT, Ungvari GS, Tang WK. Health-related quality of life in the family caregivers of stroke survivors. Int J Rehabil Res. 2010;33(3):232–237. doi: 10.1097/MRR.0b013e328338b04b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Visser-Meily A, van Heugten C, Post M, Schepers V, Lindeman E. Intervention studies for caregivers of stroke survivors: A critical review. Patient Educ Couns. 2005;56(3):257–267. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2004.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Elderkin-Thompson V, Moody T, Knowlton B, Hellemann G, Kumar A. Explicit and implicit memory in late-life depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;19(4):364–373. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181e89a5b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Norton MC, Smith KR, Ostbye T, et al. Greater risk of dementia when spouse has dementia? The Cache County study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010;58(5):895–900. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.02806.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Metzler-Baddeley C, Jones DK, Belaroussi B, Aggleton JP, O'Sullivan MJ. Frontotemporal connections in episodic memory and aging: A diffusion MRI tractography study. J Neurosci. 2011;31(37):13236–13245. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2317-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Suzuki C, Tsukiura T, Mochizuki-Kawai H, Shigemune Y, Iijima T. Prefrontal and medial temporal contributions to episodic memory-based reasoning. Neurosci Res. 2009;63(3):177–183. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2008.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Easton A, Eacott MJ. Recollection of episodic memory within the medial temporal lobe: Behavioural dissociations from other types of memory. Behav Brain Res. 2010;215(2):310–317. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bremner JD. Does stress damage the brain? Biol Psychiatr. 1999;45(7):797–805. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00009-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Lutz BJ, Young ME. Rethinking intervention strategies in stroke family caregiving. Rehabil Nurs. 2010;35(4):152–160. doi: 10.1002/j.2048-7940.2010.tb00041.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]