The etiology and pathophysiology of body dysmorphic disorder (BDD), as with most psychiatric disorders, is likely complex. Because research is limited, the causative and contributory factors to the development of BDD are unclear. Yet there is emerging evidence from research studies of various factors that may contribute to the development and maintenance of BDD symptoms.
This review of the etiology and pathophysiology of BDD explores what has been elucidated thus far from research on developmental, psychosocial, cognitive and behavioral, neuropsychological, and neurobiological (visual processing, neuroanatomical, genetic, and neurochemical) factors. We explore “submodels” of BDD within these domains, and we synthesize these to generate an overarching, yet preliminary, model of the etiological and pathophysiological processes that appear to contribute to the development and maintenance of BDD. This model involves a context of likely biological and genetic susceptibility upon which adverse life events interact with cognitive distortions and subsequent learned behavior to result in BDD symptoms. Given the limited research base, this model – while reflecting available research evidence – serves primarily a heuristic function. We hope nonetheless that this model informs clinicians’ thinking about how to understand patients with BDD.
DEVELOPMENTAL FACTORS
Developmental factors in BDD are under-researched relative to other disorders of similar prevalence, and hence remain largely unknown. Nevertheless, there is preliminary evidence that sexual, emotional, and physical abuse in childhood may be associated with BDD. One study found that 38% of 50 BDD patients reported some form of abuse during childhood (28% emotional abuse; 22% sexual abuse; 14% physical abuse) as compared to 14% of a comparison group of 50 OCD patients1. In a study of 75 BDD subjects who completed the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire, 78.7% reported a history of childhood maltreatment (68.0% emotional neglect; 56.0% emotional abuse; 34.7% physical abuse; 33.3% physical neglect; and 28.0% sexual abuse)2. However, because these two studies are cross-sectional, they cannot establish a causative role for childhood trauma in the development of BDD.
BDD most often begins in adolescence3, a period in development of accelerated changes in physical appearance as well as hormonal milieu; these factors may potentially contribute to, or trigger, the development of BDD, but have yet to be studied directly.
SOCIAL FACTORS
Social interactions can be a significant source of childhood adversity for some. Teasing related to physical stigmata, such as acne, may have a long-term impact on an individual, negatively affecting one’s thoughts and emotions. Once the negative association is made between the appearance feature and others’ reactions to it, everyday interactions and experiences can continue to imprint these thoughts and emotions, which may be theorized to contribute to symptoms of BDD4. In one study, individuals with BDD reported more incidences of teasing, especially about appearance and competency, than mentally healthy controls5.
While childhood adversity as a risk factor is relatively nonspecific, heightened aesthetic sensitivity may be more specific to BDD. Aesthetic sensitivity refers to awareness and appreciation of beauty and harmony and includes symmetry, averageness, secondary sexual characteristics, and general attractiveness6. There is evidence that individuals with BDD have heightened aesthetic sensitivity7, However, it is unclear whether this is the result of specific social influences, a trait that predisposes to BDD, or the result of having BDD symptoms.
CULTURAL INFLUENCES
Cases of BDD have been reported in various countries, including the US, Canada, Australia, China, Japan, South America, and the former Soviet Union among others8. Bohne et al. (2002) conducted the only cross cultural study published to date and found that BDD prevalence rates are fairly similar between American (4%) and Germans students (5.3%)9. Cultural factors may play a role in which body parts are of concern, and how other BDD symptoms are expressed, as different cultures (as well as races or ethnicities) may have variations in aesthetic standards of beauty10, but this has not yet been studied in relation to BDD. On the other hand, there is evidence of invariant standards of beauty such as averageness, symmetry, and sexual dimorphism; these appear to cross culture, gender, and age, and emerge early in development before individuals experience the influence of culture and the media11.
COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL AND LEARNING MODELS OF BDD
Cognitive-behavioral and learning models can provide a formulation for how biological, cultural, and social factors lead to the development and maintenance of BDD, and are based on classical and operant conditioning12. These models mainly serve a heuristic function, as research is needed to test conditioning effects in individuals with BDD.
Operant conditioning (based on reinforcement) coupled with social learning results in the development of values and beliefs about attractiveness, including a sense of value conditioned on body image13. Early experiences that positively reinforce an individual for appearance may play an important role in BDD development, but have yet to be studied directly.
In the framework of classical conditioning, negative events involving one’s physical appearance (e.g. teasing at the onset of puberty) may serve as unconditioned stimuli and cause an unconditioned negative emotional response (e.g. anxiety, disgust, or shame). Anything paired with this is also evaluated as negative, such as words or images of the body part. Higher order conditioning may account for additional appearance concerns secondary to the patient’s primary concern. As a result of higher-order conditioning the negative emotional reaction triggered by exposure to the primary body part of concern may generalize to other body parts that are noticed during this negative emotional state13.
Once BDD symptoms have been established and appearance-related beliefs, assumptions, and values developed, it has been hypothesized that secondary operant conditioning in the form of negative reinforcement serves to maintain the maladaptive behaviors and cognitions13, 14. More specifically, BDD individuals’ compulsive behaviors serve to reduce short-term distress by alleviating the negative emotional reaction triggered by either an intrusive thought about perceived appearance flaws or contact with the perceived flaw. For example, BDD patients often avoid looking into mirrors, which may result in relief as a result of not coming in contact with their image. This relief represents a negative reinforcer in that it increases the probability that the avoidance behavior will be used again when in a similar situation. However, this example of mirror use is complicated; mirror-checking is a common behavior in BDD and may also represent negative reinforcement since the patient’s negative emotional reaction may be temporarily relieved when they perceive (at times) that they look less defective.
NEUROCOGNITIVE FUNCTIONING
Researchers have begun to examine neurocognitive functioning in BDD, which may shed light on the etiology and maintenance of BDD's key symptoms. When presented with a complex visuospatial task (Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, ROCF) and the instruction to draw the figure, individuals with BDD tended to overfocus on details, which interfered with their ability to encode and ultimately recall the figure15. In a recent study female BDD subjects, relative to women with or without a disfiguring dermatological condition, were significantly more accurate at rating the degree of distortion of faces in which a feature (e.g., nose) had been changed16. Thus, BDD subjects appear to be more likely than others to notice minor deviations from beauty standards. These neuropsychological deficits related to an excessive focus on detail (versus global) processing, and sensitivity to perceived flaws may be related to one of the core clinical features of BDD. For example, when gazing in the mirror, BDD patients often selectively focus on minor flaws while ignoring global aspects of their appearance. This appears to trigger shame, anxiety and increased appearance preoccupations.
Recent research has also examined emotion recognition in BDD17. Subjects were presented with photographs of faces varying in emotional expressions18 and answered two sets of questions: one including self-referent scenarios (e.g.,, “Imagine that the bank teller is looking at you. What is his facial expression like?”) and one with other-referent scenarios (e.g., “Imagine that the bank teller is looking at a friend of yours….”). Subjects then were instructed to identify the emotional expression of the face. BDD subjects misinterpreted significantly more neutral expressions as contemptuous and angry when compared to healthy controls. This bias was found only in self-referent scenarios but not in other-referent scenarios. These results are consistent with prior research on emotion recognition in individuals with BDD19 and with the ideas and delusions of reference often experienced by individuals with BDD (e.g., thoughts that others laugh about or stare at the perceived flaw). Moreover, in a study in which BDD patients were asked to interpret various ambiguous scenarios, they reported threatening interpretations for general situations, social situations, and body-focused situations, whereas healthy control subjects showed no bias and OCD subjects had a bias toward threatening interpretations only for general situations20. It is possible that biases to interpret situations as threatening and faces as contemptuous might exacerbate social anxiety and social avoidance in BDD.
In an emotional Stroop study, BDD subjects and healthy control subjects were presented with words varying in color and emotional valence. Individuals with BDD selectively attended to words like "beauty" and "attractive," delaying their responses to these words21. This finding is consistent with clinical observations that they are preoccupied with their appearance ideal. In another recent study individuals with BDD, individuals with subclinical BDD symptoms, and healthy control participants participated in the Implicit Association Test (IAT)22. The IAT requires the rapid categorization of various stimuli, and easier pairings (and faster responses) are interpreted as being more strongly associated in memory. In comparison to control participants, BDD participants had significantly lower implicit self-esteem (automatic beliefs about one’s self that occur outside of conscious awareness or control), and the subclinical BDD participants were intermediate between these groups23. BDD participants also had significantly stronger implicit associations between attractive and competent than the other groups. Attractive Competent and Self-Esteem scores were significantly associated with BDD symptom severity, distress, and avoidance during a mirror exposure task, suggesting that maladaptive behaviors in BDD might be fueled by automatic processes outside of conscious control.
Furthermore, when rating the attractiveness of various photographs of faces, including their own, individuals with BDD underestimated their own attractiveness and overestimated the attractiveness of beautiful faces of others24. BDD subjects also endorsed high levels of perfectionistic thinking. (In terms of other personality traits, individuals with BDD have also shown high levels of neuroticism and low extroversion)25.
These findings require replication, and they do not necessarily indicate that these factors are causal mechanisms in BDD, as the studies are cross-sectional. In other words, they do not tell us whether the above processes contribute to BDD, result from BDD, both, or neither. Nonetheless, this research informs our conceptualization of this disorder and forms the basis of several proposed models of the development and maintenance of BDD4, 26, 27. Taken together, these findings indicate that individuals with BDD appear to selectively attend to details and are particularly sensitive to appearance flaws. They may also falsely interpret others’ facial expressions as negative, and may misinterpret every-day social situations as threatening. BDD sufferers appear to be preoccupied with their beauty ideal and strongly associate being attractive with being competent. They appear to overestimate the attractiveness of beautiful others and underestimate their own attractiveness. These information-processing biases likely lead to feelings of shame, depression and anxiety.
NEUROBIOLOGICAL FACTORS
Visual Processing
Clinical observation suggests that individuals with BDD may have perceptual distortions, as they perceive defects in their appearance that are not observable or appear minor to others. These observations, in addition to above-mentioned neuropsychological findings of aberrant visuospatial processing15, suggests possible disturbances in visual perception and/or visuospatial processing.
The first neuroimaging study to investigate this in BDD examined visual processing of others’ faces28. Twelve BDD subjects and thirteen healthy controls were scanned with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) while matching photographs of faces that were unaltered or altered to contain primarily low detail (conveying configural/holistic information) or high-detail information. BDD subjects demonstrated greater left hemisphere activity relative to controls in an extended face-processing network for all image types (particularly lateral prefrontal cortex and temporal lobe), which was most prominent for the low-detail faces. This imbalance in laterality suggests greater detailed and piecemeal face processing and lesser configural and holistic processing.
A subsequent fMRI study in BDD examined own-face processing. Seventeen BDD subjects and sixteen healthy controls29 viewed photographs of their own face and a familiar face that were similarly unaltered or altered to include only high- or low-detail visual information. BDD subjects demonstrated relative hypoactivity in primary and secondary visual cortical systems for low-detail images of their own and familiar faces, suggesting abnormal brain activity for configural and holistic elements. In addition, BDD subjects demonstrated hyperactivity in left orbitofrontal cortex and bilateral head of the caudate when viewing unaltered photos of their own face compared to a familiar face. Aversiveness ratings were associated with lesser activity in occipital regions for low-detail faces. Moreover, fronto-striatal activation correlated with severity of BDD-related obsessive thoughts and compulsive behaviors.
To better understand whether individuals with BDD have more general abnormalities in visual processing, the same group performed an fMRI experiment in which 14 BDD subjects and 14 healthy controls matched photographs of houses29. The BDD group demonstrated abnormal relative hypoactivity in left visual association areas, including the parahippocampal place area, for low-detail images and abnormal relative hyperactivity of prefrontal systems for high-detail images. Controlling for depression symptom severity resulted in relative hypoactivity in dorsal visual stream (used for configural and holistic as well as motion processing) for all stimulus types. Severity of BDD symptoms also correlated with decreased activity in dorsal visual stream. This abnormal hypoactivity of visual systems for configural and holistic processing in non-symptom-related stimuli suggests general abnormalities in lower- and higher-order visual processing in BDD.
Other evidence suggesting an imbalance in local (detail) vs. global (holistic) processing comes from a study of inverted faces30. Eighteen BDD subjects and 17 healthy controls viewed sets of others’ faces that were upright or inverted. Inverted faces resulted in less slowing of response time in BDD subjects relative to healthy controls. This finding suggests a greater reliance on part decomposition and detail processing, for which inversion has less of an effect than holistic processing (due to the fact that humans have a holistic template for upright faces)31. However, for the subset of short-duration stimuli, which only allow time for configural/holistic processing, the inversion effect was normal. This suggests that the imbalance of local over global processing emerges only after longer viewing times.
Taken together, these studies, while requiring replication, provide converging evidence of abnormal visual processing in BDD. Specifically, visual systems responsible for configural and holistic processing demonstrate relative hypoactivity for a variety of stimuli, including one’s own and others’ faces, as well as non-face objects. This suggests a model of distorted visual processing in which details are not contextualized or integrated into a whole percept. This may be associated with patients’ propensity to focus on details of their appearance while seeming less able to view themselves in an integrated, holistic manner. The inverted faces study additionally suggests that this global vs. local imbalance may depend on viewing duration, such that at viewing durations of several seconds to several minutes there is a greater propensity for local/detailed processing. There also appears to be abnormal hyperactivity in fronto-striatal systems specifically for own-face viewing, which may be associated with tendency for obsessive thoughts and compulsive BDD behaviors (see Didie et al in this issue). A similar pattern of fronto-striatal hyperactivity with symptom provocation has been found in studies of OCD, particularly for checking subtypes32.
Brain Morphometry in BDD
Only three small morphometric neuroimaging studies have been performed in BDD. A study of females found a leftward shift in caudate volume asymmetry and greater white matter volume in BDD than healthy control subjects33. A study of males similarly found greater white matter volume in BDD relative to healthy control subjects, as well as smaller orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate and a trend for larger thalamic volumes34. These studies’ results are consistent with abnormalities in fronto-striatal systems. However, a third study of males and females found no significant volumetric differences in BDD vs healthy controls35.
Genetics
Susceptibility for BDD may be heritable. This is evidenced by the observation that 8% of individuals with BDD have a first-degree family member with a lifetime diagnosis of BDD36, which is about 3–8 times the prevalence in the general population. Seven percent of BDD patients also have a first-degree relative with OCD37, and first-degree relatives of OCD patients have six times the lifetime prevalence of BDD than controls36, showing shared heritability between BDD and OCD. Despite limited research on specific gene involvement in BDD, in a preliminary candidate gene study of 57 BDD subjects and 58 healthy controls matched for ethnicity and gender, association was demonstrated for GABAA-γ2 (5q31.1-q33.2) (p=.032), with the 1 (A) allele occurring more frequently in BDD subjects than controls38.
Neurochemistry of BDD
There is some preliminary evidence suggesting a role for serotonin function in BDD. One study found decreased platelet serotonin transporter binding density in OCD and OCD-related disorders, including BDD39. In a case study dietary depletion of tryptophan (a serotonin precursor amino acid) led to exacerbation of BDD but not OCD symptoms in a patient with BDD and OCD40. In other case reports, the serotonin receptor partial agonist psilocybin led to decreased BDD symptoms41, and a serotonin antagonist led to the onset of BDD symptoms in a patient42. On the other hand, a mixed serotonin agonist led to increased preoccupation with perceived body defects in BDD in another case43, suggesting that the role of serotonin in BDD is likely to be complex, and not fully understood. Additionally, SRIs have been shown to be efficacious in treating BDD (see article by Phillips in this issue). This suggests possible involvement of the serotonin system in the disorder, but it does not prove that serotonergic abnormalities are part of BDD’s underlying pathophysiology.
Neuroethology
Excessive grooming behaviors, for example, excessive face washing, plucking hairs, or skin-picking, are common in BDD44. Pathological grooming conditions in animals have long been observed, and may provide analogous models for these behaviors in BDD45.
Preliminary Neurobiological Model of the Pathophysiology of BDD
Attempts to integrate these findings result in a complex proposed model that implicates brain dysfunction and neurochemistry in the development of BDD. Right hemisphere dysfunction may be involved in the development of BDD, given the right dominance related to body image46, as well as suggestions from case reports that BDD may be associated with right temporal lobe lesions47, 48. Thus, one hypothesis is that right hemisphere dysfunction could put an individual at risk for developing BDD, which could be exacerbated by dysfunction in frontal-striatal circuits, contributing to BDD-related obsessive thoughts and compulsions. Dysfunction in specific areas of the brain such as visual cortical systems could lead to distorted perception of body and face, and to an inability to correct this distortion or inhibit fronto-striatal responses to aversive stimuli related to the body. Possible dysfunction in the frontal-occipital-temporal circuit (i.e., top-down modulation) could lead to an imbalance in detail vs. holistic visual processing. Finally, serotonin may modulate BDD symptoms in frontal-subcortical circuitry and/or the limbic system, although little is known about its involvement in BDD.
CONCLUSIONS
The contribution of these different putative etiological and pathophysiologic factors to BDD’s development and maintenance, and others yet to be identified, remains unclear. A preliminary, hypothetical model involves a complex interplay in which genetic and biological susceptibilities interact with environmental events, such as teasing or abuse. These more “distal” genetic and environmental causative factors in turn may lead to more proximal contributing factors, such as neurobiologically based imbalances in global vs. local visual processing that may contribute to perceptual distortions and other information processing biases, as well as fronto-striatal abnormalities that may be associated with BDD-related obsessive-thoughts and repetitive behaviors. In individuals with such susceptibilities, adverse events such as abuse and teasing may be more likely to have influential effects. The subsequent development of distorted cognitions and reinforcing repetitive and avoidance behaviors may then serve to maintain the symptoms of the disorder. However, many questions remain; just one example is whether the above-noted brain abnormalities are inherited phenotypes or develop as a result of the disorder. Further development and testing of submodels and integrated etiological models for BDD will be important to aid in early identification and intervention, and will be crucial to guide the development of improved treatments.
References
- 1.Neziroglu F, Khemlani-Patel S, Yaryura-Tobias JA. Rates of abuse in body dysmorphic disorder and obsessive compulsive disorder. Body Image. 2006;3:189–193. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Didie ER, Tortolani CC, Pope CG, Menard W, Fay C, Phillips KA. Childhood abuse and neglect in body dysmorphic disorder. Child Abuse Negl. 2006 Oct;30(10):1105–1115. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Phillips KA, Menard W, Fay C, Weisberg R. Demographic characteristics, phenomenology, comorbidity, and family history in 200 individuals with body dysmorphic disorder. Psychosomatics. 2005 Jul-Aug;46(4):317–325. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.46.4.317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Wilhelm S. Feeling good about the way you look: A program for overcoming body image problems. New York: Guilford Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Buhlmann U, Cook LM, Fama JM, Wilhelm S. Perceived teasing experiences in body dysmorphic disorder. Body Image. 2007;4(4):381–385. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2007.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Veale D, Neziroglu F. Body Dysmorphic Disorder: A Treatment Manual. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Veale D, Ennis M, Lambrou C. Possible association of body dysmorphic disorder with an occupation or education in art and design. Am J Psychiatry. 2002 Oct;159(10):1788–1790. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Phillips KA. Body dysmorphic disorder: Recognizing and treating imagined ugliness. World Psychiatry. 2004;3(1):12–17. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bohne A, Keuthen NJ, Wilhelm S, Deckersbach T, Jenike MA. Prevalence of symptoms of body dysmorphic disorder and its correlates: a cross-cultural comparison. Psychosomatics. 2002 Nov-Dec;43(6):486–490. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.43.6.486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bernstein IH, Lin TD, McClellan P. Cross- vs. within-racial judgments of attractiveness. Percept Psychophys. 1982 Dec;32(6):495–503. doi: 10.3758/bf03204202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rhodes G. The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annual Review of Psychology. 2006;57:199–226. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Neziroglu F, Khemlani-Patel S, Jacofsky M. Body dysmorphic disorder: symptoms, models and treatment interventions. In: G S, editor. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy: A Guide for the Practising Clinician. Vol 2. Routledge, UK: 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Neziroglu F, Roberts M, Yaryura-Tobias JA. A Behavioral Model for Body Dysmorphic Disorder. Psychiatric Annals. 2004;34(12):915–920. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Neziroglu F, Khemlani-Patel S, Veale D. Social learning theory and cognitive behavioral models of body dysmorphic disorder. Body Image. 2008;5:28–38. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Deckersbach T, Savage C, Phillips K, et al. Characteristics of memory dysfunction in body dysmorphic disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2000;6(6):673–681. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700666055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Stangier U, Adam-Schwebe S, Muller T, Wolter M. Discrimination of facial appearance stimuli in body dysmorphic disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2008 May;117(2):435–443. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.117.2.435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Buhlmann U, Etcoff N, Wilhelm S. Emotional recognition bias for contempt and anger in body dysmorphic disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2006;40(2):105–111. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ekman P, Friesen WV. Pictures of Facial Affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press; 1975. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Buhlmann U, McNally R, Etcoff N, Tuschen-Caffier B, Wilhelm S. Emotion recognition deficits in body dysmorphic disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2004 Mar-Apr;38(2):201–206. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3956(03)00107-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Buhlmann U, Wilhelm S, McNally R, Tuschen-Caffier B, Baer L, Jenike M. Interpretive biases for ambiguous information in body dysmorphic disorder. CNS Spectrums. 2002;7(6):435–443. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900017946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Buhlmann U, McNally R, Wilhelm S, Florin I. Selective processing of emotional information in body dysmorphic disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2002;16(3):289–298. doi: 10.1016/s0887-6185(02)00100-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, Schwartz JL. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998 Jun;74(6):1464–1480. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.6.1464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Buhlmann U, Teachman BA, Naumann E, Fehlinger T, Rief W. The meaning of beauty: implicit and explicit self-esteem and attractiveness beliefs in body dysmorphic disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 2009 Jun;23(5):694–702. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Buhlmann U, Etcoff NL, Wilhelm S. Facial attractiveness ratings and perfectionism in body dysmorphic disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2008 Apr;22(3):540–547. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Phillips KA, McElroy SL. Personality disorders and traits in patients with body dysmorphic disorder. Compr Psychiatry. 2000 Jul-Aug;41(4):229–236. doi: 10.1053/comp.2000.7429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Veale D. Body dysmorphic disorder. Postgraduate Medical Journal. 2004 Feb;80(940):67–71. doi: 10.1136/pmj.2003.015289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wilhelm S, Buhlmann U, Hayward L, Greenberg J, Dimaite R. A cognitive-behavioral treatment approach for body dysmorphic disorder. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice. 2010;17:241–247. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Feusner JD, Townsend J, Bystritsky A, Bookheimer S. Visual information processing of faces in body dysmorphic disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007 Dec;64(12):1417–1425. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.12.1417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Feusner JD, Moody T, Townsend J, et al. Abnormalities of visual processing and fronto-striatal systems in body dysmorphic disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67(2):197–205. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Feusner JD, Moller H, Altstein L, et al. Inverted Face Processing in Body Dysmorphic Disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.03.015. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Farah MJ, Tanaka JW, Drain HM. What causes the face inversion effect? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1995 Jun;21(3):628–634. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.21.3.628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Mataix-Cols D, van den Heuvel OA. Common and distinct neural correlates of obsessive-compulsive and related disorders. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2006 Jun;29(2):391–410. viii. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2006.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rauch SL, Phillips KA, Segal E, et al. A preliminary morphometric magnetic resonance imaging study of regional brain volumes in body dysmorphic disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2003;122(1):13–19. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(02)00117-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Atmaca M, Bingol I, Aydin A, et al. Brain morphology of patients with body dysmorphic disorder. J Affect Disord. 2010 Jun;123(1–3):258–263. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Feusner JD, Townsend J, Bystritsky A, McKinley M, Moller H, Bookheimer S. Regional brain volumes and symptom severity in body dysmorphic disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2009 May 15;172(2):161–167. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bienvenu O, Samuels J, Riddle M, et al. The relationship of obsessive-compulsive disorder to possible spectrum disorders: results from a family study. Biological Psychiatry. 2000;48(4):287–293. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00831-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Phillips KA, Gunderson CG, Mallya G, McElroy SL, Carter W. A comparison study of body dysmorphic disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1998;59(11):568–575. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v59n1102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Phillips KA, Kaye WH. The relationship of body dysmorphic disorder and eating disorders to obsessive-compulsive disorder. CNS Spectr. 2007 May;12(5):347–358. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900021155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Marazziti D, Dell'Osso L, Presta S. Platelet [3H]paroxetine binding in patients with OCD-related disorders. Psychiatry Research. 1999;89(3):223–228. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(99)00102-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Barr L, Goodman W, Price L. Acute exacerbation of body dysmorphic disorder during tryptophan depletion. The Americal Journal of Psychiatry. 1992;149(10):1406–1407. doi: 10.1176/ajp.149.10.1406a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hanes K. Serotonin, psilocybin, and body dysmorphic disorder: a case report. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology. 1996;16(2):188–189. doi: 10.1097/00004714-199604000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Craven J, Rodin G. Cyproheptadine dependence associated with an atypical somatoform disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. 1987;32(2):143–145. doi: 10.1177/070674378703200211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hollander E, Wong C. Introduction: obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1995;56(suppl 4):3–6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Phillips KA. The Broken Mirror. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Feusner JD, Hembacher E, Phillips KA. The mouse who couldn't stop washing: pathologic grooming in animals and humans. CNS Spectr. 2009 Sep;14(9):503–513. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900023567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Devinsky O. Right cerebral hemisphere dominance for a sense of corporeal and emotional self. Epilepsy & Behavior. 2000;1:60–73. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Devinsky O, Khan S, Alper K. Olfactory reference syndrome in a patient with partial epilepsy. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology. 1998 Apr;11(2):103–105. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Gabbay V, Asnis G, Bello J, Alonso C, Serras S, O'Dowd M. New onset of body dysmorphic disorder following frontotemporal lesion. Neurology. 2003;61(1):123–125. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000069607.30528.d5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]