Abstract
Studies have revealed that catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) and dopaminegic receptor2 (DRD2) modulate human attention bias for palatable food or tobacco. However, the existing evidence about the modulations of COMT and DRD2 on attentional bias for facial expressions was still limited. In the study, 650 college students were genotyped with regard to COMT Val158Met and DRD2 TaqI A polymorphisms, and the attentional bias for facial expressions was assessed using the spatial cueing task. The results indicated that COMT Val158Met underpinned the individual difference in attentional bias for negative emotional expressions (P = 0.03) and the Met carriers showed more engagement bias for negative expressions than the Val/Val homozygote. On the contrary, DRD2 TaqIA underpinned the individual difference in attentional bias for positive expressions (P = 0.003) and individuals with TT genotype showed much more engagement bias for positive expressions than the individuals with CC genotype. Moreover, the two genes exerted significant interactions on the engagements for negative and positive expressions (P = 0.046, P = 0.005). These findings suggest that the individual differences in the attentional bias for emotional expressions are partially underpinned by the genetic polymorphisms in COMT and DRD2.
Introduction
Attentional bias, a tendency that individuals exhibit high sensitivity and selective attention to special stimuli or relevant information [1], [2], is essential to human survival and interactions in different situations [3], [4] because this bias is involved in many cognitive and behavioral biases such as visual searching for behavioral relevant features [5], emotional response to novel visual stimuli [6], recall of threatening words [7], and drug seeking [8].
Attentional bias to special stimuli is widely varied between individuals. It has been suggested that dopamine plays a specific role in drawing attention to emotional events [9], and attentional bias to substance-related cues emerges as a result of dopaminergic activity [10]. However, the existing evidence is insufficient to understand the molecular basis of attentional bias. In the study, the main purpose is to investigate to what content of dopaminergic modulations on the individual difference in attentional bias.
Substance-related cues strongly draw individuals' attention, and they are the causes of certain behavioral disorders such as obesity [11] and drug abuse [12]. However, these cues deliver a little interpersonal information [13]. Differently, the facial expressions, as a non-verbal social communication [14], contain much social information and have great power to effect human interpersonal activities [15], [16]. Studies have indicated that the deficits in facial expressions processing are involved in the pathogenesis of many psychiatric disorders such as autism [17] and depression [18], and children with a history of physical abuse exhibit attentional bias for angry faces [19]. Therefore, to examine what content of dopaminergic modulation on attentional bias for interpersonal information, 270 facial expressions were selected as the cues of attention task in the present study.
Recent evidence has indicated that an increased dopamine activity in the prefrontal cortex enhances the attentional bias for reward-related cues such as palatable food and tobacco [10], [20], [21]. However, only a few studies have indicated that dopamine has great affects on the processing of facial expression, of which an acute dopaminergic blockade in healthy volunteers results in a transient disruption of the recognition of facial expressions of anger, whilst leaving the intact recognition for fear and disgust [22].
One of the most concerned proteins in dopamine system is Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT). Much evidence has suggested that this protein degrades catecholamines such as dopamine and norepinephrine [23] and greatly regulates the level of dopamine in brain. The COMT Val158Met (rs4680), a single G/A base pair substitution at codon 158, is related to the dopamine levels. Studies have indicated that Met allele of Val158Met can reduce the activity of COMT to one-quarter of what is originally encoded by Val allele [24], [25].
It was shown that COMT Val158Met modulates the brain activity during processing of negative stimuli, but not the stimuli with positive valence. Specifically, the Met/Met carriers, as compared to the Val/Val carriers, are more sensitive to the unpleasant stimuli [26]. Therefore, given the regulation of COMT on dopamine activity and the link between COMT activity and Val158Met variation, we predict that Val158Met is associated with attentional bias for negative expressions and the individuals with Met allele display more attentional bias for negative emotional expressions.
The actions of dopamine in brain are mediated by multiple dopaminegic receptors. Dopamine receptor D2 (DRD2) is one of the most abundant receptors in central nervous system [27]. It has been suggested that this receptor is involved in reward-related psychiatric disorders [28], [29] such as addiction and schizophrenia [30], [31]. Studies have evidenced that the density and binding site of DRD2 are influenced by several functional polymorphisms in DRD2 [32], [33]. TaqIA (rs1800497) is the most concerned functional polymorphism in the gene. This polymorphism is related to the release of dopamine in the synaptic [34], [35], and T allele carriers are known to have a 30–40% decreased density of DRD2 [32], [34], [35]. Recent years, studies have indicated that TaqIA underlines the individual differences in work memory [36], sustained attention [37], [38], substance addiction [30], [39], and a reduced capacity in learning negative characteristics of stimuli [40]. However, the modulation of TaqIA on the attention to facial expressions, especially to the pleasure facial expressions, has not been well investigated. Here, we hypothesize that TaqIA is associated with the attentional bias for positive expressions because of the T allele carriers of this variant showing a reduced capacity for learning negative characteristics of stimuli [40].
The current study aimed to explore the modulations of COMT Val158Met and DRD2 TaqIA on attentional bias for emotional expressions in a nonclinical college student sample. Moreover, although evidence has shown that the T allele of TaqIA and Met allele of COMT both lead to increased dopamine levels of at the synaptic clef, few studies have investigated to what extent the two genes interact to attentional bias. Guided by the previous studies, we predict that COMT Val158Met and DRD2 TaqIA are associated with attentional bias for different affective facial expressions and interact to affect the bias.
Methods
Ethical Approval
The ethics committees of Peking University and Henan University of Science and Technology approved the study. Participants signed informed consent before taking part. The study was carried out according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.
Participants
Seven hundred right-handed participants, aging from 20 to 22, were recruited randomly from Henan University of Science and Technology in China. These subjects underwent mental health examinations by using Self-rating Depression scale [41], [42], Self-rating Anxiety scale [43], and UCLA (University of California, Los Angeles) Loneliness scale [44]. Thirty five participants, including 18 individuals with depressive symptoms (standard cut-off point of ≥50), 12 individuals with anxiety (standard cut-off point of ≥50) and 5 individuals with higher loneliness level (standard cut-off point of ≥50), were excluded from this study. Moreover, we surveyed the quantity of smoking (1.60 cigarettes) and the frequency of drinking (0.23) of each month, which indicated these subjects had no the habits of smoking and alcohol addiction. Finally, 665 unrelated Chinese Han volunteers (499 females and 166 males), with about 13 years education, were formally recruited. All subjects were Chinese Han individuals in origin by self-report. The hair follicle cells were collected after informed consents were obtained.
Attentional bias assessment
Two hundred and seventy facial photographs, including 90 neutral expressions, 90 negative expressions (30 sad faces, 30 fearful faces and 30 angry faces) and 90 happy expressions, were selected from the Chinese Affective Picture System [45]. All the pictures were assessed on the intensity (Mean ± SD) with 9-point rating scale (1 = most weak, 9 = most intensive) by the designers [45]. Each emotional faces category consisted of 45 female facial photographs and 45 male facial photographs. The three photograph categories were matched in intensity (negative 5.66±0.99, neutral 5.75±0.19, happy 5.22±1.06).
The spatial cueing task was used to assess attentional bias [46], [47], [48], [49]. In the task, the participants focused on a fixation point in the center of screen. Then cue was presented, and a target was followed appearing on the left or right side of screen. Participants indicated the location of the target as quickly as possible. In the trails, the cues were valid when the cues and the targets were appeared in the same locations, otherwise the cues were invalid. Valid trails are benefit to direct attention to the cued location whereas invalid trails might promote orienting to the uncued location.
In this task, the reaction times (RTs) of valid trials are shorter than those of the invalid trials when stimulus-onset asynchrony is less than 300 ms. On the contrary, the RTs of valid trials are longer than those of the invalid trials once the stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA) exceeds 300 ms [50], [51], [52]. The attentional bias was decomposed into cue validity, engagement and disengagement. Cue validity (RTinvalid cue -RTvalid cue) provides a measure of overall attention for the different cue types, of which positive scores indicate attention away valid cue whereas negative scores indicate attention toward the valid cue when SOA exceeds 300 ms. At the same time, engagement (RTvalid neutral cue -RTvalid emotional cue) and disengagement (RTinvalid emotional cue-RTinvalid neutral cue) provide measures of attentional capture and attentional holding for emotional stimuli, respectively, of which a positive score of aengagement indicates an enhanced attentional capture by emotional cues while a positive score of disengagement indicates a strong attentional holding by emotional cue [48].
In this study, the spatial cueing task comprised 270 randomly presented trails. Participants viewed the fixation point for 300 ms, then one cueing facial expression was appeared on the left or right side of screen for 500 ms, and a horizontal arrow was presented immediately in one side of the screen after the cue facial expression was disappeared. The subjects pressed a key indicating where the arrow was located. When an arrow was appeared in the right side of the screen, the participants pressed the “Alt” key with forefinger of right hand; otherwise they pressed the “Ctr” key with forefinger of left hand. The horizontal arrow would disappear in 2000 ms if the subjects did not make response. Moreover, there was a 300 ms interval between the two trials, in which a fixation point was presented.
Attentional bias is comprised of facilitated orienting, disengaging and attentional avoidance. Each of the components is related to the occurrence of attentional bias at different processes of attention. Thus, measurement of these components necessitates a task that can differentiate the components. The spatial cueing task, as comparing to the other paradigms such as dot probe task and modified stroop task, has an outstanding advantage to revealing these components. Cue validity, engagement and disengagement measured using spatial cueing task provide valid measures of attentional avoidance, facilitated orienting and disengagement, respectively [47]. In contrast, the dot probe task and the stroop task can't distinguish these components and reveal the origins of attentional bias. However, the attentional bias assessed using spatial cueing task is easily influenced by the sequence effects[53]. To diminish this effect, we displayed a 300 ms inter- trail interval in the task.
In the trails, the photographs were presented in the left or right fields with an equal number of presentations. The program was compiled by using DMDX display software[54]. The display software (version number: 3.2.6.4) was set up on the computer with video card at 640×480 with 16 bits per pixel.
Genotyping
Genomic DNA was extracted from hair follicle cells by using Chelex-100 method [55]. COMT Val158Met was amplified by polymerase chain reaction (PCR) by using upstream primer, 5′-CCAGCGGATGGTGGATTTCGCACGC-3′ and the downstream primer 5′-TGGGGGGGTCTTTCCTCAGCC-3′. The AC in the upstream primer was a site-directed mutagenesis for introducing a restriction site for MluI. The PCR was performed with a volume of 5 ul system containing 2.50 ul reaction MIX (Golden Easy PCR System, TIANGEN), 0.50 ul DNA template, 2.50 ul ddH2O, 0.25 ul (25 pmol/ul) upstream primer, and 0.25 ul (25 pmol/ul) downstream primer. A product of 206 bp was amplified with an initial 4 min denaturation at 94°C, followed by 30 cycles of 94°C for 30 s, 63.5°C for 30 seconds, 72°C for 30 s, and a final extension at 72°C for 3 min. The PCR product was incubated with MluI (FERMENTAS, MBI) at 37°C overnight. According to the provided protocols, the 5.0 ul incubation system contained 1.5 ul PCR products, 4.0 U MluI (10 U/ul), 0.4 ul R buffer, and 3.1 uL ddH2O. The digested mixture was analyzed by using 8% polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis with 200 V for 1.5 h, which was followed by silver staining. Finally, the genotypes were scanned by using the Bio-imaging System.
DRD2 TaqIA was amplified by PCR. The upstream primer, 5′- ATGCCCTGCTTTCGG -3″ and the downstream primer, 5′- GAGTGTCATCAACCTCCTA -3′ were recruited. A 201 bp product was amplified with an initial 7 min denaturation at 94°C, followed by 32 cycles of 94°C for 1 min, 62°C for 30 s, 72°C for 30 s, and a final extension at 72°C for 7 min. The PCR product was incubated with TaqI at 65°C for 5 h. The incubation was performed in a volume of 5 ul system containing 1.0 ul PCR products, 3.5 U TaqI (10 U/ul), and 0.35 ul Tango™ buffer, and 3.3 ul ddH2O. The digested mixture was resolved on 8% polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis with 250 V for 1 h, which was followed by silver staining.
Statistical Analysis
Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium tests were carried out with Finetti software. The independent samples t tests were conducted to examine the effects of COMT Val158Met on attentional bias (Met/Met and Met/Val genotypes were combined as one group because of the rare frequency of Met/Met (5.59%), which could enhance the power and reliability of the genetic behavioral study). One-way analysis of variances (ANOVAs) was performed to assess the effects of DRD2 TaqIA on the attentional bias. The statistical power was evaluated with the G*Power program [56]. Statistical significance was referred to as P<0.05.
Results
Fifteen subjects, who made more 10% errors, were excluded from our further study. The mean (410.40 ms) and standard deviation (101.31 ms) of RTs of 30 randomly selected subjects were established after the odd values were ruled out. According to the mean and standard deviation, we excluded error responses, RTs of less than 200 ms or above three standard deviations from the original data. Moreover, there were no significant gender differences in the indices of attentional biases in the present study.
The genotyping was carried out for 650 participants. Six hundred and forty four subjects were genotyped successfully at COMT Val158Met (Met/Met = 36, Met/Val = 231, Val/Val = 377) and 632 subjects were genotyped successfully at DRD2 TaqIA (TT = 92, TC = 317, CC = 223). The results indicated that the two genetic variations showed no deviations from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (χ 2 = 0.01, P = 0.94; χ 2 = 1.47, P = 0.23).
The independent samples t tests indicated that COMT Val158Met was associated with engagement for negative expressions and the 267 individuals with Met allele (Met/Met & Val/Met) showed larger engagement bias for negative emotional content expressions than the 377 Val/Val homozygotes (3.94±0.88 vs. 1.31±0.77; t (642) = 2.24, P = 0.03). The effect size indicated that this polymorphism could explain 1.00% (η2 = 0.01) variance in engagement bias for negative expressions. However, COMT Val158Met was not significantly associated with cue validities and disengagements. The effects of this polymorphism on attentional bias were displayed in table 1, and attentional bias scores of each genotype were shown as mean and standard error (SE).
Table 1. The modulations of COMT Val158Met on attentional bias.
Attentional bias | Met/Met (36) & Val/Met (231) | Val/Val (377) | t (642) | P |
Cue validities | ||||
Positive expressions | −15.57±1.88 | −14.57±1.70 | 0.39 | 0.70 |
Neutral expressions | −23.74±1.81 | −22.80±1.64 | 0.38 | 0.70 |
Negative expressions | −18.97±1.87 | −18.66±1.61 | 0.13 | 0.90 |
Engagements | ||||
Negative expressions | 3.94±0.88 | 1.31±0.77 | 2.24 | 0.03 |
Positive expressions | 3.87±0.94 | 2.08±0.83 | 1.42 | 0.16 |
Disengagements | ||||
Negative expressions | 0.83±0.81 | 2.83±0.73 | 1.81 | 0.07 |
Positive expressions | 4.30±0.98 | 6.15±0.89 | 1.39 | 0.17 |
ANOVAs showed that DRD2 TaqIA was significantly associated with attentional engagement bias for positive emotional expressions (F (2,629) = 5.81, P = 0.003). The 92 individuals with TT genotype of TaqIA (6.92±1.60) showed much engagement bias for positive emotional content expressions than the 223 individuals with CC genotype (0.42±1.00). The effect size indicated that this polymorphism could explain 2.00% (η2 = 0.02) variance in engagement for positive emotional expressions. However, TaqIA was not significantly associated with cue validities and disengagements. The effects of polymorphism on attentional bias were displayed in table 2.
Table 2. The modulations of DRD2 TaqIA on attentional bias.
Attentional bias | TT (92) | TC (317) | CC (223) | F (2,629) | P |
Cue validities | |||||
Positive expressions | −10.92±4.06 | −15.36±1.75 | −14.63±2.07 | 0.66 | 0.52 |
Neutral expressions | −25.31±3.51 | −23.70±1.64 | −20.39±2.07 | 1.12 | 0.33 |
Negative expressions | −20.86±3.87 | −18.85±1.66 | −17.30±2.02 | 0.44 | 0.65 |
Engagements | |||||
Negative expressions | 2.04±1.48 | 2.85±0.76 | 1.52±1.10 | 0.55 | 0.58 |
Positive expressions | 6.92±1.60 | 3.42±0.91 | 0.42±1.00 | 5.81 | 0.003 |
Disengagements | |||||
Negative expressions | 2.41±1.52 | 1.62±0.80 | 1.58±0.91 | 0.13 | 0.88 |
Positive expressions | 7.47±1.58 | 4.92±0.90 | 5.34±1.22 | 0.81 | 0.45 |
We further examined the interactions of COMT Val158Met and DRD2 TaqIA on attentional bias, in which COMT (Met/Met & Met/Val were combined as one group for the lower frequency of Met/Met) was an upstream gene of DRD2 according to their roles in dopamine metabolism and signaling. The results indicated that the two genes exerted significant interactions on engagements for negative and positive emotional expressions (F (5,611) = 2.27, P = 0.046, η2 = 0.02; F (5,611) = 3.36, P = 0.005, η2 = 0.03). Moreover, this analysis showed that the individuals with Met/Met (Met/Val) & TT combinations showed the largest engagements bias for positive expressions (8.60±3.31) and individuals with Val/Val & CC combinations showed the smallest engagements bias for negative expressions (−0.97±1.48). The interactions of COMT and DRD2 on the engagements bias were displayed in table 3.
Table 3. The interactions of COMT and DRD2 on the engagements bias.
COMT | DRD2 | Frequency | Negative expressions | Positive expressions |
Met/Met (Val/Met) | TT | 26 | 1.36±2.28 | 8.60±3.31 |
TC | 132 | 4.11±1.21 | 3.67±1.34 | |
CC | 96 | 4.95±1.57 | 2.54±1.34 | |
Total | 254 | 4.14±0.89 | 3.74±0.93 | |
Val/Val | TT | 58 | 2.58±1.96 | 5.89±1.86 |
TC | 186 | 2.04±1.01 | 2.86±1.28 | |
CC | 119 | −0.97±1.48 | −1.64±1.47 | |
Total | 363 | 1.14±0.78 | 1.87±0.88 |
A power analysis was implemented by using the G*Power program. The sample size revealed more than 95% power for the detection of significant associations (P<0.05), when the tested variations had a medium genetic effects, under an effect size index of 0.30 [57].
Discussion
The study investigated the modulations of COMT and DRD2 on attentional bias for emotional facial expressions in a college student population. The results demonstrated that COMT Val158Met underpinned the individual difference in attentional bias for negative emotional expressions, whereas DRD2 TaqIA underpinned the individual difference in attentional bias for positive emotional expressions.
COMT Val158Met was associated with the engagement, but not the disengagement for negative emotional expressions. The individuals with Met allele showed more engagement bias for negative emotional expressions. These findings implicated that Val158Met regulated the enhanced attentional capture by negative emotional expressions and the individuals with Met allele was more liable to be attracted by the negative emotional stimuli. We thought that the high sensitivity to unpleasant stimuli of the Met carriers might be a result of more engagement bias for negative emotional expressions [26]. Moreover, studies has evidenced that anxiety and depression-linked attentional bias is characterized at selective processing of negative stimuli [58], [59], [60]. Therefore, this attentional bias can lead to an increased perception in negative stimuli that were accompanied by more frequencies of anxiety. As a feedback, the increased perception in negative stimuli resulted in a high risk of individuals with Met allele in anxiety [61].
DRD2 TaqIA was associated with engagement for positive emotional content of expressions, and the individuals with TT genotype of TaqIA showed more engagement bias for positive emotional expressions. These findings implicated that the attention of individuals with TT genotype was liable to be captured by positive emotional stimuli. So far, previous studies have shown the T allele is a genetic marker of reward sensitivity [28] and the individuals with T allele always favor the reward-related cues such as palatable food, tobacco and heroin because these cues can decrease the negative feelings by activating the release of brain dopamine [62]. Positive facial expressions could be considered as reward related-cues which could induce the participants' pleasant experiences. As a feedback, they were able to capture more attention resources.
Evidence from psychological studied has revealed that attentional bias is related to the different processes of attention [48], [63]. The engagement expresses the bias of early vigilance while disengagement denotes the bias of later attention holding [64]. In this study, COMT and DRD2 were both associated with engagement bias, but not disengagement bias. The results indicated that COMT and DRD2 were involved in the vigilance of emotional stimuli, and Met allele of COMT facilitated the vigilance bias for negative facial expressions while T allele of TaqIA facilitated the vigilance bias for positive facial expressions.
In the study, using the single genetic loci model, COMT and DRD2 showed selective main effects on the attentional bias for negative and positive emotional expressions, respectively. This finding suggests that there was a significant difference in the genetic foundation of attentional bias between positive and negative stimuli although the T allele of TaqIA increased the release of dopamine [32], [34], [35] and Met allele of COMT leaded to an increased in dopamine levels of in the synaptic clef [24], [25]. Moreover, in the present study, COMT Val158Met and DRD2 TaqIA only accounted for 1.00% and 2.00% variances of attentional bias for facial expressions. This genetic power approximately equaled to the previous results which indicated a certain single genetic loci could explain subtle a variance in attentional bias [49], [65], [66], [67], [68]. The results further indicated that attentional bias was a polygenetic trait and each of the genes exerts a subtle effect on the individual difference although it has a substantial heritability [57]. COMT and DRD2, as well as 5-HTTLPR, ADRA2B, DBH and MAOA in previous studies [49], [65], [66], [68], were a small part of the candidate genes underlying the individual differences of attentional bias because of the complex phenotype including many components such as facilitated engagement, delayed disengagement, attentional controlling and emotional regulation [47]. Therefore, more genetic loci in dopaminergic and serotoninergic systems should be selected in the future investigation.
DRD2 TaqIA and COMT Val158Met are both related to the dopamine levels of the synaptic clef. Consistent with the expected results, the study indicated that the two genes exerted significant interactions on the attentional engagements for negative and positive expressions. The interactive effects could explain 2.00% and 3.00% variances in engagements for negative and positive emotional expressions, respectively. This finding provided strong evidence of the endogenous interactions between COMT and DRD2 on attention bias. Liking many gene-gene interaction studies, the interactions of two genes exceeded their main effects. The interesting results further indicated that attentional bias was multiple genes expressed phenotype and the interactions of the genes exerted more contributions to the individual difference and the substantial heritability of attentional bias [57]. However, a larger sample size was needed in gene-gene interaction analysis. In the research, the lower frequency of Met/Met was limitation in exploring the interactions. Therefore, further research is needed to replicate these findings.
Among the interactional effects, we also observed that Met/Met (Met/Val) & TT combinations exerted the largest impact on engagements bias for positive expressions while Met/Met (Met/Val) & CC combinations exerted the largest impact on engagements bias for negative expressions. These interesting results implicated that there was a difference in the patters of COMT and DRD2 interacting on positive and negative expressions. In the framework of COMT and DRD2 interacting on negative facial expressions, the interactions of Met/Met (Met/Val) & TT combinations and Val/Val & CC combinations showed the smallest impact on engagements bias while the interactions of Met/Met (Met/Val) & CC showed the largest impact on engagements bias. Thus, we arrived at a view that the individuals (Met/Met (Met/Val) & TT) with the lower dopamine levels and individuals (Val/Val & CC) with the higher dopamine levels showed smallest engagements bias, while the individuals (Met/Met (Met/Val) & CC) with the moderate dopamine levels showed the largest engagements bias because T allele of TaqIA and Met allele of COMT were related to the increased in dopamine levels of in the synaptic clef. These findings implicated that there was an inverted U-shaped dose-effect curve between dopamine levels [69], [70] and attentional bias for negative facial expressions. However, in the framework of COMT and DRD2 interacting on engagements bias for positive facial expressions, the effect of dopamine levels on engagements bias did not display the inverted U dose-effect curve. The Met/Met (Met/Val) & TT combinations with higher dopamine levels showed the largest impact, the Val/Val & CC combinations with lower dopamine levels showed the smallest impact, while Met/Met (Met/Val) & CC and Val/Val & TT combinations with moderate level dopamine levels exerted the moderate impacts.
Three limitations of this study need to be mentioned. Firstly, given the wide distribution of the sample, we could not exclude the potential population admixture effect although the large sample size could minimize the distortion. Secondly, only self-report scales were used to screen for neurological and psychiatric disorders of this college sample. Therefore, future research should employ an objective systematic tool. Thirdly, the adjusting for multiple testing was not performed, thus more work was needed to examine the results. However, this study has implications for understanding the genetic contributions of dopamine to cognitive bias.
Conclusions
A population-based study was performed to investigate the modulations of COMT and DRD2 on attentional bias for emotional facial expressions. We observed that COMT Val158Met and DRD2 TaqIA were associated with engagement for negative and positive facial expressions, respectively. The results suggest the inter-subject differences in attentional bias for emotional facial expressions are partially modulated by some functional polymorphisms in dopaminergic genes.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank all the participants involved in this study for their enthusiastic support and participation.
Funding Statement
This study was provided by China Postdoctoral Science Foundation (2013M530002,2012T50023, 20100470156), Henan University of Science and Technology, Scientific Research Foundation for Ph.D (09001498), and National Natural Science Foundation of China (71172216, 71132001), and Visiting Scholar Program for Harvard University and NBER (CSC 201208120037). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Hunt C, Keogh E, French CC (2007) Anxiety sensitivity, conscious awareness and selective attentional biases in children. Behav Res Ther 45: 497–509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Keogh E, Dillon C, Georgiou G, Hunt C (2001) Selective attentional biases for physical threat in physical anxiety sensitivity. J Anxiety Disord 15: 299–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Nummenmaa L, Hietanen JK, Calvo MG, Hyona J (2011) Food catches the eye but not for everyone: a BMI-contingent attentional bias in rapid detection of nutriments. PLoS One 6: e19215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Shechner T, Britton JC, Perez-Edgar K, Bar-Haim Y, Ernst M, et al. (2012) Attention biases, anxiety, and development: toward or away from threats or rewards? Depress Anxiety 29: 282–294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Hannus A, Cornelissen FW, Lindemann O, Bekkering H (2005) Selection-for-action in visual search. Acta Psychol (Amst) 118: 171–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Raymond JE, Fenske MJ, Tavassoli NT (2003) Selective attention determines emotional responses to novel visual stimuli. Psychol Sci 14: 537–542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Kulas JF, Conger JC, Smolin JM (2003) The effects of emotion on memory: an investigation of attentional bias. J Anxiety Disord 17: 103–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Hogarth L, Dickinson A, Janowski M, Nikitina A, Duka T (2008) The role of attentional bias in mediating human drug-seeking behaviour. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 201: 29–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gibbs AA, Naudts KH, Spencer EP, David AS (2007) The role of dopamine in attentional and memory biases for emotional information. Am J Psychiatry 164: : 1603–1609; quiz 1624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Luijten M, Veltman DJ, Hester R, Smits M, Pepplinkhuizen L, et al. (2012) Brain activation associated with attentional bias in smokers is modulated by a dopamine antagonist. Neuropsychopharmacology 37: 2772–2779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Frankort A, Roefs A, Siep N, Roebroeck A, Havermans R, et al. (2012) Reward activity in satiated overweight women is decreased during unbiased viewing but increased when imagining taste: an event-related fMRI study. Int J Obes (Lond) 36: 627–637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Justinova Z, Panlilio LV, Goldberg SR (2009) Drug addiction. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 1: 309–346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Hari R, Kujala MV (2009) Brain basis of human social interaction: from concepts to brain imaging. Physiol Rev 89: 453–479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Anokhin AP, Golosheykin S, Heath AC (2010) Heritability of individual differences in cortical processing of facial affect. Behav Genet 40: 178–185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Chen P, Myers CG, Kopelman S, Garcia SM (2012) The hierarchical face: higher rankings lead to less cooperative looks. J Appl Psychol 97: 479–486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Yamamoto K, Suzuki N (2008) [Facial expressions in the course of relationship formation]. Shinrigaku Kenkyu 78: 567–574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Kennedy DP, Adolphs R (2012) Perception of emotions from facial expressions in high-functioning adults with autism. Neuropsychologia 50: 3313–3319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Csukly G, Czobor P, Szily E, Takacs B, Simon L (2009) Facial expression recognition in depressed subjects: the impact of intensity level and arousal dimension. J Nerv Ment Dis 197: 98–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Stevens S, Rist F, Gerlach AL (2009) Influence of alcohol on the processing of emotional facial expressions in individuals with social phobia. Br J Clin Psychol 48: 125–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Franken IH, Hendriks VM, Stam CJ, Van den Brink W (2004) A role for dopamine in the processing of drug cues in heroin dependent patients. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 14: 503–508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Nathan PJ, O'Neill BV, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Beaver J, et al. (2012) The effects of the dopamine D(3) receptor antagonist GSK598809 on attentional bias to palatable food cues in overweight and obese subjects. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 15: 149–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Lawrence AD, Calder AJ, McGowan SW, Grasby PM (2002) Selective disruption of the recognition of facial expressions of anger. Neuroreport 13: 881–884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Zhu BT (2002) Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT)-mediated methylation metabolism of endogenous bioactive catechols and modulation by endobiotics and xenobiotics: importance in pathophysiology and pathogenesis. Curr Drug Metab 3: 321–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Lachman HM, Papolos DF, Saito T, Yu YM, Szumlanski CL, et al. (1996) Human catechol-O-methyltransferase pharmacogenetics: description of a functional polymorphism and its potential application to neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacogenetics 6: 243–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Lotta T, Vidgren J, Tilgmann C, Ulmanen I, Melen K, et al. (1995) Kinetics of human soluble and membrane-bound catechol O-methyltransferase: a revised mechanism and description of the thermolabile variant of the enzyme. Biochemistry 34: 4202–4210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Herrmann MJ, Wurflein H, Schreppel T, Koehler S, Muhlberger A, et al. (2009) Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype affects neural correlates of aversive stimuli processing. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 9: 168–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Jaber M, Robinson SW, Missale C, Caron MG (1996) Dopamine receptors and brain function. Neuropharmacology 35: 1503–1519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Davis C, Levitan RD, Kaplan AS, Carter J, Reid C, et al. (2008) Reward sensitivity and the D2 dopamine receptor gene: A case-control study of binge eating disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32: 620–628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Pecina M, Mickey BJ, Love T, Wang H, Langenecker SA, et al.. (2012) DRD2 polymorphisms modulate reward and emotion processing, dopamine neurotransmission and openness to experience. Cortex. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 30. Shahmoradgoli Najafabadi M, Ohadi M, Joghataie MT, Valaie F, Riazalhosseini Y, et al. (2005) Association between the DRD2 A1 allele and opium addiction in the Iranian population. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 134B: 39–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Kukreti R, Tripathi S, Bhatnagar P, Gupta S, Chauhan C, et al. (2006) Association of DRD2 gene variant with schizophrenia. Neurosci Lett 392: 68–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Hirvonen MM, Laakso A, Nagren K, Rinne JO, Pohjalainen T, et al. (2009) C957T polymorphism of dopamine D2 receptor gene affects striatal DRD2 in vivo availability by changing the receptor affinity. Synapse 63: 907–912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Neville MJ, Johnstone EC, Walton RT (2004) Identification and characterization of ANKK1: a novel kinase gene closely linked to DRD2 on chromosome band 11q23.1. Hum Mutat 23: 540–545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Filopanti M, Lania AG, Spada A (2011) Pharmacogenetics of D2 dopamine receptor gene in prolactin-secreting pituitary adenomas. Expert Opin Drug Metab Toxicol 6: 43–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Thompson J, Thomas N, Singleton A, Piggott M, Lloyd S, et al. (1997) D2 dopamine receptor gene (DRD2) Taq1 A polymorphism: reduced dopamine D2 receptor binding in the human striatum associated with the A1 allele. Pharmacogenetics 7: 479–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Gong P, Zhang H, Chi W, Ge W, Zhang K, et al. (2012) An association study on the polymorphisms of dopaminergic genes with working memory in a healthy Chinese Han population. Cell Mol Neurobiol 32: 1011–1019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Rodriguez-Jimenez R, Avila C, Ponce G, Ibanez MI, Rubio G, et al. (2006) The TaqIA polymorphism linked to the DRD2 gene is related to lower attention and less inhibitory control in alcoholic patients. Eur Psychiatry 21: 66–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Paclt I, Drtilkova I, Kopeckova M, Theiner P, Sery O, et al. (2010) The association between TaqI A polymorphism of ANKK1 (DRD2) gene and ADHD in the Czech boys aged between 6 and 13 years. Neuro Endocrinol Lett 31: 131–136. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Perkins KA, Lerman C, Grottenthaler A, Ciccocioppo MM, Milanak M, et al. (2008) Dopamine and opioid gene variants are associated with increased smoking reward and reinforcement owing to negative mood. Behav Pharmacol 19: 641–649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Berman SM, Ozkaragoz T, Noble EP, Antolin T, Sheen C, et al. (2003) Differential associations of sex and D2 dopamine receptor (DRD2) genotype with negative affect and other substance abuse risk markers in children of alcoholics. Alcohol 30: 201–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Zung WW (1965) A Self-Rating Depression Scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry 12: 63–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Zung WW (1967) Factors influencing the self-rating depression scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry 16: 543–547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Zung WW (1971) A rating instrument for anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics 12: 371–379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Russell DW (1996) UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. J Pers Assess 66: 20–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Bai LMH, Huang YX, Luo Y J (2005) The Development of Native Chinese Affective Picture System-A pretest in 46 College Students. CHINESE MENTAL HEALTH JOURNAL 19: 719–722. [Google Scholar]
- 46. Cisler JM, Bacon AK, Williams NL (2009) Phenomenological Characteristics of Attentional Biases Towards Threat: A Critical Review. Cognit Ther Res 33: 221–234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Cisler JM, Koster EH (2010) Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review. Clin Psychol Rev 30: 203–216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Koster EH, Crombez G, Verschuere B, Van Damme S, Wiersema JR (2006) Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety: Facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. Behav Res Ther 44: 1757–1771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Gong P, Xi S, Shen G, Li S, Zhang P, et al. (2013) The effects of DBH, MAOA, and MAOB on attentional biases for facial expressions. J Mol Neurosci 49: 606–613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Gibson BS, Amelio J (2000) Inhibition of return and attentional control settings. Percept Psychophys 62: 496–504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Chao HF (2010) Inhibition of return to negative emotion: evidence from an emotional expression detection task. Emotion 10: 272–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Klein RM (2000) Inhibition of return. Trends Cogn Sci 4: 138–147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Jongen EM, Smulders FT (2007) Sequence effects in a spatial cueing task: endogenous orienting is sensitive to orienting in the preceding trial. Psychol Res 71: 516–523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Forster KI, Forster JC (2003) DMDX: a windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 35: 116–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. de Lamballerie X, Chapel F, Vignoli C, Zandotti C (1994) Improved current methods for amplification of DNA from routinely processed liver tissue by PCR. J Clin Pathol 47: 466–467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A (2007) G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 39: 175–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Rijsdijk FV, Riese H, Tops M, Snieder H, Brouwer WH, et al. (2009) Neuroticism, recall bias and attention bias for valenced probes: a twin study. Psychol Med 39: 45–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Hankin BL, Gibb BE, Abela JR, Flory K (2010) Selective attention to affective stimuli and clinical depression among youths: role of anxiety and specificity of emotion. J Abnorm Psychol 119: 491–501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Waters FA, Badcock JC, Maybery MT (2006) Selective attention for negative information and depression in schizophrenia. Psychol Med 36: 455–464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Bishop SJ (2007) Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: an integrative account. Trends Cogn Sci 11: 307–316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Olsson CA, Anney RJ, Lotfi-Miri M, Byrnes GB, Williamson R, et al. (2005) Association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and propensity to anxiety in an Australian population-based longitudinal study of adolescent health. Psychiatr Genet 15: 109–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Bowirrat A, Oscar-Berman M (2005) Relationship between dopaminergic neurotransmission, alcoholism, and Reward Deficiency syndrome. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 132B: 29–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Posner MI (1980) Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 32: 3–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Veenstra EM, de Jong PJ (2012) Attentional bias in restrictive eating disorders. Stronger attentional avoidance of high-fat food compared to healthy controls? Appetite 58: 133–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Fani N, Gutman D, Tone EB, Almli L, Mercer KB, et al. (2013) FKBP5 and attention bias for threat: associations with hippocampal function and shape and attention bias for threat: associations with hippocampal function and shape. JAMA Psychiatry 70: 392–400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Stollstorff M, Munakata Y, Jensen AP, Guild RM, Smolker HR, et al. (2013) Individual differences in emotion-cognition interactions: emotional valence interacts with serotonin transporter genotype to influence brain systems involved in emotional reactivity and cognitive control. Front Hum Neurosci 7: 327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Gong P, Xi S, Li S, Cao G, Zhang P, et al. (2013) Effect of Val66Met polymorphism in BDNF on attentional bias in an extroverted Chinese Han population. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 73: 280–288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Naudts KH, Azevedo RT, David AS, van Heeringen K, Gibbs AA (2013) Epistasis between 5-HTTLPR and ADRA2B polymorphisms influences attentional bias for emotional information in healthy volunteers. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 15: 1027–1036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Farrell SM, Tunbridge EM, Braeutigam S, Harrison PJ (2012) COMT Val(158)Met genotype determines the direction of cognitive effects produced by catechol-O-methyltransferase inhibition. Biol Psychiatry 71: 538–544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Heinzel S, Dresler T, Baehne CG, Heine M, Boreatti-Hummer A, et al. (2012) COMT × DRD4 epistasis impacts prefrontal cortex function underlying response control. Cereb Cortex 23: 1453–1462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]