Skip to main content
International Archives of Medicine logoLink to International Archives of Medicine
. 2013 Oct 16;6:39. doi: 10.1186/1755-7682-6-39

Time perception impairs sensory-motor integration in Parkinson’s disease

Marina Lucas 1,5, Fernanda Chaves 1,5, Silmar Teixeira 1,5, Diana Carvalho 1,5, Caroline Peressutti 1,4, Juliana Bittencourt 1, Bruna Velasques 1,4, Manuel Menéndez-González 6, Mauricio Cagy 3, Roberto Piedade 1, Antonio Egidio Nardi 7,8, Sergio Machado 7,8,9,10, Pedro Ribeiro 1,2,4, Oscar Arias-Carrión 11,12,
PMCID: PMC3856585  PMID: 24131660

Abstract

It is well known that perception and estimation of time are fundamental for the relationship between humans and their environment. However, this temporal information processing is inefficient in patients with Parkinson’ disease (PD), resulting in temporal judgment deficits. In general, the pathophysiology of PD has been described as a dysfunction in the basal ganglia, which is a multisensory integration station. Thus, a deficit in the sensorimotor integration process could explain many of the Parkinson symptoms, such as changes in time perception. This physiological distortion may be better understood if we analyze the neurobiological model of interval timing, expressed within the conceptual framework of a traditional information-processing model called “Scalar Expectancy Theory”. Therefore, in this review we discuss the pathophysiology and sensorimotor integration process in PD, the theories and neural basic mechanisms involved in temporal processing, and the main clinical findings about the impact of time perception in PD.

Keywords: Parkinson’s Disease, Sensorimotor integration, Time perception

Introduction

When individuals observe a slow-motion movement, the central nervous system (CNS) speeds up or reduces the time in order to balance the movement feedback and visual perception [1]. In other words, when an individual watches a movie scene in slow-motion for a few seconds, the CNS reorganizes the time estimation according to the normal speed of movement; this leads to the impression of shortening of time [2,3]. This temporal information processing is deficient in PD patients and the range of milliseconds seems to be impaired; this results in temporal judgment deficits [4,5].

Several areas of the cerebral cortex have been associated with time perception (TP), such as prefrontal cortex (PFC), right postcentral gyrus, inferior parietal lobe (IPL), basal ganglia (BD) and cerebellum [6]. In addition to these areas, premotor cortexes (PMC), supplementary motor areas (SMA) and anterior cingulate cortexes (aCC) seem to be more active during situations in which the individual needs to discriminate between two events at different times [7,8]. Other experiments have shown that impairment of the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and basal ganglia (BG) decreases the time perception between the stimuli recognition and the utilization of the right stimulus in different occasions [9,10].

The set of PD symptoms generates a series of physical, psychological and intellectual impairments that compromise the ability to perform daily living activities such as making temporal judgments [11-14]. Furthermore, the neural networks of the human internal clock and temporal memory suffer changes in PD [15,16]. Thus, our review aims to present the Pathophysiology, sensorimotor integration process in PD, the theories and neural basic mechanisms involved in TP, and the main clinical findings about the impact of PD on TP.

Basic mechanisms involved in time perception

The way the human brain encodes and organizes information to generate a temporal response is still poorly understood [17]. Current theories support the hypothesis that human time perception is regulated by a single internal clock which can be switched off or paused [18]. However, the relativity theory indicates that time depends on different factors and its perception is recorded independently by different internal clocks, affected by intrinsic and psychological mechanisms. Considering this, researchers formulated two hypotheses: the first one states that time is independent from internal clocks; the second hypothesis supports the idea that internal clocks use different temporal contexts to judge the duration of a certain event [16,19,20].

Therefore, it seems that temporal processing requires the mutual participation of three neural systems; the first one consists of a “core timer”, the second one is an executive system that processes the stimulus duration, and the third one is an associative system for contextualizing the duration of the stimuli [21,22]. On the other hand, Gibbon et al. [23] presented the Scalar Expectancy Theory, widely used in current research as one of the most accepted models of time processing. This theory consists of an intrinsic pacemaker responsible for producing pulses to estimate the length of time. The CNS compares the different information about time and stores it inside an accumulator that counts the pulses from the pacemaker. If the duration of the signal received by the accumulator becomes meaningful, the stored content is transferred from working to long-term memory [18,24,25].

Another TP theory argues that two parallel systems operate in a temporal integration mechanism [26]; the first system, or “bottom up”, is responsible for synchronizing milliseconds, which are important for motor coordination (cerebellum), the second system, or “top down”, synchronizes seconds and minutes (frontostriatal circuits) [27]. Other experiments show that the CNS first processes time discrimination and judgment and after, it controls time and rhythm perception [28]. In this context, studies have shown activation in certain cortical areas, such as the thalamus, the substantia nigra, striatum, SMA and the prefrontal cortex, during time perception and integration. In particular, the striatum acts as the core timer, controlling the time networks [22]. Studies in rats have shown that the ability to measure the duration of a previously taught task is lost when striatal injury occurs [23,29].

The ability to focus on a particular event, as well as the intuition to anticipate occurrences can promote changes in TP. Studies show the existence of two factors that influence the subjective perception of time. The first factor is the focus, which can accumulate the number of nervous pulses per in a time unit. The second one is the level of arousal, which can increase the number of pulses emitted per time unit [16,30]. As an example, consider the group of sequential numbers (1, 2, 3, 4); the first number is processed with a longer perception than the following one, but when the numbers are shuffled (2, 4, 3, 1), they are all processed with the same duration; i.e., neural networks do not have to anticipate the order, and they have the same degree of neural processing [31]. This fact demonstrates that events requiring more attention and more neural processing lead to longer time perception [32-34].

The variation of our sensations depends on several time mechanisms. When a given temporal or non-temporal event captures our attention, processing mechanisms may go to the various time controllers, causing a decrease in the pulses of the pacemaker. Consequently, there is an underestimation of time, i.e. the individual has the perception of a time shorter than the actual one [35]. For example, the underestimation of time frequently occurs as we remember an enjoyable activity, when time seems to go faster. This happens because we pay less attention to the passing of time due to the fact that the attention is being directed to another activity; this produces a smaller number of pulses counted by the accumulator, and leads to the false impression that time passed quickly [16,36]. Moreover, time may be overrated when we experience a subjective sensation of prolonged time. This occurs when there is an increase in excitation or in attention to time and this causes an acceleration of the pacemaker, which accumulates a greater number of pulses proportionally to the physical time [37].

Pathophysiological mechanisms of PD

Approximately four million people worldwide are affected by Parkinson’s Disease. An increase in the incidence of 1.4% in individuals aged 65–75 and 4.3% in individuals aged over 85 was estimated [38]. In addition, there are reports that family members of PD patients present some olfactory and cognitive deficits. This indicates that such deficits could be a pre-clinical stage of Parkinson’s Disease [39,40]. Though, other studies show that such a relationship does not exist, and argue that occurrence is due to exposure to the same unknown environment [40,41]. However, there are two genetic factors related to cognitive dysfunction in PD: the genes for catechol-O-methyltransferase and microtubule associated tau protein [42].

In general, the pathophysiology of PD has been described as a dysfunction in the BG circuits responsible for failures in cortical and subcortical neural networks [43-47]. Meanwhile, Parkinson’s syndrome can be triggered by a variety of conditions such as exposure to dopamine-blocking agents, exporure to toxins, traumatic brain injury, cerebral infarction, encephalitis, metabolic disorders, among others [48]. In both cases, the BG play an important role in cognitive, executive and movement planning functions [49,50]. They are essential for facilitating voluntary movements and for inhibiting unwanted ones. Therefore, disturbances in BG mechanisms impair all types of motor control [51].

Among the many different causes of impairment in the basal ganglia, the decrease in dopamine release by the substantia nigra to the striatum (putamen) appears to explain movement disorders in PD [9,37,38,52-59]. Two important circuits in the basal ganglia are affected by reduced activity of dopamine-secreting cells in the substantia nigra. The first one is a direct circuit responsible for triggering the movement and for activating the neurons in the thalamocortical route. The second one is an indirect circuit that inhibits the neurons in the path from the thalamus to the cortex [60]. The impairment of these circuits is related to many of the Parkinson symptoms, such as changes in time perception [61]. During disease progression, some exacerbate symptoms and complications develop, such as: falls, postural instability, gait freezing, postural hypotension and severe dementia. Concurrent with the emergence of these events, there is a gradual loss of cognitive ability and use of language, reasoning and memory [62,63].

First movement alterations in people with Parkinson's disease

Bradykinesia was explained initially by James Parkinson, as one of the major clinical symptoms in patients with PD [64]. This term, also called akinesia or hypokinesia, characterizes the difficulty of planning, executing or starting motion and interferes especially with fine motor daily living activities in PD [65,66]. There are some factors that contribute to this phenomenon, such as muscle weakness, stiffness, tremor, thought slowness and motion instability [67]. The cause of muscle weakness still leaves open questions; however, research with electromyography (EMG) in PD showed that the trigger of a voluntary contraction is not organized the same way as in healthy individuals, causing an inability to keep a steady march; moreover, the level of akinesia changes according to the nigrostriatal dopamine decrease. The reason for muscle the relation between muscle rigidity and bradykinesia is not scientifically elucidated, but we know that it consists of a resistance increase throughout the amplitude of the passive movement [66,68]. However, there are reports that PD patients perform the activation of wrist and elbow agonist muscles at the same time that tremor activates, thus reducing the speed of movements [64,69].

Through functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, cortical changes induced by sequential and complex motor tasks were examined in both a control group and a group of akinetic parkinsonians. It was observed that hypoactivity occurred in supplementary motor and dorsolateral areas, inducing akinesia, and hyperactivity occurred in the caudal supplementary motor area, anterior cingulated and lateral premotor areas, sensorimotor activity and parietal cortex [70]. In particular, in monkeys with PD, we find a subcortical negligence, which seems to be consistent with the role of dopaminergic nigrostriatal neurons in regulating movement [71]. Thus, researches for understanding motion changes in individuals with PD have been widely studied. In general, when investigating the difficulties of coordination and visual feedback it was observed that individuals with PD significantly prolonged the time of movement; when the object was not visible, the time to reach the target increased. This suggests that individuals with PD have significant limitations in the adjustment of neural networks when they need to use internal representations, whether to catch an object or to see their hand movement [72]. Furthermore, when an individual with PD performs the task of grasping and lifting objects, the person initially shows deficiencies in strength mechanisms and finger amplitude, then recovered during movement [73].

A study with 14 PD patients was conducted in order to assess the effect of hearing and visual cues on gait initiation, using a rhythmic beep with time intervals and transverse lines on the ground with the maximum length of a step. It was observed that, after calculating the time and distance of the patient’s step through kinematic recordings, visual signals promoted better gait activity than auditory stimuli [74]. Additionally, another study examined the association between the efficiency of visuoperceptual and conceptual processing, measured by repetition priming, and by rate of change of Parkinsonian signs. It was observed that elderly subjects with better visuoperceptual conditions progressed more slowly during a follow-up (i.e., up to 11 years) towards the development of PD. The results also showed that individual differences in visuoperceptual processing efficiency occurred in older people without cognitive impairment, and promoted important changes in motor function. This may be an early sign of vulnerability in the corticostriatal circuit, which contributes to sensorimotor integration [75]. Thus, in patients with PD, the time response for motor initiation increases according to visuospatial difficulty [76,77].

Time perception and sensorimotor integration

While studying PD it is essential to consider the contribution of sensorimotor integration, since this is responsible for the adjustment of the neural networks coordinating motor action [78]. Specifically, kinesthetic awareness, i.e. motion coordination, and proprioceptive information are considerably important for motor control [79,80], especially if the patient is less able to activate the muscle spindles, thus impairing sensorimotor integration [81,82].

Furthermore, some studies investigated the neural response deficits between the BG and both thalamus and supplementary motor area (SMA), which occur because of an unbalance between the direct and indirect circuits, thus increasing the quantity of the striatum stimuli going to the substantia nigra [80,83]. In another experiment using electrical stimulation, the authors analyzed finger movements in order to verify sensorimotor skills in PD patients. They observed that, whenever the BG generated an inappropriate motor command, this resulted in a slower movement performance and in a reduced spatial awareness [81].

Moreover, changes in postural control in individuals with PD are directly related to visuospatial perception, spatial attention and visuomotor control [84-86]. Therefore, a deficit in the process of sensorimotor integration may explain an impaired planning and preparation of motor commands in PD patients. For example, in blind individuals, the CNS seeks to reorganize and hierarchically prioritize sensory inputs as well as promote postural control adjustments to compensate for the visual deficiency. Nevertheless, in patients with PD, this compensatory mechanism is inefficient because the basal ganglia does not influence properly sensorimotor integration [81,87,88]; that is, conflicts in the basal ganglia prevent the final adjustment for appropriate posture control [89,90].

With regards to TP, the most accepted theory (i.e. Scalar Expectancy Theory) is that temporal cognitive information processing occurs in three different stages. First, during the timing phase, an intrinsic pacemaker sends pulses to an accumulator, which counts them. In the second stage, the information collected by the accumulator is sent to the temporary working memory, and finally, in the decision phase the current time is compared with time previously stored in memory to promote an appropriate time to response [91,92].

Other factors such as alertness, attention and intention to perform an action may alter and determine the individual’s perception of time. Attention affects the temporal processing of visual stimuli, accelerating them [93-95], while changes in the timing or memory phase affect the perception of duration and the estimation of the event duration [96]. Thus, in individuals with PD, the temporal deficits seem to be present mainly in perception and temporal processing [5,22]. For example, in a study by Lu et al. [97], a procedure was performed to understand the role of attention in PD. They observed that patients could not properly grasp common objects when receiving visual distracting stimuli, and this may be related to a deficient perception and time processing.

The temporal forecast is a mechanism that allows the individual to use sensory stimuli or motor acts to plan and achieve goals. This control may occur by means of endogenous and exogenous stimuli, which improve the accuracy and speed of the individual [36,98,99]. In this context, PD patients produce a time estimate shorter than healthy individuals. This was observed in the study by Pouthas and Perbal [96], who analyzed patients during a reading task. The results showed that a deficit in dopamine caused a delay in the internal clock and an underestimation of time.

Dusek et al. [100] examined the precuneus of twelve PD patients through functional magnetic resonance imaging (fRMI) while performing a task that consisted of two phases: encoding and reproducing time. The results showed a tendency to overestimate or underestimate intervals, i.e. differences in precuneus activation may be related to some aspects of time perception deficits; this suggests that dopamine may allow compensatory activation of the precuneus and consequently improve the time estimation. In addition to this, Geiser and Kaelin-Lang [101] analyzed the perception of auditory pulses between two groups of subjects with early PD (with and without administration of L-DOPA) and a healthy control group; they found that the group without administration of L-DOPA performed the task as well as the control group. However, the L-DOPA medicated group showed a significantly faster pulse perception. These findings are not in agreement with the study by Harrington et al. [102], probably due to different purposes and methodological aspects. In this case, the authors investigated the neural sources and their treatment response in a task involving TP. The findings indicated that impaired TP in PD was associated with nigrostriatal and mesocortical dysfunction in systems that modulate temporal and non-temporal control (See Table 1).

Table 1.

Summary of studies investigating the impact of time perception on sensorimotor integration in PD

Author n Methods Protocol Results
Sabatini et al. [70]
12
fMRI
Sequential movement with the right hand; induced a clear activation in areas involved in motor execution and programming.
Subcortical putaminal dopamine deficit modifies cortical motor pathways in Parkinson and induces a underactivation of the rostral SMA and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
Nenadic et al. [5]
15
fMRI
Auditory time estimation task and frequency discrimination as an active control task.
Central processing of temporal information associated with basal ganglia activity. Temporal information processing in the brain might thus be a distributed process of interaction between modality-dependent sensory cortical function and attention and memory.
Pastor et al. [103]
14
fMRI
Simple detect the stimulus (control task) or to identify a stimulus property.
Pre-SMA and anterior cingulate were activated. Fronto-medial structures, directly and broadly connected with sensory and motor cortical areas of both hemispheres, as well as subcortical regions.
Jantzen et al. [104]
14
fMRI
Four conditions: synchronized pacing, synchronized continuation, syncopated pacing, and syncopated continuation.
Neural activity underlying continuation does not generalize across all timing contexts but is strongly influenced by the prior pacing context.
Pouthas and Perbal.[96]
46
Temporal accuracy
Temporal tasks, reproduction and production of duration in two conditions: control counting and concurrent reading.
The relationship between time estimation and cognitive functions – processing speed and memory – depend on the context in which duration is evaluated.
Praamtra et al. [99]
10
EEG
Choice reaction task with two different, temporally regular stimulus presentation regimes.
Interval timing for different behaviors relies on qualitatively similar mechanisms implemented in distinct cortical substrates.
Wittmann et al. [105]
13
fMRI
Six blocks of eight preference judgment trials each. On each trial, subjects were presented with a choice between a future and an immediate reward.
The striatum is critical for processing the magnitude of an option’s value over time. Inferior frontal activation pattern was associated with more consistent temporal discounting performance over time.
Lee et al. [45]
21
EEG
Subjects performed a visuospatial memory task.
Patients with Parkinson’s disease showed impairment at filtering out distracters, and they were able to hold fewer items in memory than control subjects. This indicates that the basal ganglia help controlling access to working memory.
Grahn and Brett [106] 30 NART and fMRI. Subjects were tested on a discrimination paradigm. Parkinson patients are specifically less able in processing beat-based sequences compared to non-beat-based ones. The basal ganglia and the medial pre-motor system therefore appear to be necessary for processing rhythms.

Legend: EEG = electroencephalography; fMRI = functional magnetic resonance imaging; MRI = magnetic resonance imaging; NART = National Adult Reading Test.

Conclusion

Time perception is fundamental for the relationship between humans and their environment. Indeed, difficulties in “how to see the world” may characterize certain neurobehavioral clinical conditions such as PD [107]. This pathophysiological distortion may be improved starting from an analysis of the neurobiological model of interval timing, expressed within the conceptual framework of a traditional information-processing model called “Scalar Expectancy Theory”. Furthermore, the process of sensorimotor integration in the posterior parietal cortex is impaired due to a basal ganglia dysfunction, which provokes inadequate sensory stimuli inputs [107]. The growing interest in time perception and in its modification in PD will undoubtedly speed up our understanding of time-related disorders. In addition to possible pharmacological manipulations designed to affect the clock speed, the advancement and implementation of an external therapeutic/educational support facilitating the temporal dimensions of motor behavior may be particularly effective in promoting specific rehabilitation programs.

Competing interests

The authors declared that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

ML, FC, ST, PR and OAC participated in the definition of the study design and the protocol. Authors ML, FC, ST, AEN, SM, PR, and OAC managed the literature searches. Authors ML, FC, ST, SM, PR and OAC wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Contributor Information

Marina Lucas, Email: ninabefortuna@hotmail.com.

Fernanda Chaves, Email: nanda.mgc@gmail.com.

Silmar Teixeira, Email: silmar_teixeira@yahoo.com.br.

Diana Carvalho, Email: dianasilvadecarvalho@gmail.com.

Caroline Peressutti, Email: caroldailha@gmail.com.

Juliana Bittencourt, Email: Juju_bitt@yahoo.com.br.

Bruna Velasques, Email: bruna_velasques@yahoo.com.br.

Manuel Menéndez-González, Email: manuelmenendezgonzalez@gmail.com.

Mauricio Cagy, Email: mauricio.cagy@gmail.com.

Roberto Piedade, Email: rpiedade@ig.com.br.

Antonio Egidio Nardi, Email: antonioenardi@gmail.com.

Sergio Machado, Email: secm80@yahoo.com.br.

Pedro Ribeiro, Email: rpiedade@ig.com.br.

Oscar Arias-Carrión, Email: arias@email.ifc.unam.mx.

Acknowledgements

OAC is supported by Hospital General Dr. Manuel Gea Gonzalez and Hospital General Ajusco Medio.

References

  1. Eagleman DM, Tse PU, Buonomano D, Janssen P, Nobre AC, Holcombe AO. Time and the Brain: How subjective Time Relates to Neural Time. J Neurosci. 2005;25:10369–10371. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3487-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Haggard P, Clark S, Kalogeras J. Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:382–385. doi: 10.1038/nn827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Eagleman DM, Holcombe AO. Causality and the perception of time. Trends Cogn Sci. 2002;6:323–325. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(02)01945-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Rammsayer T, Classen W. Impaired temporal discrimination in Parkinson’s disease: temporal processing of brief durations as an indicator of degeneration of dopaminergic neurons in the basal ganglia. Int J Neurosci. 1997;91:45–55. doi: 10.3109/00207459708986364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Nenadic I, Gaser C, Volz HP, Rammsayer T, Hager F, Sauer H. Processing of temporal information and the basal ganglia: new evidence from FMRI. Exp Brain Res. 2003;148:238–246. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1188-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Matell MS, Meck WH. Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. Cogn Brain Res. 2004;21:139–170. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Hernández A, Zainos A, Romo R. Temporal evolution of a decision-making process in medial premotor cortex. Neuron. 2002;33:959–972. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00613-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Pardo-Vazquez JL, Leboran V, Acuña C. Neural correlates of decisions and their outcomes in the premotor cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:12396–12408. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3396-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Matell MS, Meck WH. Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. Bioessays. 2002;22:94–103. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1521-1878(200001)22:1<94::AID-BIES14>3.0.CO;2-E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Mita A, Mushiake H, Shima K, Matsuzaka Y, Tanki J. Interval time coding by neurons in the presupplementary and supplementary motor areas. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12:502–507. doi: 10.1038/nn.2272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Schrag A, Jahanshahi M, Quinn N. What contributes to quality of life in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2000;69:308–312. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.69.3.308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Jahanshahi M, Jones CR, Zijlmans J, Katsenschlager R, Lee L, Quinn N, Frith CD, Lees AJ. Dopaminergic modulation of striato-frontal connectivity during motor timing in Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2010;133:727–745. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Jahanshahi M, Wilkinson L, Gahir H, Dharminda A, Lagnado DA. Medication impairs probabilistic classification learning in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:1096–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Lustig C, Meck WH. Modality differences in timing and temporal memory throughout the lifespan. Brain Cogn. 2011;77:298–303. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Katai S, Maruyama T, Hashimoto T, Ikeda S. Event based and time based prospective memory in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2003;74:704–709. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.74.6.704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Buhusi CV, Meck WH. Relativity theory and time perception: single or multiple clocks. Rev Plos One. 2009;4:e6268. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Karmarkar UR, Buonomano DV. Timing in the Absence of Clocks: Encoding Time in Neural Network States. Neuron. 2007;53:427–438. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.01.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fortin C, Fairhurst S, Malapani C, Morin C, Towey J, Meck WH. Expectancy in humans in multisecond peak-interval timing with gaps. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2009;71:862–868. doi: 10.3758/APP.71.4.789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Rammsayer T, Ulrich R. Counting models of temporal discrimination. Psychon Bull Rev. 2001;8:270–277. doi: 10.3758/bf03196161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Buonomano D, Karmarkar UR. How do we tell time? Rev The Neuroscientist. 2002;8:42–51. doi: 10.1177/107385840200800109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lewis PA, Miall RC. Remembering the time: a continuous clock. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10:401–406. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Meck WH, Penney TB, Pouthas V. Cortico-striatal representation of time in animals and humans. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008;18:145–152. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gibbon J, Malapani C, Dale CL, Gallistel CR. Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1997;7:170–184. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(97)80005-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Morillon B, Kell CA, Giraud AL. Three Stages and Four Neural Systems in Time Estimation. J Neurosci. 2009;29:14803–14811. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3222-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Allman MJ, Meck WH. Pathophysiological distortions in time perception and timed performance. Brain. 2011;210:2–22. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Madison G. Variability in isochronous tapping: Higher order dependencies as a function of intertap interval. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2001;27:411–422. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.27.2.411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Meck WH. Neuropsychology of timing and time perception. Brain Cog. 2005;58:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Rammsayer TH, Brandler S. Aspects of temporal information processing: A dimensional analysis. Psychol Res. 2004;69:115–123. doi: 10.1007/s00426-003-0164-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hinton SC, Meck WH. Frontal–striatal circuitry activated by human peak-interval timing in the supra-seconds range. Cogn Brain Res. 2004;21:171–182. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Wittman M, Paulus MP. Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;12:7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Ivry RB, Schlerf JE. Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12:273–280. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Tse PU, Intriligator J, Rivest J, Cavanagh P. Attention and the subjective expansion of time. Percept Psychophys. 2004;66:1171–1189. doi: 10.3758/bf03196844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kastner S, Schneider KA, Wunderlich K. Beyond a relay nucleus: neuroimaging views on the human LGN. Prog Brain Res. 2006;155:125–143. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)55008-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kanai R, Watanabe M. Visual onset expands subjective time. Percept Psychophys. 2006;8:1113–1123. doi: 10.3758/bf03193714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Buhusi CV, Meck WH. Time sharing in rats: a peak-interval procedure with gaps and distracters. Rev Elsevier. 2006;71:107–115. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2005.11.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Coull JT, Cheng RK, Meck WH. Neuroanatomical and Neurochemical Substrates of Timing. Rev Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:3–25. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Droid-Volet S, Meck WH. How emotions colour our perception of Time. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;12:504–513. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Morris ME, Watts JJ, Iansek R, Jolley D, Campbell D, Murphy AT, Martin C. Quantifying the profile and progression of impairments, activity, participation, and quality of life in people with Parkinson disease: protocol for a prospective cohort study. BMC Geriatr. 2009;9:2. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-9-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Blekher T, Weaver M, Rupp J, Nichols WC, Hui SL, Gray J, Yee RD, Wojcieszek J, Foroud T. Multiple step pattern as a biomarker in Parkinson disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2009;15:506–510. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2009.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ponsen MM, Stoffers D, Booij J, Van Eck-Smit BL, Wolters ECH, Berendse HW. Idiopathic hyposmia as a preclinical sign of Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol. 2004;56:173–181. doi: 10.1002/ana.20160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Helmchen C, Schwekendiek A, Pramstaller PP, Hedrich K, Klein C, Rambold H. Blink amplitude but not saccadic hypometria indicates carriers of Parkinson mutations. J Neurol. 2006;253:1071–1075. doi: 10.1007/s00415-006-0168-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Williams-Gray C, Evans JR, Goris A, Foltynie T, Ban M, Robbins TW, Brayne C, Kolachana BS, Weinberger DR, Sawcer SJ, Barker RA. The distinct cognitive syndromes of Parkinson’s disease: 5 year follow-up of the Campaign cohort. Brain. 2009;132:2958–2969. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Michell AW, Lewis SJG, Foltynie T, Barker RA. Biomarkers and Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2004;127:1693–1705. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Moore DJ, West AB, Dawson VL, Dawson TM. Molecular pathophysiology of Parkinson's disease. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2005;28:57–87. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lee EY, Cowan N, Vogel EK, Rolan T, Vale-Iclán F, Hackley SA. Visual working memory deficits in patients with Parkinson’s disease are due to both reduced storage capacity and impaired ability to filter out irrelevant information. Brain. 2010;133:2677–2689. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Arias-Carrión O, Stamelou M, Murillo-Rodríguez E, Menéndez-González M, Pöppel E. Dopaminergic reward system: a short integrative review. Int Arch Med. 2010;3:24. doi: 10.1186/1755-7682-3-24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Redgrave P, Rodriguez M, Smith Y, Rodriguez-Oriz MC, Lehericy S, Bergman H, Agid Y, DeLong MR, Obeso JA. Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: implications for Parkinson’s disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:760–772. doi: 10.1038/nrn2915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lang AE. When and how should treatment be started in Parkinson disease? Neurology. 2009;72:39–43. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318198e177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Graybier AM. The basal ganglia. Curr Biol. 2000;10:509–511. [Google Scholar]
  50. Wichmann T, DeLong MR. Pathophysiology of Parkinson’s Disease: The MPTP Primate Model of the Human Disorder. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;991:199–213. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07477.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Mink JW. The basal ganglia and Involuntary Movements. Arch Neurol. 2003;60:1365–1368. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.10.1365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Barker R, Dunnett S. The biology and behaviour of intracerebral adrenal transplants in animals and man. Rev Neurosci. 1993;4:113–146. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.1993.4.2.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Bezard E, Imbert C, Gross CE. Experimental models of Parkinson’s disease: from the static to the dynamic. Rev Neurosci. 1998;9:71–90. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.1998.9.2.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Obeso JA, Olanow CW, Nutt JG. Levodopa motor complications in Parkinson's disease. Trends Neurosci. 2000;23:2–7. doi: 10.1016/s1471-1931(00)00031-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Ramachandran AC, Bartlett LE, Mendez IM. A multiple target neural transplantation strategy for Parkinson’s disease. Rev Neurosci. 2002;13:243–256. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2002.13.3.243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Willis GL, Kennedy GA. The implementation of acute versus chronic animal models for treatment discovery in Parkinson’s disease. Rev Neurosci. 2004;15:75–87. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2004.15.1.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Willis GL. Parkinson’s disease as a neuroendocrine disorder of circadian function: dopamine-melatonin imbalance and the visual system in the genesis and progression of the degenerative process. Rev Neurosci. 2008;19:245–316. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2008.19.4-5.245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Romanos M, Weise D, Schliesser M, Schecklmann M, Löffle RJ, Warnke A, Gerlach M, Classen J, Mehler-Wex C. Structural abnormality of the substantia nigra in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2010;35:55–58. doi: 10.1503/jpn.090044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Vaillancourt DE, Spraker MB, Prodoehl J, Zhou XJ, Little DM. Effects of Aging on the Ventral and Dorsal Substantia Nigra Using Diffusion Tensor Imaging. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;33:35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Widnell K. Pathophysiology of Motor Fluctuations in Parkinson’s Disease. Mov Disord. 2005;20:17–22. doi: 10.1002/mds.20459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lalonde R, Hannequin D. The neurobiological basis of time estimation and temporal order. Rev Neurosci. 1999;10:151–173. doi: 10.1515/revneuro.1999.10.2.151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kummer A, Maia DP, Salgado JV, Cardoso FEC, Teixeira AL. Dopamine dysregulation syndrome in Parkinson's disease. Arq Neuropsiquiatry. 2006;64:1019–1022. doi: 10.1590/s0004-282x2006000600026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Merola A, Zibetti M, Angrisano S, Rizzi L, Ricchi V, Artusi CA, Lanote M, Rizzone MG, Lopiano L. Parkinson’s disease progression at 30 years: a study of subthalamic deep brain-stimulated patients. Brain. 2011;134:2074–2084. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Berardelli A, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, Hallett M. Pathophysiology of bradykinesia in Parkinson’s disease. Cogn Brain Res. 2001;21:171–182. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.11.2131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Giovannoni G, Schalkwyk JV, Fritz VU, Lees AJ. Bradykinesia akinesia incoordination test (BRAIN TEST): an objective computerised assessment of upper limb motor function. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1999;67:624–629. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.67.5.624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Arias-Carrion O, Machado S, Paes F, Velasques B, Teixeira S, Cardenas-Morales L, Piedade R, Ribeiro P, Nardi AE. Is rTMS an effective therapeutic strategy that can be used to treat Parkinson's disease? CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets. 2011;10(6):693–702. doi: 10.2174/187152711797247821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Pinna A, Pontis S, Borsini F, Morelli M. Adenosine A2A Receptor Antagonists Improve Deficits in Initiation of Movement and Sensory Motor Integration in the Unilateral 6-Hydroxydopamine Rat Model of Parkinson’s Disease. Wiley InterScience. 2007;61:606–614. doi: 10.1002/syn.20410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Stamey WP, Jankovic J. Shoulder pain in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. 2007;22:247–258. [Google Scholar]
  69. Deuchl G, Raethjen J, Baron R, Lindemann M, Wilms H, Krack P. The pathophysiology of parkinsonian tremor: a review. J Neurol. 2000;247:33–48. doi: 10.1007/pl00007781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Sabatini U, Boulanouar K, Fabre N, Martin F, Carel C, Colonnese C, Bozzao L, Berry I, Montastruc JL, Rascol O. Cortical motor reorganization in akinetic patients with Parkinson’s disease: a functional MRI study. Brain. 2000;2:394–403. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.2.394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Bankiewicz KS, Oldfield EH, Plunkett RJ, Schette WH, Cogan DG, Hogan N, Zuddas A, Kipon IJ. Apparent unilateral visual neglect in MPTP-hemiparkinsonian monkeys in due to delayed initiation of motion. Brain Res. 1991;541:98–102. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)91080-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Rand MK, Lemay M, Squire LM, Shimansky YP, Stelmach GE. Control of aperture closure initiation during reach-ti-grasp movements under manipulations of visual feedback and trunk involvement in Parkinson’s disease. Exp Brain Res. 2010;201:509–525. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2064-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Muratori LM, Mclsaac TL, Gordon AM, Santello M. Impaired anticipatory control of force sharing patterns during whole-hand grasping in Parkinson’s disease. Exp Brain Res. 2008;185:41–52. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1129-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Jiang Y, Norman KE. Effects of visual and auditory cues on gait initiation in people with Parkinson's disease. Clin Rehabil. 2006;20(1):36–45. doi: 10.1191/0269215506cr925oa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Fleischman DA, Buchman AS, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Visuoperceptual repetition priming and progression of parkinsonian signs in aging. Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30:441–449. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Daum I, Quinn N. Reaction times and visuospatial processing in parkinson’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1991;13:972–982. doi: 10.1080/01688639108405112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Müller C, Wenger S, Fertl L, Auff E. Initiation of visual-guided random saccades and remembered saccades in parkinsonian patients with severe motor-fluctuations. J Neural Transm Park Dis Dement Sect. 1994;7:101–108. doi: 10.1007/BF02260964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Machado S, Cunha M, Velasques B, Minc D, Teixeira S, Domingues CA, Silva JG, Bastos VH, Budde H, Cagy M, Basile L, Piedade R, Ribeiro P. Sensorimotor integration: basic concepts, abnormalities related to movement disorders and sensorimotor training-induced cortical reorganization. Rev Neurol. 2010;51:427–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Butler AJ, Fink GR, Dohle C, Wunderlich G, Tellmann L, Seitz RJ, Zilles K, Freund HJ. Neuralmechanisms underlying reaching for remembered targets cued kinesthetically or visually in left or right hemispace. Hum Brain Mapp. 2004;21:165–177. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Konczak J, Corcos DM, Horak F, Poizner H, Shapiro M, Tuite P, Volkmann J, Maschke M. Proprioception and Motor Control in Parkinson’s Disease. J Mot Behav. 2009;41:543–552. doi: 10.3200/35-09-002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Tamburim S, FiaschI A, Idone D, Lochner P, Manganotti P, Zanette G. Abnormal Sensorimotor Integration Is Related to Disease Severity in Parkinson’s Disease: A TMS Study. Mov Disord. 2003;18:1316–1324. doi: 10.1002/mds.10515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Maschke M, Gomez CM, Tuite PJ, Konczak J. Dysfunction of the basal ganglia, but not the cerebellum, impairs kinaesthesia. Brain. 2003;126:2312–2322. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Labyt E, Devos D, Bourriez JL, Cassim F, Destée A, Guieu JD, Defebvre L, Derambure P. Motor preparation is more impaired in Parkinson’s disease when sensorimotor integration is involved. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003;114:2423–2433. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(03)00278-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Adamovich SV, Berkinblit MB, Hening W, Sage J, Poizner H. The interaction of visual and proprioceptive inputs in pointing to actual and remembered targets in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience. 2001;104:1027–1041. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(01)00099-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Uc EY, Rizzo M, Anderson SW, Qian S, Rodnitzky RL, Dawson JD. Visual dysfunction in Parkinson disease without dementia. Neurology. 2005;65:1907–1913. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000191565.11065.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Beudel M, Galama S, Leenders KL, Jong BM. Time estimation in Parkinson’s disease and degenerative cerebellar disease. Cognitive Neuroscience Neuropsychology. 2009;19:1055–1058. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328303b7b9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. O’shea S, Morris ME, Iansek R. Dual Task Interference During Gait in People With Parkinson Disease: Effects of Motor Versus Cognitive Secondary Tasks. Phys Ther. 2002;82:888–897. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Tagliabue M, Ferrigno G, Horak FB. Effects of Parkinson’s disease on proprioceptive control of posture and reaching while standing. Neuroscience. 2009;158:1206–1214. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Brown LA, Cooper SA, Doam JB, Dickin DC, Whishaw IQ, Pellis SM, Suchowersky O. Parkinsonian deficits in sensory integration for postural control: Temporal response to changes in visual input. Parkinsonim Relat Disord. 2006;12:376–381. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2006.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Cameron M, Horak FB, Herndon RR. Imbalance in multiple sclerosis: A result of slowed spinal somatosensory conduction. Somatosensory Motor Res. 2009;25:113–122. doi: 10.1080/08990220802131127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Meck WH. Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 1996;3:227–242. doi: 10.1016/0926-6410(96)00009-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Pouthas V, George N, Poline JB, Pfeuty M, Vandemoorteele PF, Hugueville L, Ferrandez AM, Lehéricy S, Lebihan D, Renault B. Neural Network Involved in Time Perception: An fMRI Study Comparing Long and Short Interval Estimation. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25:433–441. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Nobre AC, O’Reilly J. Time is of the essence. Cognitive Sciences. 2004;8:387–389. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Hogendoorn H, Carlson TA, Verstraten FAJ. The time course of attentive tracking. J Vis. 2007;7:1–10. doi: 10.1167/7.14.2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Cicchini GM, Morrone MC. Shifts in spatial attention affect the perceived duration of events. J Vis. 2009;9:1–13. doi: 10.1167/9.1.9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Pouthas V, Perbal S. Time perception depends on accurate clock mechanisms as well as unimpaired attention and memory processes. Acta Neurobiol Exp. 2004;64:367–385. doi: 10.55782/ane-2004-1520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Lu C, Bharmal A, Kiss ZH, Suchowersky O, Haffenden AM. Attentional and reach-to-grasp movements in Parkinson’s disease. Exp Brain Res. 2010;1:69–80. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2341-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Correa A, Lupiane ZJ, Tudela P. Attentional preparation based on temporal expectancy modulates processing at the perceptual level. Psychon Bull Rev. 2005;5:328–334. doi: 10.3758/bf03196380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Praamstra P, Kourtis D, Kwok HF, Oostenveld R. Neurophysiology of implicit timing in serial choice reaction-time performance. J Neurosci. 2006;26:5448–5455. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0440-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Dušek P, Jech R, Sieger T, Vymazal J, Růžička E, Wackermann J, Mueller K. Abnormal activity in the precuneus during time perception in Parkinson's disease: an fMRI study. PLoS One. 2012;7(1):e29635. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Geiser E, Kaelin-Lang A. The function of dopaminergic neural signal transmission in auditory pulse perception: evidence from dopaminergic treatment in Parkinson's patients. Behav Brain Res. 2011;225(1):270–275. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.07.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Harrington DL, Castillo GN, Greenberg PA, Song DD, Lessig S, Lee RR, Rao SM. Neurobehavioral mechanisms of temporal processing deficits in Parkinson’s disease. PLoS One. 2011;6:e17461. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Pastor MA, Day BL, Macaluso E, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. The functional neuroanatomy of temporal discrimination. J Neurosci. 2004;24(10):2585–2591. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4210-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Jantzen KJ, Steinberg FL, Kelso JA. Brain networks underlying human timing behavior are influenced by prior context. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101(17):6815–6820. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0401300101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Wittman M, Leland DS, Paulus MP. Time and decision making: deferential contribution of the posterior insular cortex and the striatum during a delay discounting task. Exp Brain Res. 2007;179:643–653. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0822-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Grahn JA, Brett M. Impairment of beat-based rhythm discrimination in Parkinson's disease. Cortex. 2009;45(1):54–61. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Teixeira S, Machado S, Paes F, Velasques B, Silva JG, Sanfim AL, Minc D, Anghinah R, Menegaldo LL, Salama M, Cagy M, Nardi AE, Pöppel E, Bao Y, Szelag E, Ribeiro P, Arias-Carrión O. Time perception distortion in neuropsychiatric and neurological disorders. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets. 2013;12(5):567–582. doi: 10.2174/18715273113129990080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from International Archives of Medicine are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES