Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2013 Dec 4;8(12):e80889. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080889

Task Dependency of Grip Stiffness—A Study of Human Grip Force and Grip Stiffness Dependency during Two Different Tasks with Same Grip Forces

Hannes Höppner 1,*, Joseph McIntyre 2, Patrick van der Smagt 3
Editor: Ramesh Balasubramaniam4
PMCID: PMC3852021  PMID: 24324643

Abstract

It is widely known that the pinch-grip forces of the human hand are linearly related to the weight of the grasped object. Less is known about the relationship between grip force and grip stiffness. We set out to determine variations to these dependencies in different tasks with and without visual feedback. In two different settings, subjects were asked to (a) grasp and hold a stiffness-measuring manipulandum with a predefined grip force, differing from experiment to experiment, or (b) grasp and hold this manipulandum of which we varied the weight between trials in a more natural task. Both situations led to grip forces in comparable ranges. As the measured grip stiffness is the result of muscle and tendon properties, and since muscle/tendon stiffness increases more-or-less linearly as a function of muscle force, we found, as might be predicted, a linear relationship between grip force and grip stiffness. However, the measured stiffness ranges and the increase of stiffness with grip force varied significantly between the two tasks. Furthermore, we found a strong correlation between regression slope and mean stiffness for the force task which we ascribe to a force stiffness curve going through the origin. Based on a biomechanical model, we attributed the difference between both tasks to changes in wrist configuration, rather than to changes in cocontraction. In a new set of experiments where we prevent the wrist from moving by fixing it and resting it on a pedestal, we found subjects exhibiting similar stiffness/force characteristics in both tasks.

Introduction

A vast body of literature is devoted to the regulation of grip force. Indeed, the force necessary to stably hold an object in our hand is continuously regulated by the CNS [1], [2] in a process known as “grip-force/load-force coupling”. Already in children at the age of 2, this grip force is regulated depending on an object's weight [3]. Furthermore, the CNS is capable of modulating grip force to account for load forces acting on the hand-held object, such as the inertial forces induced by movement of the arm [4], [5], or whole body movement during running [6] and jumping [7].

It has been shown that forces of an uncompensated grip decreases for contracting and increases for expanding objects [8], which evokes the concept of grip stiffness (i.e., the change in grip force versus a change in grip aperture) and may play an important role in maintaining grip stability. In a recent study [9] we measured grip stiffness as a function of grip force applied to an object held in a pinch grip. Participants were instructed to perform a force task with visual feedback, i.e., exert a predefined force which could be monitored by displaying the exerted force measured with a load cell. By applying very fast finger position perturbations during grip, we measured the part of stiffness that is related to biomechanics only, known as passive intrinsic stiffness, excluding influences from proprioceptive feedback. With these experiments, we demonstrated a linear relationship between grip force and intrinsic grip stiffness contributed by the passive properties of the corresponding muscles. We further showed that this conforms to a model of the pinching hand in which muscle exhibit elastic properties that can be represented by (nonlinear) exponential force-generating elements. A number of studies confirm this finding of a monotonic increase of finger force or torque with stiffness[10][13].

But how do load force, grip force and grip stiffness relate to each other? Can grip stiffness be modulated independent of grip force and if so, would such modulation have functional significance? Furthermore, the way in which we required subjects to apply different grip forces in our previous study (i.e., through visual feedback) was not very natural. What would the stiffnesses be like if a subject would lift an object in a weight task without any feedback about the applied force? Would the stiffnesses measured in the two tasks be comparable? Or would subjects be able to regulate force and stiffness independently?

Two possibilities to decouple grip stiffness and force are acknowledged: either by cocontraction of antagonistic pairs of muscles or by changing the finger/wrist configuration. Carter et al. showed that for zero net torque at the interphalangeal joint in the human thumb, joint stiffness highly increases with cocontraction. This demonstrates that net torque or force alone does not determine joint stiffness [13]. Furthermore, wrist flexion and extension causes stretching and shortening of, among others, the corresponding flexor digitorum superficialis and profundus muscles [14], affecting their force/activation relationship as described in the Hill muscle model [15]. This effect can reduce the maximum grip force to 73% of its maximum [16]. Thus, changes in wrist configuration should lead to changes in both grip force and grip stiffness. But even during wrist movements of up to Inline graphic, the CNS is able to keep grip force stable [2].

Does the neuromuscular system allow for an active use of these effects on grip stiffness and an independent control of both, force and stiffness? White et al. showed that the CNS is able to decouple grip and load force in anticipation of a collision, with a rise in grip force before the expected impact and a peak in grip force around 65 ms after the impact [17]. They hypothesized that the CNS increases the net grip force in order regulate grip stiffness and damping, with the goal of optimizing stability in object manipulation. They did not, however, directly measure grip stiffness. Furthermore, not only might one wish to modulate the grip force on an object to keep it stable in the face of predictable events like a self-generated collision, one might also wish to regulate the stiffness of the grip in anticipation of unexpected perturbations depending on the constraints of a specific task.

In the current study we set out to test for grip stiffness modulation as a function of the natural tendency to increase grip force when lifting increasingly heavy objects [1], [2]. We asked human participants to perform the visually-guided force-control task (FT) described above and a task in which they lifted objects of different weight (WT), without any specific instructions or visual feedback about the forces applied to the object in the pinch grip. Given our previous conjecture for the force task:

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e002.jpg (1)

where Inline graphic is the exerted force, Inline graphic the stiffness of the pinch grip, Inline graphic the pinch grip aperture, Inline graphic the muscle activation and Inline graphic and Inline graphic are slope [1/m] and offset [N/m], we can similarly conjecture

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e009.jpg (2)

for the weight task. To compare grip-stiffness/grip-force coupling between these two tasks, we set out to test the following specific hypotheses:

  • Inline graphic: The measured stiffness in both tasks are equal for same grip force, i.e. Inline graphic;

  • Inline graphic: The relationship between grip stiffness and grip force are equal for the two tasks, i.e. Inline graphic and Inline graphic.

General model of the fingers

In this section we will introduce a general finger model which describes the influence of cocontraction and kinematics on grip stiffness. The model will help us to interpret the measurements. Eq. (3) describes the restoring finger forces Inline graphic and their relation to the Cartesian stiffness matrix Inline graphic. This stiffness matrix describes the elastic behaviour caused by a displacement Inline graphic of the end point of a finger. Eq. (4) does the same for the finger joint torques Inline graphic vs. joint stiffness Inline graphic which is caused by an angular displacement Inline graphic. Finally, Eq. (5) describes the relation between muscle forces Inline graphic, muscle stiffness matrix Inline graphic, and muscle elongation Inline graphic:

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e024.jpg (3)
graphic file with name pone.0080889.e025.jpg (4)
graphic file with name pone.0080889.e026.jpg (5)

ignoretrue The finger and joint velocities are coupled by the Jacobian matrix Inline graphic, taking into account finger phalanx lengths; while muscle and joint velocities are coupled by the Jacobian matrix Inline graphic, which corresponds to muscles moment arms:

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e029.jpg (6)
graphic file with name pone.0080889.e030.jpg (7)

ignoretrue from which we can derive that

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e031.jpg (8)

Note that Inline graphic and Inline graphic are, in fact, functions of Inline graphic, but for readability we leave this out in our notation. Eq. (8) couples the finger endpoint and muscle displacements via a ratio of the two Jacobians.

The joint torque is coupled to the Cartesian and muscle force similar to the velocities by the kinematic chain but also by the moment arms represented by the Jacobian matrices:

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e035.jpg (9)
graphic file with name pone.0080889.e036.jpg (10)

ignoretrue from which we find

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e037.jpg (11)

Again, the endpoint and muscle forces are coupled via a ratio of the two Jacobians. Using these equations and the assumption that the two Jacobians do not change for incremental angular displacements, the relations between the different stiffnesses become

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e038.jpg (12)
graphic file with name pone.0080889.e039.jpg (13)

ignoretrue leading to

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e040.jpg (14)

Combining Eqs. (11) and (14) we conclude that, if Inline graphic and Inline graphic are linearly related, then so are Inline graphic and Inline graphic. In words: if the grasp force and Cartesian stiffness are linearly dependent, then this is caused by a linear relationship between the muscle force and muscle stiffness. From these equations we can derive the influence of the two strategies for changing endpoint stiffness and its effects on the stiffness/force characteristic:

Cocontraction will increase the force and stiffness of agonist and antagonist muscles within each finger, while maintaining the same net force Inline graphic applied by the finger tips. For a given level of co-activation, the operating point on the stiffness/force characteristic of each muscle and each joint will change, but the slope of the stiffness/force characteristic will not be affected. Under the assumption that the kinematic configuration will not be changed due to increased internal forces, cocontraction will lead to an increase in the muscle stiffness Inline graphic and a proportional increase of the Cartesian stiffness Inline graphic, without a change of grip force. Thus changing stiffness caused by cocontraction will affect the offset, but not the slope of the (linear) grip force–grip stiffness relation. This principle is illustrated in Fig. 1.

Figure 1. Stiffness change due to cocontraction.

Figure 1

Increasing the cocontraction of the corresponding muscles increases the offset in the stiffness/force relationship.

From Eq. (14) we can see that a change in the Jacobian by, e.g., changing finger or wrist orientation will have a nonlinear (quadratic) effect on the stiffness/force characteristic and will affect slope and offset.

Note that both strategies will not affect the linear relation between force and stiffness while cocontraction and kinematic orientation remain the same. A change of the slope between the two tasks will indicate an influence caused by a change of the Jacobian rather than by cocontraction. A change of the offset, however, can be caused by either or both. Furthermore, a change in the kinematic configuration possibly predominates effects of cocontraction on the stiffness/force characteristics.

Methods

Device description

The Grasp Perturbator (Fig. 2) used in this experiment was a small cylindrical device with which finger stiffness during pinch grip in flexion can be identified. A spring was preloaded by an electromagnet (blue) fixed to a frame (gray) that holds a moving part (red). The grip force was measured with a load cell (black). Note that this device could only apply perturbations in one direction, measuring stiffness of an expanding object. Precise grip force measurement was obtained by guiding the grip force through a button (green) to the load cell. Releasing the spring caused the device to elongate by 7 mm within a few ms. The Perturbator weighed 187 g and its length expanded from 57 to 64 mm when activated. As noted by Van Doren [10], grasp span has a small effect on grip stiffness (stiffness changed only 5% for a change of Inline graphic 2 cm in grasp span); we therefore have chosen a fixed Perturbator size. The spring force was 140 N when loaded and 100 N when unloaded, i.e., considerably higher than human pinch-grip force, ensuring identical experimental conditions independent of how firmly the Perturbator was held.

Figure 2. Cross sectional view of the Grasp Perturbator.

Figure 2

The measurement setup consisted of a host running Windows and a real-time target machine running QNX. The real-time machine ran a Matlab/Simulink model to control the electromagnet and read out the force sensor at 10 kHz. After pressing a release button, the perturbation was applied after a random delay 4 to 7 s. The load cell consisted of a KM10 force sensor and a measurement amplifier GSV-11H (both from ME-Messsysteme GmbH) with a nominal force of 100 N and an overall accuracy of the force signal of 0.1 N were used. We verified that perturbations caused no significant phantom force changes in the device by testing it with known springs. The offset of the measured force signal was calibrated before each trial.

In this study we focused on influences of underlying (bio)mechanical principles, rather than of reflexive feedback, and measured the combination of passive (surrounding tissue and ligaments) and intrinsic stiffness only (stiffness which can be attributed to the active muscle fibres); we refrained from measuring influences of reflexive stiffness. By measuring the grip force with an internal load cell before and shortly after the perturbation—well before the effects of short-latency reflexes—the grip stiffness contributed by the intrinsic properties of the muscles could be identified.

Experimental procedure

A total of 15 healthy right-handed male subjects, age 22–45 years, performed the two experimental protocols, WT and FT, as described below. No subject had a history of neurological disorder nor neuromuscular injury affecting the CNS or the muscles. All subjects gave written consent to the procedures which were conducted partially in accordance with the principles of the Helsinki agreement. Non-conformity concerns the point B-16 of the 59th World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki, Seoul, October 2008: no physician has supervised the experiment. The collection of subject data was approved by the institutional board for protection of data privacy and by the work council of the German Aerospace Center.

All subjects had previous experience working with the Perturbator and were able to stably hold the device, even after the perturbation. Fully naive subjects would often drop the device during perturbation, leading to useless data because of a missing second static force level.

Ten subjects performed the main experiment in which the arm and wrist were free to move, although subjects were instructed to hold the forearm steady in a horizontal posture. These ten subjects were divided into two groups, counter-balanced as to whether FT or WT was done first (E1 and E2, respectively). To investigate whether changes in the wrist configuration might have an influence on grip stiffness an additional group of 6 subjects (E3) performed the two protocols with the wrist held at a constant orientation with respect to the forearm and with the relaxed arm and wrist supported by a table. We favored fixation over controlling wrist position using optical tracking in order to keep the task natural and to avoid providing visual feedback in the WT. Half of the subjects in E3 (subjects S11, S12, S13) did WT first, the rest FT first. Note that one subject took part in two experiments, and is referred to as S6-1 in E2 and S6-2 in E3. The whole experiment lasted about 90 minutes per subject. No subject reported discomfort during FT, some reported fatigue during WT. Subjects stood throughout the experiment, except for a 10-minute break between FT and WT. We found standing and lifting weights with respect to the WT intuitively more natural than sitting.

Weight Task

In the WT, six different weights of from 0.2, 0.4, 1.2 kg were attached to the Perturbator, and for 10 trials each the subjects had to lift the device off of the table (Fig. 3). The lower arm was requested to be held at approximately a 90 degree angle with the body. There was no visual feedback w.r.t. the grip force to the subject. Once the grasp was stable, the experimenter pressed the button to apply the perturbation between 4 and 7 s later (randomly chosen by the control computer).

Figure 3. Grasp Perturbator held in a pinch grasp with attached weights.

Figure 3

Force Task

The procedure of the FT was almost the same as in our previous study [9], except that the subjects lifted and held the Perturbator above the table (as in WT, but with no additional weight attached). The subject received visual feedback about the actual force applied to the Perturbator and was asked to maintain a visually instructed predefined force. Once this force level was reached, the release button was pressed by the experimenter, unknown to the subject, and the perturbation was performed between 4 and 7 s later. Six instructed force levels were randomly presented to each subject, for a total of 10 times per force level.

Since applied grip forces for lifting the weights differed considerably across subjects, we measured the natural grip force when holding the device with different weights—thus leading to different grip forces—and chose 6 different grip force levels to subsequently use for FT. If subjects were instructed to do the FT first, we asked them to lift the Perturbator once with each weight attached before the FT without applying any perturbations. If the WT had to be done first, we used the information of the WT to estimate the required force levels for the FT.

For both tasks, we preferred experimenter release over automated release because previous experiments revealed increased participant fatigue in the latter case—holding the force level steady for a while, especially at high levels is increasingly difficult and troublesome. Note that the non-rigid coupling between the Perturbator and the additional weight in the WT meant that the inertia of the Perturbator was effectively constant, so that this effect did not have to be accounted for in the data analysis.

Data processing

The force signals were first filtered using a 21-point moving average filter. The time when the electromagnet was released and the time Inline graphic at which the perturbation started (see Fig. 4) varied considerably because the breakdown of the electromagnetic field depends highly on the applied grip forces. We therefore defined Inline graphic as the end of the period Inline graphic, where Inline graphic was defined as the last 10 ms time interval before Inline graphic having a standard deviation below Inline graphic N. These numbers were empirically determined and led to stable results. The rise time between start of the perturbation and Inline graphic was on average 3.6 Inline graphic 0.62 ms (SD) and showed no significant correlation with force and weight levels.

Figure 4. Example for typical Perturbation profile.

Figure 4

Force profile before, during, and after a perturbation starting at Inline graphic. Additionally, the time windows Inline graphic and Inline graphic and the mean of force for six force levels are depicted (mean force Inline graphic subtracted). The length of Inline graphic and Inline graphic were found to be optimal at 6.8 and 16.7 ms, respectively.

The time interval Inline graphic was used to determine the force level before the perturbation; conversely, we needed to determine a time window Inline graphic over which the force level after perturbation was computed. We defined a second time Inline graphic within which one can ignore effects of fast reflex responses and trust the data to be purely intrinsic, in order to measure tendon- and muscle-based influences only. The mean onset latency of the short-latency reflex is about 30.7 Inline graphic 1.7 ms (SD) for the first dorsal interosseus in the hand [18]. In [19] Allum et al. reported a delay of about 20 ms between the onset of the short-latency reflex and first measurable changes in muscle force of stretching triceps surae muscles and releasing tibialis anterior muscles elicited by electrical stimulation. Thus, assuming that this feedback does not have a measurable influence within 40 ms, Inline graphic was allowed to vary downward from Inline graphic and the duration Inline graphic was allowed to vary between 5 and 20 ms so as to minimize an objective function.

We decided to use one time window Inline graphic for all trials with a fixed start and end rather than optimizing either subject-wise or even force level-wise. This avoided comparing different lumped stiffnesses that are affected variously by damping and inertia, even if their influence was expected to be small. Furthermore, we looked at the gradient of the calculated stiffness values normalized by their mean as a kind of measure for stability of the achieved results. The data showed that stable results were achieved if the end of the second time window Inline graphic was at least 4 ms higher than the length of the time window Inline graphic. This corresponded to a second time window not intersecting the first and left out the peak of perturbation (see Fig. 4). Thus, Inline graphic was varied so as to optimize the objective function

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e074.jpg (15)

using the whole number of trials Inline graphic, levels Inline graphic and subjects Inline graphic. We introduce the CSE Inline graphic

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e079.jpg (16)

which combines the coefficient of variation and standard error. The standard error compensates the standard deviation Inline graphic for sample size Inline graphic assessing low sample sizes with a higher standard error; the coefficient of variation is a normalized measure of the standard deviation and compensates for the sample mean Inline graphic. Since the objective function Eq. (15) mixes data sets of different size and from different dimensions, we had to compensate the standard deviation Inline graphic for both. Inline graphic denotes the CSE of force for each trial within Inline graphic and Inline graphic represents CSE of stiffness values for the different trials, for FT and for WT, respectively. The objective of this optimization was to minimize the oscillations within time interval Inline graphic and the variation of resulting stiffness values measured under exactly the same conditions. Note, that we minimized the variation of stiffness between experiments with identical conditions rather than between different stiffnesses, grip forces, or subjects. The stiffness of each trial was calculated using

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e088.jpg (17)

where Inline graphic denotes the average over time intervals Inline graphic. The length of the second time interval Inline graphic and its end Inline graphic were found to be optimal under named constraints at 6.8 and 16.7 ms, respectively.

Results

Fig. 5, Fig. 6, and Fig. 7 show the results of the experiments for each subject. Each graph depicts the measured stiffness for FT in red and WT in black and their linear regressions as dashed lines. For each force and weight level, the mean values and their SEM in force and stiffness and the mean in force and stiffness used for testing Inline graphic are plotted as circles. Additionally, the related Inline graphic coefficient (values of Inline graphic close to 1 denote a near-perfect linear regression) for a linear assumption between FT and WT and the two normalized mean inter-subject ratios of stiffness Inline graphic and linear regression slopes Inline graphic are depicted. Furthermore, Tables 1 and 2 list the results of the measured stiffnesses and the linear regressions between force and stiffness for each subject in both tasks. Based on these data and regression fits we performed statistical tests of the previously conjectured two hypotheses. For testing these, a fixed level of significance was chosen as Inline graphic for all tests.

Figure 5. Results Experiment 1.

Figure 5

Subjects doing the WT first without a cuff.

Figure 6. Results Experiment 2.

Figure 6

Subjects doing the FT first without a cuff.

Figure 7. Results Experiment 3.

Figure 7

Subjects who performed the experiment with the wrist cuff (WT first: top row, FT first: bottom row).

Table 1. Testing Inline graphic.

group subject Inline graphic [N/m] Inline graphic [N/m] Inline graphic Inline graphic [N] Inline graphic [N] No. WT No. FT F-test [%]
E1 S1 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1
E1 S2 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1
E1 S3 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1
E1 S4 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1
E1 S5 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1
E2 S6-1 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E2 S7 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E2 S8 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1
E2 S9 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1
E2 S10 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1
E3 S11 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E3 S12 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E3 S13 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E3 S6-2 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1
E3 S14 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E3 S15 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
1

Inline graphicof the F-test (Variances are equal) is rejected for probability values less than 5%.

The mean and standard deviation of stiffness of both tasks, their normalized difference Inline graphic, the percentage of data discarded for this tests and the result of the F-test are listed. Data were discarded such that mean in force of both data sets align.

Table 2. Linear regression and Mandels test for linearity.

group subject Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic M-test WT M-test FT
E1 S1 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E1 S2 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E1 S3 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E1 S4 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1
E1 S5 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1 Inline graphic1
E2 S6-1 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E2 S7 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E2 S8 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E2 S9 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E2 S10 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1 Inline graphic
E3 S11 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E3 S12 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1 Inline graphic
E3 S13 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E3 S6-2 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1
E3 S14 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
E3 S15 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1 Inline graphic1
1

For probability values less than 5% it is rejected that a linear relation is as good as a quadratic.

Slope Inline graphic [1/m], their normalized values Inline graphic, offset Inline graphic [N/m], the related Inline graphic Inline graphic coefficient for a linear model and the results of the M-test in Inline graphic are listed.

Results main experiment—groups E1 and E2

The subjects of groups E1 and E2 were asked to do either FT or WT first with an unconstrained wrist.

Inline graphic: Equal stiffness for equal grip force ( Inline graphic )

For each subject we tested whether the same stiffness was generated in each task, on average across all grip forces. Since the ranges of grip force differed considerably between the two tasks—especially for subjects doing the FT first (see, e.g., subject S9 in Fig. 6)—we algorithmically adjusted the datasets on a subject-by-subject basis by discarding trials with the highest or lowest grip-force values so as to align the mean grip forces before perturbation in WT vs. FT. We then compared the average stiffness levels. These data were only discarded for testing Inline graphic. Note that this data adaptation had no qualitative effect on the results (rejection of tested hypothesis or not). Table 1 summarizes the results for each subject in detail; the mean values in force and stiffness are additionally depicted in Fig. 56. We tested whether grip stiffness differed for the two tasks by testing if their difference was significantly different from zero, on average across subjects, by performing Student's dependent paired t-test on mean inter-subject stiffness Inline graphic, normalized by the sum of Inline graphic and Inline graphic for each subject, for groups E1 and E2. These results (Table 3) indicate that mean stiffness differed significantly (Inline graphic) with higher stiffness measured in FT, regardless of which task was performed first by each subject (see Figs. 5 and 6). Furthermore, Table 1 includes results of an F-test for testing if the variances in intra-subject stiffness of both tasks were equal. The results provides evidence that, for all subjects of groups E1 and E2 excluding subject S6-1 and S7, it can be rejected that the variances in intra-subject stiffness were equal (Table 1), even if the tested standard variation was calculated across all data.

Table 3. Testing Inline graphic.

Inline graphic E1 E2 E1+E2
Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
Inline graphic (two-tailed) Inline graphic1 Inline graphic1 Inline graphic1
1

Inline graphicis rejected for probability values less than 5% (paired t-test).

Whereby the two stiffnesses Eqs. (1) and (2) are equal Inline graphic across subjects. The mean normalized difference of the two stiffnesses Inline graphic and its standard deviation (inter-subject variability). Data were discarded such that mean in force of both data sets align.

Inline graphic: Equal grip-stiffness/grip-force slopes ( Inline graphic ) and offsets ( Inline graphic )

For each subject and task we did a linear regression between force and stiffness and calculated the slope and offset of the resulting regression (see Table 2 for details). We then tested whether the parameters of the grip-stiffness/grip-force regressions differed between the two tasks, on average across subjects via a dependent paired t-test using the normalized difference of the slopes Inline graphic and the mean of the offsets (see Table 4). The results provide evidence that, in general, the slopes in FT differed significantly from WT (Inline graphic) with higher slopes in FT, regardless of which task was performed first by each subject. Furthermore, it cannot be rejected that the mean offsets in group E1 or group E2 were equal; conversely, this can be rejected when data from the two groups were combined (E1+E2).

Table 4. Testing Inline graphic .

Inline graphic E1 E2 E1+E2 Inline graphic E1 E2 E1+E2
Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
Inline graphic (two-tailed) Inline graphic1 Inline graphic1 Inline graphic1 Inline graphic (two-tailed) Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1
1

Inline graphicis rejected for probability values less than 5% (paired t-test).

Whereby the two slopes and offsets in Eqs. (1) and (2) are equal Inline graphic and Inline graphic across subjects (inter-subject variability). Left table: The mean normalized difference of the two slopes Inline graphic and its standard deviation. Left table: The mean difference of the two offsets and its standard deviation. No data were discarded.

What can we conclude from the above? When holding the weight in the hand, higher muscle activation was required for WT in order to counteract the vertical load. Furthermore, to stabilize the wrist against this vertical load, antagonistic pairs of muscles will have been activated. One might expect a higher measured stiffnesses in comparison to FT, in which no additional weight must be supported. But the opposite was measured: by increasing the load, the stiffness decreased at constant grip force.

One could argue that the higher stiffness in the FT was required to accurately hold a certain force level using cocontraction, while for the WT it was not, because there no visual feedback of force was presented. As we discussed in section “sec:Model,” using cocontraction will affect the offset of the stiffness/force relation. Because the results of testing Inline graphic provides evidence that the regressed offsets of the two tasks were different, we were interested to know if each of them differed from zero. We found that it can be rejected for the WT across both groups E1 and E2 that the offset listed in Table 2 is equal to the origin (two-tailed t-test; Inline graphic), but not for the FT.

We furthermore looked at the correlation between slope and mean intra-subject stiffness across all subjects (no data were discarded; see Table 5). The results indicate that there is a significant correlation between mean stiffness and slope for the FT, which further argues for a stiffness/force relation going through the origin for the FT. As a corollary, the results are consistent with the finding that the stiffness/force curve of a single muscle most likely goes through the origin [20]. Note that the offset of measured stiffness/force characteristic of the antagonistic system at zero net force is not precisely zero because of the passive stiffness of surrounding tissues and ligaments in the arm and hand.

Table 5. Correlation Inline graphic between slope and mean intra-subject stiffness and its probability Inline graphic in % for groups E1 and E2.

Inline graphic E1 E2 E1+E2
Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic1 Inline graphic1
1

The correlation is significant.

For probability values less than 5% the correlation is significant. No data were discarded.

Results experiments with fixed wrist—group E3

Given that we found both higher grip stiffness and a higher grip-stiffness/grip-force slope for FT together with an offset in the FT not different from zero and an offset significantly larger than zero for the WT, our results argue strongly for a change in the kinematics as the predominately underlying mechanism rather than a change of cocontraction (see section “sec:Model”). To further test this hypothesis, subjects in group E3 performed the two tasks with the wrist held in a constant position by a rigid cuff in order to minimize the influence of a change in the kinematics. The cuff used here was made of a thermoplast with a steel plate parallel to the arm axis in order to maximize its stiffness; but still the wrist could be bent within small ranges. In order to prevent subjects from moving their wrist, we additionally rested the arm and hand on a table.

We compared the average stiffness across all grip-force levels, the slope of the grip-stiffness/grip-force relationship and their correlation, the results of which are shown in Table 6. One can see that grip stiffness – grip force relationship, i.e. the slopes and the offsets, differed much less between the two tasks when the wrist was stabilized. Nevertheless, grip stiffness was still higher for FT versus WT. Furthermore, there was a highly significant correlation between mean intra-subject stiffness and the slope of the stiffness/force curve for the WT (Inline graphic) and FT (Inline graphic). It cannot be rejected for either task that the mean offset across subjects was equal to zero. Further, the results on testing equal variances in intra-subject stiffness using the F-test provides evidence that, for all subjects of group E3 except 6-2, it cannot be rejected that these variances were equal.

Table 6. Results of group E3 when the wrist was held in a constant posture by a cuff.

E3 Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic E3 Inline graphic
Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic
Inline graphic (two-tailed) Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic Inline graphic2
Inline graphic Inline graphic2
1

Inline graphicthat Inline graphic, Inline graphic and Inline graphic is rejected for probability values less than 5% (paired t-test), respectively.

2

For probability values less than 5% the correlation is significant.

Left table: dependent paired t-test of greater inter-subject grip stiffness, grip-stiffness/grip-force slope and offset for FT versus WT. Right table: Correlation between slope and mean intra-subject stiffness and its probability in %. No data were discarded.

About linearity between force and stiffness

As we initially stated we found a strong linear correlation between force and stiffness during the FT. To close out our description of section “sec:Results” we will further test if linearity was indeed obtained and if it changed between tasks. We used Mandel's technique to test whether a linear or a quadratic model provides a significantly better fit for the relationship between grip stiffness and grip force ([21], p. 165ff). The test compares the standard deviations of the residuals

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e503.jpg (18)

where Inline graphic and Inline graphic are the residual standard deviations of a linear and quadratic fit and Inline graphic the sample size. Inline graphic and Inline graphic are computed as

graphic file with name pone.0080889.e509.jpg (19)

where Inline graphic are the measured and Inline graphic, Inline graphic the fitted values. If the linear model is a correct assumption, Inline graphic will be close to 1; if the quadratic model is a better assumption, the numerator will tend to be larger than the denominator. The Mandel test uses the F-distribution to test for significance: If Inline graphic is less than or equal to the value of the Inline graphic-distribution Inline graphic, it can be rejected that a quadratic model provides a considerably better fit to the measured values (Inline graphic is the number of degrees of freedom of the numerator; Inline graphic of the denominator).

The results of the Mandel test are listed in Table 2. Note that we also corrected the data for non-normality (see [22], p.78ff for details), because the F-test is very sensitive to non-normally distributed data. Since the results of the Mandel test are identical (rejection or not), we will refrain from a detailed explanation for correction of non-normality. The results listed in Table 2 provide evidence that a linear relationship captures the underlying relationship to a reasonable degree for most of the subjects and tasks, consistent with our findings in [9].

Note the difference between Inline graphic and the Mandel test for linearity. Inline graphic indicates the percentage of the variance which can be explained by a (cq.) linear model. Thus, by implication Inline graphic of a quadratic model is never worse than that of a linear one. The Mandel test compares the difference of both model residuals by taking also the statistical degrees of freedom into account and indicates whether the difference is significant. E.g., the amount of data that can be explained by a linear model for the WT of subject S5 is not bad (Inline graphic) and indicates a linear relation, but using a quadratic model is significantly better (Inline graphic).

Discussion and Conclusions

The main result of these experiments is that grip stiffness is regulated independently from grip force, at least to some extent. The conventional assumption that stiffness increases linearly with applied force did hold in all of our experimental conditions, but the parameters of that linear relationship varied according to the task. Mean grip stiffness was considerably higher in FT than in WT, for all subjects of the groups E1 and E2, without any significant differences between early and late trials of single subjects, in much the same way that average grip force varied between static and dynamic grasping of different weights [23], regardless of which task was performed first by each subject. But the slope of the grip stiffness/grip force relationship was also higher for FT. Furthermore, WT stiffness was higher for lower grip forces (compare subjects S1, S3, S4, S6-1, S7 and S10). This is confirmed by the finding that over all subjects of both groups the offsets were significantly higher in the WT (Inline graphic; one-tailed dependent paired t-test; find the corresponding mean and standard deviation in Table 4). Additionally, the WT offsets were significantly higher than zero, while the FT offsets were not. Together with a strong correlation between slope and mean intra-subject stiffness for the FT, these results portend to a change of finger/wrist configuration as the predominant mechanism underlying this change in the stiffness/force relationship, as opposed to a change in the level of cocontraction of antagonistic muscles of the fingers. We tested this hypothesis with a new set of subjects with their wrist fixed by a cuff and rested on a pedestal so as to maintain the same posture at the wrist. We found that both curves matched in terms of stiffness, slope, and offset and for both tasks a strong correlation between mean intra-subject stiffness and slope was observed. Furthermore, we found variances in intra-subject stiffness matched as well, which also argues in favor of data coming from the same population and thus for similar experimental conditions in both tasks. However, even if not for subjects S11 and S13, the measured mean stiffnesses in the FT were still somewhat higher for the group (see Table 6).

The limitations of our experimental conditions in E3 had to be mentioned as well: fixating the wrist and resting it on a pedestal in order to prevent the wrist from bending reduced activation in WT, possibly leading to lower WT stiffnesses. As we initially explained in section “sec:Model”, a change in kinematics possibly predominates effects of cocontraction on the stiffness/force characteristics. Thus, from measurements done within E3 it cannot be excluded, that the difference found in E1 and E2 is a combination of a change in kinematics and cocontraction, with subjects using both strategies in the WT simultaneously.

Furthermore, it should be acknowledged that our results are only valid for expanding objects and thus for the measured stiffness to force characteristics of corresponding musculotendon structure. The measured stiffness to a contracting object might be different and therefore characterizing the reaction as a linear stiffness might not be appropriate. In [10] Van Doren measured grip stiffness by measuring exerted forces of a contracting and expanding handle and used the subtraction of respective forces of both for calculating grip stiffness. But even if the meaning of stiffness measured in this work is different to our estimation, and includes information of an expanding and contracting object (and of reflexes as well), the authors still found a monotonic increase of stiffness with grip force.

Comparing the slopes and the mean stiffness between subjects, one can see that they differed considerably. Some of these large differences in inter-subject stiffness could be explained by a difference in grip force, but certainly not everything. Furthermore, for groups E1 and E2, some of the inter-subject variability can be explained by different wrist positions of the subjects. But even within group E3, where the wrist was fixed in one position, the stiffnesses differed considerably between subjects. Measuring planar human arm stiffness, Mussa-Ivaldi [24] reported that qualitative measures such as shape and orientation of a stiffness ellipse measured at the endpoint are similar over different subjects for different postures, but the quantitative measure size is not, and even varies considerably for identical subjects measured on different days.

The force ranges differed considerably between the two tasks, especially for subjects doing FT first. Since we wanted to exclude the influence of which task is done first, we let the subjects of this group only lift each weight once before the FT in order to avoid learning. This leads to a data set of 6 different force levels, while for subjects doing WT first a data set of 60 data points was used to calculate the force levels. Thus, the force ranges between both tasks differed more for the group doing FT first, leading to a larger force range of FT. However, we expect the influence of this difference to be small. Remember that to test Inline graphic we only compared data within equal force ranges by discarding extreme values (6.6%) of the data set.

A number of studies have already demonstrated how the finger [25] and wrist [16] can affect fingertip forces and stiffness in the human hand and the idea that one might adjust the configuration of a redundant multi-joint linkage to optimize impedance with respect to the task or to the environment [26], [27] has also been proposed. The question which remains to be clarified here is: Are the found differences between the two tasks actively controlled by the CNS? And if yes, why should subjects minimize the influence of the wrist in an FT where they get a visually-presented feedback about variations of the actual grip force?

To maximize the efficacy of the visually-guided control loop, one could reasonably strive to minimize the latency between commanded changes in muscle activations and the actual changes of grip force applied at the fingertips. As muscles are constrained by activation dynamics, there is a theoretical limit to the rate of change of muscle force Inline graphic with respect to time, Inline graphic, that a given muscle can produce. The rate of change of force measured at the fingertip would be modulated by the same Jacobian that governs the relationship between muscle force and finger force, and between muscle stiffness and finger stiffness, i.e., Inline graphic. Thus, by maximizing the norm of Inline graphic, one maximizes the ability to rapidly effectuate a change in grip force in response to a visually presented force error. From this point of view, the modulation of grip stiffness observed in our experiments is simply a corollary of the real optimization, that of maximizing responsivity to a visual command, rather than an optimization of grip impedance per se, to the differences in mechanical constraints between FT and WT. On the other hand, the signal-dependent noise of the corresponding sensors (viz. the Golgi tendon organs) increases with their activation, and will therefore increase its effect on fingertip force. Conclusively, we can only clarify to some extent and not with significance if the found difference is actively optimized or just passively caused by a bent wrist.

All in all, of course static grip stiffness is linearly related to grip force, caused by the exponential stiffness of tendon tissue. But by changing the kinematics of our grip—and thus just changing the force transfer function from muscle to finger—one can actively change the increase of stiffness with force by flexing our wrist and thus change the stability of the grip. In that it is highly relevant that, in our experiments, low stiffness values were obtained when holding objects of different weights rather than exerting a predefined force—a natural task, which we likely have learned to solve at minimal cost.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank the reviewers, whose competency highly improved the quality of this work, for their constructive and helpful suggestions. The authors confirm all the PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials and will make them freely available upon request.

Funding Statement

This work has been partially funded by the European Commission's Seventh Framework Programme as part of the projects STIFF under grant no. (FP7-ICT-231576) and The Hand Embodied under grant no. (FP7-ICT-248587). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Edin BB, Westling G, Johannson RS (1992) Independent control of human finger-tip forces at individual digits during precision lifting. The Journal of Physiology 450: 547–564. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Johannson RS, Westling G (1984) Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory in automatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher or more slippery objects. Experimental Brain Research 56: 550–554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Forssberg H, Eliasson A, Kinoshita H, Johansson R, Westling G (1991) Development of human precision grip. i: Basic coordination of force. Experimental Brain Research 85(2): 451–457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Flanagan JR, Wing AM (1993) Modulation of grip force with load force during point-to-point arm movements. Experimental Brain Research 95: 131–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Flanagan JR, Wing AM (1995) The stability of precision grip forces during cyclic arm movements with a hand-held load. Experimental Brain Research 105: 455–464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Kinoshita H, Kawai S, Ikuta K, Teraoka T (1996) Individual finger forces acting on a grasped object during shaking actions. Ergonomics 39: 243–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Flanagan JR, Tresilian JR (1994) Grip-load force coupling: A general control strategy for transporting objects. The Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 20: 944–957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Zatsiorsky VM, Gao F, Latash ML (2006) Prehension stability: experiments with expanding and contracting handle. J Neurophysiology 95: 2513–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Höppner H, Lakatos D, Urbanek H, Castellini C, van der Smagt P (2011) The grasp perturbator: Calibrating human grasp stiffness during a graded force task. In: Proc. ICRA|International Conference on Robotics and Automation. pp. 3312–3316. doi:10.1109/ICRA.2011.5980217
  • 10. Van Doren C (1998) Grasp stiffness as a function of grasp force and finger span. Motor Control 2: 352–378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Hajian AZ, Howe RD (1997) Identification of the mechanical impedance at the human finger tip. Journal of Biomechanical Engineering 119: 109–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Carter RR, Crago PE, Keith MW (1990) Stiffness regulation by reflex action in the normal human hand. J Neurophysiol 64: 105–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Carter RR, Crago PE, Gorman PH (1993) Nonlinear stretch reflex interaction during cocontraction. J Neurophysiol 69: 943–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Brand P, Hollister A (1999) Clinical Mechancis of the Hand. Mosby, 369 pp. [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Hill AV (1938) The heat of shortening and the dynamic constants of muscle. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B - Biological Sciences 126: 136–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. O'Driscoll SW, Horii E, Ness R, Cahalan TD, Richards RR, et al. (1992) The relationship between wrist position, grasp size, and grip strength. The Journal of Hand Surgery 17: 169–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.White O, Thonnard J, Wing A, Bracewell R, Diedrichsen J, et al. (2011) Grip force regulates hand impedance to optimize object stability in high impact loads. J Neuroscience 189.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Tarkka IM, Larsen TA (1986) Short and long latency reflex responses elicited by electrical and mechanical stimulation in human hand muscle. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica 128: 71–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Allum J, Mauritz K (1984) Compensation for intrinsic muscle stiffness by short-latency reexes in human triceps surae muscles. Journal of Neurophysiology 52: 797–818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Shadmehr R, Arbib M (1992) A mathematical analysis of the force-stiffness characteristics of muscles in control of a single joint system. Biological cybernetics 66: 463–477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Mandel J (1984) The Statistical Analysis of Experimental Data. Courier Dover Publications Inc., 432 pp. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.de Vaus D (2002) Analyzing Social Science Data: 50 Key Problems in Data Analysis. London: Sage Publications Inc., 402 pp. [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Zatsiorsky VM, Gao F, Latash ML (2005) Motor control goes beyond physics: differential effects of gravity and inertia on finger forces during manipulation of hand-held objects. Experimental Brain Research 162: 300–308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Mussa-Ivaldi F, Hogan N, Bizzi E (1985) Neural, mechanical, and geometric factors subserving arm posture in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience 5: 2732–2743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Milner T, Franklin D (1998) Characterization of multijoint finger stiffness: dependence on finger posture and force direction. Biomedical Engineering, IEEE Transactions on 45: 1363–1375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Hogan N (1985) The mechanics of multi-joint posture and movement control. Biological Cybernetics 52: 315–331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Rancourt D, Hogan N (2001) Dynamics of pushing. J Mot Behav 33: 351–362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES