Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2013 Nov 16;34(6):1225–1243. doi: 10.1007/s10764-013-9735-3

Defining the Low End of Primate Social Complexity: The Social Organization of the Nocturnal White-Footed Sportive Lemur (Lepilemur leucopus)

Iris Dröscher 1,, Peter M Kappeler 1,2
PMCID: PMC3858612  PMID: 24347750

Abstract

Whereas other species of sportive lemurs (genus Lepilemur) have been described as living in dispersed pairs, which are characterized by spatial overlap but a lack of affinity or affiliation between one adult male and female, existing reports on the social organization of the white-footed sportive lemur (Lepilemur leucopus) are conflicting, describing them as either living in dispersed one-male multifemale systems or pairs. We conducted this study in the spiny forest of Berenty Reserve, southern Madagascar, to clarify the social organization and to characterize the level of social complexity of this species. We combined 1530 h of radio-telemetry and behavioral observations over a period of 1 yr to describe the spatiotemporal stability, size, and interindividual overlap of individual home ranges as well as interindividual cohesiveness. Results revealed low intra- and high intersexual home range overlap. Although most of the social units identified consisted of dispersed pairs (N = 5), males were associated with two adult females in two cases. Furthermore, members of a social unit were never observed to groom each other or to share a daytime sleeping site, and Hutchinson’s and Doncaster’s dynamic interaction tests indicated active avoidance between pair partners. Low cohesiveness together with extremely low rates of social interactions therefore arguably places Lepilemur leucopus at the low end of primate social complexity.

Keywords: Cohesiveness, Dispersed pairs, Lepilemur, Pair-living, Social complexity

Introduction

The majority of primate field studies have been concerned with descriptions and analyses of their social systems (Crook and Gartlan 1966; Mitani et al. 2012; Smuts et al. 1987). There is an emerging consensus that the diversity of primate social systems can be analyzed at the level of their social organization, mating system and social structure (Cords 2007; Kappeler and van Schaik 2002; Kappeler et al. 2013). Interspecific variation in social systems has also been analyzed more recently as a function of social complexity (Bergman 2010; de Waal and Tyack 2003; Lehmann and Ross 2011; McComb and Semple 2005). In this context, complex social systems have been defined as those in which individuals frequently interact in many different contexts with many different individuals, and often repeatedly interact with many of the same individuals in networks over time (Freeberg et al. 2012). Social complexity is therefore an integrative measure that correlates positively with group size because, according to the social intelligence hypothesis (Dunbar 1998), animals living in larger groups should have enhanced cognitive abilities to facilitate the management of multiple social relationships, compared to those living in smaller groups or in other types of social organization (Bond et al. 2003; Byrne and Whiten 1988; Dunbar and Shultz 2007). However, social complexity has not been explicitly studied in primates that do not live in groups, so that it is currently difficult to define a baseline for comparative studies of social complexity.

The absence of group living among primates correlates strongly with nocturnal activity (van Schaik 1983). Even though about a quarter of all primates are nocturnal, their social systems have remained comparatively poorly studied (Bearder 1999). Nocturnal primates have initially been collectively characterized as “solitary foragers” (Bearder 1987; Petter et al. 1977), but methodological advances in telemetry and molecular genetics have since disclosed more details of the diversity and complexity of their social systems (summarized in Kappeler 2012; Nekaris and Bearder 2011). In particular, some species of dwarf (Cheirogaleus spp.) and sportive lemurs (Lepilemur spp.) were found to be organized into pairs, even though individuals forage solitarily (Fietz 1999; Müller 1999; Rasoloharijaona et al. 2003; Zinner et al. 2003). Because pairs are the smallest social units, and pair-living requires active coordination between pair partners (Barelli et al. 2008; Schülke and Kappeler 2003), and because its evolutionary emergence was associated with a significant increase in brain size (Shultz and Dunbar 2007), pairs represent an interesting level of analysis for comparative studies of social complexity. Species that combine aspects of the likely evolutionary transition between a solitary social organization and pair living might be particularly interesting in this context because they may represent the earliest and most primitive form of sociality.

Our study focused on a species of sportive lemur for which conflicting information about the social organization of the same population had been reported. White-footed sportive lemurs (Lepilemur leucopus) are confined to the region between the Menarandra and Mandrare Rivers in southern Madagascar (Hoffmann 2008). They have evolved adaptations to a folivorous diet despite small body size (<1 kg), including prolonged resting bouts, small night ranges, a prolonged cecum, and cecotrophy (Hladik and Charles-Dominique 1974). Two short field studies were conducted on the same population of white-footed sportive lemurs at Berenty Reserve in the 1970s and reported conflicting patterns of social organization (Charles-Dominique and Hladik 1971; Russell 1977). Charles-Dominique and Hladik (1971) described exclusive range use by both sexes, but range overlap between the sexes, and found that the largest male was associated with five females. Russell (1977) reported that no individual had an exclusive range and described range-sharing by females. He also observed males and females sleeping together during the day. Based on these observations, the social organization of Lepilemur leucopus has been classified as a “dispersed harem” (Müller and Thalmann 2000). Neither study used radio-tracking or detailed patterns of social interactions.

The present study aimed to resolve these conflicting reports by characterizing the social organization of Lepilemur leucopus during a year-long study of radio-collared individuals. In particular, we empirically identified natural social units and investigated their stability across the year. In addition, we quantified the degree of cohesiveness within social units using three different computational approaches and report on patterns of social interactions within and between social units. Together, these data also contribute to our second aim, namely the characterization of the level of social complexity in this small nocturnal lemur.

Methods

Study Site and Subjects

We conducted this study at Berenty (S 25.00°, E 46.30°), a ca. 200 km2 private ecotourism reserve located in southern Madagascar. To ensure continuing focal observations of single individuals throughout the night, we equipped subjects with radio-tracking transmitters. We captured the individuals by blow-darting in a spiny forest fragment of ca. 5 ha (HAH Reserve Forestière parcel 1), which is connected to gallery forest on one side via a transitional forest and a further 40-ha spiny forest fragment on the other side (Norscia and Palagi 2008).

We used a blowpipe and 1-ml air pressured narcotic syringe projectiles (Telinject, Germany) to anesthetize subjects with 0.4 ml of Ketanest (100 mg/L) in the mornings in their daytime resting sites. We captured anesthetized individuals with a blanket when they fell out of the tree. Alternatively, if the anesthetized individuals did not fall and it was possible to reach them by climbing the tree, we retrieved them from their resting sites by hand or with an animal capture pole (Tomahawk 7′–12′ extension restraint pole). We fitted the subjects with radio-collars (TW-3 button-cell tags, Biotrack, U.K.) while anesthetized. We kept the subjects in an animal transport box (Traveller Box Capri Mini, Trixie Heimtierbedarf, 40 × 22 × 30 cm) until they were fully recovered and released them again at their capture site in the evening. The same individuals later reused sleeping trees where they were captured.

We fitted 16 adult (8 males and 8 females) and 4 subadult individuals (3 males and 1 female) with radio-collars. We differentiated adult individuals from subadults by the degree of tooth wear and body mass. At the beginning of the study, all subadult individuals still ranged within their parental territories. Once they dispersed from their natal range, we classified them as adults. We did not radio-collar smaller juvenile individuals because radio-collars exceeded 4% of their body mass. Some members of social units were not equipped with radio-collars. However, we noted their presence during capture of subjects, focal animal observations, and a population census at the end of the study. We removed all radio-collars after the end of the study. The research followed standard protocols for animal handling, capture, and radio-tracking and was approved by the Commission Tripartite CAFF (Madagascar).

Behavioral Observations

We collected behavioral and locational data between October 2011 and October 2012 for a total of 1530 h on 20 radio-collared individuals. We divided the study period into four biologically relevant seasons: birth and offspring care with lactation (early wet season from November to January), offspring care without lactation (late wet season from February to April), mating and early gestation (early dry season from May to July), and late gestation (late wet season from August to October).We followed each radio-collared individual for up to 2 full nights during each season, with a TR-4 receiver and a RA-14K antenna (Telonics, U.S.A.).The number of focal animal follows per season decreased throughout the year owing to the disappearance of individuals, so that the total number of focal animal follows per individual ranged between 5 and 8 nights (mean ± SD: 7.7 ± 0.8 nights per individual). We restricted our analyses of static and dynamic spatial interactions to adult individuals belonging to seven different social units (Table I).

Table I.

Summary of continuous focal animal observations conducted throughout the year

Social unit Male ID Hours N location points Female ID Hours N location points
1 m10 57 642 f1B 87 990
2 m9 79 863 f2 88 996
3 m3 88 946 f3 88 964
4 m4 90 1008 f4 86 991
5 m5 88 971 f5 88 994
6 m6 87 948 f6 90 1010
7 m7 89 1012 f7 87 988

The trees of the spiny forest have small and exposed canopies (Grubb 2003), permitting observation of the subjects clearly and continuously, despite their nocturnal activity (Hladik and Charles-Dominique 1974). Continuous focal animal observations (Altmann 1974) started when an individual left its sleeping site at dusk and were continued until it returned to its daytime resting tree at dawn. On average the focal individuals were out of sight for 7.1 ± 1.8% (mean ± SD) of total observation time. We identified individuals ranging in the same area before the onset of data collection during preliminary observations on sleeping site choice and ranging behavior of radio-collared individuals. Henceforth, a second trained observer followed the range mate of a focal individual simultaneously. We tagged spatial locations of subjcts during continuous focal observations with biodegradable tape. After each full-night follow, we determined the exact position of the tagged trees with reference to a 10 × 10 m study grid system. Each morning after a full-night follow we located the sleeping trees of all radio-collared individuals by radio-tracking to determine the composition of sleeping associations.

We defined social interactions as agonistic, affiliative, or neutral. We defined all interactions that were either aggressive (chase, charge, bite, and grab) or submissive (flee, be displaced, or jump away) as agonistic (sensu Pereira and Kappeler 1997). We noted interactions during which individuals sat ≤1 m of each other and/or groomed each other as affiliative. We termed interactions during which individuals came within a distance of 5 m of each other without exhibiting agonistic or affiliative behavior as neutral. We based calculations of the frequency of social interactions on the time the focal individuals were actually in sight.

Data Analyses

To evaluate static spatial interactions between subjects, we calculated individual annual home ranges with the Animal Movement extension of ArcView. We subsampled locational data at 5-min intervals for home range analyses. We calculated home range size from 95% fixed kernel home range utilization distributions (Worton 1989) using ad hoc smoothing (Silverman 1986). We did not correct for spatial autocorrelation, as kernel densities do not require serial independence of observations when estimating home ranges size, and the accuracy and precision of home range estimates improve with the number of observations (De Solla et al. 1999). We calculated home range overlap in R (R Core Team 2012) using the package adehabitatHR (Calenge 2006). To determine whether social units were maintained throughout the year, we calculated overlap of night ranges of simultaneously followed males and females as percent overlap (Kernohan et al. 2001). We calculated overlap of annual home ranges for both, pair partners and same-sexed neighbors. We calculated seasonal influence on night range overlap for pair partners that were followed simultaneously, using one-way repeated-measures ANOVA. We excluded one pair (m10fB1) from the analyses because simultaneous follows on the pair partners were conducted only during the wet season. We averaged values for each season and pair. The data were normally distributed for each level of the within-subject factor season. We conducted the analyses in R using the function ezANOVA in the package ez (Lawrence 2012).

We examined dynamic spatial interaction to quantify the degree of sociality between pair partners, i.e., whether they associated, avoided each other, or moved randomly in relation to each other. We used three different models: the random gas model (Waser 1976), Hutchinson’s model (Hutchinson and Waser 2007), and Doncaster’s model (Doncaster 1990). We calculated expected rates of encounters between pair partners with the random gas model as f=4×ρ×vπ×2d+s, where ρ is the density of a species, υ the velocity of an individual, s the group spread, and d the distance criterion. We calculated expected rates of associations between pair partners with Hutchinson’s model as f = N × ρ × π × d 2, where N is the number of instantaneous observations, ρ is the density of a species, and d the distance criterion. For both models, we compared observed rates with expected rates, using Wilcoxon signed-ranks test across all pairs. Using Doncaster’s model, we compared N observed interindividual distances with expected ones calculated from all N 2 distances possible within a given set of spatial points. We compared observed with expected values for each pair within a 2 × 2 contingency table containing counts below and above d using a χ2 test. The significance test depends on successive data points being independent, giving each individual the opportunity to travel to any other part of its range between successive instantaneous observations (Doncaster 1990). We considered data points to be independent as the interval permits an individual to traverse its home range at maximum travel speed (Rooney et al. 1998). Here, we calculated ρ as the inverse of a pair’s union home range in square meters and υ as the average distance the male and female covered during the observation period in meters. We set s to zero and d to 15 m as this distance was close enough to allow visual contact between individuals. We used the software R for statistical analyses. We considered α levels of P ≤ 0.05 as statistically significant.

Results

Static and Dynamic Spatial Interactions

Average annual home ranges were significantly larger for males (mean ± SD: 0.33 ± 0.08 ha, N = 7) than females (0.18 ± 0.08 ha, N = 7; Wilcoxon rank sum test: W = 47, N = 14, P = 0.005; Fig. 1). Male annual home ranges overlapped on average with those of neighboring males by only 1.65 ± 1.99% and those of females with those of neighboring females by merely 0.4 ± 0.64% (mean ± SD) based on nine dyads of possible neighbor pairings. However, annual home ranges of particular males and females overlapped considerably. Average overlap between the annual home ranges of the seven pairs identified was 81 ± 20% for females and 43 ± 16% (mean ± SD) for males. Differences between male and female’s perspective are due to the smaller home ranges of females.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Annual home ranges of adult males (m) and females (f) of L. leucopus. Depicted are the 95% kernel home ranges of each individual.

Average overlap of night ranges was 73 ± 28% for females and 45 ± 24% (mean ± SD) for males based on six identified pairs. In general, overlap between pair partners was high throughout all seasons. The maximum observed night range overlap was 100% from the female’s perspective and 93% from the male’s perspective. Only during 2 out of 48 simultaneous follows did night ranges of pair partners not overlap. Otherwise, minimum observed night range overlap was 26% from the female’s perspective and 12% from the male’s perspective. Furthermore, season did not have a significant effect on night range overlap from the female’s perspective (one-way repeated-measures ANOVA: F 3,5 = 1.25, P = 0.33). However, season had an influence on night range overlap from the male’s perspective (one-way repeated-measures ANOVA: F 3,5 = 3.26, P = 0.05). Night range overlap of males with their corresponding female pair partners was significantly higher during the early dry season (corresponding to mating and early gestation) compared to the late dry season (corresponding to late gestation; Tukey’s post hoc test: Z = –3.30, P = 0.005).

Static spatial interactions between adult individuals changed during the course of the study due to confirmed deaths and dispersal events (Fig. 2). Demographic changes took place within social units 1, 2, and 7. The home range of male m3 overlapped with those of two females from the beginning of the study, whereas male m10 ranged with two females from March 2012 onwards.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Demographic changes in the study population between September 2011 and October 2012. Only adult animals are presented. Males are represented by squares; females by circles. Confirmed deaths are illustrated with a cross. No ranging data are available for individuals labeled with a question mark as they were not equipped with radio-collars or died before they could be followed.

Based on the random gas model, observed encounter rates between pair partners were significantly higher than expected (Wilcoxon signed rank test: V = 28, N = 7, P = 0.02). In contrast, using Hutchinson’s model, the number of observed associations was significantly lower than expected (Wilcoxon signed rank test: V = 0, N = 7, P = 0.02). Similarly, observed values were significantly smaller than expected for five out of seven pairs (χ2 ≥ 3.87, df = 1, P ≤ 0.049) and nonsignificant for the remaining two pairs (χ2 ≤ 0.7, df = 1, P ≥ 0.28) using Doncaster’s model.

Sleeping Associations

The focal individuals spent the day mainly in the confluence of branches of trees of the genus Alluaudia, or more rarely inside liana tangles or tree holes. A single adult used 5–11 different sleeping sites (Table II). Although adult individuals shared some of their sleeping trees with their pair partner, they never used them simultaneously (37–69 observation days per dyad). In contrast, adult females and their offspring shared sleeping trees during 79% of 42 observation days, based on six adult females that gave birth in November 2011. However, we never saw adult males sharing a sleeping tree simultaneously with any other member of their social unit. Sleeping trees were never shared with neighbors, neither simultaneously nor consecutively.

Table II.

Number of sleeping trees used exclusively, shared with pair partner and days of simultaneous use

Pair Observation days Exclusive use m/f Shared use Days simultaneous use
m10f1B 37 3/3 3 0
m9f2 61 8/4 1 0
m3f3 69 9/3 2 0
m4f4 69 8/4 2 0
m5f5 69 5/4 1 0
m6f6 69 1/2 3 0
m7f7 69 7/2 3 0

Social Interactions

In total, we observed 72 social interactions during 516 h of simultaneous focal observations on range mates. The frequency of observed social interactions was therefore low, with only 0.14 interactions/h across all pairs (Table III). Most social interactions were of the neutral type (78%), i.e., individuals sitting 1–5 m of each other. Agonistic interactions were less common than neutral interactions (21%), whereas affiliative interactions were essentially absent (1%). We never observed allogrooming between adult males and females or social interactions between neighboring males. We only observed a single affiliative social interaction between neighboring adult females (f1B and f2). We did not observe any social interactions between the females of the two social units (1 and 3) with two adult females. However, we observed agonistic interactions between resident and floating males. We did not witness any matings.

Table III.

Frequency and types of social interactions between pair partners

Pair Neutral Agonistic Affiliative Total
m10f1B 0.12 0.02 0.00 0.15
m9f2 0.24 0.03 0.01 0.28
m3f3 0.09 0.04 0.00 0.13
m4f4 0.07 0.02 0.00 0.10
m5f5 0.05 0.00 0.00 0.05
m6f6 0.12 0.05 0.00 0.17
m7f7 0.07 0.05 0.00 0.12
Mean 0.11 ± 0.06 0.03 ± 0.02 0.00 ± 0.01 0.14 ± 0.07

Total number of observation hours: 516.

Discussion

Static Spatial Interactions

Adult white-footed sportive lemurs had almost exclusive home ranges, as range overlap among neighboring adult males (1.7%) as well as among neighboring adult females (0.4%) was minimal. However, the ranges of certain adult males and females overlapped considerably. Direct social interactions were essentially limited to individuals that shared home ranges. Therefore, spatial boundaries coincided with social boundaries (sensu Schülke and Kappeler 2003), and pairs of adult males and females can be regarded as the prevailing social unit of Lepilemur leucopus.

Currently 24 species of the genus Lepilemur are recognized (Ramaromilanto et al. 2009) but only a few have been studied in any detail so far. Two of them, Lepilemur ruficaudatus and L. edwardsi, have been described as pair-living based on spatiotemporal overlap of individual home ranges (Méndez-Cárdenas and Zimmermann 2009; Zinner et al. 2003). Mean overlap of 95% KHRs between pair partners was 61% from the male’s and 89% from the female’s perspective, whereas mean overlap between neighboring males was 2.3% and 1.8% between neighboring females in Lepilemur ruficaudatus (Hilgartner et al. 2012). In Lepilemur edwardsi, mean overlap of minimum convex polygons (MCPs) between pair partners was 72% from the male’s and 87% from the female’s perspective (Méndez-Cárdenas and Zimmermann 2009), whereas median overlap between neighboring males was up to 4.9% and up to 6.6% between neighboring females (Rasoloharijaona et al. 2006). Thus, all three Lepilemur species studied in detail so far exhibit a pair-living social organization.

Other nocturnal primates vary greatly in patterns of male and female spatial distribution. Home ranges of males show mutual overlap and also overlap with the ranges of several females, whereas female home ranges never do so in the aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis: Sterling and Richard 1995). In contrast, home ranges overlap within and between the sexes in mouse lemurs (Microcebus spp.: Dammhahn and Kappeler 2009; Eberle and Kappeler 2002; Radespiel 2000), hairy-eared dwarf lemurs (Allocebus trichotis: Biebouw 2009), giant mouse lemurs (Mirza coquereli: Kappeler 1997), greater galagos (Otolemur garnettii: Nash and Harcourt 1986), and slender lorises (Loris lydekkerianus: Nekaris 2003). In addition, pair-living has been described for several other nocturnal primate taxa, including fork-marked lemurs (Phaner pallescens: Schülke and Kappeler 2003), dwarf lemurs (Cheirogaleus medius: Fietz 1999), dwarf galagos (Galagoides zanzibaricus: Nash and Harcourt 1986), pottos (Perodicticus potto: Pimley et al. 2005), slow lorises (Nycticebus coucang: Wiens and Zitzmann 2003), some tarsiers (Tarsius spp.: Driller et al. 2009; Gursky-Doyen 2010), woolly lemurs (Avahi spp.: Harcourt 1991; Norscia and Borgognini-Tarli 2008), and owl monkeys (Aotus spp.: Fernandez-Duque 2007). Thus, in terms of ranging patterns, Lepilemur leucopus do not differ fundamentally from other nocturnal primates.

Sex-Specific Ranging Behavior

The fact that males ranged over substantially larger areas than females (95% kernel: 0.33 ha vs. 0.18 ha) suggests polygynous tendencies of males. According to Schubert et al. (2009), large home ranges allow males to assess the reproductive status of neighboring females and to monitor the presence of neighboring males. Therefore, male Lepilemur leucopus may follow a mixed reproductive strategy of maintaining a pair bond while seeking extra-pair copulations, but paternity tests will be required to test this hypothesis because we did not observe any matings. Male home ranges are also larger than female home ranges in Phaner pallescens and Tupaia tana, which have high rates of extra-pair paternity (Munshi-South 2007; Schülke et al. 2004). Extra-pair copulations were also detected in Lepilemur ruficaudatus, where males also have significantly larger ranges (95% kernel: 0.99 ha vs. 0.66 ha; Hilgartner et al. 2012). Home range size did not differ between the sexes in Lepilemur edwardsi (MCP: 2.13 ha for males and 2.07 ha for females; Méndez-Cárdenas and Zimmermann 2009). However, estimates of home range size based on MCPs encompass areas that individuals may have never used and therefore may not accurately reflect patterns of range use (Schülke and Kappeler 2003).

Although the majority (70%) of social units consisted of pairs, some male Lepilemur leucopus were associated with two adult females. Similarly, two out of six males of Lepilemur ruficaudatus occupied home ranges that overlapped extensively with those of two females (Zinner et al. 2003). However, in Lepilemur ruficaudatus these two females associated within a shared home range, making it likely that they represented mother–daughter dyads. In contrast, in Lepilemur leucopus the two females had exclusive ranges as they were regularly seen within the range of the associated adult male but never within the range of the other adult female. Further, all of these females were adults because all of them were seen with dependent offspring. In Hladik and Charles-Dominique’s (1974) study of the same population, the largest of four males was associated with five females, whereas the other males were associated with either one or two females. Based on morphometric data obtained during our capture (unpubl. data), the two males that were associated with two females each were not the largest males. However, their home ranges were 40% and 52% larger than the mean home range of the other males, indicating that energetic constraints on territory defense are not a proximate cause for pair-living from the male perspective (van Schaik and Dunbar 1990).

Ecology and Ranging Behavior

Although we studied individuals of Lepilemur leucopus in a small spiny forest fragment, a crowding effect on ranging patterns seems unlikely. On the one hand, an inverse relationship between density and patch size is frequently observed owing to crowding effects of fragmentation (Bowers and Matter 1997). However, estimates of population density of Lepilemur leucopus at Berenty are much higher for the larger gallery forest (810 individuals/km2) than for the spiny forest (200–350 individuals/km2; Charles-Dominique and Hladik 1971; Hladik and Charles-Dominique 1974). On the other hand, as population densities increase owing to crowding effects, average home range size can be expected to become smaller (Cristóbal-Azkarate and Arroyo-Rodríguez 2007) and/or home range overlap between neighboring social units tends to increase (Arroyo-Rodriguez and Mandujano 2006). Although no quantitative data on ranging behavior are available for the gallery forest population, higher population densities in the gallery forest may imply that home ranges of Lepilemur leucopus are smaller in the gallery than in the spiny forest. In addition, observed home range overlap between neighboring individuals in the spiny forest population was minimal.

Dynamic Spatial Interactions

Using the random gas model, pair partners of Lepilemur leucopus approached each other more often to ≤15 m than expected by chance. Schülke and Kappeler (2003) and Hilgartner et al. (2012) also used the random gas model to calculate expected encounter rates in Phaner pallescens and Lepilemur ruficautadus, respectively, assuming that it defines the far end of interindividual spacing within pairs. The results indicated that pair partners of Phaner pallescens approached each other more often than expected by chance and that encounter rates in Lepilemur ruficaudatus did not deviate from expected values, which was interpreted as a sign of avoidance. According to the random gas model, spectral tarsiers (Tarsius spectrum) living in small family groups were found to spend more time in proximity to other group members than predicted by chance (Gursky 2005).

Hutchinson and Waser (2007) pointed out that the number of expected associations is not given correctly by the random gas model if locational data were collected instantaneously. They proposed a corrected model that is also not affected by variable speed or nonuniform distribution of directions. Using the corrected model, pair partners of Lepilemur leucopus approached each other less often to ≤15 m than expected by chance, indicating active avoidance.

Similar results were obtained using Doncaster’s model (Doncaster 1990), which allows testing for differences between pairs. Using this model, five out of seven pair partners of Lepilemur leucopus approached each other less often than expected. The individuals of the two remaining pairs moved randomly in relation to each other. These two pairs had the smallest joint home range areas. Thus, the restricted area available to them may not have allowed them to avoid each other to the same extent as the partners of the other pairs.

The results obtained using the three different models for testing cohesiveness between pair partners varied considerably. To make a more direct comparison of cohesiveness among nocturnal, pair-living primates, we compared the actual percentage of time pair partners spend within 10 m and 20 m of one another during their active period (Table IV). Whereas Aotus spp. are among the most cohesive nocturnal pair-living primates, association rates are comparatively low for Phaner spp. and Lepilemur spp.

Table IV.

Overview of percentage of time males and females of pair living nocturnal primates spent in proximity to each other during their activity period

Species ≤10 m (%) ≤20 m (%) Reference
Aouts spp. 100 100 Wright (1994)
Tarsius spectrum 28 40 Gursky (2005)
Periodictus potto ? 30 Pimley et al. (2005)
Avahi meridionalis ? 27 Norscia and Borgognini-Tarli (2008)
Phaner pallescens 9 (≤15 m) 23 (≤25 m) Schülke and Kappeler (2003)
Lepilemur ruficaudatus 9 20 Hilgartner et al. (2012)
Lepilemur leucopus 7 23 This study

Sleeping Associations

Pair partners of Lepilemur leucopus never used the same sleeping tree simultaneously, although they shared some of their sleeping trees on consecutive days. In addition, we observed females actively displacing males from their chosen sleeping tree early in the morning at the end of their active period. Lepilemur edwardsi shared sleeping trees on average every second day (Rasoloharijaona et al. 2003). Similarly, Lepilemur ruficaudatus shared sleeping trees every third to fourth day (Zinner et al. 2003), but they always occupied different tree holes within the same tree (R. Hilgartner pers. comm.). Our study does not support the observation that males and females of Lepilemur leucopus sleep together during the day (Russell 1977). However, we observed females sharing their sleeping tree frequently with their offspring.

Other nocturnal primates also exhibit much variation in the composition and stability of sleeping associations. Mouse lemurs (Microcebus spp.: Genin 2010; Radespiel et al. 2003; Weidt et al. 2004), hairy-eared dwarf lemurs (Allocebus trichotis: Biebouw 2009) and slender lorises (Loris lydekkerianus: Nekaris 2003) sleep in groups of variable size and composition during the day, whereas in aye-ayes (Daubentonia madagascariensis: Sterling and Richard 1995) and giant mouse lemurs (Mirza coquereli: Kappeler 1997) adults sleep alone. Among pair-living nocturnal primates fork-marked lemurs (Phaner pallescens: Schülke and Kappeler 2003), dwarf lemurs (Cheirogaleus medius: Fietz 1999), dwarf galagos (Galagoides zanzibaricus: Nash and Harcourt 1986), tarsiers (Tarsius spp.: Driller et al. 2009; Gursky-Doyen 2010), woolly lemurs (Avahi spp.: Harcourt 1991), and owl monkeys (Aotus spp.: Fernandez-Duque 2007) regularly sleep together, whereas pottos (Perodicticus potto: Pimley et al. 2005) and slow lorises (Nycticebus coucang: Wiens and Zitzmann 2003) rarely do so. In conclusion, considerable variation exists within nocturnal primates with regard to cohesiveness, as measured by the frequency of sleeping associations, and Lepilemur leucopus appears to be among the least cohesive species.

Social Interactions

Although males and females were found to associate in pairs, their rate of social interactions was very low (0.14 interactions/h) and most of their interactions consisted of “sitting within 1-5 m.” Similar low interaction rates were described for Lepilemur ruficaudatus (0.27/h; Hilgartner et al. 2012) and Nycticebus coucang, where social interactions made up only 3% of the activity period (Wiens and Zietzmann 2003). However, rates of agonistic interactions were more than 10 times lower in Lepilemur leucopus than in Phaner pallescens, with 0.03 compared to 0.48 interactions/h, perhaps reflecting the fact that they compete over qualitatively different nutritional resources, i.e., leaves vs. tree exudates (Schülke and Kappeler 2003). Further, whereas affiliative interactions were exchanged with a rate of 0.22 interactions/h in Phaner pallescens, they were virtually absent in Lepilemur leucopus. Similarly, affiliative interactions between pair partners were also only very rarely observed in Lepilemur ruficaudatus (Hilgartner et al. 2012). In contrast, pair partners of Perodicticus potto engaged in affiliative behavior during 30% of observations and they exhibited no agonistic interactions (Pimley et al. 2005). Rates of aggression were also much lower than the rates of affiliation in cohesive pair-living Avahi occidentalis (Ramanankirahina et al. 2011).

Rates of direct social interactions are also low in solitary nocturnal primates. For example, in Mirza coquereli, affiliative interactions were generally rare and in particular between the sexes, whereas agonistic interactions occurred disproportionately often between the sexes (Kappeler 1997). In Microcebus murinus, of the 0.12 social interactions/h more agonistic interactions occurred between nonsleeping group members and more affiliative ones between sleeping group members (Dammhahn and Kappeler 2009). Thus, solitary nocturnal primates and those living in dispersed pairs exhibit similarly low rates of social interactions, with Lepilemur leucopus being at the low end of observed values.

Low rates of social interactions do not necessarily indicate a lack of interaction between individuals. Instead, individuals may regulate their relationships mainly through vocal and olfactory signals (Charles-Dominique 1977). In this context, nocturnal primates use loud calls for sexual advertisement (Zimmermann and Lerch 1993) as well as for group aggregation and coordination (Braune et al. 2005). Lepilemur edwardsi also uses duets to regulate space use and cohesiveness (Rasoloharijaona et al. 2006), whereas adult L. ruficaudatus rarely coordinate vocal interactions and loud calling basically serves to signal an animal’s presence in its territory and to regulate spacing among conspecifics (Fichtel and Hilgartner 2012). Lepilemur leucopus produced five types of loud calls, whose functions need to be studied with future playback experiments. Olfactory sensitivity and acuity is higher for species living in dispersed pairs, compared to those living in cohesive pairs or groups (Barton 2006). Scent-marking behavior is less well developed in Lepilemur than in other lemurs because they do not have scent glands, with the exception of paired glands behind the scrotum in males (Petter et al. 1977; Schilling 1979). Lepilemur mustelinus uses nonnutritive tree gouging as a marking behavior to display ownership of sleeping sites whereas the same behavior is absent in L. edwardsi (Rasoloharijaona et al. 2010). Marking behavior in Lepilemur leucopus is inconspicuous; however, we occasionally observed males placing scent marks by rubbing their anogenital region against tree trunks, and only males performed branch bashing displays. Thus, also in terms of communicative complexity, Lepilemur leucopus ranges near the low end of among primates (McComb and Semple 2005).

Possible Causes of Pair Living

Given the virtual absence of direct male–female association and interaction, it is intriguing to speculate about the possible causes of pair living in this and other species living in dispersed pairs (Schülke 2005). Sportive lemurs are seasonal breeders, with a short mating season around May/June (Hilgartner et al. 2008; Randrianambinina et al. 2007). The short annual mating season and female spatial distribution seem to limit the potential of males to monopolize more than one female in Lepilemur ruficaudatus (Hilgartner et al. 2012), and in mammals more generally (Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2013). Thus, mate guarding and female defense may be important components of male reproductive strategies. This is reflected by increased night range overlap between pair partners during the mating season (see also Hilgartner et al. 2008). The small female ranges may facilitate monopolization of the ranges of two females for some males. However, male and female Lepilemur leucopus occupied mutually overlapping home ranges also outside the short annual mating season. Searching for a new mate every year may be more costly than defending the joint territory year-round because of the energetic costs of roaming, increased predation risk during roaming, and the risk of injuries from intrasexual competition (Ralls et al. 2007). Females may potentially profit from year-round associations with a male by territorial defense, and hence reduced food competition (Wrangham 1979), by protection against infanticide (van Schaik and Kappeler 1997) and by paternal care (van Schaik and van Hooff 1983). However, competition for food is low, even during the lean season, indicating that a possible resource defense strategy by males may play only a minor role in this species (unpubl. data). Although paternal care is absent in sportive lemurs (Hilgartner et al. 2008), infanticide was observed in Lepilemur edwardsi (Rasoloharijaona et al. 2000) and we observed one case of male infanticide, indicating that infanticide risk may play a role in the evolution and maintenance of dispersed pairs (Opie et al. 2013). Further, females may preferentially mate with males they are familiar with (Fisher et al. 2003), and the stability of pair bonds may have an effect on long-term reproductive success. In owl monkeys (Aotus azari) stable pairs reproduced once a year, whereas only about 20% of newly formed pairs produced offspring within the first year of pair formation (Fernandez-Duque and Huck 2013).

Social Complexity

On the basis of all currently recognized dimensions of social complexity (Freeberg et al. 2012), white-footed sportive lemurs lie at or near the low end of all respective measures. Their modal group size is at the theoretical minimum and they rarely interact with neighbors, i.e., they do not interact frequently with many different individuals. Moreover, observed social interactions with physical contact were limited to bouts of agonism, and neither a single bout of grooming nor mating were observed in >1500 h of observations. In fact, most pair partners actively avoided each other, and most interactions were recorded only because we defined sitting in proximity as a social interaction. Thus, social interactions did also not occur in many different contexts and they occurred with negligible frequencies. Finally, this lack of social complexity was not compensated by high levels of communicative complexity because the sizes of their vocal and olfactory repertoires were among the smallest ones reported for primates so far. Thus, we propose that this species of sportive lemur can be used to define a baseline of primate social complexity against which comparable data from other species can be scaled, so that the adjective “highly social” that is increasingly being used to characterize species (Bateman et al. 2012; Hoelzel et al. 2007) can actually be used in a meaningful way.

Conclusions

White-footed sportive lemurs were found to live in dispersed pairs, resolving questions about their social organization based on earlier studies at the same site. Males and females sharing a home range were characterized by low spatial cohesiveness, including signs of active avoidance, as well as very low rates of direct social interactions. This social system may ultimately be the result of male reproductive strategies, but the determination of the possible causes of pair living in this species requires further study. In any event, Lepilemur leucopus is the most asocial of all primates living in pairs studied to date, placing it at or near the primate baseline of social complexity.

Acknowledgments

We thank the de Heaulme family for permission to conduct this study at Berenty Nature Reserve. We thank Dr. Daniel Rakotondravony, Dr. Rodin Rasoloarison, and the other members of the Département de Biologie Animale de l’Universite´ d’Antananarivo for their cooperation and administrative support. We appreciate the help of the blow-darting expert Mamitiana Razafindrasamba as well as Mahamaro and Jo from Berenty during the capture. We especially thank field assistant Mbelomanitra Mahefa Rahajarivony for continued assistance during data collection, Hajarimanitra Rambeloarivony for logistic support in the field, as well as Sonomena for daily care. We wish to thank two anonymous reviewers, Claudia Fichtel, and Oliver Schülke for helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. The research permit was provided by Commission Tripartite CAFF (Madagascar). Financial support was provided by the German Primate Center Göttingen and the DFG (KA 1082 ⁄ 21-1).

References

  1. Altmann J. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour. 1974;49:227–265. doi: 10.1163/156853974X00534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arroyo-Rodríguez V, Mandujano S. Forest fragmentation modifies habitat quality for Alouatta palliata. International Journal of Primatology. 2006;27:1079–1096. doi: 10.1007/s10764-006-9061-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barelli C, Boesch C, Heistermann M, Reichard UH. Female white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) lead group movements and have priority of access to food resources. Behaviour. 2008;145:965–981. doi: 10.1163/156853908784089243. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barton RA. Olfactory evolution and behavioral ecology in primates. American Journal of Primatology. 2006;68:545–558. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bateman AW, Ozgul A, Coulson T, Clutton-Brock TH. Density dependence in group dynamics of a highly social mongoose, Suricata suricatta. Journal of Animal Ecology. 2012;81:628–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2656.2011.01934.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bearder SK. Lorises, bushbabies, and tarsiers: Diverse societies in solitary foragers. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT, editors. Primate societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1987. pp. 11–24. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bearder SK. Physical and social diversity among nocturnal primates: a new view based on long term research. Primates. 1999;40:267–282. doi: 10.1007/BF02557715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bergman TJ. Experimental evidence for limited vocal recognition in a wild primate: implications for the social complexity hypothesis. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 2010;277:3045–3053. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Biebouw K. Home range size and use in Allocebus trichotis in Analamazaotra special reserve, central eastern Madagascar. International Journal of Primatology. 2009;30:367–386. doi: 10.1007/s10764-009-9349-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bond AB, Kamil AC, Balda RP. Social complexity and transitive inference in corvids. Animal Behaviour. 2003;65:479–487. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2101. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bowers MA, Matter SF. Landscape ecology of mammals: relationships between density and patch size. Journal of Mammalogy. 1997;78:999–1013. doi: 10.2307/1383044. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Braune P, Schmidt S, Zimmermann E. Spacing and group coordination in a nocturnal primate, the golden brown mouse lemur. Behavioral Ecolology and Sociobiology. 2005;58:587–596. doi: 10.1007/s00265-005-0944-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Byrne RW, Whiten A. Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Oxford: Clarendon Press; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  14. Calenge C. The package adehabitat for the R software: a tool for the analysis of space and habitat use by animals. Ecological Modelling. 2006;197:516–519. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2006.03.017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Charles-Dominique P. Ecology and behaviour of nocturnal primates. London: Duckworth; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  16. Charles-Dominique P, Hladik CM. Le Lepilemur du sud de Madagascar: ecologie, alimentation et vie sociale. Terre Vie. 1971;25:3–61. [Google Scholar]
  17. Cords M. Primates in perspective. International Journal of Primatology. 2007;28:497–500. doi: 10.1007/s10764-007-9131-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cristóbal-Azkarate J, Arroyo-Rodríguez V. Diet and activity pattern of howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) in Los Tuxtlas, Mexico: effects of habitat fragmentation and implications for conservation. American Journal of Primatology. 2007;69:1013–1029. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Crook JH, Gartlan JC. Evolution of primate societies. Nature. 1966;210:1200–1203. doi: 10.1038/2101200a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dammhahn M, Kappeler PM. Females go where the food is: does the socio-ecological model explain variation in social organisation of solitary foragers? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2009;63:939–952. doi: 10.1007/s00265-009-0737-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. De Solla SR, Bonduriansky R, Brooks RJ. Eliminating autocorrelation reduces biological relevance of home range estimates. Journal of Animal Ecology. 1999;68:221–234. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2656.1999.00279.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. de Waal FBM, Tyack PL. Animal social complexity: Intelligence, culture, and individualized societies. Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  23. Doncaster CP. Nonparametric estimates of interaction from radio-tracking data. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 1990;143:431–443. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80020-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Driller C, Perwitasari-Farajallah D, Zischler H, Merker S. The social system of Lariang tarsiers (Tarsius lariang) as revealed by genetic analyses. International Journal of Primatology. 2009;30:267–281. doi: 10.1007/s10764-009-9341-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dunbar RIM. The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology. 1998;6:178–190. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1998)6:5&#x0003c;178::AID-EVAN5&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Dunbar RIM, Shultz S. Evolution in the social brain. Science. 2007;317:1344–1347. doi: 10.1126/science.1145463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Eberle M, Kappeler PM. Mouse lemurs in space and time: a test of the socioecological model. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2002;51:131–139. doi: 10.1007/s002650100409. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fernandez-Duque E. The Aotinae: Social monogamy in the only nocturnal haplorhines. In: Bearder S, Campbell C, Fuentes A, MacKinnon K, Panger M, editors. Primates in perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 139–154. [Google Scholar]
  29. Fernandez-Duque E, Huck M. Till death (or an intruder) do us part: intrasexual-competition in a monogamous primate. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e53724. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fichtel C, Hilgartner R. Noises in the dark: Vocal communication in nocturnal pair-livingprimates. In: Master J, Gamba M, Génin F, editors. Leaping ahead: Advances in prosimian biology. New York: Springer Science+Business Media; 2012. pp. 297–304. [Google Scholar]
  31. Fietz J. Monogamy as a rule rather than exception in nocturnal lemurs: the case of the fat-tailed dwarf lemur, Cheirogaleus medius. Ethology. 1999;105:259–272. doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0310.1999.00380.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fisher HS, Swaisgood RR, Fitch-Snyder H. Odor familiarity and female preferences for males in a threatened primate, the pygmy loris (Nycticebus pygmaeus): applications for genetic management of small populations. Naturwissenschaften. 2003;90:509–512. doi: 10.1007/s00114-003-0465-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Freeberg TM, Dunbar RIM, Ord TJ. Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 2012;367:1785–1801. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Genin F. Who sleeps with whom? Sleeping association and socio-territoriality in Microcebus griseorufus. Journal of Mammalogy. 2010;91:942–951. doi: 10.1644/09-MAMM-A-239.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Grubb P. Interpreting some outstanding features of the flora and vegetation of Madagascar. Perspectives in Plant Ecology, Evolution and Systematics. 2003;6:125–146. doi: 10.1078/1433-8319-00046. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Gursky S. Associations between adult spectral tarsiers. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2005;128:74–83. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.20104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Gursky-Doyen S. Intraspecific variation in the mating system of spectral tarsiers. International Journal of Primatology. 2010;31:1161–1173. doi: 10.1007/s10764-010-9450-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Harcourt C. Diet and behaviour of a nocturnal lemur, Avahi laniger, in the wild. Journal of Zoology. 1991;223:667–674. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1991.tb04395.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hilgartner R, Zinner D, Kappeler PM. Life history traits and parental care in Lepilemur ruficaudatus. American Journal of Primatology. 2008;70:2–11. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hilgartner R, Fichtel C, Kappeler PM, Zinner D. Determinants of pair-living in red-tailed sportive lemurs (Lepilemur ruficaudatus) Ethology. 2012;118:466–479. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2012.02033.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Hladik CM, Charles-Dominique P. The behavior and ecology of the sportive lemur (Lepilemur mustelinus) in relation to its dietary peculiarities. In: Martin RD, Doyle GA, Walker AC, editors. Prosimian biology. London: Duckworth; 1974. pp. 23–37. [Google Scholar]
  42. Hoelzel AR, Hey J, Dahlheim ME, Nicholson C, Burkanov V, Black N. Evolution of population structure in a highly social top predator, the killer whale. Molecular Biology and Evolution. 2007;24:1407–1415. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msm063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hoffmann, M. (2008). Lepilemur leucopus. In IUCN 2012. IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2012.2. http://www.iucnredlist.org (Accessed December 12, 2012).
  44. Hutchinson JMC, Waser PM. Use, misuse and extensions of “ideal gas” models of animal encounter. Biological Reviews. 2007;82:335–359. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00014.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kappeler PM. Intrasexual selection in Mirza coquereli: evidence for scramble competition polygyny in a solitary primate. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1997;41:115–127. doi: 10.1007/s002650050371. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kappeler PM. Behavioral ecology of strepsirrhines and tarsiers. In: Mitani J, Call J, Kappeler PM, Palombit R, Silk JB, editors. Evolution of primate societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2012. pp. 17–42. [Google Scholar]
  47. Kappeler PM, van Schaik CP. Evolution of primate social systems. International Journal of Primatology. 2002;23:707–740. doi: 10.1023/A:1015520830318. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kappeler PM, Barrett L, Blumstein DT, Clutton-Brock TH. Constraints and flexibility in mammalian social behaviour: introduction and synthesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2013;368(1618):20120337. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kernohan BJ, Gitzen RA, Millspaugh JJ. Analysis of animal space use and movements. In: Millspaugh JJ, Marzluff JM, editors. Radio tracking animal populations. San Diego: Academic Press; 2001. pp. 125–166. [Google Scholar]
  50. Lawrence, M. A. (2012). ez: Easy analysis and visualization offactorial experiments. R package version 4.1-1. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ez
  51. Lehmann J, Ross C. Baboon (Papio anubis) social complexity—a network approach. American Journal of Primatology. 2011;73:775–789. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lukas D, Clutton-Brock TH. The evolution of social monogamy in mammals. Science. 2013;341:526–530. doi: 10.1126/science.1238677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. McComb K, Semple S. Coevolution of vocal communication and sociality in primates. Biology Letters. 2005;1:381–385. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2005.0366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Méndez-Cárdenas MG, Zimmermann E. Duetting—a mechanism to strengthen pair bonds in a dispersed pair-living primate (Lepilemur edwardsi)? American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2009;139:523–532. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.21017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Mitani J, Call J, Kappeler PM, Palombit R, Silk JB. The evolution of primate societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  56. Müller AE. Aspects of social life in the fat-tailed dwarf lemur (Cheirogaleus medius): inferences from body weights and trapping data. American Journal of Primatology. 1999;49:265–280. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(199911)49:3&#x0003c;265::AID-AJP4&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-U. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Müller AE, Thalmann U. Origin and evolution of primate social organisation: a reconstruction. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 2000;75:405–435. doi: 10.1017/S0006323100005533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Munshi-South J. Extra-pair paternity and the evolution of testis size in a behaviorally monogamous tropical mammal, the large tree shrew (Tupaia tana) Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2007;62:201–212. doi: 10.1007/s00265-007-0454-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Nash LT, Harcourt CS. Social organization of galagos in Kenyan coastal forests: I. Galago zanzibaricus. American Journal of Primatology. 1986;10:339–355. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350100406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Nekaris KAI. Spacing system of the Mysore slender loris (Loris lydekkerianus lydekkerianus) American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2003;121:86–96. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.10189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Nekaris KAI, Bearder SK. The Lorisiform primates of Asia and mainland Africa. In: Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, MacKinnon KC, Bearder SK, Stumpf RM, editors. Primates in perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2011. pp. 34–54. [Google Scholar]
  62. Norscia I, Borgognini-Tarli SM. Ranging behavior and possible correlates of pair-living in southeastern avahis (Madagascar) International Journal of Primatology. 2008;29:153–171. doi: 10.1007/s10764-007-9219-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Norscia I, Palagi E. Berenty 2006: census of Propithecus verreauxi and possible evidence of population stress. International Journal of Primatology. 2008;29:1099–1115. doi: 10.1007/s10764-008-9259-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Opie, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Dunbar, R. I. M., Shultz, S. (2013). Male infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 110, 13328–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  65. Pereira ME, Kappeler PM. Divergent systems of agonistic behaviour in lemurid primates. Behaviour. 1997;134:225–274. doi: 10.1163/156853997X00467. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. Petter JJ, Albignac R, Rumpler Y. Faune de Madagascar 44: Mammifères lémuriens (Primates Prosimiens) Paris: ORSTROM/CNRS; 1977. [Google Scholar]
  67. Pimley ER, Bearder SK, Dixson AF. Social organization of the Milne-Edward's potto. American Journal of Primatology. 2005;66:317–330. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. R Core Team (2012). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. http://www.R-project.org/.
  69. Radespiel U. Sociality in the gray mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus) in northwestern Madagascar. American Journal of Primatology. 2000;51:21–40. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(200005)51:1&#x0003c;21::AID-AJP3&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Radespiel U, Ehresmann P, Zimmermann E. Species-specific usage of sleeping sites in two sympatric mouse lemur species (Microcebus murinus and M. ravelobensis) in northwestern Madagascar. American Journal of Primatology. 2003;59:139–151. doi: 10.1002/ajp.10071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Ralls K, Cypher B, Spiegel LK. Social monogamy in kit foxes: formation, association, duration, and dissolution of mated pairs. Journal of Mammalogy. 2007;88:1439–1446. doi: 10.1644/06-MAMM-A-348R.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Ramanankirahina R, Joly M, Zimmermann E. Peaceful primates: affiliation, aggression, and the question of female dominance in a nocturnal pair-living lemur (Avahi occidentalis) American Journal of Primatology. 2011;73:1261–1268. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Ramaromilanto B, Lei R, Engberg SE, Johnson SE, Sitzmann BD, Louis EE. Sportive lemur diversity at Mananara-Nord Biosphere Reserve, Madagascar. Occasional Papers, Museum of Texas Tech University. 2009;286:1–22. [Google Scholar]
  74. Randrianambinina B, Mbotizafy S, Rasoloharijaona S, Ravoahangimalala RO, Zimmermann E. Seasonality in reproduction of Lepilemur edwardsi. International Journal of Primatology. 2007;28:783–790. doi: 10.1007/s10764-007-9158-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rasoloharijaona S, Rakotosamimanana B, Zimmermann E. Infanticide by a male Milne-Edwards’ sportive lemur (Lepilemur edwardsi) in Ampijoroa NW-Madagascar. International Journal of Primatology. 2000;21:41–45. doi: 10.1023/A:1005419528718. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  76. Rasoloharijaona S, Rakotosamimanana B, Randrianambinina B, Zimmermann E. Pair-specific usage of sleeping sites and their implications for social organization in a nocturnal Malagasy primate, the Milne Edwards’ sportive lemur (Lepilemur edwardsi) American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2003;122:251–258. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.10281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Rasoloharijaona S, Randrianambinina B, Braune P, Zimmermann E. Loud calling, spacing, and cohesiveness in a nocturnal primate, the Milne Edwards’ sportive lemur (Lepilemur edwardsi) American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2006;129:591–600. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.20342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Rasoloharijaona S, Randrianambinina B, Joly-Radko M. Does nonnutritive tree gouging in a rainforest-dwelling lemur convey resource ownership as does loud calling in a dry forest-dwelling lemur? American Journal of Primatology. 2010;72:1062–1072. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Rooney SM, Wolfe A, Hayden TJ. Autocorrelated data in telemetry studies: time to independence and the problem of behavioural effects. Mammal Review. 1998;28:89–98. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2907.1998.00028.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Russell, R. J. (1977). The behavior, ecology, and environmental physiology of a nocturnal primate, Lepilemur mustelinus. PhD thesis, Duke University, Durham, NC.
  81. Schilling A. Olfactory communication in prosimians. In: Doyle GA, Martin RD, editors. The study of prosimian behavior. New York: Academic Press; 1979. pp. 461–542. [Google Scholar]
  82. Schubert M, Pillay N, Ribble DO, Schradin C. The round-eared sengi and the evolution of social monogamy: factors that constrain males to live with a single female. Ethology. 2009;115:972–985. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01684.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  83. Schülke O. Evolution of pair-living in Phaner furcifer. International Journal of Primatology. 2005;26:903–919. doi: 10.1007/s10764-005-5328-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  84. Schülke O, Kappeler PM. So near and yet so far: territorial pairs but low cohesion between pair partners in a nocturnal lemur, Phaner furcifer. Animal Behaviour. 2003;65:331–343. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Schülke O, Kappeler PM, Zischler H. Small testes size despite high extra-pair paternity in the pair-living nocturnal primate Phaner furcifer. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2004;55:293–301. doi: 10.1007/s00265-003-0709-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  86. Shultz S, Dunbar RIM. The evolution of the social brain: Anthropoid primates contrast with other vertebrates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 2007;274:2429–2436. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.0693. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Silverman BW. Density estimation for statistics and data analyses. London: Chapman and Hall; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  88. Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT. Primate societies. Chicago: Chicago University Press; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  89. Sterling EJ, Richard AF. Social organization in the aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis) and the perceived distinctiveness of nocturnal primates. In: Alterman L, Doyle GA, Izard MK, editors. Creatures of the dark: The nocturnal prosimians. New York: Plenum Press; 1995. pp. 439–451. [Google Scholar]
  90. van Schaik CP. Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour. 1983;87:120–144. doi: 10.1163/156853983X00147. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. van Schaik CP, Dunbar RIM. The evolution of monogamy in large primates: a new hypothesis and some crucial tests. Behaviour. 1990;115:30–62. doi: 10.1163/156853990X00284. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  92. van Schaik CP, Kappeler PM. Infanticide risk and the evolution of male-female association in primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 1997;264:1687–1694. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. van Schaik CP, van Hooff JARAM. On the ultimate causes of primate social systems. Behaviour. 1983;85:91–117. doi: 10.1163/156853983X00057. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  94. Waser PM. Cercocebus albigena—site attachment, avoidance, and intergroup spacing. American Naturalist. 1976;110:911–935. doi: 10.1086/283117. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  95. Weidt A, Hagenah N, Randrianambinina B, Radespiel U, Zimmermann E. Social organization of the golden brown mouse lemur (Microcebus ravelobensis) American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2004;123:40–51. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.10296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Wiens F, Zitzmann A. Social structure of the solitary slow loris Nycticebus coucang (Lorisidae) Journal of Zoology. 2003;261:35–46. doi: 10.1017/S0952836903003947. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  97. Worton BJ. Kernel methods for estimating the utilization distribution in home-range studies. Ecology. 1989;70:164–168. doi: 10.2307/1938423. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Wrangham RW. On the evolution of ape social systems. Social Science Information. 1979;18:335–368. doi: 10.1177/053901847901800301. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. Wright, P. C. (1994). The behavior and ecology of the owl monkey. In J. F. Baer, R. E. Weller, & I. Kakoma (Eds.), Aotus: the owl monkey (pp. 97–112). San Diego: Academic Press.
  100. Zimmermann E, Lerch C. The complex acoustic design of an advertisement call in male mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus, Prosimii, Primates) and sources of its variation. Ethology. 1993;93:211–224. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1993.tb00990.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  101. Zinner D, Hilgartner RD, Kappeler PM, Pietsch T, Ganzhorn JU. Social organization of Lepilemur ruficaudatus. International Journal of Primatology. 2003;24:869–888. doi: 10.1023/A:1024684907250. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Articles from International Journal of Primatology are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES