Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Pediatr Diabetes. 2013 Oct 14;14(8):10.1111/pedi.12088. doi: 10.1111/pedi.12088

Glycemic extremes in youth with T1DM: Effects on the developing brain's structural and functional integrity

Ana Maria Arbelaez 1, Katherine Semenkovich 1, Tamara Hershey 2,3,4
PMCID: PMC3857606  NIHMSID: NIHMS529182  PMID: 24119040

Abstract

Hypoglycemia and hyperglycemia are rare occurrences in normal individuals, but they occur commonly in patients with type 1 diabetes mellitus (T1DM) due to a dysfunction of peripheral glucose-insulin-glucagon responses and non-physiologic doses of exogenous insulin, which imperfectly mimic normal physiology. These extremes can occur more frequently in children and adolescents, due to events leading to the diagnosis of T1DM (prolonged untreated hyperglycemia, diabetic ketoacidosis), and to behavioral factors interfering with optimal treatment. Given that these events are occurring at a time of dynamic changes in brain structure and metabolic demand, it has been hypothesized that glycemic extremes could alter normal brain developmental trajectories depending on the age and severity at which these extremes are experienced. Researchers have attempted to determine the impact of diabetes, hypoglycemia, and hyperglycemia on brain structure and function in vivo using various neuroimaging techniques although few studies have examined these questions in children and youth with T1DM within a neurodevelopmental context. In this article, we review the current neuroimaging techniques available, describe the limited data in youth that address whether exposure to glycemic extremes in T1DM has any long-lasting consequences on the brain and its functions within a developmental perspective, and provide commentary on future directions for this field.

Keywords: brain, type 1 diabetes mellitus, children, neuroimaging, cognition


The adult brain accounts for a disproportionally large percentage of the body's total energy consumption (1). However, during brain development, energy demand is even higher, reaching the adult rate by age 2 and increasing to nearly twice the adult rate by age 10, followed by gradual reduction towards adult levels in the next decade (1-2). The dramatic changes in brain metabolism occurring over the first two decades of life coincide with the initial proliferation and then pruning of synapses to adult levels. The brain derives its energy almost exclusively from glucose and is largely driven by neuronal signaling, biosynthesis and neuroprotection (3-6). Glucose homeostasis in the body is tightly regulated by a series of hormones and physiologic responses. As a result, hypoglycemia and hyperglycemia are rare occurrences in normal individuals, but they occur commonly in patients with type 1 diabetes mellitus (T1DM) due to a dysfunction of peripheral glucose-insulin-glucagon responses and non-physiologic doses of exogenous insulin, which imperfectly mimic normal physiology. These extremes can occur more frequently in children and adolescents with T1DM due to the inadequacies of insulin replacement therapy, events leading to the diagnosis (prolonged untreated hyperglycemia, diabetic ketoacidosis), and to behavioral factors interfering with optimal treatment. When faced with fluctuations in glucose supply the metabolism of the body and brain change dramatically, largely to conserve resources and, at a cost to other organs, to preserve brain function (7). However, if the normal physiological mechanisms that prevent these severe glucose fluctuations and maintain homeostasis are impaired, neuronal function and potentially viability can be affected (8-11).

Given that these events are occurring at a time of dynamic changes in brain structure and metabolic demand, it has been hypothesized that exposure to glycemic extremes during childhood could alter normal brain developmental trajectories (12) depending on the age and severity at which these extremes are experienced (13). Data suggest that early exposure to T1DM, and specifically to severe hypoglycemia, may lead to subtle differences in cognitive and academic function (14-26). However, our understanding of the neurobiological underpinnings of these cognitive differences in youth with T1DM is very limited. Researchers have attempted to determine the impact of diabetes, hypoglycemia, and hyperglycemia on brain structure and function in vivo using various neuroimaging techniques. This literature has been reviewed extensively in adults (27-29). However, despite advances in neuroimaging techniques amenable to studying children, relatively few studies using these techniques have been conducted in youth with T1DM and thus few reviews have examined these questions within a neurodevelopmental context (14,30) (Table 1). In this article, we review the current neuroimaging techniques available, describe the limited data in youth that address whether exposure to glycemic extremes in T1DM has any long-lasting consequences on the brain and its functions, and provide commentary on future directions for this field.

Table 1.

Summary of published neuroimaging papers examining the relationship between a history of severe hypoglycemia and hyperglycemia exposure and brain structure and function in youth with type 1 diabetes. (No evidence available about DKA).

First Author Year Groups N Age Range (years) Neuroimaging variables examined Effects of past severe hypoglycemia Effects of past hyperglycemia
*Perantie (26) 2007 T1DM Controls 108
51
7-17 Voxel-wise gray and white matter volume; multiple comparison correction Greater number of severe hypoglycemic episodes related to reduced volume in left superior temporal region Weighted A1c associated with reduced volume in cuneus, precuneus; larger gray matter in PFC
Ho (73) 2008 T1DM 62 Mean age = 10 Clinical reading, hippocampal volume, total gray, white matter volume Hypoglycemia seizure history associated with lower total gray matter Hyperglycemia not analyzed
Northam (63) 2009 T1DM Controls 79
51
Mean age= 20 Voxel-wise gray and white matter volume and MRS Reduced thalamic gray matter volume Higher glucose level was associated with elevated myoinositol and decreased T2 in thalamus
*Hershey (68) 2010 T1DM Controls 95
49
7-17 Hippocampal, whole brain volumes Greater number of severe episodes related to larger hippocampal volumes No relationship to weighted A1c
+Aye (67) 2011 T1DM Controls 27
18
3-10 Whole brain gray and white matter volume; hippocampal volume Smaller whole brain gray and white matter volume in those with hypo seizures No relationship to weighted A1c
*Perantie (62) 2011 T1DM Controls 75
25
7-17 Voxel-wise gray and white matter volume; multiple comparison correction; Prospective analysis Presence of severe hypo associated with reduced white matter volume over time in occipital-parietal cortex Weighted A1c related to greater decrease in whole brain gray matter over time
+Aye (74) 2012 T1DM Controls 22
14
3-10 Diffusion tensor imaging; voxel-wise; multiple comparison correction None Weighted A1c correlated with radial diffusivity.
*Antenor-Dorsey (87) 2013 T1DM Controls 73
30
9-22 Diffusion tensor imaging; region of interest & voxel-wise; multiple comparison correction None No relationship with weighted A1c. DKA episodes included in severe hyperglycemia category. Greater number of severe hyperglycemic episodes related to lower white matter microstructural integrity in superior parietal lobule, hippocampus.

Overlapping cohort from Hershey Lab at Washington University

+Overlapping cohort from the Aye Lab at Stanford University

Brain Development

Brain synaptic development and metabolic demands vary throughout childhood and continue into young adulthood. Synaptic density increases rapidly during early postnatal cortical development, followed by a discrete period of synaptic pruning that typically occurs during adolescence (31-38). Synaptic and spine numbers subsequently stabilize during adulthood. Brain glucose metabolism (39) and oxygen consumption (40-41) follow a similar course with some regional variability (39), presumably reflecting associated changes in synaptic density and proliferation. From 2–3 months of age until 4 years of age, glucose use in the brain increases dramatically, until it is double the rate of the adult brain by the end of the first decade of life. It then begins to decline and reaches adult levels in the third decade of life, where it accounts for approximately 20% of the energy consumed in the body despite representing only 2% of the body weight (2).

In addition to changes in brain metabolism during development, there is a complex array of structural changes in the normally developing brain. Between the ages of 1-6 years old total brain volume increases, and by the age of six, it reaches about 90 percent of the adult brain volume (42-44). However, total gray matter volume, cortical volume and average cortical thickness increase in childhood up to ages 9-11, and then begin to decrease over the rest of childhood and adolescence as synaptic pruning occurs. In contrast, myelination of white matter (as reflected in white matter volume) increases gradually throughout childhood, adolescence and even early adulthood (44) (Figure 1). In addition, within gray white matter, there is regional and sex-related variability in these developmental trajectories (45-46).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Schematic representation of the known normal trajectory of whole brain glucose consumption, and whole brain white and gray matter volume during childhood and adolescence. These trajectories are dynamic during the time frame of exposure to many type 1 diabetes-related glycemic extremes.

Many critical events in the evolution of T1DM disease co-occur with these dynamic developmental trajectories (Figure 1). T1DM can be diagnosed at any age, but incidence rates generally increase with age until mid-puberty and then decline (47). Similarly, glycemic extremes in T1DM can occur at any age, but a period of untreated hyperglycemia occurs prior to diagnosis and its risk is also higher during adolescence; while severe hypoglycemia is more likely to occur in younger children with T1DM or in those with tighter glycemic control. The co-occurrence of these events with the different neurodevelopmental processes may interact to produce either qualitatively or quantitatively different effects. This question has been discussed much in the T1DM literature, but few articles have directly addressed the issue (14,48-49). If there were critical developmental time periods when the brain was particularly vulnerable to severe hypoglycemia or hyperglycemia, then perhaps a rational argument for periods of tighter or looser control during some ages but not others could be made and outcomes could be improved.

Mechanisms of neuronal damage due to glycemic changes

Various mechanisms of how glycemic extremes might induce neuronal or axonal injury or dysfunction have been proposed (8-11). Neurons have a constantly high glucose demand. However, glucose uptake in neurons appears to be independent of insulin action. Thus, neuronal glucose uptake depends on the extracellular concentration of glucose (50-51), making them unable to efficiently down-regulate glucose transport inside the cell. This confers a higher risk for toxic effects in neurons vs. other cells from exposure to severe glycemic extremes and acute or chronic glycemic fluctuations (10-11). Glucose neurotoxicity could be induced through oxidative stress, which is promoted by glucose through a combination of free-radical generation and impaired free-radical scavenging ability (11). Induction of oxidative stress can generate more reactive molecules from glucose that combine with cellular and extracellular macromolecules, altering their functional properties and causing downstream adverse effects on cell function, which all lead to mitochondrial dysfunction and cell damage. Hypoglycemia has also been linked to overstimulation of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors during episodes, resulting in excitotoxicity and cell death (21,52), although apoptotic mechanism have also been proposed (8-9,53). Furthermore T1DM is also associated with fluctuations of a number of endocrine factors (insulin, c-peptide, cortisol, IGF-1) and paracrine factors (ketone bodies, lactate), which may have complex effects on the brain development of children with T1DM (54-56).

Assessing the impact of the glycemic extremes in the developing brain

Assessing the impact of the glycemic extremes in the developing brain can be difficult due to the inherent glucose variability of the disease but more so due to the inability to precisely quantify the degree of exposure to these glycemic extremes. For example, the definition and severity of hypoglycemia is often disputed, inconsistent between studies and can be hard to confirm retrospectively. There are no laboratory tests to quantify past exposure, clinical recall is imperfect, and there can be unrecognized and unrecorded episodes, particularly when there is hypoglycemia unawareness from recurrent hypoglycemia and in young prepubertal children asymptomatic nocturnal hypoglycemic episodes are common (57-59). Furthermore, within groups of well-controlled patients with T1DM, the frequency of severe hypoglycemia events can be bimodal, with a large proportion reporting no previous events, and a few individuals having a large number of events. Severe acute hyperglycemia (with or without diabetic ketoacidosis) can also be difficult to characterize, unless patients were hospitalized and full medical records are available. Chronic hyperglycemia exposure can be more easily captured through HbA1c levels, but only if they were routinely and reliably captured over the entire duration of diabetes. Researchers have attempted to weight degree of hyperglycemia experienced by the length of time it was experienced in order to get at a dose response/exposure score in different ways. Others have focused on variability of glucose levels over time, either from A1c levels or from short term continuous glucose monitoring. All of these efforts will at best be estimates of true exposure until affordable, reliable and non-invasive continuous blood glucose monitoring from diagnosis on is widely used. Nevertheless, some progress has been made in understanding the impact of these more traditional measures of glycemic extreme exposure on the brain through neuroimaging.

Modern neuroimaging techniques allow us to explore alterations in the brain's macrostructure ((e.g. regional volumes and shapes) using standard magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)), microstructural integrity of white matter (using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI)), and the brain's activity at rest or in reaction to a specific challenge (using positron emission tomography (PET) and functional MRI (fMRI)) (60-61) presumably accumulated over time and after repeated exposure to extreme glycemic states. All these techniques rely on multiple assumptions and require careful and skillful interpretation of their findings to avoid false positive findings (type 1 error). Although these techniques can answer questions about how brain structure and function are affected by certain diseases or conditions, they cannot directly address the mechanisms by which these changes are produced and each has their own strengths and weaknesses. Given that MRI only requires a relatively short amount of time to acquire, uses no radiation or contrast agent, and has no known risks for human health it has been most widely used for studying children with T1DM. In contrast, PET is not appropriate for studying children due to inherent radiation exposure. Thus, assessment of the effects of glycemic extremes in the brain of children and adolescents with T1DM is restricted to measurements based on MRI techniques.

Glycemic Effects on Regional Brain Volumes

Structural MRI techniques allow for the measurement of whole brain and regional white and grey matter volumes. These volumes can be compared across groups and correlated with age or critical clinical variables (e.g. exposure to hypoglycemia or hyperglycemia). Studies have found lower regional gray matter volumes in children with T1DM compared to healthy controls (49,62-63) as well as a decrease in T2 relaxation time in subjects with T1DM, which was interpreted as being characteristic of normal aging and suggesting accelerated brain aging of T1DM children (49). However, only one study is truly prospective (62) and none have specifically examined the timing of exposure to glycemic extremes during development. Thus our understanding of the impact of T1DM, hypoglycemia and hyperglycemia on the dynamic developmental trajectory of gray and white matter volumes is still relatively limited.

Early MRI studies hypothesized that severe hypoglycemia might negatively affect hippocampal volumes in children with T1DM, based on case studies and animal models (8,64-66). One study found no differences in hippocampal volumes between those children with T1DM and a past history of hypoglycemic seizures and those with T1DM but without a hypoglycemic seizure history (67), and one found enlarged hippocampal volumes in those with three or more past severe hypoglycemic episodes (68). This latter study suggested that the hippocampus might react to hypoglycemia in a way that results in greater gray matter volumes (e.g. via reactive neurogenesis, gliosis, loss of synaptic pruning, compensation). However, the consensus from this limited body of work is that the hippocampus does not suffer long-term atrophic changes due to severe hypoglycemic events during development. Volumetric differences in other brain regions that are known to be acutely hypoglycemia sensitive (69-72), such as the thalamus and medial prefrontal cortex, have not been explicitly studied in youth with T1DM.

More recent work has taken a whole brain approach, searching the entire brain for hypoglycemia-related volumetric differences in youth with T1DM. One retrospective analysis found lower gray matter volume in the left superior temporal cortex (26), one found lower thalamic gray matter (63), one found lower whole brain gray volume (73) in T1DM youth who had experienced severe hypoglycemia in the past compared to those who had not. Another cross-sectional study found decreased whole brain gray and white matter volume in children with T1DM who experienced hypoglycemic seizures (67). The single prospective study in this field found that severe hypoglycemia experienced between scans was related to reduced white matter volume growth in the parietal occipital cortex over a 2 year time period (62). Clearly, there is a lack of consistent findings of the impact of previous severe hypoglycemia on regional or whole brain volumes. This could be due to differences across studies in the ages studied, definitions of severe hypoglycemia, sample sizes, analytical approaches, or a failure to account for the ages at which severe hypoglycemia was experienced. It also indicates that, even if there truly are long-lasting effects of severe hypoglycemia on the macrostructural features of the developing brain, these are likely subtle.

Given that chronic hyperglycemia is a default state of T1DM and the most concerning for long-term consequences in other organ systems, its effects on the developing and aging brain have been of great interest. Degree of chronic hyperglycemia has been associated with both gray and white matter volume differences in youth with T1DM. A retrospective, whole brain analysis found that greater prior hyperglycemia was related to smaller right cuneus and precuneus cortical gray matter volume and smaller white matter volume in the right superior parietal region. Subthreshold results were also found in the homologous left sided regions in these areas (26). Both regions are adjacent to each other and reside in the parietal/occipital cortex. Replication of these findings in other samples of youth has been limited, although similar regional cortical findings also have been reported in adults with T1DM. One group of investigators found an association between decreased gray matter in the right cuneus and higher lifetime HbA1c averages (75-76) and another study found reduced gray matter density in the right occipital lobe in diabetic adults with retinopathy compared to healthy controls (77). The consistent effects of hyperglycemia on the macrostructure of the precuneus/cuneus region may suggest special vulnerability of this region to the neurotoxic effects of chronic hyperglycemia, particularly given that this region is highly glycolytic at rest (78).

More extreme hyperglycemia that includes diabetic ketoacidosis (DKA), which has been associated with subclinical cerebral edema (79-80), has not been analyzed separately in the existing volumetric MRI studies of youth with T1DM, perhaps due to difficulty in documenting and defining severity, or lack of exposure in those samples. However, neuroimaging studies in children with DKA have documented narrowing of the lateral ventricles in greater than 50% of children during DKA treatment (81), despite only mild mental status changes in these children. This supports a much greater incidence of subclinical cerebral edema in pediatric patients with DKA than was previously suspected.

Glycemic Effects on White Matter Integrity

Early neuroimaging studies of white matter typically used qualitative measures of white-matter hyperintensities. However, those methods are limited in sensitivity and the scope of white matter effects they can detect. Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) provides a quantitative assessment of white matter integrity through the measurement of diffusion of water molecules in brain tissue. In white matter fibers, this diffusion is constrained and thus directional; when degraded, diffusion is less constrained and more random (82-83). DTI is sensitive to subtle white matter brain injury in humans (84) and can detect changes even when standard T2-weighted images appear normal and the white matter region volumes are similar (85). In addition, DTI measures have been directly validated by means of comparison with immunohistochemical indicators of axonal injury in animal models (86). Just like brain volumes, white matter properties change dynamically during normal development and these processes need to be considered in group and individual level analyses.

Although the effects of severe hypoglycemia on white matter integrity have been examined in adults with T1DM, the issue has not been fully explored in children with T1DM. One retrospective analysis found no significant effects of severe hypoglycemia on white matter integrity across the brain in T1DM youth (87). As noted above, a prospective study found that exposure to severe hypoglycemia inhibited white matter volume development (62). Such changes could indicate that white matter integrity might also be affected over time, however, further prospective research using DTI is needed to test this hypothesis.

Given the impact of hyperglycemia on peripheral axonal integrity (88), it has been hypothesized that white matter integrity in the brain might also be affected. Many studies in adults with T1DM have explored this possibility, but to date there only two published studies in youth with T1DM. Exposure to chronic hyperglycemia, weighted by degree and duration (74) and repeated severe hyperglycemic episodes (most involving DKA) (87); were both associated with lower white matter structural integrity in and near the precuneus cortex. This study also found altered DTI parameters in the hippocampus and thalamus associated with severe hyperglycemia (87). This latter finding is notable because alterations in the thalamus have also been associated with T1DM and glycemic exposure in other imaging and neuropathological studies of adults (75,89-90) and youth (63). Specifically, DKA has been reported to acutely affect the diffusivity of the thalamus in a pediatric population (91-92). Importantly, severe hyperglycemia and DKA can affect water accumulation in the brain (edema) leading to altered DTI parameters which tend to normalize once DKA has resolved (93). Our findings suggest that the thalamus may exhibit long-term consequences of diabetes-related complications, such as multiple severe hyperglycemic episodes (87). Differences in DTI parameters in gray matter regions could reflect either direct pathologic damage or secondary degeneration due to the disruption of white matter tracts linking these structures to other areas (94).

Effects on Brain Metabolite Concentrations

Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy (MRS) is similar to MRI in that it also depends on the signaling of hydrogen protons. However, while MRI uses the signaling of hydrogen protons to construct structural images of the brain, MRS uses such signaling to determine the concentration of brain metabolites in certain areas of the body. MRS examines concentration of metabolites including N-acetyl aspartate (NAA), lactate, choline (Cho), and Creatine (Cr). Increases or decreases in metabolites in the brain can signal various phenomena, such as neuronal integrity. Even though there are many benefits of MRS, such as the fact that it can be performed quickly and is low in cost, MRS also has limitations. One limitation is that it is not very specific; many artifacts appear on MRS scans performed on tissue close to bone or air, which makes obtaining images of brain regions close to areas such as the base of the skull challenging (95). In addition, MRS can only sample one or two broad regions of the brain at a time and thus cannot perform whole brain analyses.

Not many studies have used this imaging method in children with T1DM. However some have explored if differences in the metabolic milieu exist between T1DM and controls. One study found that children with T1DM had a lower NAA/Cr and Cho/Cr ratio in the pons and a lower NAA/Cr ratio in the posterior parietal white matter compared to healthy controls (96). Decreases in NAA metabolites may indicate a loss of neurons, while a decrease in Cho may be due to a change in membrane lipids or a decrease in membrane turnover. However, they did not find a significant association between hyperglycemia or the number of previous hypoglycemic episodes and the ratio of metabolite concentrations in the brain (96). Others explored if there are regional differences between brain metabolites of children with T1DM compared to healthy control children 12 years after diagnosis (63), and found a decrease in NAA in the frontal lobes and basal ganglia, and greater Cho in the temporal and frontal lobes and basal ganglia compared to controls. Suggesting a loss of neuronal cells in the regions of the frontal lobe and basal ganglia as well as an increase in membrane turnover in the temporal, frontal lobes and basal ganglia. Others have used MRS to examine the impact of acute DKA on metabolites and chart recovery after treatment as well (97-98). One study found that during treatment for DKA there was a lower NAA/Cr ratio in periaqueductal gray matter and occipital gray matter compared to the recovery phase (98). It was not possible to compare baseline (pre DKA) to post-DKA scans, thus it is unclear if there are any long-term consequences of DKA (or chronic hyperglycemia) on MRS measured metabolites.

Effects on Brain Blood Flow

Changes in local blood flow (measured directly with PET or indirectly with Arterial Spin Labeling (ASL-fMRI)) can provide a surrogate marker of global or regional neuronal activation during different cognitive tasks, pharmacologic challenges, or glycemic extremes such as hypoglycemia (69-71), hyperglycemia or hypoglycemia associated autonomic failure (HAAF) (99). An increase in regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) is thought to reflect increases in neuronal activity primarily in the terminal fields (100), due to increases in synaptic metabolic activity, but not in the spike firing rate. Interpretation of these changes depends on whether there are inhibitory or excitatory (101) input pathways to the region, as increases in both can cause increased blood flow (102). Furthermore, they could reflect dysfunction of a particular brain region or network that is not necessarily visible as a structural defect. When interpreting these data, it must be determined whether the challenge given (e.g. drug, hypoglycemia) changes whole brain blood flow, so that relative regional blood flow changes can be properly normalized. PET methods unfortunately are invasive and require the use of radioactivity, which makes them inappropriate for use in children and adolescents. ASL-MRI, in contrast, is non-invasive, does not require radiation, and can be repeatedly performed on the same subject; therefore it can be used in children. We found comparable blood flow estimates during hypoglycemia using ASL-MRI and PET, demonstrating that such methods are likely replicable (70). However, ASL requires a very high signal-to noise ratio and cannot accurately map either low (10 mL/100g/minute) or high (150 mL/100 g/minute) rCBF states. To our knowledge, this technique has not yet been used in studies of children with T1DM.

Effects on Brain Metabolism

When a brain region is active in performance of a task, or in response to a physiologic change such as hypoglycemia, there will be a change in metabolism in the neurons in that region, which can be imaged by PET using radioactively labeled glucose analogues. These analogues are taken up by cells as native glucose but are either arrested early in the intracellular metabolic pathway and retained within the cells for longer (if using fluorodeoxy glucose (FDG)) or incorporated into the glucose metabolism pathways (if using 1-C11 labeled glucose). The tracer uptake can be modeled mathematically to derive measures of glucose uptake, content, and cerebral metabolic rate (103-105). Although these are great methods to assess mechanistic questions related to diabetes pathophysiology, their use is limited to adults with intact renal function due to the exposure to radioactivity, and invasiveness of the studies.

Effects on Brain Function and Functional Connectivity

Functional MRI allows us to measure the brain's blood flow response to different tasks, and its resting state functional organization. Task based studies must consider changes in the subjects’ behavior across stimulation conditions as a potential confounder when interpreting findings. If the goal of a study is to assess the neuropathophysiology of a condition such as hypoglycemia, changes in behavior within the scanner across conditions or groups could make such conclusions difficult. For example, if a subject performs differently on a cognitive challenge while hypoglycemic compared to while euglycemic, differences in brain activation patterns between the two conditions could have little to do with the direct action of hypoglycemia on brain pathways, but instead could merely reflect the effects of poor performance or difficulty level on brain activity. Functional connectivity analyses (based on resting state blood oxygen level-dependence (BOLD)) avoid this potential performance confound. Furthermore, resting-state fMRI BOLD signal might relate to the underlying metabolism of the brain and, in particular, glycolysis. A regional increase in metabolism (particularly in glycolysis) can significantly exceed the change in oxygen consumption (106). This change in oxygenation status can be captured using BOLD MRI, which depends on the difference in magnetic signal generated by deoxyhemoglobin vs. hemoglobin. Functional networks and BOLD signal can be altered by disease states, including T1DM. Researchers have found that adults with T1DM with microangiopathy have decreased functional connectivity between brain regions involved in working memory (the left frontoparietal network), language (the left supramarginal gyrus), attention (the left temporal pole, and inferior and superior frontal gyrus), motor control (the motor cortex, and pre and post central gyri) and visual processing (the visual cortex) (107). However, to our knowledge, there are no published fMRI-BOLD studies in children with T1DM.

Differences in the central nervous system in diabetes have been linked to severe hypoglycemia, severe hyperglycemia and chronic exposure to hyperglycemia in children. Although there are diverse findings across these studies, there does seem to be an emerging consensus that the posterior cortical regions of the brain, most commonly the precuneus/cuneus cortex, may be most commonly affected by hyperglycemia in children and adults (Figure 2). The precuneus/cuneus region has many interesting properties that could provide the basis for its vulnerability to hyperglycemia. This region is a critical player in the “default mode network” or DMN. Regions in this network (precuneus/cuneus/posterior cingulate, medial prefrontal, medial temporal and lateral temporal/parietal cortex) are highly active at rest (the brain's ‘default’ state; as measured by PET blood flow and glucose metabolism), but dramatically decrease activity during goal-oriented tasks (108-111). Regions in this network also have high functional connectivity as detected by resting state fMRI (rs-fMRI) (112). Further, the precuneus/cuneus is somewhat unique within the default network in that it shares with the primary visual cortex the highest baseline metabolism of the whole brain, and is the most functionally and structurally ‘connected’ part of the brain (113-114). Recent data has demonstrated that the precuneus/cuneus and the default network have the highest resting state glycolytic rate in the brain, a feature distinguishing them from the primary visual cortex (78). Finally, it is also one of the regions most affected in early Alzheimer disease (115) for which diabetes is thought to be a risk factor (116-117). It is possible that the high baseline demand on blood flow and glucose supply in the DMN may create a heightened vulnerability to alterations in blood glucose delivery (118). Regions of high aerobic glycolysis, such as the precuneus region, may be more vulnerable to this disruption, or disruption could be more likely to lead to cell dysfunction and ultimately cell death. However, it is unclear how developmental processes may be involved in any of these hypothesized effects.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

A) Schematic representation of brain regions in young individuals with T1DM affected by hypoglycemia (green) (thalamus and hippocampus) and hyperglycemia (red) (cuneus and precuneus). Sagittal midline view. B) Schematic representation of brain regions in young individuals with T1DM affected by hypoglycemia (green) (temporal-occipital-prefrontal cortex) and hyperglycemia (red) (prefrontal cortex) lateral view.

Functional correlates of neuroimaging findings

The relationship between the described brain differences and cognitive, academic and occupational function in youth with T1DM have yet to be determined. Although there is ample evidence to suggest that children with T1DM have differences in cognitive functioning depending on their exposure to glycemic extremes, these data have not been linked to observed brain structural or functional differences in any coherent way. Cross-sectional, retrospective studies find that a higher rate of severe hypoglycemic exposure is associated with poorer cognitive function, particularly memory and attention, even when controlling for age of onset and hyperglycemia exposure (19,119-122). Meta-analyses have identified a medium effect size for the relationship between severe hypoglycemia and lower memory performance and small to medium effect sizes for learning, intelligence and verbal tasks (123). Further, effects on memory appear to be modulated by the timing of hypoglycemia, with earlier exposure leading to worse outcomes (120). Prospective studies are less common, and primarily represented by Northam et al's work in assessing a large sample of newly diagnosed children with T1DM and community controls over 12 years (63). Importantly, at diagnosis, children with T1DM did not perform differently than their control group on any cognitive test (124). After 12 years of diabetes, severe hypoglycemia exposure in the past was related to working memory, speed of processing and verbal skills whereas hyperglycemia was related only to working memory (125). An important caveat to these findings is that the test battery given had to change over time and the analyses presented are necessarily cross-sectional, and thus do not reflect within-subject change. In addition, the data have not been analyzed with respect to the timing of hypoglycemia exposure during development. Nonetheless, this study strongly supports the concept that severe hypoglycemia and hyperglycemia exposure during childhood predict cognition outcomes. Other work has suggested that children with T1DM have lower academic performance in school than controls (126-128). Elucidation of the role hypoglycemic and hyperglycemic episodes play in development of specific cognitive defects and how these lead to reduced academic performance is needed to merge these two lines of work.

Conclusions

Research to date examining the effects of hypoglycemia and hyperglycemia on brain structure and function allow us to make several tentative conclusions and point out several gaps in the literature. Over the past decade, much progress has been made in understanding the effects of glycemic extremes on brain structure and function in adults and to a lesser extent in youth. The work that does exist suggests several potential conclusions and areas of needed future study. First, there are relatively subtle, though detectable relationships between glycemic extremes and brain function and structure in youth with T1DM. Second, the pattern of regional brain effects appears different depending on which glycemic extreme is examined. However, in practice, most children have exposure to both glycemic extremes. It may be more clinically relevant — though also much more statistically challenging — to consider the interactive effects of both glycemic extremes over time. Similarly, the effects of DKA or interactions between DKA and other glycemic exposures have not been well addressed in existing analyses. Third, it is not fully understood whether these relatively subtle brain differences are the cause of the cognitive and academic differences reported, and if these effects have a clinically significant impact on daily function. Fourth, there is insufficient evidence regarding whether the timing of exposure to glycemic extremes modulates effects on brain structure and function. Researchers continue to define the fluid, non-linear and diverse trajectory of brain and cognitive development in normal children. More complete understanding of this complex process and its normal variation is necessary before one can fully understand any systematic alterations that may occur in children with T1DM due to glycemic extremes. Fifth, longitudinal, prospective assessment over a wide age range and glycemic experiences is necessary to establish whether glycemic extremes precede brain volume and function changes or whether brain volume changes precede or are unrelated to glycemic extremes. These types of studies are underway in young children with T1DM (U10 HD41890), newly diagnosed children with T1DM (R01 DK064832), and older school aged children and adolescents with T1DM (R01 DK064832 (62). However, determining the effects of specific variables on brain development remains a challenge even in prospective studies, when there are many interacting factors that can influence brain development and functional outcomes, such as nocturnal hypoglycemia, environmental enrichment, life stressors, etc., many of which can be hard to measure accurately. Forthcoming analyses from these groups will begin to answer these difficult questions in a more conclusive manner. The important challenge in this field is to turn these primarily descriptive results into mechanistic, testable hypotheses. To do this, we will need to be careful not to force new data to fit with old hypotheses and instead be willing to accept more complex and nuanced patterns and interactions. By doing so, we may be able to identify networks of critical, glucose-influenced structures and construct optimized treatment protocols over different developmental time periods.

References

  • 1.Mink JW, Blumenschine RJ, Adams DB. Ratio of central nervous system to body metabolism in vertebrates: its constancy and functional basis. Am J Physiol. 1981;241:R203–12. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1981.241.3.R203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Clarke DD, Sokoloff L. Circulation and Energy Metabolism of the Brain. In: Agranoff B, Siegel GJ, editors. Basic Neurochemistry Molecular, Cellular and Medical Aspects. 6th Ed. Lippencott-Raven; Philadelphia: 1999. pp. 637–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Buzsaki G, Kaila K, Raichle M. Inhibition and brain work. Neuron. 2007;56:771–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Vannucci RC, Vannucci SJ. Glucose metabolism in the developing brain. Semin Perinatol. 2000;24:107–15. doi: 10.1053/sp.2000.6361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lunt SY, Vander Heiden MG. Aerobic glycolysis: meeting the metabolic requirements of cell proliferation. Annu Rev Cell Dev Biol. 2011;27:441–64. doi: 10.1146/annurev-cellbio-092910-154237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Petanjek Z, Judas M, Simic G, et al. Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:13281–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105108108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Peters A, Schweiger U, Pellerin L, et al. The selfish brain: competition for energy resources. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;28:143–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Auer RN, Hugh J, Cosgrove E, Curry B. Neuropathologic findings in three cases of profound hypoglycemia. Clin Neuropathol. 1989;8:63–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Yamada KA, Rensing N, Izumi Y, et al. Repetitive hypoglycemia in young rats impairs hippocampal long-term potentiation. Pediatr Res. 2004;55:372–9. doi: 10.1203/01.PDR.0000110523.07240.C1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Russo VC, Higgins S, Werther GA, Cameron FJ. Effects of fluctuating glucose levels on neuronal cells in vitro. Neurochem Res. 2012;37:1768–82. doi: 10.1007/s11064-012-0789-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Tomlinson DR, Gardiner NJ. Glucose neurotoxicity. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:36–45. doi: 10.1038/nrn2294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ludwig A, Ziegenhorn K, Empting S, et al. Glucose metabolism and neurological outcome in congenital hyperinsulinism. Semin Pediatr Surg. 2011;20:45–9. doi: 10.1053/j.sempedsurg.2010.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Gataullina S, De Lonlay P, Dellatolas G, et al. Topography of brain damage in metabolic hypoglycaemia is determined by age at which hypoglycaemia occurred. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55:162–6. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Desrocher M, Rovet J. Neurocognitive correlates of type 1 diabetes mellitus in childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 2004;10:36–52. doi: 10.1076/chin.10.1.36.26241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Eeg-Olofsson O, Petersen I. Childhood diabetic neuropathy: A clinical and neuropsychological study. Acta Pediatrica Scandinavica. 1966;55:163–76. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Frier B. Hypoglycaemia-Clinical consequences and morbidity. International Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2001;3(Supplement 11):51–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Golden MP, Ingersoll GM, Brack CJ, Russell BA, Wright JC, Huberty TJ. Longitudinal relationship of asymptomatic hypoglycemia to cognitive function in IDDM. Diabetes Care. 1989;12:89–93. doi: 10.2337/diacare.12.2.89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gschwend S, Ryan C, Atchison J, Arslanian S, Becker D. Effects of acute hyperglycemia on mental efficiency and counterregulatory hormones in adolescents with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. J Pediatr. 1995;126:178–84. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(95)70542-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hershey T, Bhargava N, Sadler M, White NH, Craft S. Conventional versus intensive diabetes therapy in children with type 1 diabetes: effects on memory and motor speed. Diabetes Care. 1999;22:1318–24. doi: 10.2337/diacare.22.8.1318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hershey T, Craft S, Bhargava N, White NH. Memory and insulin dependent diabetes mellitus (IDDM): effects of childhood onset and severe hypoglycemia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1997;3:509–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.McCall AL. The impact of diabetes on the CNS. Diabetes. 1992;41:557–70. doi: 10.2337/diab.41.5.557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Patino-Fernandez AM, Delamater AM, Applegate EB, et al. Neurocognitive functioning in preschoolage children with type 1 diabetes mellitus. Pediatr Diabetes. 2010;11:424–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5448.2009.00618.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Rovet JF. Neurological sequelae of pediatric diabetes. In: Yeates KO, Taylor HG, Ris MD, B.F. P, editors. Pediatric neuropsychology: Research, theory and practice. Guilford Press; New York: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ryan C, Vega A, Drash A. Cognitive deficits in adolescents who developed diabetes early in life. Pediatrics. 1985;75:921–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ryan CM. Diabetes and brain damage: more (or less) than meets the eye? Diabetologia. 2006;49:2229–33. doi: 10.1007/s00125-006-0392-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Perantie DC, Wu J, Koller JM, et al. Regional brain volume differences associated with hyperglycemia and severe hypoglycemia in youth with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2007;30:2331–7. doi: 10.2337/dc07-0351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.van Harten B, de Leeuw FE, Weinstein HC, Scheltens P, Biessels GJ. Brain imaging in patients with diabetes: a systematic review. Diabetes Care. 2006;29:2539–48. doi: 10.2337/dc06-1637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jongen C, Biessels GJ. Structural brain imaging in diabetes: a methodological perspective. Eur J Pharmacol. 2008;585:208–18. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.11.085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kodl CT, Seaquist ER. Cognitive dysfunction and diabetes mellitus. Endocr Rev. 2008;29:494–511. doi: 10.1210/er.2007-0034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Northam EA, Rankins D, Cameron FJ. Therapy insight: the impact of type 1 diabetes on brain development and function. Nat Clin Pract Neurol. 2006;2:78–86. doi: 10.1038/ncpneuro0097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Cragg BG. The density of synapses and neurons in normal, mentally defective ageing human brains. Brain. 1975;98:81–90. doi: 10.1093/brain/98.1.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Huttenlocher PR, de Courten C, Garey LJ, Van der Loos H. Synaptogenesis in human visual cortex--evidence for synapse elimination during normal development. Neurosci Lett. 1982;33:247–52. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(82)90379-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Lund JS, Boothe RG, Lund RD. Development of neurons in the visual cortex (area 17) of the monkey (Macaca nemestrina): a Golgi study from fetal day 127 to postnatal maturity. J Comp Neurol. 1977;176:149–88. doi: 10.1002/cne.901760203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Rakic P, Bourgeois JP, Eckenhoff MF, Zecevic N, Goldman-Rakic PS. Concurrent overproduction of synapses in diverse regions of the primate cerebral cortex. Science. 1986;232:232–5. doi: 10.1126/science.3952506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Glasser MF, Goyal MS, Preuss TM, Raichle ME, Van Essen DC. Trends and properties of human cerebral cortex: Correlations with cortical myelin content. Neuroimage. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Glasser MF, Van Essen DC. Mapping human cortical areas in vivo based on myelin content as revealed by T1- and T2-weighted MRI. J Neurosci. 2011;31:11597–616. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2180-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Smyser CD, Inder TE, Shimony JS, et al. Longitudinal analysis of neural network development in preterm infants. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20:2852–62. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Van Essen DC, Ugurbil K, Auerbach E, et al. The Human Connectome Project: a data acquisition perspective. Neuroimage. 2012;62:2222–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Chugani HT, Phelps ME, Mazziotta JC. Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development. Ann Neurol. 1987;22:487–97. doi: 10.1002/ana.410220408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kennedy C, Sokoloff L. An adaptation of the nitrous oxide method to the study of the cerebral circulation in children; normal values for cerebral blood flow and cerebral metabolic rate in childhood. J Clin Invest. 1957;36:1130–7. doi: 10.1172/JCI103509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Takahashi T, Shirane R, Sato S, Yoshimoto T. Developmental changes of cerebral blood flow and oxygen metabolism in children. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1999;20:917–22. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Giedd JN. Structural magnetic resonance imaging of the adolescent brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1021:77–85. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Reiss AL, Abrams MT, Singer HS, Ross JL, Denckla MB. Brain development, gender and IQ in children. A volumetric imaging study. Brain. 1996;119(Pt 5):1763–74. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.5.1763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Tamnes CK, Ostby Y, Fjell AM, Westlye LT, Due-Tonnessen P, Walhovd KB. Brain maturation in adolescence and young adulthood: regional age-related changes in cortical thickness and white matter volume and microstructure. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20:534–48. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Giedd JN, Rapoport JL. Structural MRI of pediatric brain development: what have we learned and where are we going? Neuron. 2010;67:728–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Raznahan A, Shaw P, Lalonde F, et al. How does your cortex grow? J Neurosci. 2011;31:7174–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0054-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Craig ME, Hattersley A, Donaghue KC. Definition, epidemiology and classification of diabetes in children and adolescents. Pediatr Diabetes. 2009;10(Suppl 12):3–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5448.2009.00568.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hershey T, Perantie DC, Warren SL, Zimmerman EC, Sadler M, White NH. Frequency and timing of severe hypoglycemia affects spatial memory in children with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2005;28:2372–7. doi: 10.2337/diacare.28.10.2372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Pell GS, Lin A, Wellard RM, et al. Age-related loss of brain volume and T2 relaxation time in youth with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2012;35:513–9. doi: 10.2337/dc11-1290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Heilig CW, Concepcion LA, Riser BL, Freytag SO, Zhu M, Cortes P. Overexpression of glucose transporters in rat mesangial cells cultured in a normal glucose milieu mimics the diabetic phenotype. J Clin Invest. 1995;96:1802–14. doi: 10.1172/JCI118226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kaiser N, Sasson S, Feener EP, et al. Differential regulation of glucose transport and transporters by glucose in vascular endothelial and smooth muscle cells. Diabetes. 1993;42:80–9. doi: 10.2337/diab.42.1.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Wieloch T. Hypoglycemia-induced neuronal damage prevented by an N-methyl-D-aspartate antagonist. Science. 1985;230:681–3. doi: 10.1126/science.2996146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ouyang YB, He QP, Li PA, Janelidze S, Wang GX, Siesjo BK. Is neuronal injury caused by hypoglycemic coma of the necrotic or apoptotic type? Neurochem Res. 2000;25:661–7. doi: 10.1023/a:1007563104170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Lupien SB, Bluhm EJ, Ishii DN. Systemic insulin-like growth factor-I administration prevents cognitive impairment in diabetic rats, and brain IGF regulates learning/memory in normal adult rats. J Neurosci Res. 2003;74:512–23. doi: 10.1002/jnr.10791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Mason GF, Petersen KF, Lebon V, Rothman DL, Shulman GI. Increased brain monocarboxylic acid transport and utilization in type 1 diabetes. Diabetes. 2006;55:929–34. doi: 10.2337/diabetes.55.04.06.db05-1325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Sima AA, Kamiya H, Li ZG. Insulin, C-peptide, hyperglycemia, and central nervous system complications in diabetes. Eur J Pharmacol. 2004;490:187–97. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2004.02.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Jones TW, Davis EA. Hypoglycemia in children with type 1 diabetes: current issues and controversies. Pediatr Diabetes. 2003;4:143–50. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-5448.2003.00025.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Matyka KA, Crowne EC, Havel PJ, Macdonald IA, Matthews D, Dunger DB. Counterregulation during spontaneous nocturnal hypoglycemia in prepubertal children with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 1999;22:1144–50. doi: 10.2337/diacare.22.7.1144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Porter PA, Keating B, Byrne G, Jones TW. Incidence and predictive criteria of nocturnal hypoglycemia in young children with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. J Pediatr. 1997;130:366–72. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(97)70197-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Aine CJ. A conceptual overview and critique of functional neuroimaging techniques in humans: I. MRI/FMRI and PET. Crit Rev Neurobiol. 1995;9:229–309. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Assaf Y, Pasternak O. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI)-based white matter mapping in brain research: a review. J Mol Neurosci. 2008;34:51–61. doi: 10.1007/s12031-007-0029-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Perantie DC, Koller JM, Weaver PM, et al. Prospectively determined impact of type 1 diabetes on brain volume during development. Diabetes. 2011;60:3006–14. doi: 10.2337/db11-0589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Northam EA, Rankins D, Lin A, et al. Central nervous system function in youth with type 1 diabetes 12 years after disease onset. Diabetes Care. 2009;32:445–50. doi: 10.2337/dc08-1657. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Chalmers J, Risk MT, Kean DM, Grant R, Ashworth B, Campbell IW. Severe amnesia after hypoglycemia. Clinical, psychometric, and magnetic resonance imaging correlations. Diabetes Care. 1991;14:922–5. doi: 10.2337/diacare.14.10.922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Holemans X, Dupuis M, Misson N, Vanderijst JF. Reversible amnesia in a Type 1 diabetic patient and bilateral hippocampal lesions on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Diabet Med. 2001;18:761–3. doi: 10.1046/j.1464-5491.2001.00481.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kalimo H, Olsson Y. Effects of severe hypoglycemia on the human brain. Neuropathological case reports. Acta Neurol Scand. 1980;62:345–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1980.tb03047.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Aye T, Reiss AL, Kesler S, et al. The feasibility of detecting neuropsychologic and neuroanatomic effects of type 1 diabetes in young children. Diabetes Care. 2011;34:1458–62. doi: 10.2337/dc10-2164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Hershey T, Perantie DC, Wu J, Weaver PM, Black KJ, White NH. Hippocampal volumes in youth with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes. 2010;59:236–41. doi: 10.2337/db09-1117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Arbelaez AM, Rutlin JR, Hershey T, Powers WJ, Videen TO, Cryer PE. Thalamic activation during slightly subphysiological glycemia in humans. Diabetes Care. 2012;35:2570–4. doi: 10.2337/dc12-0297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Arbelaez AM, Su Y, Thomas JB, Hauch AC, Hershey T, Ances BM. Comparison of regional cerebral blood flow responses to hypoglycemia using pulsed arterial spin labeling and positron emission tomography. PLoS One. 2013;8:e60085. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0060085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Teves D, Videen TO, Cryer PE, Powers WJ. Activation of human medial prefrontal cortex during autonomic responses to hypoglycemia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:6217–21. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0307048101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Teh MM, Dunn JT, Choudhary P, et al. Evolution and resolution of human brain perfusion responses to the stress of induced hypoglycemia. Neuroimage. 2010;53:584–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Ho MS, Weller NJ, Ives FJ, et al. Prevalence of structural central nervous system abnormalities in early-onset type 1 diabetes mellitus. J Pediatr. 2008;153:385–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Aye T, Barnea-Goraly N, Ambler C, et al. White matter structural differences in young children with type 1 diabetes: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Diabetes Care. 2012;35:2167–73. doi: 10.2337/dc12-0017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Musen G, Lyoo IK, Sparks CR, et al. Effects of type 1 diabetes on gray matter density as measured by voxel-based morphometry. Diabetes. 2006;55:326–33. doi: 10.2337/diabetes.55.02.06.db05-0520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Musen G, Lyoo KL, Sparks C, et al. Evidence for reduced gray matter density in patients with type 1 diabetes as measured by magnetic resonance imaging. Diabetes. 2004;52(Suppl. 2):A57–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Wessels AM, Simsek S, Remijnse PL, et al. Voxel-based morphometry demonstrates reduced grey matter density on brain MRI in patients with diabetic retinopathy. Diabetologia. 2006;49:2474–80. doi: 10.1007/s00125-006-0283-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Vaishnavi SN, Vlassenko AG, Rundle MM, Snyder AZ, Mintun MA, Raichle ME. Regional aerobic glycolysis in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:17757–62. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010459107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Hoffman WH, Steinhart CM, el Gammal T, Steele S, Cuadrado AR, Morse PK. Cranial CT in children and adolescents with diabetic ketoacidosis. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1988;9:733–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Krane EJ, Rockoff MA, Wallman JK, Wolfsdorf JI. Subclinical brain swelling in children during treatment of diabetic ketoacidosis. N Engl J Med. 1985;312:1147–51. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198505023121803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Glaser NS, Wootton-Gorges SL, Buonocore MH, et al. Frequency of sub-clinical cerebral edema in children with diabetic ketoacidosis. Pediatr Diabetes. 2006;7:75–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-543X.2006.00156.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Concha L, Gross DW, Wheatley BM, Beaulieu C. Diffusion tensor imaging of time-dependent axonal and myelin degradation after corpus callosotomy in epilepsy patients. Neuroimage. 2006;32:1090–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Thomalla G, Glauche V, Koch MA, Beaulieu C, Weiller C, Rother J. Diffusion tensor imaging detects early Wallerian degeneration of the pyramidal tract after ischemic stroke. Neuroimage. 2004;22:1767–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Mac Donald CL, Johnson AM, Cooper D, et al. Detection of blast-related traumatic brain injury in U.S. military personnel. N Engl J Med. 2011;364:2091–100. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1008069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Shimony JS, Sheline YI, D'Angelo G, et al. Diffuse microstructural abnormalities of normal-appearing white matter in late life depression: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:245–52. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Budde MD, Xie M, Cross AH, Song SK. Axial diffusivity is the primary correlate of axonal injury in the experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis spinal cord: a quantitative pixelwise analysis. J Neurosci. 2009;29:2805–13. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4605-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Antenor-Dorsey JA, Meyer E, Rutlin J, et al. White matter microstructural integrity in youth with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes. 2013;62:581–9. doi: 10.2337/db12-0696. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Sharma R, Buras E, Terashima T, et al. Hyperglycemia induces oxidative stress and impairs axonal transport rates in mice. PLoS One. 2010;5:e13463. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Ohara S, Nakagawa S, Tabata K, Hashimoto T. Hemiballism with hyperglycemia and striatal T1-MRI hyperintensity: an autopsy report. Mov Disord. 2001;16:521–5. doi: 10.1002/mds.1110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Postuma RB, Lang AE. Hemiballism: revisiting a classic disorder. Lancet Neurol. 2003;2:661–8. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(03)00554-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Glaser NS, Marcin JP, Wootton-Gorges SL, et al. Correlation of clinical and biochemical findings with diabetic ketoacidosis-related cerebral edema in children using magnetic resonance diffusion-weighted imaging. J Pediatr. 2008;153:541–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2008.04.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Vavilala MS, Marro KI, Richards TL, et al. Change in mean transit time, apparent diffusion coefficient, and cerebral blood volume during pediatric diabetic ketoacidosis treatment. Pediatr Crit Care Med. 2011;12:e344–9. doi: 10.1097/PCC.0b013e3182196c9c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Glaser NS, Wootton-Gorges SL, Marcin JP, et al. Mechanism of cerebral edema in children with diabetic ketoacidosis. J Pediatr. 2004;145:164–71. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2004.03.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.O'Sullivan M, Singhal S, Charlton R, Markus HS. Diffusion tensor imaging of thalamus correlates with cognition in CADASIL without dementia. Neurology. 2004;62:702–7. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000113760.72706.d2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Gujar SK, Maheshwari S, Bjorkman-Burtscher I, Sundgren PC. Magnetic resonance spectroscopy. J Neuroophthalmol. 2005;25:217–26. doi: 10.1097/01.wno.0000177307.21081.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Sarac K, Akinci A, Alkan A, Aslan M, Baysal T, Ozcan C. Brain metabolite changes on proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in children with poorly controlled type 1 diabetes mellitus. Neuroradiology. 2005;47:562–5. doi: 10.1007/s00234-005-1387-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Wootton-Gorges SL, Buonocore MH, Caltagirone RA, Kuppermann N, Glaser NS. Progressive decrease in N-acetylaspartate/Creatine ratio in a teenager with type 1 diabetes and repeated episodes of ketoacidosis without clinically apparent cerebral edema: Evidence for permanent brain injury. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2010;31:780–1. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A1829. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Wootton-Gorges SL, Buonocore MH, Kuppermann N, et al. Cerebral proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in children with diabetic ketoacidosis. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2007;28:895–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Arbelaez AM, Powers WJ, Videen TO, Price JL, Cryer PE. Attenuation of counterregulatory responses to recurrent hypoglycemia by active thalamic inhibition: a mechanism for hypoglycemia-associated autonomic failure. Diabetes. 2008;57:470–5. doi: 10.2337/db07-1329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Schwartz WJ, Smith CB, Davidsen L, et al. Metabolic mapping of functional activity in the hypothalamo-neurohypophysial system of the rat. Science. 1979;205:723–5. doi: 10.1126/science.462184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Ackermann RF, Finch DM, Babb TL, Engel J., Jr. Increased glucose metabolism during long-duration recurrent inhibition of hippocampal pyramidal cells. J Neurosci. 1984;4:251–64. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-01-00251.1984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Tagamets MA, Horwitz B. A model of working memory: bridging the gap between electrophysiology and human brain imaging. Neural Netw. 2000;13:941–52. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(00)00063-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Powers WJ, Dagogo-Jack S, Markham J, Larson KB, Dence CS. Cerebral transport and metabolism of 1-11C-D-glucose during stepped hypoglycemia. Ann Neurol. 1995;38:599–609. doi: 10.1002/ana.410380408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Segel SA, Fanelli CG, Dence CS, et al. Blood-to-brain glucose transport, cerebral glucose metabolism, and cerebral blood flow are not increased after hypoglycemia. Diabetes. 2001;50:1911–7. doi: 10.2337/diabetes.50.8.1911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Antenor-Dorsey JA, Khoury N, Su Y, et al. Adrenomedullary Epinephrine Response to Declining Plasma Glucose Concentrations is a Signaling Event that is Not Caused by a Decrease in the Cerebral Metabolic Rate of Glucose. Diabetes. 2013 392-P abstract. [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Fox PT, Raichle ME, Mintun MA, Dence C. Nonoxidative glucose consumption during focal physiologic neural activity. Science. 1988;241:462–4. doi: 10.1126/science.3260686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.van Duinkerken E, Schoonheim MM, Sanz-Arigita EJ, et al. Resting-state brain networks in type 1 diabetic patients with and without microangiopathy and their relation to cognitive functions and disease variables. Diabetes. 2012;61:1814–21. doi: 10.2337/db11-1358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Gusnard DA, Raichle ME. Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:685–94. doi: 10.1038/35094500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Gusnard DA, Raichle ME. Functional imaging, neurophysiology, and the resting state of the human brain. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The Cognitive Neurosciences. The MIT Press; Cambridge: 2004. pp. 1267–80. [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Raichle ME, Snyder AZ. A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1083–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.041. discussion 97-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Shulman GL, Fiez JA, Corbetta M, et al. Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1997;9:648–63. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005;102:9673–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Buckner RL, Sepulcre J, Talukdar T, et al. Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci. 2009;29:1860–73. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5062-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Hagmann P, Kurant M, Gigandet X, et al. Mapping human whole-brain structural networks with diffusion MRI. PLoS One. 2007;2:e597. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, Shannon BJ, et al. Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer's disease: evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J Neurosci. 2005;25:7709–17. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2177-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Luchsinger JA, Tang MX, Stern Y, Shea S, Mayeux R. Diabetes mellitus and risk of Alzheimer's disease and dementia with stroke in a multiethnic cohort. Am J Epidemiol. 2001;154:635–41. doi: 10.1093/aje/154.7.635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Messier C, Gagnon M. Glucose regulation and cognitive functions: relation to Alzheimer's disease and diabetes. Behav Brain Res. 1996;75:1–11. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00153-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Reusch JE. Diabetes, microvascular complications, and cardiovascular complications: what is it about glucose? J Clin Invest. 2003;112:986–8. doi: 10.1172/JCI19902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Northam EA, Anderson PJ, Werther GA, Warne GL, Andrewes D. Predictors of change in the neuropsychological profiles of children with type 1 diabetes 2 years after disease onset. Diabetes Care. 1999;22:1438–44. doi: 10.2337/diacare.22.9.1438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Perantie DC, Lim A, Wu J, et al. Effects of prior hypoglycemia and hyperglycemia on cognition in children with type 1 diabetes mellitus. Pediatr Diabetes. 2008;9:87–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5448.2007.00274.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Rovet J, Alvarez M. Attentional functioning in children and adolescents with IDDM. Diabetes Care. 1997;20:803–10. doi: 10.2337/diacare.20.5.803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Rovet JF, Ehrlich RM. The effect of hypoglycemic seizures on cognitive function in children with diabetes: a 7-year prospective study. J Pediatr. 1999;134:503–6. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(99)70211-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Naguib JM, Kulinskaya E, Lomax CL, Garralda ME. Neuro-cognitive performance in children with type 1 diabetes--a meta-analysis. J Pediatr Psychol. 2009;34:271–82. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Northam EA, Anderson PJ, Werther GA, Warne GL, Adler RG, Andrewes D. Neuropsychological complications of IDDM in children 2 years after disease onset. Diabetes Care. 1998;21:379–84. doi: 10.2337/diacare.21.3.379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Lin A, Northam EA, Rankins D, Werther GA, Cameron FJ. Neuropsychological profiles of young people with type 1 diabetes 12 yr after disease onset. Pediatr Diabetes. 2010;11:235–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5448.2009.00588.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Hannonen R, Komulainen J, Riikonen R, et al. Academic skills in children with early-onset type 1 diabetes: the effects of diabetes-related risk factors. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2012;54:457–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04248.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.McCarthy AM, Lindgren S, Mengeling MA, Tsalikian E, Engvall J. Factors associated with academic achievement in children with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2003;26:112–7. doi: 10.2337/diacare.26.1.112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Parent KB, Wodrich DL, Hasan KS. Type 1 diabetes mellitus and school: a comparison of patients and healthy siblings. Pediatr Diabetes. 2009;10:554–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5448.2009.00532.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES