Abstract
Schizophrenia is a disabling, chronic psychiatric disorder with a prevalence rate of 0.5–1% in the general population. Symptoms include positive (e.g., delusions, hallucinations), negative (e.g., blunted affect, social withdrawal), as well as cognitive symptoms (e.g., memory and attention problems). Although 75–85% of patients with schizophrenia report cognitive impairments, the underlying neuropharmacological mechanisms are not well understood and currently no effective treatment is available for these impairments. This has led to the Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) initiative, which established seven cognitive domains that are fundamentally impaired in schizophrenia. These domains include verbal learning and memory, visual learning and memory, working memory, attention and vigilance, processing speed, reasoning and problem solving, and social cognition. Recently, a growing number of studies have been conducted trying to identify the underlying neuropharmacological mechanisms of cognitive impairments in schizophrenia patients. Specific cognitive impairments seem to arise from different underlying neuropharmacological mechanisms. However, most review articles describe cognition in general and an overview of the mechanisms involved in these seven separate cognitive domains is currently lacking. Therefore, we reviewed the underlying neuropharmacological mechanisms focusing on the domains as established by the MATRICS initiative which are considered most crucial in schizophrenia.
Keywords: pharmacology, schizophrenia, cognition, MATRICS, neurotransmitters
Introduction
Schizophrenia is a disabling, chronic psychiatric disorder with a prevalence rate of 0.5–1% in the general population. Symptoms include positive and negative symptoms and disorganization. Approximately 75–85% of the schizophrenia patients report cognitive impairments as well (1). Although this aspect of schizophrenia was already described by Kraepelin nearly a century ago, cognitive impairments have long been under-identified as a symptom of schizophrenia and relatively little research has been conducted on this topic. Previous research showed that cognitive functioning is strongly associated with functional outcome in schizophrenia [e.g., skill acquisition in psychosocial rehabilitation treatment, demonstration of ability to solve simulated interpersonal problems, and community functioning (2, 3)]. Cognitive impairments often precede the onset of other symptoms and persist after other psychotic symptoms have been effectively treated (4). Furthermore, severity of cognitive impairments is predictive of poorer medication compliance (5), treatment adherence (6), and increased tendency for relapse in first-episode patients (7). At present, no effective treatment is available as existing (antipsychotic) medication mainly targets positive symptoms and does not improve cognition. Although it is sometimes assumed by clinicians (possibly due to marketing) that atypical antipsychotics are superior to typical antipsychotics, results from recent studies contradict this theory. Presently available guidelines for treatment of schizophrenia, such as the NICE (8), PORT (9), and WFSBP (10) guidelines, indicate that there is no superiority of atypical antipsychotics on positive symptoms with the exception of clozapine, and instead state that use of antipsychotics should be evaluated based on their side effects. Since cognitive dysfunction is associated with functional outcome, development of an effective intervention strategy for these symptoms and corresponding guidelines is essential, as such guidelines are currently still lacking.
Lack of effective treatment strategies has over recent years led to an increase in studies investigating underlying neurobiological mechanisms of cognitive impairments and potential new pharmacological targets to enhance cognition in schizophrenia. Research has mainly focused on the role of neurotransmitters such as dopamine, serotonin, γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA), glutamate, and acetylcholine. Previous research indicated that specific cognitive impairments seem to arise from different underlying neurobiological mechanisms (11). For example, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) has been implicated in the executive functioning aspect of cognition (12) whereas the hippocampus has been linked to, e.g., episodic memory (13). This suggests that specific pharmacological agents could enhance domains of cognition differentially. Nonetheless, still little is known about the underlying neurobiology of cognition. Knowledge about these neurobiological mechanisms is highly necessary for development of new pharmacological intervention strategies.
To improve cognition research in schizophrenia the Measurement and Treatment to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) was developed. The MATRICS identified seven cognitive domains that are fundamentally impaired in schizophrenia: verbal learning and memory, visual learning and memory, working memory, attention and vigilance, processing speed, reasoning and problem solving, and social cognition (14). It was decided that cognition research in schizophrenia should mainly focus on these domains in order to identify the neurobiological mechanisms, ultimately to facilitate development of new pharmacological treatment strategies.
Although specific domains of cognition have been identified, most studies tend to describe cognition in general terms using a composite score. Currently, a review differentiating between separate MATRICS domains is lacking. Therefore, the aim of this review is to provide an outline of the underlying neuropharmacological mechanisms of each individual cognitive domain. We will focus on pharmacological intervention studies which used validated instruments to measure the effect on the MATRICS cognitive domains.
Methods
Search strategy
A literature search was conducted in medical database PubMed. The following keywords were used: “pharmacology,” “schizophrenia,” and “Cognition.” Subsequently, a separate search was conducted for each individual domain combining the keywords “pharmacology” and “schizophrenia” with the following keywords: “verbal learning,” “verbal memory,” “visual learning,” “visual memory,” “working memory,” “attention,” “vigilance,” “processing speed,” “reasoning,” “problem solving,” and “social cognition.”
Inclusion and exclusion criteria
Papers that met the following inclusion criteria were included: (1) original research papers, both single challenge and clinical trials (full text available); (2) published in English; (3) use of a pharmacological intervention (pharmacological interventions used and their main mechanism of action are displayed in Table 1); (4) use of validated cognitive tests to measure one or more of the MATRICS domains; (5) subjects were patients with schizophrenia; and (6) were published between January 2000 and May 2013. Papers were excluded if: (1) only healthy subjects were included; (2) also schizoaffective disorders and other psychotic disorders were included; (3) cognitive domains were measured with non-validated tests; (4) cognitive domains other than the MATRICS domains were measured; and (5) only a composite score of cognition was reported.
Table 1.
General pharmacological domain | Pharmacological agent | Agents main mechanism of action |
---|---|---|
Antipsychotics | Haloperidol | D2 antagonist |
Chlorprothixene | D2 antagonist | |
Perazine | D2 antagonist | |
Flupenthixol | D2 antagonist | |
Amisulpride | D2/D3 antagonist | |
Risperidone | D2 and 5-HT2a antagonist | |
Paliperidone | D2 and 5-HT2a antagonist (active metabolite of risperidone) | |
Olanzapine | D2 and 5-HT2a antagonist | |
Loxapine | D2 and 5-HT2a antagonist | |
Sertindole | D2 and 5-HT2a antagonist | |
Ziprasidone | D2 and 5-HT2a antagonist | |
Quetiapine | D2 and 5-HT2a antagonist partial 5-HT1a agonist | |
Aripiprazole | 5-HT2a antagonist and partial D2 agonist and partial 5-HT1a agonist | |
Perospirone | D2 and 5-HT2a antagonist and 5-HT1a agonist | |
Clozapine | D2, D4, and 5-HT2a antagonist partial 5-HT1a agonist (metabolite shows weak partial agonistic effects on D2 and D3) | |
BL-1020 | D2 and 5-HT2a antagonist GABAA agonist | |
Dopaminergic | Dihydrexidine | D1 agonist |
Serotonergic | Ondansetron | 5-HT3a antagonist |
Tandospirone | 5-HT1a agonist | |
Tropisetron | 5-HT3a antagonist | |
Fluvoxamine | Reuptake inhibition and sigma-1 receptor agonist | |
Citalopram | Reuptake inhibition | |
Cholinergic | AZD3480 | nicotinic α4β2 agonist |
DMXB-A | Partial nicotinic α7 agonist | |
Donepezil | Acetylcholinesterase inhibition | |
Galantamine | Acetylcholinesterase inhibition | |
Rivastigmine | Acetylcholinesterase inhibition | |
Glutamatergic | Minocycline | (Mainly used as antibiotic) blocks nitric oxide induced neurotoxicity |
d-Cycloserine | NMDA partial agonist | |
Lamotrigine | Glutamate release regulation | |
Topiramate | AMPA/kainite antagonist | |
GABAergic | MK-0777 | GABA α2/α3 agonist |
Lorazepam | GABAA agonist | |
Flumazenil | GABAA antagonist | |
Noradrenergic | Atomoxetine | Reuptake inhibition |
Stimulants | d-Amphetamine | Dopamine/norepinephrine releaser thus indirect dopamine agonist |
Modafinil | Unknown | |
Armodafinil | Unknown | |
Other domains | Mifepristone | Glucocorticoid receptor antagonist |
Sildenafil | PDE5 inhibition |
Results
In total, the search strategy yielded 938 articles of which 293 articles were found using the keywords “pharmacology,” “cognition,” and “schizophrenia.” The separate searches yielded 158 articles for verbal learning and memory, 85 for visual learning and memory, 100 for working memory, 234 for attention and vigilance, 22 for processing speed, 17 for reasoning and problem solving, and 29 for social cognition. After final screening 44 articles were included for verbal learning and memory, 26 for visual learning and memory, 43 for working memory, 22 for attention and vigilance, 31 for processing speed, 30 for reasoning and problem solving, and 7 for social cognition. Study characteristics and specifics are shown in Table 2 (studies investigating antipsychotics) and Table 3 (studies investigating non-antipsychotic intervention strategies).
Table 2.
Study | Agent(s) | Sample size (in analyses) | Design | MATRICS domains measured | Main results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Wagner et al. (16) | Olanzapine and amisulpride | N = 36 | 8-week randomized clinical trial | Ver. l and m; wm; attention; ps | No difference between treatments conditions in ver. l and m, wm, attention, and ps. Both the olanzapine and the amisulpride group improved on ver. l and m and wm. Attention and ps did not improve in both treatment conditions |
Tyson et al. (17) | Risperidone, olanzapine, clozapine, quetiapine, and amisulpride | N = 44 | 18-month clinical trial | Vis. l and m; wm; r and ps | Vis. l and m, wm, and r and ps improved in the low affinity group (quetiapine and amisulpride) whereas vis. l and m and r and ps declined in the high-affinity group (risperidone, olanzapine, and clozapine) |
Purdon et al. (20) | Risperidone, olanzapine, and haloperidol | N = 55 | 54-week randomized, double-blind trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; ps; r and ps | After Bonferroni correction, none of the cognitive domains improved in any of the treatment conditions |
Malhotra et al. (21) | Ziprasidone | N = 10 | 12-week open-label clinical trial | Ver. l and m; ps | Significant improvement in ver. l and m. Ps did not improve significantly |
Rémillard et al. (22) | Risperidone and haloperidol | N = 28 | 12-month randomized | Ver. l and m | Both types of antipsychotics did not improve ver. l and m |
Harvey et al. (23) | Quetiapine and risperidone | N = 289 | 8-week, randomized, double-blind clinical trial | Ver. l and m; attention; ps | Both treatment conditions improved on ver. l and m. Although no differences were found between treatment conditions, attention, and ps improved only in the risperidone group |
Purdon et al. (24) | Quetiapine and haloperidol | N = 22 | 6-month, randomized, double-blind clinical trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; ps; r and ps | Ver. l and m, vis. l and m, and r and ps did not improve in both treatment conditions. Ps was found to improve in both treatment conditions but this was not significant after corrections for multiple testing |
Velligan et al. (25) | Quetiapine and haloperidol | N = 58 | 24-week, randomized, double-blind clinical trial | Ver. l and m; ps | After Bonferroni correction, no significant improvement on ver. l and m and ps in all treatment conditions |
Kivircik Akdede et al. (26) | Quetiapine | N = 14 | 8-week clinical trial | Ver. l and m; wm; ps | Improvement was found in wm and ps. Ver. l and m did not improve |
Kim et al. (27) | Risperidone depot | N = 36 | 26-week, open-label trial | Ver. l and m; attention; ps; r and ps | Significant improvement in ver. l and m, attention, and r and ps. Ps did not improve |
Suzuki and Gen (28) | Risperidone and haloperidol depot | N = 20 | 24-week, open-label trial | Ver. l and m; r and ps | Compared to the haloperidol group, ver. l and m and r and ps improved in the risperidone depot group |
Surguladze et al. (30) | Risperidone depot and typical antipsychotics depot (haloperidol, fluphenazine, flupenthixol, zuclopenthixol) | N = 32 patients and 8 healthy controls | 4-month clinical trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m | Although no differences were found between treatment conditions, both groups improved on ver. l and m and vis. l and m |
Kim et al. (31) | Paliperidone and risperidone | N = 58 | 12-week, randomized open-label clinical trial | Ver. l and m; wm; attention; ps | Compared to the risperidone group, ver. l and m improved more in the paliperidone group. No differences between treatment conditions were found in wm, attention, and ps |
Purdon et al. (32) | Clozapine | N = 18 | 6-week clinical trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; ps; r and ps | Significant improvement in ver. l and m, vis. l and m, and ps. No improvement in wm and r and ps |
Ertugrul et al. (33) | Clozapine | N = 20 patients and 20 healthy controls | 8-week clinical trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; r and ps | No significant improvement in ver. l and m, vis. l and m, and r and ps. Performance on one of the wm tasks improved |
Sumiyoshi et al. (34) | Clozapine | N = 27 | 6-week clinical trial | Ver. l and m | No significant improvement in ver. l and m |
Geffen et al. (35) | BL-1020 and risperidone | N = 363 | 6-week, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial | Ver. l and m; wm; ps; r and ps | Compared to placebo, improvement in ver. l and m and wm was found with a high dose of BL-1020. Ps and r and ps did not improve |
Riedel et al. (39) | Aripiprazole | N = 56 | 8-week, open-label clinical trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; ps | Improvement in ver. l and m and ps. Vis. l and m and wm did not improve |
Bervoets et al. (40) | Aripiprazole | N = 238 | 12-week, open-label clinical trial | Ver. l and m | Ver. l and m improved significantly |
Suzuki et al. (41) | Aripiprazole, perospirone, and olanzapine | N = 31 | 8-week, clinical trial | Ver. l and m; r and ps | None of the treatment conditions showed improved on ver. l and m and r and ps. No differences were found between groups |
Yasui-Furukori et al. (42) | Aripiprazole (added to risperidone or olanzapine) | N = 36 | 12-week, randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Ver. l and m; wm; ps; r and ps | None of the cognitive domains measured improved significantly after adjunctive aripiprazole treatment |
Rollnik et al. (69) | Atypical (olanzapine, risperidone, clozapine) and typical antipsychotics (haloperidol, chlorprothixene, perazine, flupenthixol) | N = 20 patients and 14 healthy controls | 12-week, clinical trial | Vis. l and m; wm; ps | No significant differences were found between typical and atypical antipsychotics for all cognitive domains |
McGurk et al. (70) | Risperidone and haloperidol | N = 27 | 4-week, randomized double-blind clinical trial | Vis. l and m; wm | After corrections for anticholinergic co-medication, no differences were found between risperidone and haloperidol on vis. l and m and wm |
Mori et al. (75) | Risperidone, olanzapine, perospirone; quetiapine | N = 77 | 8-week, randomized clinical trial | wm | Wm improved in the risperidone group and declined in the quetiapine group. Wm performance did not change in the olanzapine and perospirone group |
Papageorgiou et al. (76) | Olanzapine and clozapine | N = 16 | 6-week, randomized, double-blind clinical trial | wm | Wm improved in both groups. No significant differences between treatment conditions |
Nielsen et al. (77) | Sertindole (added to clozapine) | N = 41 | 12-week, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial | Wm; r and ps | Adjunctive sertindole did not improve wm and r and ps |
Molina et al. (85) | Olanzapine | N = 17 | 24-week clinical? Trial | Attention | No improvement in attention |
Liu et al. (86) | Risperidone and haloperidol | N = 38 | 12-week, randomized, double-blind clinical trial | Attention | No improvement in attention |
Mizrahi et al. (97) | Clozapine, risperidone, olanzapine, and loxapine | N = 71 | 6-week, clinical trial | sc | Improvement in sc |
Sumiyoshi et al. (100) | Perospirone | N = 5 | 6-month, clinical trial | sc | Improvement in sc |
Behere et al. (101) | Risperidone | N = 25 patients and 30 healthy controls | Mean duration 38 days, clinical trial | sc | Improvement in sc |
Ver. l and m, verbal learning and memory; vis. l and m, visual learning and memory; wm, working memory; ps, processing speed; r and ps, reasoning and problem solving; sc, social cognition.
Table 3.
Study | Agent | Mechanism of action | Sample size (in analyses) | Design | MATRICS domains measured | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
George et al. (48) | Dihydrexidine | D1 agonist | N = 20 | Single-challenge, randomized placebo-controlled cross-over trial | Ver. l and m; wm; attention; ps | No improvement in any of the measured domains |
Pietrzak et al. (72) | d-Amphetamine | Indirect D1 agonist | N = 32 | Single-challenge, double-blind, placebo-controlled cross-over trial | Vis. l and m; ps; r and ps | Improvement in ps and r and ps. No improvement in vis. l and m |
Sumiyoshi et al. (38) | Tandospirone | 5-HT1a agonist | N = 31 | 4-week, clinical trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m | Ver. l and m improved after adjunctive tandospirone treatment. Vis. l and m did not improve |
Akhondzadeh et al. (43) | Ondansetron | 5-HT3a antagonist | N = 30 | 12-week, randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; r and ps | Vis. l and m improved significantly after adjunctive ondansetron treatment. None of the other domains improved |
Levkovitz et al. (44) | Ondansetron | 5-HT3a antagonist | N = 21 | 2-week, double-blind placebo-controlled cross-over trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; ps | Significant improvement in vis. l and m in the ondansetron group. None of the others domains improved significantly |
Zhang et al. (45) | Tropisetron | 5-HT3a antagonist | N = 38 | 10-day, randomized double-blind, placebo-controlled trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm | Ver. l and m improved significantly after adjunctive tropisetron treatment. Other domains did not improve significantly |
Niitsu et al. (71) | Fluvoxamine | σ-1 Agonist | N = 47 | 12-week, randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Vis. l and m; wm; r and ps | None of the cognitive domains measured improved significantly after adjunctive fluvoxamine treatment |
Friedman et al. (47) | Citalopram | Serotonin reuptake inhibition | N = 18 | 24-week, randomized double-blind placebo-controlled cross-over trial | Ver. l and m; wm; attention; ps; r and ps | None of the cognitive domains measured improved significantly after adjunctive citalopram treatment |
Velligan et al. (51) | AZD3480 | Nicotinic α4β2 agonist | N = 440 | 12-week, randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; attention; ps; r and ps | None of the cognitive domains measured improved |
Freedman et al. (52) | DMXB-A | Nicotinic α7 agonist | N = 31 | 4-week, double-blind placebo-controlled cross-over trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; attention; ps; r and ps | No significant improvement in any of the cognitive domains over three treatment arms. After 4 weeks (one treatment arm) attention improved in both DMXB-A conditions compared to placebo |
Tugal et al. (50) | Donepezil | Acetylcholinesterase inhibition | N = 12 | 12-week randomized double-blind placebo-controlled cross-over trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; ps; r and ps | None of the cognitive domains measured improved after adjunctive donepezil treatment |
Akhondzadeh et al. (53) | Donepezil | Acetylcholinesterase inhibition | N = 30 | 12-week randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; r and ps | None of the cognitive domains measured improved after adjunctive donepezil treatment |
Friedman et al. (58) | Donepezil | Acetylcholinesterase inhibition | N = 34 | 12-week randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Ver. l and m; wm; attention; ps; r and ps | None of the cognitive domains measured improved after adjunctive donepezil treatment |
Freudenreich et al. (57) | Donepezil | Acetylcholinesterase inhibition | N = 30 | 8-week double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Ver. l and m; wm; ps | None of the cognitive domains measured improved after adjunctive donepezil treatment |
Fagerlund et al. (55) | Donepezil | Acetylcholinesterase inhibition | N = 11 patients and 11 healthy controls | 12-week randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; ps; r and ps | None of the cognitive domains measured improved after adjunctive donepezil treatment |
Lee et al. (54) | Galantamine | Acetylcholinesterase inhibition | N = 24 | 12-week, randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; ps | Visual recognition improved in the galantamine group. None of the other measures improved significantly |
Sharma et al. (56) | Rivastigmine | Acetylcholinesterase inhibition | N = 21 | 24-week, randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Ver. l and m; wm; attention; ps; r and ps | No significant improvement in any of the measured cognitive domains after adjunctive rivastigmine treatment |
Lenzi et al. (88) | Rivastigmine | Acetylcholinesterase inhibition | N = 10 | 52-week clinical trial | Ver. l and m; wm; attention; ps; r and ps | No significant improvement in any of the cognitive domains after adjunctive rivastigmine treatment |
Duncan et al. (81) | d-Cycloserine | NMDA partial agonist | N = 22 | 4-week, randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Wm; attention | Both domains did not improve significantly after adjunctive d-cycloserine treatment |
Muscatello et al. (92) | Topiramate | AMPA/kainite antagonist | N = 43 | 24-week randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Attention; r and ps | Both domains did not improve significantly after adjunctive topiramate treatment |
Zoccali et al. (93) | Lamotrigine | Glutamate release regulation | N = 51 | 24-week randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Attention; r and ps | After Bonferroni correction, no significant improvement was found in both domains |
Levkovitz et al. (73) | Minocycline | Blockade nitric oxide induced neurotoxicity | N = 21 | 22-week randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Vis. l and m; wm; r and ps | Vis. l and m, wm and r and ps improved after adjunctive minocycline treatment. |
Buchanan et al. (59) | MK-0777 | GABA α2/α3 agonist | N = 53 | 4-week randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; attention; ps; r and ps; sc | After correction for multiple testing, none of the domains improved significantly after adjunctive MK-077 treatment |
Menzies et al. (80) | Lorazepam and attention flumazenil | GABAA agonist and GABAA antagonist | N = 11 patients and 11 healthy controls | Single-challenge randomized double-blind placebo-controlled cross-over trial | wm | Wm performance declined significantly after lorazepam administration whereas no significant differences were found in the flumazenil group |
Friedman et al. (60) | Atomoxetine | Norepinephrine reuptake inhibition | N = 20 | 8-week randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Ver. l and m; wm; ps; r and ps | No significant improvement in any of the cognitive domains after adjunctive atomoxetine treatment. |
Turner et al. (63) | Modafinil | Unknown | N = 20 | Single-challenge, randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled cross-over trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; attention; ps; r and ps | Wm improved in the modafinil condition compared to placebo. Response latency on a r and ps task was significantly lower in the modafinil condition. No differences were found in number of attempts on the same task |
Spence et al. (82) | Modafinil | Unknown | N = 17 | Single-challenge, randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled cross-over trial | wm | No significant difference between the modafinil and placebo condition |
Kane et al. (61) | Armodafinil | unknown | N = 49 | 4-week, randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; attention; ps; r and ps; sc | No significant improvement in any of the cognitive domains after adjunctive armodafinil treatment |
Gallagher et al. (66) | Mifepristone | Glucocorticoid antagonist | N = 19 | 2-week, randomized double-blind placebo-controlled cross-over trial | Ver. l and m; vis. l and m; wm; attention | No significant improvement in any of the cognitive domains after adjunctive mifepristone treatment. |
Goff et al. (67) | Sildenafil | PDE5 inhibition | N = 15 | Single-challenge randomized placebo-controlled cross-over trial | Ver. l and m; wm; attention | No significant improvement in any of the cognitive domains after adjunctive sildenafil treatment |
Ver. l and m, verbal learning and memory; vis. l and m, visual learning and memory; wm, working memory; ps, processing speed; r and ps, reasoning and problem solving; sc, social cognition.
Verbal Learning and Memory
It has been proposed that impairments in verbal learning and memory are one of the most consistent cognitive deficits seen in schizophrenia (15) and is one of the most examined cognitive domains in these patients. A majority of the 44 included studies investigated the effects of typical and atypical antipsychotic medication on verbal learning and memory.
In the past, it was assumed that atypical antipsychotics are superior to typical antipsychotics in enhancing cognition in schizophrenia due to their affinity for both dopamine D2 receptors and serotonin 5-HT2A receptors (16, 17). However large studies such as the Clinical Antipsychotic Trials of Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) (18) and the European First Episode Schizophrenia Trial (EUFEST) (19) did not find differences between typical and atypical antipsychotics in cognitive enhancing effects. This theory was tested by Wagner et al. (16) who compared olanzapine, a dopamine D2, and serotonin 5-HT2A antagonist, to amisulpride, a dopamine D2/D3 antagonist in 52 patients. No significant differences were found after 8 weeks between the two groups in terms verbal memory; both groups improved on verbal learning and memory tasks. Olanzapine was compared to risperidone and haloperidol by Purdon et al. (20). Although not significant after corrections for multiple comparisons, olanzapine was found to be superior to risperidone and haloperidol in enhancing verbal learning and memory.
The atypical antipsychotic ziprasidone was found to improve verbal learning and memory after 12 weeks of monotherapy (21). However, the study did not include a control group and practice effects were not adequately controlled for. Therefore the results have to be interpreted with caution. A study comparing the effects of risperidone and haloperidol on verbal memory found that performance on a verbal memory task remained essentially unchanged in both groups (22). Since the authors did not include a non-medicated schizophrenia group but included a healthy group as control this mainly provides evidence that there is no difference between risperidone and haloperidol. Nevertheless it suggests that both medications did not cause a clinically relevant enhancement of verbal memory. However, Harvey et al. (23) compared the effects of risperidone and quetiapine and found improvement in verbal memory in both treatment groups. The dropout rate of 57% in this trial was high which may have caused a selection bias. These results are not consistent with other findings. A small study with substantial methodological shortcomings by Purdon et al. (24) found no effect of haloperidol and quetiapine. Velligan et al. (25) also compared quetiapine to haloperidol and found that patients in the quetiapine group improved to a greater extent than the patients in the haloperidol group. However, these findings were not significant after correction for multiple comparisons. Moreover, Kivircik Akdede et al. (26) did not find improvement on a verbal learning and memory task after 8 weeks of quetiapine treatment.
Kim et al. (27) found that switching from oral atypical antipsychotics to depot risperidone led to significant improvement in verbal learning and memory. However, they also did not include a control group and did not correct for possible practice effects. Furthermore a high percentage of the patients dropped out in an early stage of the study which could have caused a bias in the sample. Despite these limitations, their results are supported by Suzuki and Gen (28) who also found an improvement in verbal learning and memory in patients treated with depot risperidone compared to patients who received haloperidol treatment (depot). However, the differences between these two treatments might be caused by the high rate of anticholinergic co-medication in the haloperidol group which is well known for its adverse effects on cognition (29). Additionally, improvement in verbal learning and memory was found by Surguladze et al. (30) with both depot risperidone and typical antipsychotics (depot). Performance did not significantly differ between the groups. However, they did not control for practice effects. Therefore improvement cannot reliably be attributed to medication effects. In addition, Kim et al. (31) found that compared to patients using risperidone, verbal memory improved more in patients using paliperidone extend release (ER), an active metabolite of risperidone with an extended delivery system that decreases fluctuations in serum drug concentrations.
Hence, the theory that atypical antipsychotic medications are superior to typical antipsychotics in enhancing verbal learning and memory is not supported by these findings. Nonetheless, some of the atypical antipsychotics, especially risperidone depot, have shown beneficial effects on this aspect of cognition.
Results regarding the effects of clozapine on verbal learning and memory are inconclusive. Three studies which investigated the effects of clozapine on verbal learning and memory were found. Purdon et al. (32) reported significant improvement in verbal learning and memory after treatment with clozapine. Their results were not replicated by Ertugrul et al. (33) and Sumiyoshi et al. (34) as both of these studies did not find improvement in verbal memory in clozapine-treated patients. This discrepancy in findings could be due to methodological differences such as different test batteries and trial duration. Moreover, the study by Purdon et al. (32) did not correct for possible practice effects and since they did not include a control group, improvement in verbal memory cannot be reliably attributed to the effects of clozapine. Thus, although results are inconclusive, at present no convincing evidence is available for the effectiveness of clozapine in enhancing verbal learning and memory in schizophrenia.
Geffen et al. (35) compared the effects of 10 and 20–30 mg of BL-1020, a new antipsychotic drug, to risperidone and placebo. BL-1020 is an antagonist for D2 en 5-HT2a receptors and agonist for GABAA receptors (35). Post-mortem studies have found altered GABAergic transmission in schizophrenia, predominantly in the PFC (36, 37). Treatment with 20–30 mg BL-1020 was found to improve performance on a verbal learning memory task compared to placebo, whereas 10 mg of BL-1020 and risperidone did not have enhancing effects compared to placebo. A substantial number of patients, especially in the risperidone group, received anticholinergics as well, which could have influenced the results.
Although the findings described above do not support the theory that blocking the 5-HT2A receptors improves verbal learning and memory, a role for serotonin in this domain of cognition cannot be ruled out. Sumiyoshi et al. (38) found a significant improvement on verbal memory after adjunctive tandospirone (a 5-HT1a agonist) treatment compared to patients who did not receive adjunctive tandospirone. Riedel et al. (39) and Bervoets et al. (40) both found a significant improvement in verbal memory in patients treated with aripiprazole, an atypical antipsychotic drug with partial D2 agonistic and antagonistic and 5-HT2a antagonistic properties in addition to partial agonistic activity at the 5-HT1a receptors. However, this is contradicted by Suzuki et al. (41) who compared the effects of aripiprazole, olanzapine, and perospirone on verbal memory. None of the three conditions showed significant changes in verbal memory scores and no differences were found between the three treatment conditions. Moreover, aripiprazole added to atypical antipsychotics did not improve verbal learning and memory in a trial by Yasui-Furukori et al. (42). This discrepancy in findings could be due to practice effects in the studies of Riedel et al. (39) and Bervoets et al. (40), since both studies did not include a control group and did not correct for possible practice effects. Furthermore, different tasks were used which makes it difficult to directly compare the results.
The role of the serotonin 5-HT3a receptor in verbal learning and memory has been examined as well. Akhondzadeh et al. (43) and Levkovitz et al. (44) investigated the effects of ondansetron, a serotonin 5-HT3a receptor antagonist, added to respectively risperidone and clozapine treatment, and found no enhancing effects on verbal learning and memory. However, the sample size of both studies was small. In contrast to these results, Zhang et al. (45) did find improvement in verbal memory after add-on tropisetron (a 5-HT3a receptor antagonist and nicotinic α7 agonist) to risperidone treatment compared to placebo. This discrepancy in findings could be explained by the fact that the two studies used different pharmacological interventions. Both Akhondzadeh et al. (43) and Levkovitz et al. (44) administered ondansetron whereas Zhang et al. (45) used tropisetron. Since ondansetron has been found to have low affinity for the nicotinic α7 receptors (46), the positive effects of tropisetron on verbal learning and memory could be due to its nicotinic α7 agonistic properties rather than its antagonistic effects on the 5-HT3a receptors.
Effects of a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) were examined by Friedman et al. (47) who added citalopram to treatment with atypical antipsychotics but found no improvement in verbal learning and memory.
Thus, although results are inconclusive there is preliminary evidence of positive effects of 5-HT1a agonists, but not 5-HT3a antagonists on verbal learning and memory in schizophrenia.
One study examined the effects of a dopamine D1 agonist on verbal learning and memory in schizophrenia. George et al. (48) administered dihydrexidine as a pharmacological challenge and found no effects of dihydrexidine on verbal learning and memory.
The role of the neurotransmitter acetylcholine in cognition has been widely investigated and established (36). Post-mortem, a correlation was found between cognitive impairment and decreased levels of brain choline acetyltransferase in schizophrenia (49). Post-mortem studies have shown changes in both muscarinic and nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in patients with schizophrenia (50). Velligan et al. (51) conducted a trial to assess the effects adjunctive therapy with the selective nicotinic α4β2 receptor agonist AZD3480 on verbal learning and memory in patients but found no improvement. An important limitation of this study is that only patients who smoked were included. Therefore, the lack of effect could be due to desensitization of the nicotinic receptors caused by chronic tobacco use. However, Freedman et al. (52) also did not find an improvement in verbal learning and memory after administration of the partial nicotinic α7 agonist DMXB-A, added to antipsychotic treatment in patients, who abstained from nicotine at least 1 month prior to participation. Studies using acetylcholinesterase (enzyme that breaks down acetylcholine, thereby increasing acetylcholine levels) inhibitors (AChE-Is) as adjunctive therapy to antipsychotic medication also found no improvement on verbal learning and memory (53–58). These results suggest that acetylcholinesterase inhibitors do not effectively enhance verbal memory in patients with schizophrenia. However, all studies used a small sample size and one study included patients who were taking anticholinergic medication as well. This could have influenced the outcome of the study. Another important limitation is that none of these studies controlled for the effects of smoking. Hence, although there is theoretical evidence for a role of acetylcholine in cognition, various (possibly underpowered) intervention studies have not yielded positive results. Since Zhang et al. (45) found improvement in verbal learning and memory after adjunctive tropisetron treatment, it remains possible that enhancement of verbal learning and memory can be achieved with nicotinic α7 agonists while simultaneously blocking the 5-HT3a receptors.
The role of the neurotransmitters GABA and norepinephrine in verbal learning in memory has also been investigated. Buchanan et al. (59) found no improvement in verbal learning and memory with adjunctive MK-0777, a partial GABA α2/α3 agonist, therapy to antipsychotics. However, the authors argue that a lack of effect may be due to the fact that MK-0777 is a weak GABA α2/α3 agonist. A pilot study by Friedman et al. (60) found no effects of the norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor atomoxetine, on verbal memory when added to treatment with atypical antipsychotics.
Hence, although positive effects were found with the antipsychotic BL-1020 with GABAA agonistic properties, the available studies were not able to detect positive effects of both a partial GABA α2/α3 agonist and a norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor on verbal learning and memory. Nonetheless, these results suggest that GABAA receptors may be a potential target for future studies in verbal learning and memory.
Effects of the psychostimulant armodafinil were investigated by Kane et al. (61). Armodafinil is a longer-lasting isomer of modafinil, which is an alertness-promoting medication with mechanisms of action different from those of amphetamine (62) and has been found to improve cognition in healthy subjects and adults with ADHD (63). However, the exact mechanisms of action are complex and not entirely understood (64). Armodafinil, added to atypical antipsychotics, did not enhance verbal learning and memory.
The effects of several other pharmacological interventions on verbal learning and memory in schizophrenia have been investigated. Six months of daily creatine administration added to antipsychotic treatment did not have beneficial effects on verbal learning and memory (65). Effects of adjunctive mifepristone, a glucocorticoid receptor (GR) antagonist, on verbal learning and memory was examined by Gallagher et al. (66). They found no improvement in this domain of cognition. Goff et al. (67) examined the effects of sildenafil, a phosphodiesterase 5 (PDE5) inhibitor used to treat erectile dysfunction, on verbal learning and memory. PDE5 inhibitors increase cyclic guanine monophosphate (cGMP) which is thought to modulate long-term potentiation. They found single dose of 50 and 100 mg not to improve verbal learning and memory. On the other hand, Feifel et al. (15) found that patients who received intranasal oxytocin for 3 weeks performed better on a verbal learning and memory task. Additionally, Stone et al. (68) found that additional glucose administration in patients who were stable on clozapine, enhanced verbal learning and memory. All studies used a small sample and the dropout rate in the study by Feifel et al. (15) was 25%. Moreover, the majority of the patients in the study of Stone et al. (68) received additional medication besides clozapine. Therefore the effect cannot be attributed to a unique interaction of clozapine and glucose.
To summarize, the proposed superior effect on verbal learning and memory of atypical to typical antipsychotics, is not supported by the available data and any effect does not result from blocking the 5-HT2a receptors. Despite the fact that multiple studies describe a role of acetylcholine in cognition, nicotinic receptor agonist and acetylcholinesterase inhibitors do not seem to effectively improve verbal learning and memory in schizophrenia. However, some positive results have been found with risperidone depot, possibly because of medication compliance. In addition, serotonin 5-HT1a receptors and GABAA receptors may be molecular targets for enhancing this aspect of cognition. Additionally, there is preliminary evidence that adjunctive oxytocin and glucose treatment may be beneficial in enhancing verbal learning and memory. However, results have to be interpreted with care as the available studies are mostly pilot studies which, although important in early stages of research, inherently suffer from methodological shortcomings.
Visual Learning and Memory
Compared to verbal learning and memory, considerably less research has been conducted regarding visual learning and memory in schizophrenia. Results of studies that examined the effects of antipsychotic medication on visual learning and memory are, similar to verbal learning and memory, indecisive. Tyson et al. (17) compared the effects of atypical antipsychotic medication with high and low affinity for the 5-HT2a receptors on visual learning and memory. Patients treated with risperidone, olanzapine, and clozapine were assigned to the high-affinity group and patients using quetiapine and amisulpride were assigned to the low affinity group. Performance on visual memory tasks improved in patients in the low affinity group whereas performance in the high-affinity group declined. A limitation is that the authors did not correct for multiple testing. However, the effects were already present after 9 months of treatment and show a consistent pattern. Since performance declined in the high-affinity group, it is not likely that the improvement in the low affinity group is driven by practice effects. However, Rollnik et al. (69) found atypical antipsychotics (olanzapine, risperidone, or clozapine) to be superior to typical antipsychotics (haloperidol, chlorprothixene, perazine, or flupenthixol) in improving visual learning and after 3 weeks of treatment, although at the final assessment, this difference was no longer significant. Purdon et al. (20) compared the effects of olanzapine, risperidone, and haloperidol. After correction for multiple comparisons, no significant differences were found between the three treatment conditions; visual learning and memory did not improve in all three groups. As mentioned earlier, lack of effect could be caused by concomitant anticholinergic treatment and the small sample size.
Two studies compared the effects of haloperidol to respectively quetiapine (24) and risperidone (70). In both studies haloperidol and both quetiapine and risperidone were found not to enhance visual learning and memory. However, a substantial number of patients in both studies were treated with anticholinergics in addition to antipsychotics, which could have influenced the results. Surguladze et al. (30) found no differences in visual learning and memory between depot risperidone and a typical antipsychotic depot treatment, although performance in both groups improved. As discussed earlier, it cannot be ruled out that this improvement is caused by practice effects rather than the effects of medication.
Although results are inconclusive, clozapine does not seem to effectively enhance visual learning and memory. A study by Ertugrul et al. (33) did not find improvement with clozapine whereas Purdon et al. (32) found significant improvement on performance on one of three assessed visual memory subtests. However, this study has important limitations, which were described earlier.
Treatment with aripiprazole did not significantly improve visual learning and memory in the study by Riedel et al. (39). Tandospirone, which like aripiprazole has serotonin 5-HT1a agonistic properties, also did not significantly enhance visual learning and memory in the study of Sumiyoshi et al. (38). These findings suggest that stimulating the serotonin 5-HT1a receptors does not enhance visual learning and memory in schizophrenia. However, the serotonin 5-HT3a receptors may play a role in visual memory in schizophrenia. Both Akhondzadeh et al. (43) and Levkovitz et al. (44) found visual learning and memory to improve with adjunctive ondansetron treatment. Although both studies did not correct for multiple testing and possible practice effects, visual memory was the only aspect of the cognitive domains measured that improved in both studies. Adjunctive tropisetron treatment on the other hand did not enhance visual memory (45). As mentioned earlier, tropisetron is both a 5-HT3a antagonist and partial nicotinic α7 agonist whereas ondansetron has low affinity for the nicotinic α7 receptors (46). Hence, the 5-HT3a receptors may play a role in visual learning and memory. However, enhancement of this aspect of cognition may depend on blockade of nicotinic α7 receptors. The 5-HT1a receptor does not seem to be a promising target for enhancing visual learning and memory.
A preliminary study by Niitsu et al. (71) investigated the effect of the SSRI fluvoxamine, a sigma-1 receptor agonist, on cognition and found it not to enhance visual learning and memory. These preliminary results indicate sigma-1 receptor agonism does not enhance visual learning and memory in schizophrenia patients.
Studies investigating the role acetylcholine in visual learning and memory in schizophrenia have not yielded positive results. Selective nicotinic α4β2 receptor agonist AZD3480 and partial nicotinic α7 agonist DMXB-A both did not enhance visual learning and memory when added to antipsychotic treatment (51, 52). As mentioned earlier, the lack of effect in the study of Velligan et al. (51) could be due to desensitization of the nicotinic receptors caused by chronic tobacco use since only smokers were included. Studies conducted with acetylcholinesterase inhibitors do not report positive results either. Although Lee et al. (54) found improvement on the recognition subtest of Rey Complex Figure Test (RCTF) with galantamine, the other aspects of the task did not improve. Studies using donepezil also did not find improvement in visual learning and memory (50, 53, 55). Hence, both nicotinic receptor agonists and acetylcholinesterase inhibitors do not seem to effectively enhance visual learning and memory in schizophrenia.
Based on preliminary results of Buchanan et al. (59), GABA does not seem to enhance visual learning and memory in schizophrenia as they found no improvement with partial GABAA α2/α3 agonist MK-0777. However, this is the only study investigating the role of GABA in visual learning and memory and MK-0777 is a weak GABA α2/α3 agonist.
The possible enhancing effect of psychostimulant drugs on visual learning and memory has been investigated in schizophrenia as well. A single dose of d-amphetamine, an indirect dopamine D1 agonist, did not improve visual learning and memory (72). A single administration of modafinil did not significantly enhance visual memory either (63). In addition, adjunctive armodafinil treatment did not improve visual learning and memory (61). Hence, based on these findings d-amphetamine and modafinil do not seem to be promising intervention strategies for enhancing visual learning and memory.
Results of other pharmacological interventions on visual learning and memory are mixed. Gallagher et al. (66) found no improvement after adjunctive mifepristone (a GR antagonist) treatment. The effect of adjunctive creatine treatment was investigated by Kaptsan et al. (65) but they found it not to enhance visual learning and memory. Levkovitz et al. (73) investigated the effects of minocycline, a second generation tetracycline with anti-inflammatory and antimicrobial effects. Minocycline modulates the glutamate pathway by blocking nitric oxide induced neurotoxicity. Hyperactivity of glutamatergic neurotransmission [possibly caused by hypofunction of N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptors] has been found in schizophrenia (36). Stimulated by glutamate, the NMDA receptors activate production of nitric oxide. The authors found improvement on a spatial recognition memory task after minocycline administration, added to antipsychotic treatment. Task performance showed a decrease after 10 weeks, but compared to baseline, performance improved at the final assessment. The drop-out rate in this study was high (due to adverse events and non-adherence) and although it did not differ between the two treatment groups, this could have biased the results.
To summarize, results regarding visual learning and memory are inconclusive. At present, there is some evidence for a role of the serotonin 5-HT3a receptors in this cognitive domain. 5-HT1a and sigma-1 receptor agonist on the other hand, did not yield promising results. Additionally, minocycline was found to have positive effects on visual learning memory when added to antipsychotic treatment. Both typical and atypical antipsychotics do not seem to enhance visual learning and memory. Studies investigating nicotinic receptor agonist, acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, GABA α2/α3 agonist, psychostimulants, and GR antagonist did not yield positive results but are in need of replication as they were possibly underpowered. Overall, results need be interpreted with care as the described studies are often small pilot studies with inherent limitations.
Working Memory
Working memory refers to a system with limited capacity for temporary storage and manipulation of information, necessary for cognitive tasks (74), and has been the subject of many studies investigating cognition in schizophrenia. Most of these studies focused on the effects of antipsychotic medication on working memory.
Although results are inconclusive at present, both typical and atypical antipsychotics show little beneficial effects on working memory. Rollnik et al. (69) compared the effectiveness of typical and atypical antipsychotics and found no differences between the two groups. Quetiapine and amisulpride, which both have low affinity for the 5-HT2a receptors, improved performance on a verbal working memory task compared to risperidone, olanzapine, and clozapine (high affinity for 5-HT2a) treatment (17). However, performance on a visual working memory tasks did not significantly improve in both groups. No differences were found in working memory performance between amisulpride and olanzapine by Wagner et al. (16). However, working memory performance did improve in both groups. Effects of olanzapine, risperidone, and haloperidol were compared by Purdon et al. (20). After correction for multiple comparisons, no significant differences were found between the three treatment conditions and working memory did not improve in any group. Mori et al. (75) examined the effects of switching from typical antipsychotics to olanzapine, quetiapine, risperidone, or perospirone on working memory and found improvement in working memory with risperidone, while working memory performance of the patients treated with quetiapine decreased. Working memory performance did not change in the olanzapine and perospirone group. It must be noted that the mean age in the sample was high (59.9 years) and correlated negatively with working memory scores. Therefore age related cognitive decline may have confounded these results. On the contrary, Kivircik Akdede et al. (26) found improvement on working memory with quetiapine. However, this study did not include a control group and did not correct for possible practice effects. Risperidone treatment was found to be superior to haloperidol in the study by McGurk et al. (70), but after correcting for anticholinergic co-medication, this effect was no longer significant. Working memory performance did not improve in both groups. Paliperidone treatment was not superior to risperidone treatment in enhancing working memory in the study by Kim et al. (31) and working memory did not improve in both groups.
Results regarding clozapine are inconclusive. Papageorgiou et al. (76) compared the effects of clozapine and olanzapine on working memory and found that working memory improved in both groups. No differences were found between the two treatment conditions. Ertugrul et al. (33) conducted two working memory tasks (Digit Span Test and the Auditory Consonant Trigram Test) and found that performance on the Digit Span test improved with clozapine treatment, whereas performance on the Auditory Consonant Trigram Test did not. Both studies did not correct for possible practice effects or multiple testing. These results are contradicted by Purdon et al. (32) who did not find improvement in working memory with clozapine treatment. This discrepancy in findings could be due to methodological shortcomings and differences such as different test batteries and trial duration.
Riedel et al. found that aripiprazole did not significantly enhance working memory (39). Aripiprazole also did not improve working memory when added to atypical antipsychotics (42). Adjunctive sertindole, an atypical antipsychotic with high affinity for the D2 and 5-HT2a receptors, did not improve working memory in patients treated with clozapine (77). Finally, working memory did improve after 20–30 mg of the novel antipsychotic BL-1020 (a dopamine antagonist with GABA agonistic properties) compared to placebo whereas 10 mg BL-1020 and risperidone did not (35).
Hence, although results are preliminary and inconclusive, at present no convincing evidence is available for the effectiveness of both typical and atypical antipsychotics on working memory in schizophrenia. However, the antipsychotic BL-1020 was found to improve working memory, possibly through its GABAA agonistic properties.
Single administration of the dopamine D1 agonist dihydrexidine did not enhance working memory in a trial by George et al. (48). Given the small sample size and the fact that dihydrexidine was only administered a single time, beneficial effects of long term dihydrexidine administration on working memory cannot be ruled out.
Serotonin 5-HT3a receptors seem to be less important for working memory functioning. Both Akhondzadeh et al. (43) and Levkovitz et al. (44) found no improvement in working memory with adjunctive ondansetron (5-HT3a antagonist) treatment. Moreover, adjunctive tropisetron treatment also did not enhance working memory (45).
In addition, Niitsu et al. (71) found no improvement in working memory after adjunctive fluvoxamine treatment. Moreover, 12 weeks of adjunctive citalopram treatment did not enhance working memory (47). Hence, these results suggest that serotonin modulation pharmacological agents do not significantly enhance working memory in schizophrenia.
Both the selective nicotinic α4β2 receptor agonist AZD3480 and the partial nicotinic α7 agonist DMXB-A did not enhance working memory performance (51, 52). Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors donepezil and rivastigmine did not enhance working memory either (50, 53–55, 57, 58, 77). Jacobsen et al. (78) examined the effects of a single administration of nicotine [nicotinic acetylcholine receptor agonist (79)] on working memory in tobacco using patients and tobacco using healthy subjects. Although patients performed significantly worse on the N-back task compared to healthy controls, nicotine administration did improve performance on the two-back in patients while performance of the healthy controls declined. Thus, although the acetylcholinesterase inhibitors donepezil and rivastigmine and nicotinic receptor agonists do not show positive effects on working memory in schizophrenia, positive results were found with a single dose of nicotine administration. Therefore, a role of acetylcholine and nicotinic receptors in enhancing working memory cannot be ruled out.
Working memory did not improve in a study by Buchanan et al. (59) after 4 weeks of treatment with partial GABA α2/α3 agonist MK-0777. Menzies et al. (80) studied the effects of lorazepam, a GABAA receptor agonist, and flumazenil, a GABAA receptor antagonist, on working memory in patients and healthy subjects. Working memory performance declined after a single dose of lorazepam whereas performance did not change significantly after flumazenil. Thus, although results are inconclusive, there is some evidence that in GABAA agonists may lower working memory. However positive effects on working memory were found with an antipsychotic with additional GABAA agonistic properties.
Friedman et al. (60) found no effects of atomoxetine (norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor) on working memory in patients. A lack of effect could be due to insufficient power.
Glutamatergic pathways have been examined by Duncan et al. (81). They added 50 mg of d-cycloserine, an antituberculous drug which, at low doses has agonistic properties at the NDMA receptors, to typical antipsychotics. After 4 weeks of treatment, working memory did not improve. However, these results need to be replicated before the role of NMDA receptors in working memory can be established.
Armodafinil, a longer-lasting isomer of modafinil, did not enhance performance on working memory task when added to atypical antipsychotics for 4 weeks (61). However, Turner et al. (63) found that a single dose of modafinil improved performance on a verbal working memory task compared to placebo. Performance on a spatial working memory task did not differ between modafinil condition and placebo condition. These results were not replicated by Spence et al. (82) who did not find differences in performance on a (verbal) working memory task after a single administration of modafinil compared to placebo. This discrepancy in findings could be due to differences in modafinil dose (200 and 100 mg, respectively). Moreover, both studies used different tasks which make direct comparison of results difficult. Nonetheless, a single dose of modafinil might enhance working memory in schizophrenia patients, although recurrent treatment with a longer-lasting variant showed no effect.
Add-on intranasal oxytocin treatment did not improve working memory (15). Furthermore, adjunctive mifepristone (GR antagonist) treatment did not enhance working memory (66). Goff et al. (67) did not find improvement in working memory after both 50 and 100 mg sildenafil administration. Furthermore, daily creatine administration added to antipsychotic treatment did not have beneficial effects on working memory either (65). Add-on minocycline treatment on the other hand, improved working memory in a study by Levkovitz et al. (73); the number of errors on a working memory task decreased significantly in the minocycline group whereas the number of errors in the placebo group increased. However, this study has some limitations which are described earlier. Thus, apart from minocycline, other lines of research did not identify possible new intervention strategies for working memory enhancement in schizophrenia.
To summarize, even though some positive results were found with minocycline, nicotine, and modafinil, the presently available studies did not identify promising molecular targets for enhancement of working memory in schizophrenia. GABA agonists have shown mixed results. Most of the studies examined the effects of antipsychotic treatment on working memory and only a limited number of studies used other pharmacological interventions, which makes it difficult to draw definite conclusions. More research needs to be conducted, especially on the potential role of GABA, norepinephrine, acetylcholine, glutamate, and psychostimulants.
Attention and Vigilance
The construct of attention refers to a core cognitive function that relates to the ability to select, filter, focus, and process different stimuli in the environment. Attention is closely related to working memory and executive functioning (83) and therefore, it is difficult to distinguish between these constructs. Because of the broad definition, impairment in virtually any task can be considered as impairment in attention (83). Therefore, in this review, we only included articles which used the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) to specifically measure attention as this test is recommended by the MATRICS (84).
Studies using antipsychotics to enhance attention report little beneficial effects. The theory that atypical antipsychotics are superior to typical antipsychotics in enhancing cognition was tested for attention as well. A trial comparing olanzapine to amisulpride did not find differences in effects on attention; attention did not significantly improve in both groups (16). Olanzapine did not enhance attention in a study by Molina et al. (85), who examined the effects of switching from typical antipsychotics or risperidone to olanzapine. Attention did not improve with olanzapine treatment.
The effects of risperidone on attention were compared to the effects of respectively haloperidol (86) and quetiapine (23). In the study of Liu et al. (86) no improvement was found in both the haloperidol and the risperidone group. Additionally, although no differences were found between the risperidone and quetiapine in the study by Harvey et al. (23), within group analyses showed significant improvement in attention in the risperidone group, whereas the quetiapine group did not improve. Kim et al. (27) reported improvement with depot risperidone. However, paliperidone did not improve attention (31). To summarize, both typical and atypical antipsychotics have shown little beneficial effects on attention in schizophrenia patients.
The possible role of dopamine and serotonin in attention has also been examined with intervention strategies other than antipsychotic treatment. George et al. (48) examined the effects of a full dopamine D1 agonist, dihydrexidine but found it not to enhance attention.
Studies using serotonin intervention strategies have not yielded positive results either. Golightly et al. (87) examined the role of serotonin in attention by using acute tryptophan, a precursor for serotonin, depletion. Depletion had no effect on performance on the CPT. Niitsu et al. (71) investigated the effect of adjunctive fluvoxamine treatment on attention and found no improvement in attention either. The effects of citalopram, added to treatment with atypical antipsychotics was examined by Friedman et al. (47). They found that adjunctive citalopram treatment did not enhance attention. Thus, although serotonin has been implicated in both verbal and visual memory, it does not seem to play a prominent role in attention.
Based on results of mostly preliminary studies, acetylcholine does not seem to play an important role in attention. Selective nicotinic α4β2 receptor agonist AZD3480, did not enhance attention in a study by Velligan et al. (51). Nonetheless, Freedman et al. (52) found some beneficial effects of partial nicotinic α7 agonist DMXB-A on attention. In their cross-over study, patients were treated with 75, 150 mg DMXB-A, and placebo. All treatment arms lasted 4 weeks, followed by a 1-week washout period. Over the course of the trial, attention improved in all treatment groups. The authors suggested that possible practice effects had obscured the potential effect of treatment, and therefore also examined the scores after the first 4 weeks of treatment. They found that attention improved with both doses of DMXB-A compared to the placebo. Two types of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, rivastigmine, and donepezil, did not enhance attention in schizophrenia (56, 58, 88). On the other hand, a single nicotine administration improved attention in schizophrenia patients but not in healthy controls (89). Thus, despite negative results with acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, some promising results have been obtained with nicotine and a partial nicotinic α7 agonist. These preliminary results suggest that the nicotinic receptors are a potential target for enhancement of attention in schizophrenia.
Partial GABA α2/α3 agonist MK-0777 did not enhance attention when added to antipsychotic treatment (59). However, as mentioned earlier, MK-0777 is a weak GABA α2/α3 agonist and this is the only study investigating the potential role of GABA in attention in schizophrenia. Therefore, these results need to be replicated using specific and more potent GABA modulatory agents.
Other lines of research have not yielded positive results. The psychostimulant drug armodafinil did not enhance attention (61). Furthermore, adjunctive mifepristone (GR antagonist) therapy did not improve attention (66). Duncan et al. (81) found no improvement in attention with adjunctive d-cycloserine treatment. Also, sildenafil (PDE5 inhibitor) did not enhance attention in schizophrenia in a trial by Goff et al. (67). Stone et al. (68) compared the effects of single administration of glucose to placebo in clozapine-treated patients and found attention to be significantly worse in the glucose condition. Thus, several other lines of research did not identify potential new intervention strategies enhancement of attention in schizophrenia.
To summarize, at present only a few pharmacological intervention strategies have been effective in enhancing attention in schizophrenia. Although results are not consistent, positive results have been found with both oral and depot risperidone treatment. Furthermore, a partial nicotinic α7 agonist, and a single administration of nicotine did improve attention in schizophrenia, suggesting a role for acetylcholine and nicotinic receptors in attention. Overall, results need to be considered preliminary and more research needs to be conducted to replicate these findings as these studies have some important limitations.
Processing Speed
Processing speed is considered a core cognitive function and refers to the speed at which the brain processes information. It can be measured as the number of correct responses during a task within a given amount of time (90).
As for other domains of cognition, a majority of the studies examined the effects of antipsychotic medication on processing speed. The theory that atypical antipsychotics are superior to typical antipsychotics in improving cognition in patients was tested for processing speed as well. A study comparing amisulpride with olanzapine found that processing speed did not improve in both groups (16). Moreover, performance on a processing speed task did not differ between patients treated with different typical antipsychotics and atypical antipsychotics and performance did not improve in both groups (69). Purdon et al. (20) compared the effects of olanzapine, risperidone, and haloperidol on processing speed. After correction for multiple comparisons, no significant differences were found between the three treatment conditions and processing speed did not improve in any of the groups. Thus, atypical antipsychotics do not appear to be superior to typical antipsychotics. Furthermore, no beneficial effects of the atypical antipsychotic ziprasidone were found on processing speed (21). Anticholinergics were allowed during the trial which might have influenced the results. Risperidone depot treatment did not enhance processing speed in the study by Kim et al. (27). Moreover, paliperidone ER was not found to be superior to risperidone (31). Unfortunately, within group comparisons were not reported. However, although no significant differences were found between two treatment groups, risperidone was found to improve processing speed whereas quetiapine did not in a study comparing these two antipsychotics (23). Contrary to those results, quetiapine was found to enhance processing speed in a trial by Kivircik Akdede et al. (26). As described earlier, this study did not include a control group and did not correct for possible practice effects. These results were not replicated by Purdon et al. (24). Although processing speed improved in both the quetiapine and haloperidol group, these results were not significant after correcting for multiple comparisons. Velligan et al. (25) compared the effectiveness on processing speed between quetiapine and haloperidol treatment. They found no differences between the two treatment groups. Purdon et al. (32) found that processing speed improved with clozapine treatment. However, this improvement cannot reliably be attributed to the effects of clozapine as the study did not include a control group en they did not correct for possible practice effects. Beneficial effects of aripiprazole on processing speed were found by Riedel et al. (39). Yasui-Furukori et al. (42) found no improvement in processing speed with adjunctive aripiprazole treatment to olanzapine or risperidone. Furthermore, a new antipsychotic with GABAA receptor agonistic properties in addition to dopamine D2 and serotonin 5-HT2a receptor blockade (BL-1020) did not improve processing speed (35).
To summarize, both typical and atypical antipsychotics do not seem to effectively enhance processing speed in schizophrenia patients. Although some studies reported positive effects of clozapine, quetiapine, and risperidone, these results should be interpreted with care as these studies have important limitations.
Studies examining the role of serotonin in processing speed did not yield positive results. Adjunctive ondansetron (5-HT3a receptor antagonist) therapy to antipsychotic medication did not enhance processing speed in a study by Levkovitz et al. (44). Adding citalopram to atypical antipsychotic treatment also did not enhance processing speed in the study by Friedman et al. (47). In addition, tryptophan depletion did not affect processing speed in the study by Golightly et al. (87). Thus, the available studies do not provide evidence for a prominent role of serotonin in processing speed in schizophrenia. However, all studies used small samples and the study of Golightly et al. (87) allowed concomitant anticholinergics which could have influenced the results.
Studies using acetylcholine related intervention strategies did not yield positive results either. Both the selective nicotinic α4β2 receptor agonist AZD3480 and the partial nicotinic α7 agonist DMXB-A did not enhance processing speed (51, 52). Moreover, three types of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, donepezil, galantamine, and rivastigmine, did not improve processing speed (50, 54–58). Thus, although the role of acetylcholine in memory and attention has been well established, it does not seem to be a potential target in enhancing processing speed in schizophrenia. However, these studies have limitations such as small sample sizes.
Effects of GABA and norepinephrine on processing speed were also examined. Processing speed did not improve after adjunctive treatment with partial α2/α3 agonist MK-0777 (59). As mentioned earlier, BL-1020 (antipsychotic with GABAA agonistic properties) enhanced antipsychotic also did not enhance processing speed (35). In the pilot study of Friedman et al. (60), processing speed did not improve after adjunctive treatment with the norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor atomoxetine. Thus, norepinephrine and GABA α2/α3 receptors do not seem to play an important role in processing speed in schizophrenia as the available studies were not able to detect positive effects. However, lack of effect could be due to the small sample size of both studies.
Processing speed did increase after a single dose of d-amphetamine in the study of Pietrzak et al. (72). Adjunctive armodafinil treatment had no beneficial effects on processing speed in the study by Kane et al. (61). Hence, since d-amphetamine is an indirect dopamine D1 agonist, dopamine D1 receptors could be a possible target for enhancing processing speed in schizophrenia although the study by Pietrzak et al. (72) needs replication.
Lastly, there are some studies of agents that have no direct (known) effect on neurotransmission. Processing speed did not improve after single dose of 50 and 100 mg sildenafil (a PDE5 inhibitor commonly used for erectile dysfunction) in the cross-over trial by Goff et al. (67). Furthermore, adjunctive dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) therapy, a corticosteroid that serves as a precursor for both androgenic and estrogenic steroids, did not enhance processing speed (91). Thus, other lines of research did not identify promising intervention strategies for enhancement of processing speed in schizophrenia.
To summarize, at present no convincing evidence exist for the effectiveness of both typical and atypical antipsychotics on processing speed in schizophrenia. Although, positive results were found with quetiapine, risperidone, clozapine, and aripiprazole, these studies have important limitations and therefore the effects cannot be reliably attributed to the effects of the medication. One positive result was found with single dose d-amphetamine, suggesting a potential role of dopamine D1 receptors in processing speed. Studies investigating 5-HT3a receptor antagonist, SSRI, nicotinic receptor agonist, acetylcholinesterase inhibitors, a GABA α2/α3 agonist, and a norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor did not find clinically relevant improvement in processing speed but are in need for replication as the described studies used small samples and were possibly underpowered.
Reasoning and Problem Solving
We found 30 studies that measured reasoning and problem solving after a pharmacological intervention. Reasoning and problem solving is considered an aspect of executive functioning. As for the other cognitive domains, a relatively large number of studies investigated the effects of antipsychotics on reasoning and problem solving.
Tyson et al. (17) compared antipsychotics with low affinity for 5-HT2a receptors to antipsychotics with high affinity for these receptors. Response latency on a reasoning and problem solving task decreased in the low affinity group, whereas latency increased in the high-affinity group. The authors did not correct for multiple comparisons. Since response latency increased in the high-affinity group, it is not likely that the improvement in the low affinity group is (only) driven by practice effects. Purdon et al. (20) compared the effects of olanzapine, risperidone, and haloperidol and found that olanzapine was superior in enhancing reasoning and problem solving. However, after correcting for multiple comparisons, none of the medications improved reasoning and problem solving. On the contrary, both Kim et al. (27) and Suzuki and Gen (28) found improvement in performance on a reasoning and problem solving task. However, Kim et al. (27) did not include a control group and did not correct for possible practice effects. Therefore, improvement cannot be reliably attributed to the effects of medication. Moreover, in the study of Suzuki and Gen (28) a high percentage of the patients in the haloperidol group was using concomitant anticholinergics whereas in the risperidone group anticholinergics were tapered during the first weeks of the trial. Therefore improvement in this group could be due to effects of diminishing anticholinergics.
Purdon et al. (24) investigated the effects of haloperidol and quetiapine and found that reasoning and problem solving did not improve in both groups. Olanzapine, perospirone, and aripiprazole did not improve reasoning and problem solving either (41). Moreover, adding aripiprazole to atypical antipsychotics also did not yield positive results (42). Furthermore, no improvement was found with clozapine by Ertugrul et al. (33) and Purdon et al. (32). Nielsen et al. (77) added sertindole to clozapine but found no improvement in reasoning and problem solving either. Finally, the new antipsychotic with GABA agonistic properties BL-1020 also did not improve reasoning and problem solving (35). Hence, although positive results were found with risperidone depot, both typical and atypical antipsychotics have shown little clinically relevant effects on reasoning and problem solving.
Serotonergic intervention strategies have not yielded positive results. Adjunctive ondansetron (5-HT3 antagonist) treatment had no beneficial effects on reasoning and problem solving (43). Fluvoxamine and citalopram both did not enhance reasoning and problem solving either (47, 71). On the contrary, Golightly et al. (87) found that patients who underwent tryptophan depletion during the first session performed significantly worse on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) compared to placebo. However, this effect was not present during the second session. The WCST requires subjects to sort cards by a certain parameter. The subject is not told which parameter. The sorting principle changes after 10 correct responses in a row. Thus subjects have to acquire a certain strategy to sort the cards. The authors concluded that tryptophan depletion only affected strategy acquisition and that once this is learned; tryptophan depletion did not interfere with application of this strategy. Thus, based on the results described above, 5-HT3a antagonist and SSRI’s did not effectively enhance reasoning and problem solving. However, a role for serotonin in this aspect of cognition cannot be ruled out since tryptophan depletion did interfere with strategy acquisition.
Partial nicotinic α7 agonist DMXB-A had no enhancing effects on reasoning and problem solving in the study by Freedman et al. (52). In addition, studies investigating the effects of the acetylcholinesterase inhibitors donepezil and rivastigmine did not find improvement in performance on a reasoning and problem solving task either (50, 53, 56, 58). Hence, based on these results, nicotinic α7 agonists and acetylcholinesterase inhibitors do not appear to significantly enhance reasoning and problem solving in schizophrenia.
In line with the results of Geffen et al. (35) described earlier, no positive results were obtained with other GABAergic interventions strategies. Adjunctive therapy with partial GABA α2/α3 agonist MK-0777 did not enhance reasoning and problem solving abilities in the study by Buchanan et al. (59). In addition, adjunctive topiramate (an antiepileptic drug which potentiates GABAergic transmission probably through its AMPA/kainite receptor antagonistic properties) treatment did not enhance reasoning and problem solving either (92). Zoccali et al. (93) examined the effects of adjunctive lamotrigine, an anticonvulsant drug which reduces excessive glutamate release in the brain via inhibition of voltage-gated sodium and calcium channels (94). Reasoning and problem solving did not improve.
The potential role of norepinephrine in reasoning and problem solving in schizophrenia was examined in a pilot study by Friedman et al. (60) by adding atomoxetine to antipsychotic treatment. They found no improvement in this aspect of cognition.
In conclusion, the available evidence suggests that modulation of GABA and glutamate transmission and norepinephrine reuptake inhibition does not have enhancing effects on reasoning and problem solving abilities in schizophrenia.
Some positive results have been found with psychostimulant drugs. Compared to placebo, performance on a reasoning and problem solving task improved after a single dose of d-amphetamine [indirect dopamine D1 agonist, (72)]. Contrary to these results, Turner et al. (63) found that, compared to placebo, response latency on the Tower of London task was significantly lower after modafinil administration. The number of attempts to obtain the correct solution did not differ between the two groups. Armodafinil, the longer-lasting isomer of modafinil, did not affect reasoning and problem solving abilities (61). Thus, although armodafinil and modafinil did not enhance reasoning and problem solving, d-amphetamine did improve this aspect of cognition. This suggests that dopamine D1 agonists are a potential area of research for further study of reasoning and problem solving in schizophrenia.
Pharmacological intervention strategies that do not directly influence neurotransmission were also investigated. Levkovitz et al. (73) found that reasoning and problem solving abilities improved after add-on minocycline treatment whereas performance in the placebo group did not change. Both creatine and glucose had no enhancing effects on reasoning and problem solving; daily creatine administration in addition to antipsychotic treatment did not have beneficial effects on working memory in the study by Kaptsan et al. (65). Stone et al. (68) found no improvement in reasoning and problem solving after a single dose of glucose. Hence, despite of the study’s limitations, minocycline’s putative ability to improve reasoning and problem solving abilities should be studied further.
In conclusion, although positive results were found with depot risperidone treatment in studies with important limitations, antipsychotics do not seem to effectively improve reasoning and problem solving in schizophrenia. The available studies did not show enhancing effects of the neurotransmitters serotonin, acetylcholine, GABA, or norepinephrine on reasoning and problem solving. However, dopamine D1 agonists may have potential in this cognitive domain since one positive result was found with a single dose d-amphetamine. Finally, minocycline had enhancing effects on reasoning and problem solving abilities. However, this study has important limitations. Therefore these results are in need for replication.
Social Cognition
In general terms, social cognition refers to the cognitive processes used to decode and encode the social world (95). Of all the MATRICS cognitive domains, social cognition has received the least attention in research. This is probably due to the fact that it is a relatively new area in schizophrenia research (96) and that the boundaries of this domain are not entirely clear (96). Final screening yielded no more than seven articles on social cognition and pharmacology using validated outcome measures.
A study by Mizrahi et al. (97) examined the effects of antipsychotic treatment on social cognition in schizophrenia. They investigated the effects of clozapine, risperidone, olanzapine, and loxapine on Theory of Mind (TOM). TOM is an aspect of social cognition which refers to the ability to understand intentions of others and to recognize that their actions are guided by beliefs about the world (97). They found that TOM improved after 2 weeks of medication use and continued to improve during the rest of the trial. Unfortunately, the authors did not differentiate between the four different antipsychotics. However, all four types of medication have high affinity for dopamine D2 receptors as well as the 5-HT2a receptors (98, 99). These results are in line with the results of Sumiyoshi et al. (100) who found that perospirone treatment improved performance on a social cognition task. Nonetheless, both studies did not include a control group and used small samples. Hence, given the longitudinal design of this study, natural progression and practice effects cannot be excluded. Additionally, the high drop-out rate in the study of Sumiyoshi et al. (100) could have led to a bias in the sample. Behere et al. (101) found that performance on an emotion recognition task improved after risperidone treatment. However, it must be noted that they did not include a control group and that time of follow-up assessment was not equal for all patients. Opposed to these results, the study by Harvey et al. (23) discussed earlier did not find improvement in performance on an emotional recognition task in patients treated with either quetiapine or risperidone.
Hence, these results imply preliminary evidence that dopamine and serotonin are important for TOM related aspects of social cognition but not for emotion recognition.
The possible role of GABA in social cognition was investigated by Buchanan et al. (59). They found no improvement with the partial GABA α2/α3 agonist MK-0777 when added to antipsychotic treatment. The psychostimulant armodafinil also did not improve social cognition (61). Hence, GABA α2/α3 receptors are not prime candidates for enhancing social cognition in schizophrenia.
Another line of research focused on the role of oxytocin in social cognition. This neuropeptide is known for its role in positive social behavior (102). Pedersen et al. (103) found that intranasal oxytocin administration improved social cognition. This implicates a role for oxytocin in social cognition. Since previous research showed that plasma oxytocin levels are lower in schizophrenia compared to controls (104), it could be hypothesized that increasing oxytocin levels improves social cognition in schizophrenia. However, the study of Pedersen et al. (103) has some important limitations. First, the sample size was small. Moreover, they carried out multiple tests in this small sample; therefore the results have to be interpreted with caution. Studies investigating the role of oxytocin in social cognition are sparse. Therefore, to determine the exact role of oxytocin in social cognition more research needs to be conducted on this topic.
In conclusion, the available seven studies suggest preliminary evidence for a role of dopamine, serotonin, and oxytocin in social cognition in schizophrenia patients. GABA does not seem to be a promising target for enhancement of social cognition. However, these studies have important limitations. Therefore future research needs to confirm the role of these transmitters in social cognition.
Discussion
With this review we aimed to provide an outline of the underlying neuropharmacological mechanisms of the separate MATRICS domains. Although some potential targets were identified, overall, results of previous studies attempting to identify potential pharmacological targets for cognitive enhancement have been unsatisfactory. This review has shown that dysfunction in separate cognitive domains seems to arise from different underlying neuropharmacological mechanisms. This suggests that schizophrenia patients with different cognitive impairments could benefit from different (adjunctive) pharmacological agents. The identified potential molecular targets, which include dopamine D1 receptors, serotonin 5-HT1a and 5-HT3a receptors, nicotinic α7 receptors, GABAA receptors, and NMDA receptors, are described more extensively below.
Dopamine D1
Although George et al. (48) were not able to detect positive results with a single dose of D1 agonist dihydrexidine, Pietrzak et al. (72) found improvement in both processing speed and reasoning and problems solving abilities (both aspects of executive functioning) after a single dose of d-amphetamine, an indirect D1 agonist. Decreased dopaminergic neurotransmission in the PFC has been hypothesized to be associated with cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia (36). Since D1 receptors are highly abundant in the PFC (105), this receptor subtype has been particularly associated with executive function and working memory. A PET study by Okubo et al. (106) found decreased D1 receptors in the PFC in schizophrenia which was indeed associated with poorer executive functioning. Animal studies provide additional evidence for cognitive enhancing effects of D1 receptor agonists as low doses of several D1 agonists were found to enhance cognition in non-human primates (107, 108). Because a majority of dopamine receptors in the PFC belong to the D1 subtype, and not to the D2 subtype (109), which is related to psychotic symptom severity, D1 receptors may be a feasible molecular target for enhancement of executive function related aspects of cognition without exacerbation of psychotic symptoms.
Serotonin 5-HT1a and 5-HT3a receptors
(Partial) 5-HT1a receptor agonism was found to improve verbal learning and memory (38–40). These enhancing effects may be due to the high density of 5-HT1a receptors in the hippocampus (36), which is an area in the brain well known for its role in memory. In addition, preclinical studies showed that both 5-HT1a agonists and antagonist enhanced cognition in rats (110, 111). 5-HT3a receptor antagonism was associated with improvement in visual learning and memory (43, 44). Contrary to other subtypes of serotonin receptors, the 5-HT3a receptor is the only ligand-gated ion channel subtype (43). To date, not many studies investigated the potential role of 5-HT3a receptors in cognition. However, since both Akhondzadeh et al. (43) and Levkovitz et al. (44) found improvement in visual learning and memory with the 5-HT3a receptor antagonist ondansetron, these receptors might be a promising molecular target for enhancement of visual learning and memory in schizophrenia.
Acetylcholine nicotinic α7 receptors
The role of acetylcholine in cognition (particularly in learning, memory, and attention) has been widely established and central dysfunction of the cholinergic system has been found to be associated with cognitive symptoms in neurological diseases as Alzheimer and Parkinson’s disease (112–114). However, with the exception of positive results found with nicotine and a partial nicotinic α7 agonist on attention (52, 89), cholinergic interventions strategies did not affect any of the cognitive domains in the available studies. However, all the available studies that met the inclusion criteria used acetylcholinesterase inhibitors or nicotinic agonists. Although (mostly post-mortem) both nicotinic and muscarinic receptor abnormalities have been repeatedly found in schizophrenia (115–120), nicotinic receptor antagonists do not appear to impair cognition in the same manner as antimuscarinic drugs (121). This might explain the lack of improvement in the described studies. Indeed, a small pilot study by Shekar et al. (122) found that the muscarinic receptor agonist xanomeline improves verbal, visual, and working memory in patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Moreover, xanomeline improved cognition in Alzheimer disease (123). Thus, although positive effects on attention were found with nicotine and a partial nicotinic α7 agonist, future studies should focus on muscarinic agents.
GABAA receptors
Both GABAA receptor antagonists and agonists were found to affect working memory and verbal learning and memory (35, 80). Multiple studies have shown reduced GABAergic transmission, especially in the PFC, in schizophrenia (124). The PFC is strongly involved in working memory functioning (125) and animal studies have shown that appropriate GABA transmission in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DPFC) is essential to adequate working memory functioning (36, 126). Lewis et al. (37) showed that altered GABA transmission in the DPFC is possibly limited to certain cell classes, such as the chandelier cells, which synchronize the activation of the pyramidal neurons via the GABAA receptor subtypes. Therefore, GABAA receptors may be a promising molecular target for enhancement of working memory.
Glutamate NMDA receptors
Glutamate, the primary excitatory neurotransmitter in the mammalian brain, has been linked to learning and memory because of its principal role in modulating long-term potentiation (127), and hyperactivity of glutamatergic neurotransmission has been implicated in schizophrenia (36). It has been hypothesized that cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia is due to hypofunction of the NMDA receptor (81). Small increases in NMDA-dependent glutamate transmission might enhance cognition, whereas excessive stimulation might have neurodegenerative consequences (36). Activation of the NMDA receptors leads to synthesis of nitric oxide, which is able to further increase the excitotoxicity by increasing glutamate release from presynaptic neurons and inhibition of glial glutamate transporters (73). Indeed, minocycline (which blocks nitric oxide induced neurotoxicity) was found to improve visual learning and memory, working memory, and reasoning and problem solving abilities (73). Although d-cycloserine (which non-competitively enhances NMDA neurotransmission) was not found to improve attention and working memory (81), pharmacological agents reducing the neurotoxic effects of extensive glutamate might still be a promising intervention strategy for cognitive enhancement.
Can the Negative Results be Explained by Studies Limitations?
A substantial number of the studies evaluated in this review addressing pharmacological cognitive enhancement in schizophrenia report negative results. It is debatable whether this is due to ineffectiveness of the agents used, or that potential results were obscured by methodological shortcomings as cognition research in schizophrenia deals with pertinent limitations. First, in many of the studies included in this review, cognition was not the primary outcome parameter. As a result, the design of these studies was not always optimal to measure cognitive enhancement. Second, the sample sizes are often too small to detect clinically relevant effects. Third, concurrent medications may interfere with the investigated pharmacological agents. Patients with schizophrenia are generally treated with antipsychotics. Because the entire mechanism of action of these medications is not completely understood, it cannot be ruled out that antipsychotics alter the effects of the added agents (62). Moreover, patients often use concomitant medications such as benzodiazepines, antidepressants, and anticholinergics, which could influence the effects of the adjunctive pharmacological agents as well. Especially anticholinergic medication is well known for its adverse effects on cognition (29). Therefore, studies including only medication-naïve patients at early stages of the disease are necessary. Fourth, studies often do not use fixed doses. It is possible that certain agents are only effective in a certain dose. If studies do use a fixed dose, this dose is often established for treating the illness or symptoms it was originally used for. However, it is not necessarily so that the same dose is required for cognitive enhancement in schizophrenia. Fifth, research seems to focus on the direct or semi-direct enhancing effect of modulating certain receptors while cognitive decline has most often been a process of a longer period of time possibly implying that enhancement studies should equally allow for more time to yield effects. Also, different paradigms could focus on neuroprotective targets and preventing cognitive decline early in the disorder in contrast to enhancing cognition after impairment. Sixth, not all studies report information about substance use of the participants. Patients with schizophrenia often abuse substances as tobacco, alcohol and cannabis, and epidemiological studies found high co-morbidity rates of substance abuse (40–60%) (128, 129). Particularly tobacco use can interfere with nicotinic receptor agents as it causes desensitization of this receptor subtype. Although most of the studies excluded patients with a (recent) co-morbid diagnosis of alcohol or drug abuse/dependence, tobacco using patients are almost never excluded. Seventh, studies often use different neuropsychological tasks to measure the same aspects of cognition which makes it difficult to compare the results. Therefore, studies should use a standardized cognitive battery, such as the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery [MCCB; (84)] or the CANTAB cognitive battery (130). The MCCB in particular, has been composed to reliably assess cognition in schizophrenia patients. At present, only three of the reported studies used this battery to measure cognition (50, 52, 59). Finally, not all studies used a control group and repeatedly conducted the same test battery without adequately correcting for potential practice effects. Therefore, improvement often cannot be reliably attributed to the effect of the pharmacological agent. Future research should take these limitations in to account in experimental setup and optimize their design for cognition measurement. Ideally, future studies should include a placebo or other control group and should ensure sufficient power by including enough participants. Furthermore, medication-naïve patients should be recruited, preferably non-smokers who do not use other substances as cannabis and alcohol. Regarding the design, future studies should choose cognition as primary outcome measure and optimize the design by using a fixed dose and make sure to choose a dose suitable for schizophrenia patients. Finally, future studies should make sure to choose sufficient trial duration and to use a standardized cognitive test battery which covers all the MATRICS domains. In this manner, studies will be better comparable and the effects of possible confounding variables will be limited. To summarize, the negative results of “pharmacological cognitive enhancement studies” could partially be explained by studies’ limitations.
Conclusion
Although some potential targets were identified, overall, results of previous studies attempting to identify potential pharmacological targets for cognitive enhancement have been disappointing. This review has shown that dysfunction in separate cognitive domains may arise from different underlying neuropharmacological mechanisms which suggests that schizophrenia patients with different cognitive impairments could benefit from different intervention strategies. Although development of effective cognitive enhancers is a complex process, it is an exciting challenge for this area of research as improvement of cognition contributes significantly to the quality of life of these patients.
Author Contributions
Wilhelmina A. M. Vingerhoets wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
- 1.Reichenberg A, Weiser M, Caspi A, Knobler HY, Lubin G, Harvey PD, et al. Premorbid intellectual functioning and risk of schizophrenia and spectrum disorders. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (2006) 28:193–207 10.1080/13803390500360372 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Green MF, Kern RS, Heaton RK. Longitudinal studies of cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: implications for MATRICS. Schizophr Res (2004) 72:41–51 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Green MF. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry (1996) 153:321–30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Heinrichs RW. The primacy of cognition in schizophrenia. Am Psychol (2005) 60:229–42 10.1037/0003-066X.60.3.229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Burton SC. Strategies for improving adherence to second-generation antipsychotics in patients with schizophrenia by increasing ease of use. J Psychiatr Pract (2005) 11:369–78 10.1097/00131746-200511000-00003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Prouteau A, Verdoux H, Briand C, Lesage A, Lalonde P, Nicole L, et al. Cognitive predictors of psychosocial functioning outcome in schizophrenia: a follow-up study of subjects participating in a rehabilitation program. Schizophr Res (2005) 77:343–53 10.1016/j.schres.2005.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Chen EY, Hui CL, Dunn EL, Miao MY, Yeung WS, Wong CK, et al. A prospective 3-year longitudinal study of cognitive predictors of relapse in first-episode schizophrenic patients. Schizophr Res (2005) 77:99–104 10.1016/j.schres.2005.02.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence Schizophrenia: Core Interventions in the Treatment and Management of Schizophrenia in Adults in Primary and Secondary Care (2009). Available from: http://guidance.nice.org.uk/CG82 (Clinical guideline 82).
- 9.Buchanan RW, Kreyenbuhl J, Kelly DL, Noel JM, Boggs DL, Fischer BA, et al. The 2009 schizophrenia PORT psychopharmacological treatment recommendations and summary statements. Schizophr Bull (2010) 36:71–93 10.1093/schbul/sbp116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hasan A, Falkai P, Wobrock T, Lieberman J, Glenthoj B, Gattaz WF, et al. World Federation of Societies of Biological Psychiatry (WFSBP) guidelines for biological treatment of schizophrenia, part 1: update 2012 on the acute treatment of schizophrenia and the management of treatment resistance. World J Biol Psychiatry (2012) 13:318–78 10.3109/15622975.2012.696143 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Stip E, Chouinard S, Boulay LJ. On the trail of a cognitive enhancer for the treatment of schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2005) 29:219–32 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2004.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Volk DW, Lewis DA. Impaired prefrontal inhibition in schizophrenia: relevance for cognitive dysfunction. Physiol Behav (2002) 77:501–5 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00936-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Cohen NJ, Ryan J, Hunt C, Romine L, Wszalek T, Nash C. Hippocampal system and declarative (relational) memory: summarizing the data from functional neuroimaging studies. Hippocampus (1999) 9:83–98 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Nuechterlein KH, Barch DM, Gold JM, Goldberg TE, Green MF, Heaton RK. Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2004) 72:29–39 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Feifel D, Macdonald K, Cobb P, Minassian A. Adjunctive intranasal oxytocin improves verbal memory in people with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2012) 139:207–10 10.1016/j.schres.2012.05.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Wagner M, Quednow BB, Westheide J, Schlaepfer TE, Maier W, Kühn K-U. Cognitive improvement in schizophrenic patients does not require a serotonergic mechanism: randomized controlled trial of olanzapine vs amisulpride. Neuropsychopharmacology (2005) 30:381–90 10.1038/sj.npp.1300626 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Tyson PJ, Roberts KH, Mortimer AM. Are the cognitive effects of atypical antipsychotics influenced by their affinity to 5HT-2A receptors? Int J Neurosci (2004) 114:593–611 10.1080/00207450490430552 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Keefe RS, Bilder RM, Davis SM, Harvey PD, Palmer BW, Gold JM, et al. Neurocognitive effects of antipsychotic medications in patients with chronic schizophrenia in the CATIE trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2007) 64:633–47 10.1001/archpsyc.64.6.633 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Davidson M, Galderisi S, Weiser M, Werbeloff N, Ph D, Fleischhacker WW, et al. Cognitive effects of antipsychotic drugs in first-episode schizophrenia and schizophreniform disorder: a randomized, open-label clinical trial (EUFEST). Am J Psychiatry (2009) 166:675–82 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08060806 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Purdon SE, Jones BD, Stip E, Labelle A, Addington D, David SR, et al. Neuropsychological change in early phase schizophrenia during 12 months of treatment with olanzapine, risperidone, or haloperidol. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2000) 57:249–58 10.1001/archpsyc.57.3.249 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Malhotra AK, Burdick KE, Razi K, Bates JA, Sanders M, Kane JM. Ziprasidone-induced cognitive enhancement in schizophrenia: specificity or pseudospecificity? Schizophr Res (2006) 87:181–4 10.1016/j.schres.2006.05.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Rémillard S, Pourcher E, Cohen H. Long-term effects of risperidone versus haloperidol on verbal memory, attention, and symptomatology in schizophrenia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2008) 14:110–8 10.1017/S1355617708080090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Harvey PD, Patterson TL, Potter LS, Zhong K, Brecher M. Improvement in social competence with short-term double-blind comparison of quetiapine versus and neuropsychological functioning. Am J Psychiatry (2006) 163:1918–25 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.11.1918 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Purdon SE, Malla A, Labelle A, Lit W. Neuropsychological change in patients with schizophrenia after treatment with quetiapine or haloperidol. J Psychiatry Neurosci (2001) 26:137–49 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Velligan DI, Newcomer J, Pultz J, Csernansky J, Hoff AL, Mahurin R, et al. Does cognitive function improve with quetiapine in comparison to haloperidol? Schizophr Res (2002) 53:239–48 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00268-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kivircik Akdede BB, Alptekin K, Kitis A, Arkar H, Akvardar Y. Effects of quetiapine on cognitive functions in schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2005) 29:233–8 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2004.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kim S, Shin I, Kim J, Lee S, Lee Y, Yang S, et al. Effects of switching to long-acting injectable risperidone from oral atypical antipsychotics on cognitive function in patients with schizophrenia. Hum Psychopharmacol (2009) 24:565–73 10.1002/hup.1057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Suzuki H, Gen K. The influence of switching from haloperidol decanoate depot to risperidone long-acting injection on the clinical symptoms and cognitive function in schizophrenia. Hum Psychopharmacol (2012) 27:470–5 10.1002/hup.2249 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Campbell N, Boustani M, Limbil T, Ott C, Fox C, Maidment I, et al. The cognitive impact of anticholinergics: a clinical review. Clin Interv Aging (2009) 4:225–33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Surguladze SA, Chu EM, Evans A, Anilkumar AP, Patel MX, Timehin C, et al. The effect of long-acting risperidone on working memory in schizophrenia: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Clin Psychopharmacol (2007) 27:560–70 10.1097/jcp.0b013e31815a256c [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kim S-W, Chung Y-C, Lee Y-H, Lee J-H, Kim S-Y, Bae K-Y, et al. Paliperidone ER versus risperidone for neurocognitive function in patients with schizophrenia: a randomized, open-label, controlled trial. Int Clin Psychopharmacol (2012) 27:267–74 10.1097/YIC.0b013e328356acad [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Purdon SE, Labelle A, Boulay L. Neuropsychological change in schizophrenia after 6 weeks of clozapine. Schizophr Res (2001) 48:57–67 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00101-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ertugrul A, Ucar G, Basar K, Demir B, Yabanoglu S, Ulug B. Influence of clozapine on platelet serotonin, monoamine oxidase and plasma serotonin levels. Psychiatry Res (2007) 149:49–57 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.12.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sumiyoshi T, Roy A, Kim C-H, Jayathilake K, Lee MA, Sumiyoshi C, et al. Prediction of changes in memory performance by plasma homovanillic acid levels in clozapine-treated patients with schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology (2004) 177:79–83 10.1007/s00213-004-1924-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Geffen Y, Keefe R, Rabinowitz J, Anand R, Davidson M. Bl-1020, a new γ-aminobutyric acid-enhanced antipsychotic: results of 6-week, randomized, double-blind, controlled, efficacy and safety study. J Clin Psychiatry (2012) 73:1168–74 10.4088/JCP.12m07642 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Gray JA, Roth BL. Molecular targets for treating cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull (2007) 33:1100–19 10.1093/schbul/sbm074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Lewis DA, Pierri JN, Volk DW, Melchitzky DS, Woo TU. Altered GABA neurotransmission and prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (1999) 46:616–26 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00061-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Sumiyoshi T, Matsui M, Yamashita I, Nohara S, Kurachi M, Uehara T, et al. The effect of tandospirone, a serotonin1A agonist, on memory function in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2001) 49:861–8 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01025-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Riedel M, Spellmann I, Schennach-Wolff R, Musil R, Dehning S, Cerovecki A, et al. Effect of aripiprazole on cognition in the treatment of patients with schizophrenia. Pharmacopsychiatry (2010) 43:50–7 10.1055/s-0029-1239539 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bervoets C, Morrens M, Vansteelandt K, Kok F, de Patoul A, Halkin V, et al. Effect of aripiprazole on verbal memory and fluency in schizophrenic patients: results from the ESCAPE study. CNS Drugs (2012) 26:975–82 10.1007/s40263-012-0003-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Suzuki H, Gen K, Inoue Y. An unblinded comparison of the clinical and cognitive effects of switching from first-generation antipsychotics to aripiprazole, perospirone or olanzapine in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2011) 35:161–8 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.10.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Yasui-Furukori N, Kaneda A, Sugawara N, Tomita T, Kaneko S. Effect of adjunctive treatment with aripiprazole to atypical antipsychotics on cognitive function in schizophrenia patients. J Psychopharmacol (2012) 26:806–12 10.1177/0269881111405555 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Akhondzadeh S, Mohammadi N, Noroozian M, Karamghadiri N, Ghoreishi A, Jamshidi A-H, et al. Added ondansetron for stable schizophrenia: a double blind, placebo controlled trial. Schizophr Res (2009) 107:206–12 10.1016/j.schres.2008.08.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Levkovitz Y, Arnest G, Mendlovic S, Treves I, Fennig S. The effect of Ondansetron on memory in schizophrenic patients. Brain Res Bull (2005) 65:291–5 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.09.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Zhang XY, Liu L, Liu S, Hong X, Chen DC, Xiu MH, et al. Short-term tropisetron treatment and cognitive and P50 auditory gating deficits in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2012) 169:974–81 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11081289 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ishikawa M, Sakata M, Toyohara J, Oda K, Ishii K, Wu J, et al. Occupancy of α7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in the brain by tropisetron: a positron emission tomography study using [(11)C]CHIBA-1001 in healthy human subjects. Clin Psychopharmacol Neurosci (2011) 9:111–6 10.9758/cpn.2011.9.3.111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Friedman JI, Ocampo R, Elbaz Z, Parrella M, White L, Bowler S, et al. The effect of citalopram adjunctive treatment added to atypical antipsychotic medications for cognitive performance in patients with schizophrenia. J Clin Psychopharmacol (2005) 25:237–42 10.1097/01.jcp.0000161499.58266.51 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.George MS, Molnar CE, Grenesko EL, Anderson B, Mu Q, Johnson K, et al. A single 20 mg dose of dihydrexidine (DAR-0100), a full dopamine D1 agonist, is safe and tolerated in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2007) 93:42–50 10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Karson CN, Mrak RE, Husain MM, Griffin WS. Decreased mesopontine choline acetyltransferase levels in schizophrenia. Mol Chem Neuropathol (1996) 29:181–91 10.1007/BF02815001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Tugal O, Yazici KM, Anil Yagcioglu AE, Gögüs A. A double-blind, placebo controlled, cross-over trial of adjunctive donepezil for cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol (2004) 7:117–23 10.1017/S1461145703004024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Velligan D, Brenner R, Sicuro F, Walling D, Riesenberg R, Sfera A, et al. Assessment of the effects of AZD3480 on cognitive function in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2012) 134:59–64 10.1016/j.schres.2011.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Freedman R, Olincy A, Buchanan RW, Harris JG, Gold JM, Johnson L, et al. Initial phase 2 trial of a nicotinic agonist in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2008) 165:1040–7 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07071135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Akhondzadeh S, Gerami M, Noroozian M, Karamghadiri N, Ghoreishi A, Abbasi S-H, et al. A 12-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of donepezil adjunctive treatment to risperidone in chronic and stable schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2008) 32:1810–5 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2008.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Lee S, Lee J, Lee B, Hoon Y. Galantamine adjunctive treatment to conventional antipsychotics for the cognitive impairments in chronic schizophrenia. Int Clin Psychopharmacol (2007) 22:63–8 10.1097/YIC.0b013e3280117feb [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Fagerlund B, Søholm B, Fink-Jensen A, Lublin H, Glenthøj BY. Effects of donepezil adjunctive treatment to ziprasidone on cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Clin Neuropharmacol (2007) 30:3–12 10.1097/01.WNF.0000240940.67241.F6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Sharma T, Reed C, Aasen I, Kumari V. Cognitive effects of adjunctive 24-weeks rivastigmine treatment to antipsychotics in schizophrenia: a randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind investigation. Schizophr Res (2006) 85:73–83 10.1016/j.schres.2006.03.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Freudenreich O, Herz L, Deckersbach T, Evins AE, Henderson DC, Cather C, et al. Added donepezil for stable schizophrenia: a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Psychopharmacology (2005) 181:358–63 10.1007/s00213-005-2235-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Friedman JI, Adler DN, Howanitz E, Harvey PD, Brenner G, Temporini H, et al. A double blind placebo controlled trial of donepezil adjunctive treatment to risperidone for the cognitive impairment of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2002) 51:349–57 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01342-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Buchanan RW, Keefe RS, Lieberman JA, Barch DM, Csernansky JG, Goff DC, et al. A randomized clinical trial of MK-0777 for the treatment of cognitive impairments in people with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2011) 69:442–9 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Friedman JI, Carpenter D, Lu J, Fan J, Tang CY, White L, et al. A pilot study of adjunctive atomoxetine treatment to second-generation antipsychotics for cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. J Clin Psychopharmacol (2008) 28:59–63 10.1097/jcp.0b013e318161318f [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Kane JM, D’Souza DC, Patkar AA, Youakim JM, Tiller JM, Yang R, et al. Armodafinil as adjunctive therapy in adults with cognitive deficits associated with schizophrenia: a 4-week, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. J Clin Psychiatry (2010) 71:1475–81 10.4088/JCP.09m05950gry [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Harvey PD. Pharmacological cognitive enhancement in schizophrenia. Neuropsychol Rev (2009) 19:324–35 10.1007/s11065-009-9103-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Turner DC, Clark L, Pomarol-Clotet E, McKenna P, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Modafinil improves cognition and attentional set shifting in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology (2004) 29:1363–73 10.1038/sj.npp.1300457 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Ballon JS, Feifel D. A systematic review of modafinil: potential clinical uses and mechanisms of action. J Clin Psychiatry (2006) 67:554–66 10.4088/JCP.v67n0406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Kaptsan A, Odessky A, Osher Y, Levine J. Lack of efficacy of 5 grams daily of creatine in schizophrenia: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. J Clin Psychiatry (2007) 68:881–4 10.4088/JCP.v68n0609 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Gallagher P, Watson S, Smith MS, Ferrier IN, Young AH. Effects of adjunctive mifepristone (RU-486) administration on neurocognitive function and symptoms in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2005) 57:155–61 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Goff DC, Cather C, Freudenreich O, Henderson DC, Evins AE, Culhane MA, et al. A placebo-controlled study of sildenafil effects on cognition in schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology (2009) 202:411–7 10.1007/s00213-008-1278-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Stone WS, Seidman LJ, Wojcik JD, Green AI. Glucose effects on cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2003) 62:93–103 10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00406-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Rollnik JD, Borsutzky M, Huber TJ, Mogk H, Seifert J, Emrich HM, et al. Short-term cognitive improvement in schizophrenics treated with typical and atypical neuroleptics. Neuropsychobiology (2002) 45:74–80 10.1159/000048680 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.McGurk SR, Green MF, Wirshing WC, Wirshing DA, Marder SR, Mintz J, et al. Antipsychotic and anticholinergic effects on two types of spatial memory in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2004) 68:225–33 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00123-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Niitsu T, Fujisaki M, Shiina A, Yoshida T, Hasegawa T, Kanahara N, et al. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of fluvoxamine in patients with schizophrenia: a preliminary study. J Clin Psychopharmacol (2012) 32:593–601 10.1097/JCP.0b013e3182664cfc [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Pietrzak RH, Snyder PJ, Maruff P. Use of an acute challenge with d-amphetamine to model cognitive improvement in chronic schizophrenia. Hum Psychopharmacol (2010) 25:353–8 10.1002/hup.1118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Levkovitz Y, Mendlovich S, Riwkes S, Braw Y, Levkovitch-Verbin H, Gal G, et al. A double-blind, randomized study of minocycline for the treatment of negative and cognitive symptoms in early-phase schizophrenia. J Clin Psychiatry (2010) 71:138–49 10.4088/JCP.08m04666yel [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Hasselmo ME, Stern CE. Mechanisms underlying working memory for novel information. Trends Cogn Sci (2006) 10:487–93 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Mori K, Nagao M, Yamashita H, Morinobu S, Yamawaki S. Effect of switching to atypical antipsychotics on memory in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2004) 28:659–65 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2004.01.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Papageorgiou C, Oulis P, Vasios C, Kontopantelis E, Uzunoglu N, Rabavilas A, et al. P300 alterations in schizophrenic patients experiencing auditory hallucinations. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol (2004) 14:227–36 10.1016/S0924-977X(03)00147-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Nielsen RE, Levander S, Thode D, Nielsen J. Effects of sertindole on cognition in clozapine-treated schizophrenia patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2012) 126:31–9 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2012.01840.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Jacobsen LK, D’Souza DC, Mencl WE, Pugh KR, Skudlarski P, Krystal JH. Nicotine effects on brain function and functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2004) 55:850–8 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.12.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Rezvani AH, Levin ED. Cognitive effects of nicotine. Biol Psychiatry (2001) 49:258–67 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01094-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Menzies L, Ooi C, Kamath S, Suckling J, McKenna P, Fletcher P, et al. Effects of γ-aminobutyric acid-modulating drugs on working memory and brain function in patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2007) 64:156–67 10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.156 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Duncan EJ, Szilagyi S, Schwartz MP, Bugarski-Kirola D, Kunzova A, Negi S, et al. Effects of D-cycloserine on negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2004) 71:239–48 10.1016/j.schres.2004.03.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Spence SA, Green RD, Wilkinson ID, Hunter MD. Modafinil modulates anterior cingulate function in chronic schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry (2005) 187:55–61 10.1192/bjp.187.1.55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Luck SJ, Gold JM. The construct of attention in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2008) 64:34–9 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.02.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Kern RS, Baade LE, Barch DM, Cohen JD, et al. The MATRICS consensus cognitive battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am J Psychiatry (2008) 165:203–13 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07010042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Molina V, Gispert JD, Reig S, Pascau J, Martinez R, Sanz J, et al. Olanzapine-induced cerebral metabolic changes related to symptom improvement in schizophrenia. Pharmacopsychiatry (2003) 20:13–8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Liu SK, Chen WJ, Chang CJ, Lin HN. Effects of atypical neuroleptics on sustained attention deficits in schizophrenia: a trial of risperidone versus haloperidol. Neuropsychopharmacology (2000) 22:311–9 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00137-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Golightly KL, Lloyd JA, Hobson JE, Gallagher P, Mercer G, Young AH. Acute tryptophan depletion in schizophrenia. Psychol Med (2001) 31:75–84 10.1017/S0033291799003062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Lenzi A, Maltinti E, Poggi E, Fabrizio L, Coli E. Effects of rivastigmine on cognitive function and quality of life in patients with schizophrenia. Clin Neuropharmacol (2003) 26:317–21 10.1097/00002826-200311000-00011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Dépatie L, O’Driscoll GA, Holahan AL, Atkinson V, Thavundayil JX, Kin NN, et al. Nicotine and behavioral markers of risk for schizophrenia: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over study. Neuropsychopharmacology (2002) 27:1056–70 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00372-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Knowles EE, David AS, Reichenberg A. Processing speed deficits in schizophrenia: reexamining the evidence. Am J Psychiatry (2010) 167:828–35 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09070937 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Strous RD, Stryjer R, Maayan R, Gal G, Viglin D, Katz E, et al. Analysis of clinical symptomatology, extrapyramidal symptoms and neurocognitive dysfunction following dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) administration in olanzapine treated schizophrenia patients: a randomized, double-blind placebo controlled trial. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2007) 32:96–105 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Muscatello MR, Bruno A, Pandolfo G, Micò U, Bellinghieri PM, Scimeca G, et al. Topiramate augmentation of clozapine in schizophrenia: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study. J Psychopharmacol (2011) 25:667–74 10.1177/0269881110372548 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Zoccali R, Muscatello M, Bruno A, Cambria R. The effect of lamotrigine augmentation of clozapine in a sample of treatment-resistant schizophrenic patients: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Schizophr Res (2007) 93:109–16 10.1016/j.schres.2007.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Kalyoncu A, Mirsal H, Pektas O, Unsalan N, Tan D, Beyazyürek M. Use of lamotrigine to augment clozapine in patients with resistant schizophrenia and comorbid alcohol dependence: a potent anti-craving effect? J Psychopharmacol (2005) 19:301–5 10.1177/0269881105051542 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Beer JS, Ochsner KN. Social cognition: a multi level analysis. Brain Res (2006) 1079:98–105 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Adolphs R. The neurobiology of social cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2001) 11:231–9 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00202-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Mizrahi R, Korostil M, Starkstein SE, Zipursky RB, Kapur S. The effect of antipsychotic treatment on theory of mind. Psychol Med (2007) 37:595–601 10.1017/S0033291706009342 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Burns MJ. The pharmacology and toxicology of atypical antipsychotic agents. Clin Toxicol (2001) 39:1–14 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Singh AN, Barlas C, Saeedi H, Mishra RK. Effect of loxapine on peripheral dopamine-like and serotonin receptors in patients with schizophrenia. J Psychiatry Neurosci (2003) 28:39–47 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Sumiyoshi T, Higuchi Y, Itoh T, Matsui M, Arai H, Suzuki M. Effect of perospirone on P300 electrophysiological activity and social cognition in schizophrenia: a three-dimensional analysis with sloreta. Psychiatry Res (2009) 172:180–3 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Behere RV, Venkatasubramanian G, Arasappa R, Reddy N, Gangadhar BN. Effect of risperidone on emotion recognition deficits in antipsychotic-naïve schizophrenia: a short-term follow-up study. Schizophr Res (2009) 113:72–6 10.1016/j.schres.2009.05.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E. Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature (2005) 435:673–6 10.1038/nature03701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Pedersen CA, Gibson CM, Rau SW, Salimi K, Smedley KL, Casey RL, et al. Intranasal oxytocin reduces psychotic symptoms and improves theory of mind and social perception in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2011) 132:50–3 10.1016/j.schres.2011.07.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Kéri S, Kiss I, Kelemen O. Sharing secrets: oxytocin and trust in schizophrenia. Soc Neurosci (2009) 4:287–93 10.1080/17470910802319710 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Takahashi H, Yamada M, Suhara T. Functional significance of central D1 receptors in cognition: beyond working memory. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab (2012) 32:1248–58 10.1038/jcbfm.2011.194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Okubo Y, Suhara T, Suzuki K, Kobayashi K, Inoue O, Terasaki O, et al. Decreased prefrontal dopamine D1 receptors in schizophrenia revealed by PET. Nature (1997) 385:634–6 10.1038/385634a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Cai JX, Arnsten AF. Dose-dependent effects of the dopamine D1 receptor agonists A77636 or SKF81297 on spatial working memory in aged monkeys. J Pharmacol Exp Ther (1997) 283:183–9 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Arnsten AF, Cai JX, Murphy BL, Goldman-Rakic PS. Dopamine D1 receptor mechanisms in the cognitive performance of young adult and aged monkeys. Psychopharmacology (1994) 116:143–51 10.1007/BF02245056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Galletly C. Recent advances in treating cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology (2009) 202:259–73 10.1007/s00213-008-1302-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Winstanley CA, Chudasama Y, Dalley JW, Theobald DE, Glennon JC, Robbins TW. Intra-prefrontal 8-OH-DPAT and M100907 improve visuospatial attention and decrease impulsivity on the five-choice serial reaction time task in rats. Psychopharmacology (2003) 167:304–14 10.1007/s00213-003-1398-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Boast C, Bartolomeo AC, Morris H, Moyer JA. 5HT antagonists attenuate MK801-impaired radial arm maze performance in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem (1999) 71:259–71 10.1006/nlme.1998.3886 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Van der Zee EA, Platt B, Riedel G. Acetylcholine: future research and perspectives. Behav Brain Res (2011) 221:583–6 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.01.050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Lieberman JA, Javitch JA, Moore H. Cholinergic agonists as novel treatments for schizophrenia: the promise of rational. Am J Psychiatry (2008) 165:931–6 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050769 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Friedman JI. Cholinergic targets for cognitive enhancement in schizophrenia: focus on cholinesterase inhibitors and muscarinic agonists. Psychopharmacology (2004) 174:45–53 10.1007/s00213-004-1794-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Raedler TJ, Knable MB, Jones DW, Urbina RA, Gorey JG, Lee KS, et al. In vivo determination of muscarinic acetylcholine receptor availability in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2003) 160:118–27 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Crook JM, Tomaskovic-Crook E, Copolov DL, Dean B. Low muscarinic receptor binding in prefrontal cortex from subjects with schizophrenia: a study of Brodmann’s areas 8, 9, 10, and 46 and the effects of neuroleptic drug treatment. Am J Psychiatry (2001) 158:918–25 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.987.918 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Crook JM, Tomaskovic-Crook E, Copolov DL, Dean B. Decreased muscarinic receptor binding in subjects with schizophrenia: a study of the human hippocampal formation. Biol Psychiatry (2000) 48:381–8 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00918-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Breese CR, Lee MJ, Adams CE, Sullivan B, Logel J, Gillen KM, et al. Abnormal regulation of high affinity nicotinic receptors in subjects with schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology (2000) 23:351–64 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00121-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Dean B, Crook J, Opeskin K, Hill C, Keks N, Copolov D. The density of muscarinic M1 receptors is decreased in the caudate-putamen of subjects with schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry (1996) 1:54–8 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Freedman R, Hall M, Adler LE, Leonard S. Evidence in postmortem brain tissue for decreased numbers of hippocampal nicotinic receptors in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (1995) 38:22–33 10.1016/0006-3223(94)00252-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Central cholinergic systems and cognition. Annu Rev Psychol (1997) 48:649–84 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.649 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Shekhar A, Potter WZ, Lightfoot J, Lienemann J, Dubé S, Mallinckrodt C, et al. Selective muscarinic receptor agonist xanomeline as a novel treatment approach for schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2008) 165:1033–9 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.06091591 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Bodick NC, Offen WW, Levey AI, Cutler NR, Gauthier SG, Satlin A, et al. Effects of xanomeline, a selective muscarinic receptor agonist, on cognitive function and behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol (1997) 54:465–73 10.1001/archneur.1997.00550160091022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Blum BP, Mann JJ. The GABAergic system in schizophrenia. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol (2002) 5:159–79 10.1017/S1461145702002894 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Kane MJ, Engle RW. The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual-differences perspective. Psychon Bull Rev (2002) 9:637–71 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Lewis DA, Volk DW, Hashimoto T. Selective alterations in prefrontal cortical GABA neurotransmission in schizophrenia: a novel target for the treatment of working memory dysfunction. Psychopharmacology (2004) 174:143–50 10.1007/s00213-003-1673-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Javitt DC. Glutamate as a therapeutic target in psychiatric disorders. Mol Psychiatry (2004) 9:984–97 10.1038/sj.mp.4001551 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Malchow B, Hasan A, Fusar-Poli P, Schmitt A, Falkai P, Wobrock T. Cannabis abuse and brain morphology in schizophrenia: a review of the available evidence. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2013) 263:3–13 10.1007/s00406-012-0346-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Lubman DI, King JA, Castle DJ. Treating comorbid substance use disorders in schizophrenia. Int Rev Psychiatry (2010) 22:191–201 10.3109/09540261003689958 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Levaux M-N, Potvin S, Sepehry AA, Sablier J, Mendrek A, Stip E. Computerized assessment of cognition in schizophrenia: promises and pitfalls of CANTAB. Eur Psychiatry (2007) 22:104–15 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2006.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]