Abstract
It has been widely accepted that the younger the age and/or immaturity of the organism, the greater the brain plasticity, the young age plasticity privilege. This paper examines the relation of a young age to plasticity, reviewing human pediatric brain disorders, as well as selected animal models, human developmental and adult brain disorder studies. As well, we review developmental and childhood acquired disorders that involve a failure of regulatory homeostasis. Our core arguments are:
Plasticity is neutral with respect to outcome. Although the effects of plasticity are often beneficial, the outcome of plasticity may be adaptive or maladaptive.
The young age plasticity privilege has been overstated.
Plastic change operates in concert with homeostatic mechanisms regulating change at every point in the lifespan.
The same mechanisms that propel developmental change expose the immature brain to adverse events, making it more difficult for the immature than for the mature brain to sustain equilibrium between plasticity and homeostasis.
Poor outcome in many neurodevelopmental disorders and childhood acquired brain insults is related to disequilibrium between plasticity and homeostasis.
Keywords: Plasticity, homeostasis, neurodevelopmental disorders, childhood-acquired brain damage, age at brain injury, time since brain injury
1. Introduction
This paper considers the role of age in brain and behavioral plasticity. It has been widely accepted that the younger the age and/or immaturity of he organism, the greater the brain plasticity. We term this the young age plasticity privilege. Focusing primarily on human pediatric brain disorders, as well as selected animal models, human developmental and adult brain disorder studies, we examine how the idea of a young age plasticity privilege came about; identify historical and current challenges to the young age plasticity privilege; discuss regulatory homeostasis in brain and behavioral function; and review childhood brain disorders that involve a failure of regulatory homeostasis. These issues address the general question, how do plastic change and homeostatic regulation operate over age to shape outcome in disorders of the immature brain?
Our core arguments are:
Plasticity is neutral with respect to outcome and it is not designed to be adaptive. Although the effects of plasticity are often beneficial, the outcome of plasticity may be adaptive or maladaptive.
The young age plasticity privilege has been overstated, and important evidence shows plasticity to operate in mature as well as immature organisms.
Plastic change operates in concert with homeostatic mechanisms regulating change at every point in the lifespan.
The same mechanisms that propel developmental change expose the immature brain to adverse events, making it more difficult for the immature than for the mature brain to sustain equilibrium between plasticity and homeostasis.
Poor outcomes in many neurodevelopmental disorders and childhood acquired brain insults are related to disequilibrium between plasticity and homeostasis.
1. 1 The term plasticity
Plasticity is the capacity of a system to respond to normal or aberrant developmental or lesion-induced changes in the internal or external environments by adopting new, stable, developmentally appropriate phenotypes and/or restoring old phenotypes. The term plasticity has deep historical roots (Berlucchi & Buchtel, 2009) ranging from William James, who used the term for changes in neural paths that establish habits (James, 1890), to Ramón y Cajal, who used the term for regenerative capacities of peripheral and central nervous systems (Stahnisch & Nitsch, 2002), and Lugaro (1913), who suggested that chemotropic activities promote new synaptic functions (la plasticità).
Even today, the term plasticity is used in many different ways (Will et al., 2008) and refers variously to molecular, cellular, neural, or behavioral systems (Cramer et al., 2011) that achieve novel functions (Paillard, 1976; see Will et al., 2008 for English translation and commentary). Plasticity may refer to events at a microlevel (e.g., the fine-tuning of prewired circuits favoring activation of specific granule cell groups in the olivo-cerebellar system; D’Angelo & De Zeeuw, 2009) or at a macrolevel (e.g., an approach to memory research; Matthies, 1982). Plasticity may refer either to normal states (e.g., the springtime reemergence of bird song; Lenn, 1992, or changes in oscillatory brain activity with musical training; Trainor, Shahin, & Roberts, 2009) or to abnormal states (e.g., changes in white matter tracts of aphasic patients undergoing intonation therapy; Schlaug, Marchina, & Norton, 2009). Plasticity may refer to a belief about the immature brain (e.g., that functional recovery will be greater in a younger organism; Webb et al. 1996) or to evidence that plasticity is a general property of the brain at any age (e.g., experience-dependent structural synaptic plasticity in the adult brain; Holtmaat & Svoboda, 2009). Plasticity is a reparative mechanism for the brain to adjust to lesions by remyelination, reorganization of circuits, and/or neural and behavioral compensation (Castellanos et al., 2010; Nudo et al., 1996, 2006; Leocani & Comi, 2006). Finally, in a longer, evolutionary time frame, plasticity refers to the development of culturally specific skills, like reading, in brain substrates originally used for other functions (Liberman, 1998; Sacks, 2010).
1. 2 The young age plasticity privilege and the “Kennard Principle’
Chronological age has long been linked to plasticity, such that greater plasticity is associated with a younger age and/or immaturity, an idea we term the young age plasticity privilege. This idea arose in part from misreading of Kennard’s work in the 1930s and 1940s (see Dennis, 2010). Kennard had shown that age, but also factors other than age, predicted outcome after early brain lesions. Despite later support for Kennard’s conclusions (e.g., Feldman, 2009; Giza & Prins, 2006; Goldman-Rakic, 1980; Pullela et al., 2006), the ‘Kennard Principle’ persists as a belief – even in the face of invented evidence (fictitious traumatic brain injury (TBI) case histories varying only in the age of the patient) - that children exhibit fewer problems and better recovery after brain insult than adolescents or adults (Hart & Faust, 1988; Webb et al., 1996).
2. Plasticity May Be Adaptive Or Maladaptive
Three questionable presuppositions continue to dominate discussions of plasticity. The first is that plasticity is yoked to functional outcome. The second is that plasticity is functionally adaptive (as evidenced by recent titles like “Harnessing neuroplasticity,” Cramer et al., 2011). The third, an argument from design, is that plasticity is designed to be adaptive.
Almost 40 years ago, Schneider (1974; Schneider & Jhaveri, 1974) dissociated quantum of plasticity from functional outcome, finding that structural plasticity after lesions in the immature midbrain was more extensive than after lesions in the mature midbrain, but that function was poorer..
Although plasticity provides an evolutionary advantage in adapting to constantly changing internal and external environments, plastic mechanisms produce both adaptive and maladaptive change (Elbert & Heim, 2001 term the latter the ‘dark side’ of plasticity). Conditions such as musician’s dystonia or writer’s cramp occur because of neural plasticity in representational zones but involve dysfunction. Focal dystonia in musicians is associated with the same plastic brain changes that generate high levels of skilled performance (reorganization of the digits in the primary sensory area, Tamura et al., 2009). Therapy for focal dystonia involves returning the cortical finger representation to its original state (Candia et al., 2003). Immobilizing the dystonic hand in patients with writer’s cramp produces a relative gray matter decrease in the contralateral primary motor hand area and a decrease in corticomotor excitability (Granert et al., 2011).
Plastic change is not designed to be adaptive or maladaptive, powerful or impotent. As Nava and Röder (2011) suggest, some forms of maladaptation may be like Gould’s evolutionary spandrels (Gould & Lewontin, 1979), a random side effect, not an intended direct result, of plasticity.
3. The Young Age Plasticity Privilege Has Been Overstated
Mental retardation has long been the elephant in the young age plasticity privilege room. Referring to the ‘myth’ of recovery from early brain damage, Isaacson noted,
If the developing brain were completely “plastic” (a most unfortunate word) and any part capable of doing the work of any other, how are we to explain the tragedies of mental retardation resulting from biological problems occurring before birth? (Isaacson, 1975, p. 1)
Why are pervasive cognitive deficits more common after neurodevelopmental and early-acquired brain insults than after later childhood or adult lesions? How can disruptions of brain development generate plastic brain changes but the most widespread and life-long functional impairments? Why are the most profoundly debilitating and long-lasting cognitive-behavioral effects not associated with insults to the mature brain? In addition to the questions raised by mental retardation, the young age plasticity privilege has been challenged in a number of ways.
3.1 Epigenetic rescue in the adult brain
Epigenetics refers to heritable changes in gene function mediated by modifications to chromatin structure and covalent DNA modifications (methylation), but which do not involve permanent DNA alteration (Franklin & Mansuy, 2010; McGowan, Meaney, & Szyf, 2008). Epigenetic mechanisms program tissue specific patterns of gene expression; throughout life, epigenetics is like biochemical instructions written in pencil rather than indelible ink that can lead to dramatic changes in expression and/or function in response to environment and experience (Gosden & Feinberg, 2007). Epigenetic markers laid down in development dynamically regulate gene transcription in the adult brain (Miller & Sweatt, 2007).
Epigenetic restoration in adulthood may reverse neurological deficits. Rett syndrome, an X-linked neurodevelopmental disorder caused by mutations in the MeCP2 gene, is associated with aberrant synaptic function and behavior (Boggio et al., 2010; D’Cruz et al., 2010). Targeted reintroduction of functional MeCP2 rescues the behavioral abnormalities of MeCP2-deficient adult animals (Guy, Gan, Selfridge, Cobb, & Bird, 2007; Jugloff et al., 2008; Luikenhuis et al., 2004). In autism, a functionally impaired locus coeruleus-noradrenergic system may be transiently restored in the presence of fever (Mehler & Purpura, 2009). Treatment with a GABA receptor antagonist rescues cognitive function in adult mouse models of Down Syndrome (Fernandez et al., 2007). Pharmacological treatment in adult mice rescues the physiological and behavioral deficits of neurofibromatosis (Ehninger et al., 2008). While it has long been known that neurodevelopmental, neurodegenerative, and neuropsychiatric disorders are associated with aberrant epigenetic modifications (Gräff et al., 2011; Bartzokis, 2011), studies of epigenetic rescue in adulthood demonstrate the plasticity of epigenetic modification in the mature organism, although, to be sure, the mechanisms of action remain to be fully understood.
3.2 Adult neurogenesis
Neurogenesis, production of astrocytes, glia, and neural cells that connect and form pathways (Levitt, 2003), is largely completed during the pre-/perinatal period, but adult neurogenesis (Altman, 1962; see review in Kriegstein & Alvarez-Buylla, 2009) continues in the subgranular zone of the dentate gyrus and the subventricular zone of the lateral ventricle, recapitulating stages of neural maturation during perinatal development, albeit at a slower pace (Overstreet-Wadiche et al., 2006). Adult neurogenesis promoes function; for example, neurogenesis in the adult forebrain is required for predator avoidance and sex-specific behaviors (Sakamoto et al., 2011). Neurogenesis increases after cerebral ischemia, epilepsy and bacterial meningitis, and decreases with chronic stress and aging. (Fields, 2005). Neonatal brain insults, such as repetitive seizures and hypoxia/ischemia, alter the integration, although not the production, of adult-generated dentate gyrus neurons (Pugh et al., 2011).
Neurons generated in adult life must form synapses with existing neurons and integrate with existing circuits (for review, see Imayoshi et al., 2011). The young age neurogenesis advantage rests not so much in the production of new neurons, as in integrating newly-formed neurons with existing networks without disturbing circuit function, a less complex problem in the immature brain.
3.3 Age-conserved plasticity mechanisms
Neural migration, survival, myelination, and synaptic function are subject to similar regulation throughout development. For example, master regulators of neural stem cells and neural development, such as the Notch signalling pathway, are expressed and active in the adult brain (Ables et al., 2011). Basic processes of myelination are disrupted in neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., mutations in genes that control oligodendroglia or myelin development, such as Pelizaeus-Merzbacher disease, Fancy et al., 2011) and in birth pathologies (e.g., periventricular leukomalacia, Billiards et al., 2008). Regenerative remyelination shares mechanisms with primary myelination. Remyelination after white matter injury involves upregulation of several genes associated with the generation of oligodendrocytes during development. The recruitment phase of remyelination recapitulates aspects of primary myelination (e.g., platelet-derived growth factor is a mitogen in both development and remyelination; Fancy et al., 2011).
The development and regeneration of axons and dendrites share core mechanisms. Related processes may regulate Wallerian degeneration, degenerative forms of axon pruning in development, and models of axon degeneration in neurological disease (Low & Cheng, 2006). Mature CNS neurons regenerate injured axons by a molecular process apparently conserved across development; the limiting factor is neuronal competence to sustain long-distance regrowth by trophic factors and molecules in the extracellular matrix, a greater challenge in the adult brain.
3.4 Critical (but elastic) periods for development
Critical periods are time points in development when experiences strongly influence neurogenesis, brain sculpting, and learning (Hubel & Wiesel, 1963); for example, establishing ocular dominance columns in the visual cortex are established during early post-natal life (reviewed in Berardi et al., 2000). Critical periods are less precise in age timing and specificity than originally proposed (Johnson, 2005). Abnormal visual input can be disruptive even when it begins in adulthood (Daw, 1998; Lewis & Maurer, 2005). Practice improves visual acuity in adults with amblyopia (Levi, 2005). Age-at-cochlear implant variations in prelingually deaf children do not represent critical periods (Harrison et al., 2005). Real-time brain activation for artificial language learninng is similar in adults and children (Friederici, Steinhauer, & Pfeiger, 2002).
Multiple critical periods exist for different domains, different functions within a domain, and different time points. Visual deprivation in the monkey before 3 months of age affects scotopic sensitivity; deprivation before 6 months of age affects photopic spectral sensitivity; and deprivation before 18–24 months affects spatial contrast sensitivity (although visual deprivation can affect binocularity even in adulthood, Harwerth et al., 1986).
Importantly, controllers other than age drive the onset and offset of critical periods (Michel & Tyler, 2005). Intracortical inhibition thresholds in visual cortex, not age, control onset and offset of the critical period, which can be reinstated in the mature brain by pharmacological or environmental manipulations (Spolidoro et al., 2009; Hensch, 2005; Sale et al., 2009).
3.5 Critical periods that minimize the effects of brain injury?
Are there critical periods when the brain can be injured with impunity, as Anderson et al. (2009) and Kolb et al. (2011) suggest? Early comparisons supporting this hypothesis confounded age at injury with differences in pathology, e.g. finding better recovery from aphasia in children with head injuries than in adults with arteritic strokes (e.g., Guttmann, 1942). In school age children and adults, recovery from aphasia induced by head trauma is better than that caused by vascular lesions – regardless of age at injury (e.g., Dennis, 1980b; Dennis, 2009). More recent papers continue to conflate age, pathology, and lesion laterality. Anderson et al. (2009) argued that children with brain injury in infancy have lower IQ scores than those injured in middle childhood, but the infancy group included 61% neoplasms and 9% trauma, whereas the middle childhood injury group had 32% neoplasms and 26% trauma. In the same data set, comparison of lesions before and after age 2 showed bilateral lesions to be significantly more frequent in the early group (analyses of Anderson et al., 2009, data by Lidzba et al., 2009). To demonstrate a critical period for immunity to brain damage, it would be necessary not only account for outcome-relevant variables other than age (pathology, etiology, laterality (or bilaterality) of lesions, seizures), but also to compare acquired disorders with the same pathology occurring at different ages. Because neuropathology itself is age-related, inferences about plasticity from comparisons across age are difficult and have frequently been over interpreted as demonstrating age-related plasticity.
3.6 “Recovery” and age at injury
The pathology confound is apparent in many animal and human studies that report better functional outcomes and recovery after an earlier age at injury. For example, early studies asserted that children with left-sided brain injury rarely showed adult aphasic syndromes (e.g., Basser, 1962; Lenneberg, 1967). Later studies challenged the proposal that language recovery is better with early lesions (e.g., Dennis 1980; Dennis, 2009; Dennis & Whitaker, 1977).
While one type of cortical-spinal reorganization (strengthening of fast-conducting ipsilateral projections) is better with a younger age at injury (reviewed in Staudt, 2010), there is no overall advantage to earlier rather than later brain injury in children. Especially when higher cortical functions are considered, outcome also depends on pathology, how outcome is measured, and time since injury.
Age at injury effects vary within pathology, a better test of age-related plasticity hypotheses. Children treated for malignant posterior fossa medulloblastomas have poorer outcomes with a younger age at diagnosis and treatment (Dennis et al., 1996). With active, ongoing pathological processes such as seizures, in contrast, an early age at treatment may be functionally positive. For example, children with congenital Sturge-Weber syndrome have better outcomes with earlier surgery and seizure control (Hoffman et al., 1979). Differences between (rather than within) pathologies may be less salient when seizures drive aberrant function (Kadis et al., 2009).
Plastic reorganization after early-onset brain lesions may produce considerable but incomplete functionality. After early-onset pre- and perinatal lesions, controlling both paretic and non-paretic hand with the contralesional hemisphere, while advantageous, comes with a cost that adult-onset lesions does not bear - involuntary synkinesias or mirror movements (Müller et al., 1997). After left hemisphere surgery for early-onset seizures, language is functional but syntactically limited (Dennis & Kohn, 1975; Dennis & Whitaker, 1976; Kohn, 1980). While IQ scores are generally within normal limits after childhood TBI, social-cognitive and social-affective functions are impaired (Dennis et al., 2013).
Age at injury effects must be considered in the context of time since injury. After childhood-acquired aphasia, better language function is associated with activation of left anterior language regions over time (Elkana et al., 2011). Time since injury effects operate differently after childhood- and adult-onset injuries. After TBI, decrease in cerebral blood flow is similar in juvenile and adult mice and both lose ipsilateral cortical volumes, but in different time frames (Claus et al., 2010).
Diverse outcome patterns over time since injury have been identified for children with brain disorders. The recovery pattern involves deficits maximal soon after the injury but abating thereafter (recovery pattern). For childhood-acquired injuries, some age-at-injury effects are more pronounced and behavioral effects are more volatile in the period soon after the injury (e.g., Wu et al., 2010). The response inhibition deficit in ADHD following childhood TBI is most pronounced soon after the injury but recovers with increasing time since injury (Schachar et al., 2004). The growing into a deficit (Kennard, 1944) pattern is characterized by mild deficits soon after the injury that increase with time. Children with preschool mild TBI grow into deficits, exhibiting increasing cognitive-behavioral deficits over 7–13 years of age (McKinlay et al., 2010). The arrested development pattern involves deficits that improve but fail to advance to developmentally appropriate levels. Compared to his co-twin, a child with an arterial stroke in the language areas of the left hemisphere at age 6 developed immediate, severe syntactic deficits that improved over some years to their level at the time of the stroke, but failed to develop thereafter (Hetherington & Dennis, 2004). After cerebellar tumor removal in children, behavioral improvements continue beyond 3 months, but are developmentally incomplete at one year post-injury (Küper et al., 2013).
3.7 Evidence for greater impairment with a younger age at injury
Comparisons of outcomes within pathologies in individuals varying in age show greater morbidity in children vs. adults, and in younger vs. older children. A younger age at diagnosis and radiation treatment in children with primitive neuroectoermal cerebellar tumors is associated with poorer outcome (Hoppe-Hirsch et al., 1990; Radcliffe et al., 1994). A form of acquired aphasia, mutism with subsequent dysarthria in which previously acquired language skills are lost (DiCataldo et al., 2001; Dailey et al., 1995; Doxey et al., 1999; Huber-Okrainec et al., 2001; Humphreys, 1989; Rekate et al., 1985; Van Dongen et al., 1994; Van Mourik et al., 1998) has been identified in children, but not in adults, after surgery for posterior fossa tumors. While both childhood and adult cerebellar lesions produce a “cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome” (Schmahmann, 2010), adult cerebellar damage generally results in milder and/or more transient cognitive difficulties (Alexander et al., 2012). Compared to children with an older age at TBI, children with TBI in infancy and toddlerhood have more severe and persisting sequelae and more protracted recovery (Ewing-Cobbs et al., 2006; Koskiniemi et al., 1995).
3.8 Experiential plasticity over the lifespan
Over the lifespan, brain and behavior change in response to use, disuse, training, and enrichment (Angelucci et al., 2009; Chaddock et al., 2011; Cotman & Berchtold, 2002; Jaeggi et al., 2011; Klingberg, 2010). Early environments, maternal behavior and diet shape gene expression, tissue function and disease risk throughout life (Caldji et al., 1998; McGowan, Meaney, & Szyf, 2008; Sandovici et al., 2011). Training changes the adult brain (e.g., adult second language proficiency changes language-related brain regions, Hernandez & Li, 2007; Stein et al., 2012). Even experience-dependent plasticity of white matter microstructure (e.g., Mackey, Whitaker, & Bunge, 2012) is similar in younger and older adults (Lövdén et al., 2010).
Experiential plasticity operates in individuals with brain compromise at any age. Musical training enhances function in individuals with Williams Syndrome (Martens et al., 2011). Enriched environments compensate for the effects of acquired cerebellar damage (Cutuli et al., 2011; Foti et al., 2011) and traumatic brain injury (Kolb et al., 2011; Kovesdi et al., 2011). Even with degenerative brain damage, exposure to an enriched environment may sustain cognitive function (Green & Bavelier, 2008) and be associated with a slower rate of decline.
4. Plastic Change Operates In Concert With Homeostatic Mechanisms Regulating Change
Unbridled plasticity would generate an infinitely malleable, novelty-adaptive, and lesion-resilient brain that, at the same time, would be slow to learn and unable to automatize learning (or fast to learn but poor to retain). Unbridled stable homeostasis would generate a brain that could respond quickly to existing routines and information, but respond poorly to novelty or insult. Whether plasticity will be adaptive or maladaptive depends on homeostatic regulation, which exists at the level of gene, transcript, protein, metabolite, cell, and brain (Broderick & Craddock, 2012).
Traditional forms of synaptic plasticity (long-term potentiation or depression) occur in the context of stabilizing forces that provide homeostatic stability to neurons and networks. At the cellular level, excitation operates within an optimal range around a set point, beyond which excitation is adjusted by scaling up or down to maintain an appropriate level of function. At the network level, homeostasis stabilizes learning and changing connectivity via synaptic scaling, which adjusts the gain of the input and modifies the excitation-inhibition balance, and mechanisms that target intrinsic neuronal excitability, which modify contribution of a neuron to circuit function without changing synaptic currents (Pozo & Goda, 2010; Turrigiano & Nelson, 2004; Turrigiano, 2011). At the neuronal level, homeostasis balances excitation and inhibition in the face of morphological change or protein turnover, preventing activity-dependent plasticity from driving neural activity to excess excitation or quiescence (Marder & Prinz, 2002). The formation of connections governed by neurons driven to reach homeostasis can account for critical connectivity in developmental neural networks (Tetzlaff et al., 2010). Whereas synaptic plasticity and learning are achieved by positive feedback, homeostasis generally involves negative feedback.
5. Mechanisms of Plastic Developmental Change Expose The Immature Organism To Adverse Events
Brain development involves a complex sequence of plastic changes occurring over a protracted time span (Fields, 2005; Yakovlev & Lecours, 1967), and occurs in the context of age-dependent variations in metabolic rate, blood flow, neurotransmitter activity, and ability to tolerate oxidative stress (Morrison, Fraser, & Cepinskas, 2012). The same developmental mechanisms that promote plastic change may expose the organism to adverse outcomes.
Insult to an immature brain alters not only currently active brain development, including myelination, axon and dendrite growth, synaptogenesis, and proliferation of microglia and astrocytes (Rees et al., 2011), but also future brain development, involving myelination and cortical thinning (Ewing-Cobbs et al., 2008; Wilde, Hunter, & Bigler, 2012), and even neurodegeneration (Wu et al., 2010). A focus on plasticity as the mechanism for responding to environmental manipulations and recovery from brain injury has downplayed how dynamic change elevates the risk for a range of physiological and functional perturbations.
Newly formed neurons, the products of plasticity, have a low threshold for disturbance. The peak of human subplate neuron development in premature infants is coincident with the gestational ages of greatest vulnerability to perinatal brain injury (McQuillen & Ferriero, 2005). Compared to mature neurons, newly-formed dentate gyrus neurons in the adult brain exhibit increased neural plasticity, revealed by enhanced long-term potentiation (Schmidt-Hieber et al., 2004). However, although enhanced ability for long-term potentiation facilitates learning, neurons forming synapses have a low perturbation threshold (Kelsch, Sim, & Lois, 2010; Deisseroth et al., 2004; Schmidt-Hieber et al., 2004).
Three interconnected processes – oxidative stress, microglia phagocytosis, and neuroinflammation – have both beneficial and harmful effects. These processes are operative throughout the lifespan and are activated by brain injury, so open the immature and aging organisms, particularly, to both negative and positive effects. Developing neurons are susceptible to excitotoxicity, oxidative stress, and to inflammation (Morrison, Fraser, & Cepinskas, 2012).
Oxidative stress is caused by the imbalance between generation and detoxification of reactive oxygen and nitrogen species (Wang & Michaelis, 2010), and select neuron populations are vulnerable to oxidative stress, including those in the hippocampus and cerebellar granule cell layer (Wang et al, 2007). While reactive oxygen and nitrogen species are harmful, they have a beneficial, signaling, function that neurons use to responds to environmental cues (Gutierrez et al., 2006). Phagocytosis by microglia has a beneficial effect on development because microglia eat apoptotic cells during development and in adult neurogenesis, promoting brain modeling; but phagocytosis can also kill normal cells during inflammation (Sierra, Abiega, Shahraz, & Neumann, 2013).
Neuroinflammation is a common response to trauma, has beneficial effects on injured brain parenchyma and neuronal survival, and may pave the way for reparative processes. On the other hand, it also generates adverse effects including cell death, which contribute to secondary brain damage and neurological dysfunction (Lenzlinger, Morganti-Kossmann, Laurer, & McIntosh, 2001; Morganti-Kossmann et al., 2002). Neuroinflammation is related to changes in the brain following traumatic brain injury (TBI), including suppressed neurogenesis (Acosta et al., 2013) and chronic white matter alterations (Johnson et al., 2013). Individual differences in chronic neuroinflammatory change associated with TBI may account for variability in delayed effects in TBI survivors, such as post-traumatic epilepsy and the onset of neuropsychiatric disorders (Bigler, 2012).
Some metabolic effects of TBI may be especially disruptive in the immature organism. Increased anisotropic diffusion produces cytotoxic edema after TBI, and the magnitude and duration of these abnormalities appear to be greater in pediatric patients (Mayer et al., 2012). Apoptotic neurodegeneration following trauma is markedly enhanced in the immature brain (Bittigau, Sifringer, Pohl et al. 1999). Mechanisms that facilitate developmental plasticity may exacerbate damage resulting from by brain injury during development.
6. Many Childhood Disorders Involve Plasticity-Homeostasis Disequilibrium
Do maladaptive outcomes arise from disequilibrium of plasticity and homeostasis? Certainly, synaptic plasticity-homeostasis equilibrium is disrupted in several pediatric neurological disorders (Johnston, 2009; Johnston et al., 2009). The SynGAP protein may act as a developmental repressor of neural excitability and promote dendritic spine synapse maturation and behavior; pathogenic SynGAP1 mutations prompt early maturation and enhanced excitation of hippocampal spine synapses and behavioral abnormalities (Clement et al., 2012). Chondroitin sulphate proteoglycans in extracellular matrix of the adult brain restrict plasticity and their digestion reactivates it. Animals lacking cartilage link protein (upregulated in visual cortex as perineuronal nets form during development and after dark rearing) have attenuated perineuronal nets and persistent plasticity (Carulli et al., 2010). Both attenuated and persisting plasticity are forms of homeostatic dysequilibrium.
6.1 Epilepsy as a disorder of neuron circuit homeostasis
Epilepsy is a neuronal circuit excitability disorder characterized by spontaneous recurrent seizures. A balance of excitatory (glutamate) and inhibitory (GABA) neurotransmission controls excitability of neuronal circuits and homeostasis is important for epileptogenesis, a process involving complex disruption of self-regulatory mechanisms (Boison, Sandau, Ruskin, Kawamura, & Masino, 2013) arising from insufficient or excessive compensatory mechanisms in response to a change in network activity (Fritschy, 2008; Transande & Ramirez, 2007)..
Balancing homeostasis and plasticity involves myriad presynaptic, postsynaptic, ion channel, and extracellular matrix signaling mechanisms (Turrigiano, 2011; Pozo & Goda, 2010; Transande & Ramirez, 2007; Dityatev, 2010). A complex cascade of events leads to the evolution of epilepsy; for example, the spatial pattern of trauma (whether compact or diffuse) affects the propensity for developing posttraumatic epileptic activity (Volman, Bazhenov, & Sejnowski, 2011).
In post-traumatic epilepsy emergent after a latent period after TBI, trauma produces damaged neurons and physical undercutting of neuronal circuits; this causes deafferentation and neuronal injury and fewer neurons in the circuits. As homeostatic mechanisms attempt to re-establish the baseline level of excitation, the network becomes synaptically reorganized and hyperexcitable, producing post-traumatic seizures (Timofeev, Bazhenov, Avramescu, & Nita, 2010; Avramescu & Timofeev, 2008). Post-traumatic generation of paroxysmal events does not require structural changes in connectivity, and trauma-induced change in functional rather than anatomical connectivity might be sufficient for the evolution of epilepsy (Volman, Sejnowski, & Bashenov, 2011). Homeostatic plasticity following TBI may contribute to both adaptive functional recovery and in maladaptive epileptogenesis occurring between an initial cortical insult and an explicit onset of late epilepsy (Prince et al., 2009; Timofeev et al., 2010). The challenge in targeted treatments of TBI is to prevent epileptogenesis and post-traumatic epilepsy with pharmacological interventions without compromising adaptive function (Prince et al., 2009).
6.2 Dysregulated neurogenesis
Some neurodevelopmental disorders feature plastic but dysregulated neurogenesis. In DiGeorge syndrome, the 22q11.2 deletion results in diminished gene dosage, reduced cortical neurogenesis, and disrupted interneuron migration (Meechan et al., 2009), including clinical manifestation of autism (Wegiel, Kuchna et al. 2010). Disturbances of cortical development produce abnormalities of cortical migration, including lissencephaly (reduction in cerebral folding), cortical dysplasia (disorganization of cortical lamination), heterotopia (malposition of cortical gray matter in white matter fiber tracts), and polymicrogyria (excessive folding of cortical surface) (Golden & Harding, 2010).
6.3. Underpruning and overpruning
Pruning of developing synapses and dendritic spines during CNS development (Buss, Sun, & Oppenheim, 2006; Huttenlocher, 1979; Paolicelli et al., 2011; Saxena & Caroni, 2007; Vanderhaeghen & Cheng, 2010) regulates brain size and shape (Haydar, Kuan et al. 1999). Apoptosis continues into the third decade of life (Casey, Giedd et al., 2000; Giorgio, Watkins et al., 2010; O’Donnell et al., 2005; Paus, Keshavan, & Geidd, 2008; Petanjek et al., 2011).
Synaptic and dendrite instability has been implicated in neurodevelopmental disorders (Jan & Jan, 2010; Parrish et al., 2006). Autism (Pardo & Eberhart, 2007; Kelleher & Bear, 2008), fragile X syndrome (Bagni & Greenough, 2005); and Rett syndrome (Ramocki & Zoghbi, 2008) are associated with synaptic underpruning, resulting in enlarged regional brain size, due to overgrowth or lack of dendrite pruning and modification in neuron number.
Disorders with later onset show synaptic overpruning or failed maintenance of dendrites. Schizophrenia is associated with loss of dendritic spines on cerebral neocortical pyramidal neurons, decreased density of glutaminergic neurons, and microgliosis (Garey, 2010; Glantz & Lewis, 2000). Progressive gray matter loss occurs concomitant with the development of late adolescence schizophrenia, suggesting synaptic overpruning (Garey et al., 1998; Steen et al., 2005). Overexpression or underexpression of genetic variation or mutations in factors such as DISC-1, whose protein binding is regulated by activation of the NMDA-type glutamate receptor, have been implicated in the loss of synaptic function in schizophrenia (Hayashi-Takagi, Barker, & Sawa, 2011). Dysbindin, a gene linked to schizophrenia, is involved in the homeostatic modulation of neurotransmission (Dickman & Davis, 2009).
Acquired childhood lesions disrupt normal apoptosis. Cortical thinning after TBI in several brain regions overlaps with reduced gray matter that occurs with normal pruning, so areas destined for reduction as part of the normal pruning/programmed cell death get extra reduction (Merkley et al., 2008). Loss of cortical thickness and connectivity in more severe TBI (Bigler et al., 2010; Bigler, Abildskov et al., 2010) may disrupt developmentally programmed pruning (Bigler, Abildskov et al., 2010; Bendlin, Ries et al., 2008; Levine, Kovacevic et al., 2008; Wilde, Hunter et al., 2005; Wilde, Hunter, & Bigler, 2012).
6.4 Too-fat and too-thin brains
Neurodevelopmental disorders produce both overgrowth and undergrowth of cortical volume, cortical thickness, and cortical complexity. The homeostatic failure to regulate plastic growth in the cerebral cortex is associated with dysfunction.
Reduced total or regional cortical volume has been reported in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (Eliez et al. 2000), Angelman syndrome (Tan et al. 2011), Down syndrome (Weis et al. 1991), Fragile X syndrome (Kates et al. 2002), Rett syndrome (Subramaniam et al.1997), and Cohen syndrome (Kivitie-Kallio et al. 1998).. Excess superior and middle temporal lobe volume in Turner syndrome is associated with poor language (Rae et al. 2004). Congenital amusia is associated with extra volume in the right inferior frontal gyrus and auditory cortex (Hyde et al. 2007). More radiate white matter in the primary motor cortex is associated with motor impairment in autism (Mostofsky et al. 2007).
Cortical volume reflects both cortical thickness and cortical gyrification (the process by which the cortical surface folds to create gyral and sulcal regions), both of which change over normal development (e.g., Armstrong, Schleicher, Omran, Curtis, & Zilles, 1995; Giedd et al. 1999; Giedd 2004; Gogtay et al. 2004; Mangin et al. 2010; Magnotta et al. 1999; Sowell et al. 2003; Shaw et al. 2008; Raznahan et al. 2011; White et al. 2010), and are aberrant in neurodevelopmental disorders. Regions of thicker cortex have been reported in fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (Sowell et al. 2008) and Williams syndrome (Thompson et al. 2005); regions of thinner cortex in adolescents born preterm (Nagy et al. 2011), ADHD (Makris et al. 2007; Narr et al. 2009; Batty et al. 2010), childhood- and adolescent-onset schizophrenia (White et al. 2003), and temporal lobe epilepsy (Lin et al. 2007; Mueller et al. 2009). Thicker and thinner cortical regions co-exist in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (Bearden et al. 2007, 2009; Schaer et al. 2009), autism spectrum disorder (Hyde et al. 2010; Jiao et al. 2010), and Turner syndrome (Raznahan et al. 2010). Regions of higher gyrification have been reported in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (Bearden et al. 2009), autism spectrum disorder (Awate et al. 2008), children born preterm (Kesler et al. 2006), and Williams syndrome (Thompson et al. 2005; Gaser et al. 2006); regions of lower gyrification in ADHD (Wolosin et al. 2009), dyslexia (Casanova et al. 2004), intellectual disability (Zhang et al. 2010), obsessive-compulsive disorder (Wobrock et al. 2010), and Turner syndrome (Raznahan et al. 2010). Regions of both higher and lower gyrification have been reported in schizophrenia (White and Hilgetag 2011), and temporal lobe epilepsy (Lin et al. 2007; Voets et al. 2011).
Individuals with spina bifida myelomeningocele exhibit bidirectional anomalies of cortical thickness and cortical gyrification (Juranek et al. 2008; Juranek & Salman 2010 (Fig. 1). Treble et al (2012) investigated the functional effects of these failures of homeostatic regulation of cortical development. They found a negative association between cortical thickness and gyrification, such that aberrant plasticity in one feature was correlated with compensatory homeostasis in the second feature. For example, excessive gyrification in the inferior parietal and supramarginal cortices resulted in an aberrantly thin cortex that folded more compactly. More deviant levels of cortical thickness and gyrification—whether higher or lower relative to the typically developing comparison group—were associated with impaired function.
Cerebellar resculpting in spina bifida meningomyelocele, on the other hand, may promote increased functionality of eye movements. The small posterior fossa in this condition is caused by cerebrospinal fluid leak through the spinal defect that prevents distension of the embryonic ventricular system (McLone & Knepper, 1989), which prevents expansion of the posterior fossa and reconfigures the cerebellum (Juranek et al., 2010). Hyperdevelopment of the midsagittal vermis in spina bifida meningomyelocele is associated with sparing of ocular motor function (Salman, Dennis, & Sharpe, 2009).
6.5 Unbalanced global and local circuit connectivity
Over development, brain circuits become established as a series of hubs and connectors (van den Heuvel & Sporns, 2011) promoting both local and remote connectivity (Vogel et al., 2010). Hubs within a brain network tend to be more densely connected among themselves than with nodes of a lower degree, a Rich-Club organization (Colizza et al., 2006) that confers a level of resilience and plasticity not available to functionally specialized networks lacking such structure.
Circuitry is important in considering age and plasticity. Functional connectivity of the same brain region is different in children and adults (e.g., Ezekiel, Bosma, & Morton, 2013), so it is not surprising that connectivity develops abnormally in a number of neurodevelopmental disorders or that Rich-Club organization may be important for long-distance brain connectivity (van der Heuvel et al., 2009) in these conditions.
Individuals with autism show evidence of both local over-connectivity and long-distance under-connectivity (e.g., Belmonte et al., 2004; Casanova & Trippe, 2009), with more severe disruptions in later-developing cortical regions (Wass, 2011), and weak long-range inhibitory connections (JS Anderson et al., 2011). This aberrant connectivity has functional consequences. Short-range dynamic connectivity increases and long-range dynamic connectivity decreases with increasing autism severity (Barttfield et al., 2011). Individuals with autism use atypical brain regions to perform tasks (Di Martino et al., 2009). Behavioral markers of autism (impaired language, lack of social reciprocity, repetitive behavior) may involve disordered communication between frontal and posterior brain regions (Just et al., 2007; Schipul, Keller & Just, 2011).
Experience changes brain circuits in both typically developing children and special populations (Gabrieli, 2009; Hoeft et al., 2011; Perfetti et al., 2007; Rezaie et al., 2011). Reading intervention in children with dyslexia shifts neural activity from a more bilateral pattern reflecting underactivation of the left middle temporal and/or lateral occipital regions to more lateralized pattern involving greater left temporo-occipital cortical activity (Simos et al., 2002; Shaywitz et al., 2004; Spironelli et al. 2010).
6.6 Heterosynchrony
Dysregulation of homeostatic processes may create a heterosynchrony, a mismatch between two processes that are normally linked in time whereby accelerating or decelerating of one or both shifts their relative standing at various points in development. Heterosynchrony may help explain why some functional impairments after childhood brain disorders, rather than abating with age and development, become increasingly apparent over time.
Rats exposed to valproate, a neuronal maturational promoter, exhibit hyper-connectivity in cortical circuits. Postnatal rat pups treated with valproate show premature adult-like intrinsic electrophysiological properties in 40% of the temporal association cortex, cortical hypertrophy, and reduced social play, suggesting an association of behavioral dysfunction with accelerated timing of cortical maturation (Chomiak et al., 2010).
Spina bifida meningomyelocele involves attenuation of long association pathways including the corticopontocerebellar tract, frontostriatal and thalamofrontal tracts, limbic and commissural tracts, and association and projection pathways (Fletcher et al. 2005; Hasan, Eluvathingal et al. 2008a; Hasan, Sankar et al. 2008b; Juranek & Salman, 2010; Vachha et al. 2006). Over ages 8–16 years, more efficient myelination of the inferior longitudinal fasciculus (connecting the occipital and temporal lobes) is reflected in a decrease in radial diffusion on DTI. Children with spina bifida meningomyelocele, however, show no change, or even increased diffusivity in this tract, indicating poorer myelination and even early degenerative processes over age (Hasan et al., 2008). An asynchronous relation between myelination and age means that a key segment of childhood development lacks white matter functionality.
The timing of sleep relative to the 24-hour day changes across the lifespan, with progressively later sleep-wake times during childhood and adolescence (Carskadon et al., 1993), and earlier times in aging adults (Carrier et al., 1997). Edelstein et al., 2012 measured sleep chronotype (Roenneberg et al., 2003; Roenneberg, 2007) in 7–55 year old individuals with spina bifida meningomyelocele, finding the typical relationship between chronotype and age, even though the phase relationship between sleep-wake timing and 24 hour day in controls trended downward at the age when it peaked in spina bifida meningomyelocele (Edelstein et al., 2012). Aberrant synchronization of circadian rhythms to the 24-hour day may contribute to sleep problems in this and in other neurodevelopmental disorders (Glickman, 2010; Harvey & Kennedy, 2002).
Wu et al. (2010) found heterosynchronous processes over time since injury in an adolescent with TBI. Microstructure regrowth (evidenced by increased fractional anisotropy and fiber density) was accompanied by accelerated degenerative volume loss at an earlier-than-normal point in the lifespan.
6.7 Synaptic plasticity and learning
The model for learning has been activity-dependent adaptation, Hebbian plasticity, a positive feedback mechanism that includes long-term potentiation and long term depression (Feldman, 2009). Positive feedback mechanisms must be balanced by compensatory and homeostatic negative feedback and prediction mechanisms.
Neurons change the strength of their synapses and connections in response to extrinsic stimuli. Hebbian synaptic changes are associative, rapid and input specific (Pozo & Goda, 2010); because repeatedly and persistently co-active cells increase connective strength among populations of interconnected neurons, they reinforce synapses that are co-activated with certain experiences, thereby providing a putative cellular basis for learning and memory (Neves et al., 2008). Cortical networks undergo adaptations during learning including specific increases in dendritic complexity and spines. Structural elaborations during learning involve discrete subsets of cells preferentially activated by experience (Wang et al., 2011).
Novel stimuli become predictable after association with other neural signals, so the brain needs to distinguish predictable and novel signals. Learning is facilitated by the homeostatic mechanisms of anti-Hebbian plasticity, a form of learning involving reducing synaptic strength between neurons after one neuron produces an action potential in another, and which provides the basis for novelty detection (Roberts & Leen, 2010). Spike-timing-dependent plasticity, which modifies the strength of connections between neurons based on the relative timing of a particular neuron’s output and input spikes, is also important (Markram et al., 2011). Spike-timing-dependent synaptic plasticity at a synapse carrying predictive signals sculpts a negative image of predictable sensory patterns from background sensorimotor activity.
6.8 Behavioral homeostasis
At the level of behavior, across groups and within individuals, homeostasis involves adjusting behavior to maintain a target level of response to environmental cues or triggers A range of behavioral dysfunction in neurodevelopmental and childhood acquired brain disorders is associated with dysregulation of behavior around a homeostatic baseline.
Attention, normally maintained within a target range, may be dysregulated and subject to performance extremes. Russell et al. (2006) propose that ADHD increases trial-by-trial performance variability (Scaglione et al., 2011) because of inefficient astrocyte function from deficient ATP production in neurons, over milliseconds, and deficient developmental myelination of axons. Inhibition of return is a form of automatic attention and is indexed by a longer time to return to a previously attended cue location compared to a novel location (Klein, 2000). Children with spina bifida meningomyelocele and beaking of the midbrain tectum exhibit attenuated inhibition of return (Dennis et al., 2005), showing dysregulation of automatic attention. When bisecting horizontal and vertical lines, these children exhibit an enhanced Weber fraction, a larger zone of subjective variability (Dennis et al., 2005).
Acquired deafness results in a sensory deprived cortical region with decreased inhibitory synaptic transmission. Hearing-lesioned animals exhibit tinnitus with a pitch in the hearing loss range, and drugs that enhances inhibition (but not those that reduce excitation) eliminate tinnitus, suggesting that sensory-induced homeostatic down-regulation of inhibitory synapses generates tinnitus (Yang et al., 2011).
Restoring cellular homeostasis may rescue behavior in mouse models of complex behavioral disorders. Increased cellular inhibition moderately ameliorates social deficits in mice experimentally subjected to elevation of cellular excitation-inhibition balance (Yizhar et al., 2011).
The symptom spectrum in children and adults with acquired cerebellar lesions has been proposed to reflect exaggeration (overshoot, dysmetria) and/or diminution (hypotonia, hypometria), (Schmahmann, 2010). The immature and adult cerebellum may modulate the speed, capacity, consistency, and appropriateness of cognitive processes, maintaining them around a homeostatic, context-appropriate baseline (Schmahmann, 2010).
7. Discussion
This review has considered major challenges to the idea of a global young age plasticity privilege: Plasticity itself is neutral and ‘intends’ no particular outcome, which may be either adaptive or maladaptive; many plasticity mechanisms are not intrinsically tied to age; homeostatic mechanisms regulate plastic change at any age; and the outcome of many neurodevelopmental disorders and childhood acquired brain insults is related to disequilibrium of plasticity and homeostasis. Plasticity in any neural or behavioral system is not unbridled, and homeostatic processes that limit change counter plastic processes that promote change at the level of brain and behavior. Plasticity – related to change – and homeostasis –related to stability – are properties of brain and behavior at every point in the lifespan. To the extent that plasticity is an intrinsic property of the brain, it exists in a normal young brain, a normal older brain, and in brains of any age compromised by congenital malformations or acquired insult. Whether the outcome of plasticity is adaptive or maladaptive depends on the equilibrium between plastic and homeostatic processes.
Is there a basis for the continuing belief in the young age plasticity principle? The answer depends on how the question is posed. It is hardly news that damage to the young brain has functional consequences (Cotard, 1868), that some differences in the effects of brain injury are related to age at injury, or that particular outcomes may be more favorable with an earlier age at injury (Staudt, 2010). Whether an early age at injury provides a general adaptive advantage depends on when and how outcome is assessed, and even adaptive outcomes often come with symptoms or developmental truncation not observed with later age at injury.
Age is one factor in describing the outcomes of neurodevelopmental disorders or acquired brain lesions (Dennis, 2000, Taylor & Alden, 1997), but is not a mechanism to explain plasticity. Age marks programmed events that are malleable over the lifespan but that typically occur at a particular age point. Age-based plasticity has sometimes been assumed rather than proven; age may be a proxy for more fundamental processes, such as those in the epigenome, that are the causal factor in outcome; and age is correlated with differences in pathology. The age span for neural plasticity can be shrunk or expanded by manipulations of gene expression or by experience. Mutable function is tied not to age but to key developmental processes; for example, intracortical inhibition thresholds – not age- control critical periods for monocular vision. A focus on age an explanation distracts from the discovery of these mechanisms (e.g., how endogenous and self-terminating learning processes control the duration of critical periods; Johnson, 2005).
If the age issue is conceived as a contest between early plasticity and later specialization, then the answer is no, there is little basis for a continuing belief in the young age plasticity privilege. Plasticity vs. specialization is simply a variation of the nature-nurture debate, a false dichotomy (Traynor & Singleton, 2010) since a functional brain must be both adaptable and specialized. If, on the other hand, the age question concerns identifying brain regions and cell populations with age-related reactions to excitotoxicity, oxidative stress, and inflammation (e.g., Ferriero & Miller, 2010), then the answer is yes because it prompts the search for both adaptive processes like neural repair and maladaptive processes like accelerated apoptosis and degeneration.
Age is also a marker for environmental problems that plastic neural and behavioral processes have to solve, which alter as the environment changes (Nava & Röder, 2011), within and between points in development. Brain and behavioral development occurs within a highly constrained, genetically organized but changing environment that has bidirectional effects on the brain; at each point in development, the child has a stable state and a plasticity history that limit which factors will influence its further development (Stiles & Jernigan, 2010). By typicality, enrichment, or deprivation, the environment affords experiences that iteratively shape brain development (Greenough et al., 1987).
Age-conserved and age-related plastic processes and homeostatic mechanisms provide flexibility in negotiating changing environmental challenges to meet life demands (Deisseroth et al., 2004; Kelsch, Sim & Lois, 2010). As internal and external environments change over age and development, a combination of plastic and homeostatic mechanisms solve emerging neural and behavioral problems. The tango between short-lived plasticity and longer-term robust specialization, on the one hand, and homeostatic forces, on the other, enables the immature brain to shape and reshape itself with experience, use, and disuse; adopt new stable phenotypes; and reengage, reorganize and compensate to restore old phenotypes shattered by brain insult.
But the outcome of the dance may be maladaptive as well as adaptive. The same mechanisms that propel developmental change expose the immature brain to adverse events. In attempting to understand age, plasticity, and homeostasis, the traditional, roseate view of a universal young age plasticity privilege needs to be tempered with an awareness of the huge challenge facing an immature brain: maintaining equilibrium between change and stability when the arc of both is in a state of constant jitter and when novel environmental demands emerge constantly.
Highlights.
Plasticity is neutral with respect to outcome. Although the effects of plasticity are often beneficial, the outcome of plasticity may be adaptive or maladaptive.
The young age plasticity privilege has been overstated.
Plastic change operates in concert with homeostatic mechanisms regulating change at every point in the lifespan.
The same mechanisms that propel developmental change expose the immature brain to adverse events, making it more difficult for the immature than for the mature brain to sustain equilibrium between plasticity and homeostasis.
Poor outcome in many neurodevelopmental disorders and childhood acquired brain insults is related to disequilibrium between plasticity and homeostasis.
Acknowledgments
Supported in part by National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Grants P01 HD35946 & P01 HD35946–06, “Spina Bifida: Cognitive and Neurobiological Variability” and by National Institutes of Health Grant 1RO1 HD04946, “Social Outcomes in Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury.” We thank Deborah Budding, Kim Edelstein, Darren Kadis, Nevena Simic, and Amy Wilkinson for comments on the manuscript and Beverly Andres for help with manuscript preparation.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributor Information
Maureen Dennis, Program in Neurosciences and Mental Health, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Departments of Surgery, Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto.
Brenda J. Spiegler, Department of Psychology, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Department of Pediatrics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto
Jenifer J. Juranek, Department of Pediatrics, University of Texas Health Science Center, Houston, Texas
Erin D. Bigler, Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, Brigham Young University, Provo UT, Department of Psychiatry, University of Utah, Salt Lake City UT
O. Carter Snead, Centre for Brain and Behaviour, Division of Neurology, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Department of Pediatrics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto.
Jack M. Fletcher, Department of Psychology, University of Houston, Houston, Texas
References
- Ables JL, Breunig JJ, Eisch AJ, Rakic P. Not(ch) just development: Notch signalling in the adult brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:269–283. doi: 10.1038/nrn3024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Acosta S, Tajiri N, Shinozuka K, Ishikawa H, Grimmig B, Diamond D, Sanberg P, Bickford P, Kaneko Y, Borlongan C. Long-term upregulation of inflammation and suppression of cell proliferation in the brain of adult rats exposed to traumatic brain injury using the controlled cortical impact model. PLoS One. 2013;8:e53376. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander MP, Gillingham S, Schweizer T, Stuss DT. Cognitive impairments due to focal cerebellar injuries in adults. Cortex. 2012;48:980–990. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Altman J. Are new neurons formed in the brains of adult mammals? Science. 1962;135:1127–1128. doi: 10.1126/science.135.3509.1127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JS, Nielsen JA, Froehlich AL, DuBray MB, Druzgal TJ, Cariello AN, Cooperrider JR, Zielinski BA, Ravichandran C, Fletcher PT, Alexander AL, Bigler ED, Lange N, Lainhart JE. Functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging classification of autism. Brain. 2011;134:3742–3754. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson V, Spencer-Smith M, Leventer R, Coleman L, Anderson P, Williams J, Greenham M, Jacobs R. Childhood brain insult: can age at insult help us predict outcome? Brain. 132:45–56. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Angelucci F, De Bartolo P, Gelfo F, Foti F, Cutuli D, Bossu P, Caltagirone C, Petrosini L. Increased concentrations of nerve growth factor and brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the rat cerebellum after exposure to environmental enrichment. Cerebellum. 2009;8:499–506. doi: 10.1007/s12311-009-0129-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong E, Schleicher A, Omran H, Curtis M, Zilles K. The ontogeny of human gyrification. Cereb Cortex. 1995;5:56–63. doi: 10.1093/cercor/5.1.56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avramescu S, Timofeev I. Synaptic strength modulation after cortical trauma: a role in epileptogenesis. J Neurosci. 2008;28:6760–6772. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0643-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Awate SP, Win L, Yushkevich P, Schultz RT, Gee JC. 3D cerebral cortical morphometry in autism: increased folding in children and adolescents in frontal, parietal, and temporal lobes. Med Image Comput Comput Assist Interv. 2008;11:559–567. doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-85988-8_67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bagni C, Greenough WT. From mRNP trafficking to spine dysmorphogenesis: the roots of fragile X syndrome. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:376–387. doi: 10.1038/nrn1667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barttfeld P, Wicker B, Cukier S, Navarta S, Lew S, Sigman M. A big-world network in ASD: dynamical connectivity analysis reflects a deficit in long-range connections and an excess of short-range connections. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:254–263. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartzokis G. Neuroglialpharmacology: white matter pathophysiologies and psychiatric treatments. Front Biosci. 2011;16:2695–2733. doi: 10.2741/3881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Basser LS. Hemiplegia of early onset and the faculty of speech with special reference to the effects of hemispherectomy. Brain. 1962;85:427–460. doi: 10.1093/brain/85.3.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Batty MJ, Liddle EB, Pitiot A, Toro R, Groom MJ, Scerif G, Liotti M, Liddle PF, Paus T, Hollis C. Cortical gray matter in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a structural magnetic resonance imaging study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:229–238. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2009.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bearden CE, van Erp TG, Dutton RA, Lee AD, Simon TJ, Cannon TD, Emanuel BS, McDonald-McGinn D, Zackai EH, Thompson PM. Alterations in midline cortical thickness and gyrification patterns mapped in children with 22q11.2 deletions. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19:115–126. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bearden CE, van Erp TG, Dutton RA, Tran H, Zimmermann L, Sun D, Geaga JA, Simon TJ, Glahn DC, Cannon TD, Emanuel BS, Toga AW, Thompson PM. Mapping cortical thickness in children with 22q11.2 deletions. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:1889–1898. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belmonte MK, Allen G, Beckel-Mitchener A, Boulanger LM, Carper RA, Webb SJ. Autism and abnormal development of brain connectivity. J Neurosci. 2004;24:9228–9231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3340-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bendlin BB, Ries ML, Lazar M, Alexander AL, Dempsey RJ, Rowley HA, Sherman JE, Johnson SC. Longitudinal changes in patients with traumatic brain injury assessed with diffusion-tensor and volumetric imaging. Neuroimage. 2008;42:503–514. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berardi N, Pizzorusso T, Maffei L. Critical periods during sensory development. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2000;10:138–145. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(99)00047-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berlucchi G, Buchtel HA. Neuronal plasticity: historical roots and evolution of meaning. Exp Brain Res. 2009;192:307–319. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1611-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bigler E. Neuroinflammation and the dynamic lesion in traumatic brain injury. Brain. 2012:1–3. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws1342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bigler ED, Abildskov TJ, Wilde EA, McCauley SR, Li X, Merkley TL, Fearing MA, Newsome MR, Scheibel RS, Hunter JV, Chu Z, Levin HS. Diffuse damage in pediatric traumatic brain injury: a comparison of automated versus operator-controlled quantification methods. Neuroimage. 2010a;50:1017–1026. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bigler ED, McCauley SR, Wu TC, Yallampalli R, Shah S, MacLeod M, Chu Z, Hunter JV, Clifton GL, Levin HS, Wilde EA. The temporal stem in traumatic brain injury: preliminary findings. Brain Imaging Behav. 2010b;4:270–282. doi: 10.1007/s11682-010-9105-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Billiards SS, Haynes RL, Folkerth RD, Borenstein NS, Trachtenberg FL, Rowitch DH, Ligon KL, Volpe JJ, Kinney HC. Myelin abnormalities without oligodendrocyte loss in periventricular leukomalacia. Brain Pathol. 2008;18:153–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2007.00107.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bittigau P, Sifringer M, Pohl D, Stadhaus D, Ishimaru M, Shimuzu H, Ikeda M, Lang D, Speer A, Olney JW, Ikonomidou C. Apoptotic neurodegenertio followng trauma is markedly enhanced in the immature brain. Ann Neurol. 45:7240735. doi: 10.1002/1531-8249(199906)45:6<724::aid-ana6>3.0.co;2-p. 199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boggio EM, Lonetti G, Pizzorusso T, Giustetto M. Synaptic determinants of Rett syndrome. Front Synaptic Neurosci. 2010;2:28. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boison D, Sandai US, Ruskin DN, Kawamura M, Masino SA. Homeostatic control of brain function – new approaches to understanding epileptogenesis. Front Cell Neurosci. 2013:7. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2013.00109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broderick G, Craddock TJ. Systems biology of complex symptom profiles: Capturing interactivity across behavior, brain and immune regulation. Brain Behav Immun. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buss RR, Sun W, Oppenheim RW. Adaptive roles of programmed cell death during nervous system development. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2006;29:1–35. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caldji C, Tannenbaum B, Sharma S, Francis D, Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. Maternal care during infancy regulates the development of neural systems mediating the expression of fearfulness in the rat. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95:5335–5340. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.9.5335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Candia V, Wienbruch C, Elbert T, Rockstroh B, Ray W. Effective behavioral treatment of focal hand dystonia in musicians alters somatosensory cortical organization. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:7942–7946. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1231193100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrier J, Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Kupfer DJ. Sleep and morningness-eveningness in the ‘middle’ years of life (20–59 y) J Sleep Res. 1997;6:230–237. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1997.00230.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carskadon MA, Vieira C, Acebo C. Association between puberty and delayed phase preference. Sleep. 1993;16:258–262. doi: 10.1093/sleep/16.3.258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carulli D, Pizzorusso T, Kwok JC, Putignano E, Poli A, Forostyak S, Andrews MR, Deepa SS, Glant TT, Fawcett JW. Animals lacking link protein have attenuated perineuronal nets and persistent plasticity. Brain. 2010;133:2331–2347. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casanova M, Trippe J. Radial cytoarchitecture and patterns of cortical connectivity in autism. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364:1433–1436. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casanova MF, Araque J, Giedd J, Rumsey JM. Reduced brain size and gyrification in the brains of dyslexic patients. J Child Neurol. 2004;19:275–281. doi: 10.1177/088307380401900407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey BJ, Giedd JN, Thomas KM. Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biol Psychol. 2000;54:241–257. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(00)00058-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Castellanos NP, Paul N, Ordonez VE, Demuynck O, Bajo R, Campo P, Bilbao A, Ortiz T, del-Pozo F, Maestu F. Reorganization of functional connectivity as a correlate of cognitive recovery in acquired brain injury. Brain. 2010;133:2365–2381. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaddock L, Pontifex MB, Hillman CH, Kramer AF. A review of the relation of aerobic fitness and physical activity to brain structure and function in children. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17:975–985. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chomiak T, Karnik V, Block E, Hu B. Altering the trajectory of early postnatal cortical development can lead to structural and behavioural features of autism. BMC Neurosci. 2010;11:102. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-11-102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Claus CP, Tsuru-Aoyagi K, Adwanikar H, Walker B, Manvelyan H, Whetstone W, Noble-Haeusslein LJ. Age is a determinant of leukocyte infiltration and loss of cortical volume after traumatic brain injury. Dev Neurosci. 2010;32:454–465. doi: 10.1159/000316805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clement JP, Aceti M, Creson TK, Ozkan ED, Shi Y, Reish NJ, Almonte AG, Miller BH, Wiltgen BJ, Miller CA, Xu X, Rumbaugh G. Pathogenic SYNGAP1 mutations impair cognitive development by disrupting maturation of dendritic spine synapses. Cell. 2012;151:709–723. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.08.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colizza V, Flammini A, Serrano MA, Vespignani A. Detecting rich-club ordering in complex networks. Nature physics. 2006;2:110–115. [Google Scholar]
- Cotard J. Etude sur l’atophie partielle du cerveau. These de Paris; 1868. [Google Scholar]
- Cotman CW, Berchtold NC. Exercise: a behavioral intervention to enhance brain health and plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 2002;25:295–301. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(02)02143-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cramer SC, Sur M, Dobkin BH, O’Brien C, Sanger TD, Trojanowski JQ, Rumsey JM, Hicks R, Cameron J, Chen D, Chen WG, Cohen LG, deCharms C, Duffy CJ, Eden GF, Fetz EE, Filart R, Freund M, Grant SJ, Haber S, Kalivas PW, Kolb B, Kramer AF, Lynch M, Mayberg HS, McQuillen PS, Nitkin R, Pascual-Leone A, Reuter-Lorenz P, Schiff N, Sharma A, Shekim L, Stryker M, Sullivan EV, Vinogradov S. Harnessing neuroplasticity for clinical applications. Brain. 2011;134:1591–1609. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cutuli D, Rossi S, Burello L, Laricchiuta D, De Chiara V, Foti F, De Bartolo P, Musella A, Gelfo F, Centonze D, Petrosini L. Before or after does it matter? Different protocols of environmental enrichment differently influence motor, synaptic and structural deficits of cerebellar origin. Neurobiol Dis. 2011;42:9–20. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2010.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Angelo E, De Zeeuw CI. Timing and plasticity in the cerebellum: focus on the granular layer. Trends Neurosci. 2009;32:30–40. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Cruz JA, Wu C, Zahid T, El-Hayek Y, Zhang L, Eubanks JH. Alterations of cortical and hippocampal EEG activity in MeCP2-deficient mice. Neurobiol Dis. 2010;38:8–16. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2009.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dailey AT, McKhann GM, 2nd, Berger MS. The pathophysiology of oral pharyngeal apraxia and mutism following posterior fossa tumor resection in children. J Neurosurg. 1995;83:467–475. doi: 10.3171/jns.1995.83.3.0467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daw NW. Critical periods and amblyopia. Arch Ophthalmol. 1998;116:502–505. doi: 10.1001/archopht.116.4.502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deisseroth K, Singla S, Toda H, Monje M, Palmer TD, Malenka RC. Excitation-neurogenesis coupling in adult neural stem/progenitor cells. Neuron. 2004;42:535–552. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00266-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M. Capacity and strategy for syntactic comprehension after left or right hemidecortication. Brain Lang. 1980;10:287–317. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(80)90058-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M. Childhood medical disorders and cognitive impairment: Biological risk, time, development and reserve. In: Yeates KO, Ris MD, Taylor HG, editors. Pediatric Neuropsychology: Research, Theory and Practice. Guilford Press; New York: 2000. pp. 3–22. [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M. Language disorders in children with central nervous system injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2009;32:417–432. doi: 10.1080/13803390903164355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M. Margaret Kennard (1899–1975): not a ‘principle’ of brain plasticity but a founding mother of developmental neuropsychology. Cortex. 2010;46:1043–1059. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M, Edelstein K, Frederick J, Copeland K, Francis D, Blaser SE, Kramer LA, Drake JM, Brandt M, Hetherington R, Fletcher JM. Peripersonal spatial attention in children with spina bifida: associations between horizontal and vertical line bisection and congenital malformations of the corpus callosum, midbrain, and posterior cortex. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:2000–2010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M, Kohn B. Comprehension of syntax in infantile hemiplegics after cerebral hemidecortication: left-hemisphere superiority. Brain Lang. 1975;2:472–482. doi: 10.1016/s0093-934x(75)80084-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M, Simic N, Bigler ED, Abildskov T, Agostino A, Taylor HG, Rubin K, Vannatta K, Gerhardt CA, Stancin T, Yeates KO. Cognitive, affective, and conative theory of mind (ToM) in children with traumatic brain injury. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2013;5C:25–39. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M, Spiegler BJ, Hetherington CR, Greenberg ML. Neuropsychological sequelae of the treatment of children with medulloblastoma. J Neurooncol. 1996;29:91–101. doi: 10.1007/BF00165522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M, Whitaker HA. Language acquisition following hemidecortication: linguistic superiority of the left over the right hemisphere. Brain Lang. 1976;3:404–433. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(76)90036-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis M, Whitaker HA. Hemispheric equipotentiality and language acquisition. In: Segalowitz S, Gruber F, editors. Language Development and Neurological Theory. Academic Press; New York: 1977. pp. 93–106. [Google Scholar]
- Di Cataldo A, Dollo C, Astuto M, La Spina M, Ippolito S, Papotto M, Giuffrida S. Mutism after surgical removal of a cerebellar tumor: two case reports. Pediatr Hematol Oncol. 2001;18:117–121. doi: 10.1080/088800101300002946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Martino A, Ross K, Uddin LQ, Sklar AB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. Functional brain correlates of social and nonsocial processes in autism spectrum disorders: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:63–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickman DK, Davis GW. The schizophrenia susceptibility gene dysbindin controls synaptic homeostasis. Science. 2009;326:1127–1130. doi: 10.1126/science.1179685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dityatev A. Remodeling of extracellular matrix and epileptogenesis. Epilepsia. 2010;51(Suppl 3):61–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02612.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doxey D, Bruce D, Sklar F, Swift D, Shapiro K. Posterior fossa syndrome: identifiable risk factors and irreversible complications. Pediatr Neurosurg. 1999;31:131–136. doi: 10.1159/000028848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edelstein K, Cirino PT, Hasher L, Fletcher JM, Dennis M. Sleep problems, chronotype, and diurnal preferences in children and adults with spina bifida. J Biol Rhythms. 2012;27:172–175. doi: 10.1177/0748730411435209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehninger D, Li W, Fox K, Stryker MP, Silva AJ. Reversing neurodevelopmental disorders in adults. Neuron. 2008;60:950–960. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elbert T, Heim S. A light and a dark side. Nature. 2001;411:139. doi: 10.1038/35075678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eliez S, Schmitt JE, White CD, Reiss AL. Children and adolescents with velocardiofacial syndrome: a volumetric MRI study. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:409–415. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.3.409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elkana O, Frost R, Kramer U, Ben-Bashat D, Hendler T, Schmidt D, Schweiger A. Cerebral reorganization as a function of linguistic recovery in children: An fMRI study. Cortex. 2011;47:202–216. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ewing-Cobbs L, Prasad MR, Kramer L, Cox CS, Jr, Baumgartner J, Fletcher S, Mendez D, Barnes M, Zhang X, Swank P. Late intellectual and academic outcomes following traumatic brain injury sustained during early childhood. J Neurosurg. 2006;105:287–296. doi: 10.3171/ped.2006.105.4.287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ewing-Cobbs L, Prasad MR, Swank P, Kramer L, Cox CS, Jr, Fletcher JM, Barnes M, Zhang X, Hasan KM. Arrested development and disrupted callosal microstructure following pediatric traumatic brain injury: relation to neurobehavioral outcomes. Neuroimage. 2008;42:1305–1315. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ezekiel F, Bosma R, Morton JB. Dimensional Change Card Sort performance associated with age-related differences in functional connectivity of lateral prefrontal cortex. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2013;5:40–50. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2012.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fancy SP, Chan JR, Baranzini SE, Franklin RJ, Rowitch DH. Myelin regeneration: a recapitulation of development? Annu Rev Neurosci. 2011;34:21–43. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman DE. Synaptic mechanisms for plasticity in neocortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32:33–55. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez F, Morishita W, Zuniga E, Nguyen J, Blank M, Malenka RC, Garner CC. Pharmacotherapy for cognitive impairment in a mouse model of Down syndrome. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:411–413. doi: 10.1038/nn1860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferriero DM, Miller SP. Imaging selective vulnerability in the developing nervous system. J Anat. 2010;217:429–435. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2010.01226.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fields RD. Myelination: an overlooked mechanism of synaptic plasticity? Neuroscientist. 2005;11:528–531. doi: 10.1177/1073858405282304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher JM, Copeland K, Frederick JA, Blaser SE, Kramer LA, Northrup H, Hannay HJ, Brandt ME, Francis DJ, Villarreal G, Drake JM, Laurent JP, Townsend I, Inwood S, Boudousquie A, Dennis M. Spinal lesion level in spina bifida: a source of neural and cognitive heterogeneity. J Neurosurg. 2005;102:268–279. doi: 10.3171/ped.2005.102.3.0268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foti F, Laricchiuta D, Cutuli D, De Bartolo P, Gelfo F, Angelucci F, Petrosini L. Exposure to an enriched environment accelerates recovery from cerebellar lesion. Cerebellum. 2011;10:104–119. doi: 10.1007/s12311-010-0236-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franklin TB, Mansuy IM. The prevalence of epigenetic mechanisms in the regulation of cognitive functions and behaviour. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2010;20:441–449. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friederici AD, Steinhauer K, Pfeifer E. Brain signatures of artificial language processing: evidence challenging the critical period hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:529–534. doi: 10.1073/pnas.012611199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritschy JM. Epilepsy, E/I Balance and GABA(A) Receptor Plasticity. Front Mol Neurosci. 2008;1:5. doi: 10.3389/neuro.02.005.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabrieli JD. Dyslexia: a new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience. Science. 2009;325:280–283. doi: 10.1126/science.1171999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garey L. When cortical development goes wrong: schizophrenia as a neurodevelopmental disease of microcircuits. J Anat. 2010;217:324–333. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2010.01231.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garey LJ, Ong WY, Patel TS, Kanani M, Davis A, Mortimer AM, Barnes TR, Hirsch SR. Reduced dendritic spine density on cerebral cortical pyramidal neurons in schizophrenia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1998;65:446–453. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.65.4.446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaser C, Luders E, Thompson PM, Lee AD, Dutton RA, Geaga JA, Hayashi KM, Bellugi U, Galaburda AM, Korenberg JR, Mills DL, Toga AW, Reiss AL. Increased local gyrification mapped in Williams syndrome. Neuroimage. 2006;33:46–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giedd JN. Structural magnetic resonance imaging of the adolescent brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1021:77–85. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, Paus T, Evans AC, Rapoport JL. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giorgio A, Watkins KE, Chadwick M, James S, Winmill L, Douaud G, De Stefano N, Matthews PM, Smith SM, Johansen-Berg H, James AC. Longitudinal changes in grey and white matter during adolescence. Neuroimage. 2010;49:94–103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giza CC, Prins ML. Is being plastic fantastic? Mechanisms of altered plasticity after developmental traumatic brain injury. Dev Neurosci. 2006;28:364–379. doi: 10.1159/000094163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glantz LA, Lewis DA. Decreased dendritic spine density on prefrontal cortical pyramidal neurons in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:65–73. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.1.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glickman G. Circadian rhythms and sleep in children with autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34:755–768. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC, Nugent TF, 3rd, Herman DH, Clasen LS, Toga AW, Rapoport JL, Thompson PM. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golden JA, Harding BN. Cortical malformations: unfolding polymicrogyria. Nat Rev Neurol. 2010;6:471–472. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2010.118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman-Rakic PS. Morphological consequences of prenatal injury to the primate brain. Prog Brain Res. 1980;53:1–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gosden RG, Feinberg AP. Genetics and epigenetics--nature’s pen-and-pencil set. N Engl J Med. 2007;356:731–733. doi: 10.1056/NEJMe068284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gould SJ, Lewontin RC. The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1979;205:581–598. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1979.0086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gräff J, Kim D, Dobbin MM, Tsai LH. Epigenetic regulation of gene expression in physiological and pathological brain processes. Physiological reviews. 2011;91:603–649. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00012.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Granert O, Peller M, Gaser C, Groppa S, Hallett M, Knutzen A, Deuschl G, Zeuner KE, Siebner HR. Manual activity shapes structure and function in contralateral human motor hand area. Neuroimage. 2011;54:32–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green CS, Bavelier D. Exercising your brain: a review of human brain plasticity and training-induced learning. Psychol Aging. 2008;23:692–701. doi: 10.1037/a0014345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenough WT, Black JE, Wallace CS. Experience and brain development. Child Dev. 1987;58:539–559. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gutierrez J, Ballinger SW, Darley-Usmar VM, Landar A. Free radicals, mitochondria, and oxidized lipids: the emerging role in signal transduction in vascular cells. Circ Res. 2006;99:924–932. doi: 10.1161/01.RES.0000248212.86638.e9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guttmann E. Aphasia in children. Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 1942;65:205–219. [Google Scholar]
- Guy J, Gan J, Selfridge J, Cobb S, Bird A. Reversal of neurological defects in a mouse model of Rett syndrome. Science. 2007;315(5815):1143–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1138389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison RV, Gordon KA, Mount RJ. Is there a critical period for cochlear implantation in congenitally deaf children? Analyses of hearing and speech perception performance after implantation. Dev Psychobiol. 2005;46:252–261. doi: 10.1002/dev.20052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart K, Faust D. Prediction of the effects of mild head injury: a message about the Kennard Principle. J Clin Psychol. 1988;44:780–782. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198809)44:5<780::aid-jclp2270440519>3.0.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harvey MT, Kennedy CH. Polysomnographic phenotypes in developmental disabilities. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2002;20:443–448. doi: 10.1016/s0736-5748(02)00008-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harwerth RS, Smith EL, 3rd, Duncan GC, Crawford ML, von Noorden GK. Multiple sensitive periods in the development of the primate visual system. Science. 1986;232:235–238. doi: 10.1126/science.3952507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasan KM, Eluvathingal TJ, Kramer LA, Ewing-Cobbs L, Dennis M, Fletcher JM. White matter microstructural abnormalities in children with spina bifida myelomeningocele and hydrocephalus: a diffusion tensor tractography study of the association pathways. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2008a;27:700–709. doi: 10.1002/jmri.21297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hasan KM, Sankar A, Halphen C, Kramer LA, Ewing-Cobbs L, Dennis M, Fletcher JM. Quantitative diffusion tensor imaging and intellectual outcomes in spina bifida: laboratory investigation. J Neurosurg Pediatr. 2008b;2:75–82. doi: 10.3171/PED/2008/2/7/075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayashi-Takagi A, Barker PB, Sawa A. Readdressing synaptic pruning theory for schizophrenia: Combination of brain imaging and cell biology. Commun Integr Biol. 2011;4:211–212. doi: 10.4161/cib.4.2.14492. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haydar TF, Kuan CY, Flavell RA, Rakic P. The role of cell death in regulating the size and shape of the mammalian forebrain. Cereb Cortex. 1999;9:621–626. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.6.621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hensch TK. Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:877–888. doi: 10.1038/nrn1787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hernandez AE, Li P. Age of acquisition: its neural and computational mechanisms. Psychol Bull. 2007;133:638–650. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hetherington R, Dennis M. Plasticity for recovery, plasticity for development: cognitive outcome in twins discordant for mid-childhood ischemic stroke. Child Neuropsychol. 2004;10:117–128. doi: 10.1080/09297040490911122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoeft F, McCandliss BD, Black JM, Gantman A, Zakerani N, Hulme C, Lyytinen H, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Glover GH, Reiss AL, Gabrieli JD. Neural systems predicting long-term outcome in dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:361–366. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008950108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman HJ, Hendrick EB, Dennis M, Armstrong D. Hemispherectomy for Sturge-Weber syndrome. Childs Brain. 1979;5:233–248. doi: 10.1159/000119821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holtmaat A, Svoboda K. Experience-dependent structural synaptic plasticity in the mammalian brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:647–658. doi: 10.1038/nrn2699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoppe-Hirsch E, Renier D, Lellouch-Tubiana A, Sainte-Rose C, Pierre-Kahn A, Hirsch JF. Medulloblastoma in childhood: progressive intellectual deterioration. Childs Nerv Syst. 1990;6:60–65. doi: 10.1007/BF00307922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Receptive fields of cells in striate cortex of very young, visually inexperienced kittens. J Neurophysiol. 1963;26:994–1002. doi: 10.1152/jn.1963.26.6.994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huber-Okrainec J, Dennis M, Bradley K, Spiegler B. Motor speech deficits in long-term survivors of childhood cerebellar tumors: effects of tumor type, radiation, age at diagnosis, and survival years. Neurooncology. 2001;3:371. doi: 10.1177/0883073807303995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humphreys RP. Mutism after posterior fossa surgery. Concepts Pediatr Neurosurg. 1989;9:57–64. [Google Scholar]
- Huttenlocher PR. Synaptic density in human frontal cortex - developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Res. 1979;163:195–205. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90349-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyde KL, Lerch JP, Zatorre RJ, Griffiths TD, Evans AC, Peretz I. Cortical thickness in congenital amusia: when less is better than more. J Neurosci. 2007;27:13028–13032. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3039-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyde KL, Samson F, Evans AC, Mottron L. Neuroanatomical differences in brain areas implicated in perceptual and other core features of autism revealed by cortical thickness analysis and voxel-based morphometry. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31:556–566. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imayoshi I, Sakamoto M, Kageyama R. Genetic methods to identify and manipulate newly born neurons in the adult brain. Front Neurosci. 2011;5:64. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isaacson RL. The myth of recovery from early brain damage. In: Ellis NR, editor. Aberrant development in infancy: Human and animal studies. Laurence Erlbaum Associates; Hillsdale, NJ: 1975. pp. 1–25. [Google Scholar]
- Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Shah P. Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:10081–10086. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1103228108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James W. The principles of psychology. MacMillan; London: 1890. [Google Scholar]
- Jan YN, Jan LY. Branching out: mechanisms of dendritic arborization. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:316–328. doi: 10.1038/nrn2836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiao Y, Chen R, Ke X, Chu K, Lu Z, Herskovits EH. Predictive models of autism spectrum disorder based on brain regional cortical thickness. Neuroimage. 2010;50:589–599. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson MH. Sensitive periods in functional brain development: problems and prospects. Dev Psychobiol. 2005;46:287–292. doi: 10.1002/dev.20057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson V, Stewart J, Begbie F, Trojanowski J, Smith D, Stewart W. Inflammation and white matter degeneration persist for years after a single traumatic brain injury. Brain. 2013 doi: 10.1093/brain/aws322. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston MV. Plasticity in the developing brain: implications for rehabilitation. Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2009;15:94–101. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston MV, Ishida A, Ishida WN, Matsushita HB, Nishimura A, Tsuji M. Plasticity and injury in the developing brain. Brain Dev. 2009;31:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2008.03.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jugloff DG, Vandamme K, Logan R, Visanji NP, Brotchie JM, Eubanks JH. Targeted delivery of an Mecp2 transgene to forebrain neurons improves the behavior of female Mecp2-deficient mice. Hum Mol Genet. 2008;17:1386–1396. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddn026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juranek J, Fletcher JM, Hasan KM, Breier JI, Cirino PT, Pazo-Alvarez P, Diaz JD, Ewing-Cobbs L, Dennis M, Papanicolaou AC. Neocortical reorganization in spina bifida. Neuroimage. 2008;40:1516–1522. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juranek J, Salman MS. Anomalous development of brain structure and function in spina bifida myelomeningocele. Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2010;16:23–30. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Kana RK, Minshew NJ. Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: evidence from an FMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:951–961. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kadis DS, Kerr EN, Rutka JT, Snead OC, 3rd, Weiss SK, Smith ML. Pathology type does not predict language lateralization in children with medically intractable epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2009;50:1498–1504. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2008.02014.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kates WR, Folley BS, Lanham DC, Capone GT, Kaufmann WE. Cerebral growth in Fragile X syndrome: review and comparison with Down syndrome. Microsc Res Tech. 2002;57:159–167. doi: 10.1002/jemt.10068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelleher RJ, 3rd, Bear MF. The autistic neuron: troubled translation? Cell. 2008;135:401–406. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelsch W, Sim S, Lois C. Watching synaptogenesis in the adult brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2010;33:131–149. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kennard MA. Reactions of monkeys of various ages to partial and complete decortication. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 1944;3:289–310. [Google Scholar]
- Kesler SR, Vohr B, Schneider KC, Katz KH, Makuch RW, Reiss AL, Ment LR. Increased temporal lobe gyrification in preterm children. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kivitie-Kallio S, Autti T, Salonen O, Norio R. MRI of the brain in the Cohen syndrome: a relatively large corpus callosum in patients with mental retardation and microcephaly. Neuropediatrics. 1998;29:298–301. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-973581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein RM. Inhibition of return. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4:138–147. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01452-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klingberg T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14:317–324. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohn B. Right-hemisphere speech representation and comprehension of syntax after left cerebral injury. Brain Lang. 1980;9:350–361. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(80)90154-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolb B, Mychasiuk R, Williams P, Gibb R. Brain plasticity and recovery from early cortical injury. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2011;53(Suppl 4):4–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04054.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koskiniemi M, Kyykka T, Nybo T, Jarho L. Long-term outcome after severe brain injury in preschoolers is worse than expected. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1995;149:249–254. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1995.02170150029004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovesdi E, Gyorgy AB, Kwon SK, Wingo DL, Kamnaksh A, Long JB, Kasper CE, Agoston DV. The effect of enriched environment on the outcome of traumatic brain injury; a behavioral, proteomics, and histological study. Front Neurosci. 2011;5:1–12. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kriegstein A, Alvarez-Buylla A. The glial nature of embryonic and adult neural stem cells. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32:149–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Küper M, Döring K, Spangenberg C, Konczak J, Gizewski ER, Schoch B, Timmann D. Location and restoration of function after cerebellar tumor removal-a longitudinal study of children and adolescents. Cerebellum. 2013;12:48–58. doi: 10.1007/s12311-012-0389-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenn NJ. Brain plasticity and regeneration. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1992;13:505–515. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenneberg EH. Biological foundations of language. Wiley; New York: 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Lenzlinger PM, Morganti-Kossmann MC, Laurer HL, McIntosh TK. The duality of the inflammatory response to traumatic brain injury. Molec Neurobiol. 2001;24:169–181. doi: 10.1385/MN:24:1-3:169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leocani L, Comi G. Electrophysiological studies of brain plasticity of the motor system. Neurol Sci. 2006;27(Suppl 1):S27–29. doi: 10.1007/s10072-006-0542-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levi DM. Perceptual learning in adults with amblyopia: a reevaluation of critical periods in human vision. Dev Psychobiol. 2005;46:222–232. doi: 10.1002/dev.20050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine B, Kovacevic N, Nica EI, Cheung G, Gao F, Schwartz ML, Black SE. The Toronto traumatic brain injury study: injury severity and quantified MRI. Neurology. 2008;70:771–778. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000304108.32283.aa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levitt P. Structural and functional maturation of the developing primate brain. J Pediatr. 2003;143:S35–45. doi: 10.1067/s0022-3476(03)00400-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis TL, Maurer D. Multiple sensitive periods in human visual development: evidence from visually deprived children. Dev Psychobiol. 2005;46:163–183. doi: 10.1002/dev.20055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liberman AM. Speech: A special code. Cambridge MA: MIT Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Lidzba K, Wilke M, Staudt M, Krageloh-Mann I. Early plasticity versus early vulnerability: the problem of heterogeneous lesion types. Brain. 2009;132:45–56. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin JJ, Salamon N, Lee AD, Dutton RA, Geaga JA, Hayashi KM, Luders E, Toga AW, Engel J, Jr, Thompson PM. Reduced neocortical thickness and complexity mapped in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy with hippocampal sclerosis. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:2007–2018. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lövdén M, Bodammer NC, Kühn S, Kaufmann J, Schütze H, Tempelmann C, Heinze HJ, Düzel E, Schmiedek F, Lindenberger U. Experience-dependent plasticity of white-matter microstructure extends into old age. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:3878–3883. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Low LK, Cheng HJ. Axon pruning: an essential step underlying the developmental plasticity of neuronal connections. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2006;361:1531–1544. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lugaro E. In: Modern problems in psychiatry. Orr D, Rows RG, translators. Univ. Press; Manchester: 1913. [Google Scholar]
- Luikenhuis S, Giacometti E, Beard CF, Jaenisch R. Expression of MeCP2 in postmitotic neurons rescues Rett syndrome in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101:6033–6038. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0401626101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackey AP, Whitaker KJ, Bunge SA. Experience-dependent plasticity in white matter microstruture: Reasoning training alters structural connectivity. Front Neuroanat. 2012;6:Article 32. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2012.00032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magnotta VA, Andreasen NC, Schultz SK, Harris G, Cizadlo T, Heckel D, Nopoulos P, Flaum M. Quantitative in vivo measurement of gyrification in the human brain: changes associated with aging. Cereb Cortex. 1999;9:151–160. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.2.151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Makris N, Biederman J, Valera EM, Bush G, Kaiser J, Kennedy DN, Caviness VS, Faraone SV, Seidman LJ. Cortical thinning of the attention and executive function networks in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:1364–1375. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mangin JF, Jouvent E, Cachia A. In-vivo measurement of cortical morphology: means and meanings. Curr Opin Neurol. 2010;23:359–367. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32833a0afc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marder E, Prinz AA. Modeling stability in neuron and network function: the role of activity in homeostasis. Bioessays. 2002;24:1145–1154. doi: 10.1002/bies.10185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markram H, Gerstner W, Sjöström PJ. A history of spike-timing-dependent plasticity. Frontiers in Synaptic Neuroscience. 2011;3:4. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2011.00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martens MA, Jungers MK, Steele AL. Effect of musical experience on verbal memory in Williams syndrome: Evidence from a novel word learning task. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:3093–3102. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthies H. Plasticity in the nervous system--an approach to memory research. In: Ajmone Marsan C, Matthies H, editors. Neuronal plasticity and memory formation. Raven Press; New York: 1982. pp. 1–15. [Google Scholar]
- Mayer AR, Ling JM, Yang Z, Pena A, Yeo RA, Klimaj S. Diffusion abnormalities in pediatric mild traumatic brain injury. J Neurosci. 2012;32:17961–17969. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3379-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGowan PO, Meaney MJ, Szyf M. Diet and the epigenetic (re)programming of phenotypic differences in behavior. Brain Res. 2008;1237:12–24. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKinlay A, Grace RC, Horwood LJ, Fergusson DM, MacFarlane MR. Long-term behavioural outcomes of pre-school mild traumatic brain injury. Child Care Health Dev. 2010;36:22–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2009.00947.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLone DG, Knepper PA. The cause of Chiari II malformation: a unified theory. Pediatr Neurosci. 1989;15:1–12. doi: 10.1159/000120432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McQuillen PS, Ferriero DM. Perinatal subplate neuron injury: implications for cortical development and plasticity. Brain Pathol. 2005;15:250–260. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2005.tb00528.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meechan DW, Tucker ES, Maynard TM, LaMantia AS. Diminished dosage of 22q11 genes disrupts neurogenesis and cortical development in a mouse model of 22q11 deletion/DiGeorge syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:16434–16445. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905696106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehler MF, Purpura DP. Autism, fever, epigenetics and the locus coeruleus. Brain Res Rev. 2009;59:388–392. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merkley TL, Bigler ED, Wilde EA, McCauley SR, Hunter JV, Levin HS. Diffuse changes in cortical thickness in pediatric moderate-to-severe traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. 2008;25:1343–1345. doi: 10.1089/neu.2008.0615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michel GF, Tyler AN. Critical period: a history of the transition from questions of when, to what, to how. Dev Psychobiol. 2005;46:156–162. doi: 10.1002/dev.20058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller CA, Sweatt JD. Covalent modification of DNA regulates memory formation. Neuron. 2007;53:857–869. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morganti-Kossmann MC, Rancan M, Stahel PF, Kossmann T. Inflammatory response in acute traumatic brain injury: a double-edged sword. Current opinion in critical care. 2002;8:101–105. doi: 10.1097/00075198-200204000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morrison G, Fraser DD, Cepinskas G. Mechanisms and consequences of acquired brain injury during development. Pathophysiology. 2012;20:49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.pathophys.2012.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mostofsky SH, Burgess MP, Gidley Larson JC. Increased motor cortex white matter volume predicts motor impairment in autism. Brain. 2007;130:2117–2122. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mueller SG, Laxer KD, Barakos J, Cheong I, Garcia P, Weiner MW. Widespread neocortical abnormalities in temporal lobe epilepsy with and without mesial sclerosis. Neuroimage. 2009;46:353–359. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller K, Kass-Iliyya F, Reitz M. Ontogeny of ipsilateral corticospinal projections: a developmental study with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Ann Neurol. 1997;42:705–711. doi: 10.1002/ana.410420506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagy Z, Lagercrantz H, Hutton C. Effects of preterm birth on cortical thickness measured in adolescence. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21:300–306. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Narr KL, Woods RP, Lin J, Kim J, Phillips OR, Del’Homme M, Caplan R, Toga AW, McCracken JT, Levitt JG. Widespread cortical thinning is a robust anatomical marker for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48:1014–1022. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b395c0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nava E, Röder B. Adaptation and maladaptation insights from brain plasticity. In: Green AM, Chapman CE, Kalaska JF, Lepore F, editors. Progress in Brain Research. Elsevier; 2011. pp. 177–194. 2011/07/12 ed. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neves G, Cooke SF, Bliss TV. Synaptic plasticity, memory and the hippocampus: a neural network approach to causality. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:65–75. doi: 10.1038/nrn2303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nudo RJ. Plasticity. NeuroRx. 2006;3:420–427. doi: 10.1016/j.nurx.2006.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nudo RJ, Wise BM, SiFuentes F, Milliken GW. Neural substrates for the effects of rehabilitative training on motor recovery after ischemic infarct. Science. 1996;272:1791–1794. doi: 10.1126/science.272.5269.1791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Donnell S, Noseworthy MD, Levine B, Dennis M. Cortical thickness of the frontopolar area in typically developing children and adolescents. Neuroimage. 2005;24:948–954. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Overstreet-Wadiche LS, Bensen AL, Westbrook GL. Delayed development of adult-generated granule cells in dentate gyrus. J Neurosci. 2006;26:2326–2334. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4111-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paolicelli RC, Bolasco G, Pagani F, Maggi L, Scianni M, Panzanelli P, Giustetto M, Ferreira TA, Guiducci E, Dumas L, Ragozzino D, Gross CT. Synaptic pruning by microglia is necessary for normal brain development. Science. 2011;333:1456–1458. doi: 10.1126/science.1202529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pardo CA, Eberhart CG. The neurobiology of autism. Brain Pathol. 2007;17:434–447. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2007.00102.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parrish JZ, Kim MD, Jan LY, Jan YN. Genome-wide analyses identify transcription factors required for proper morphogenesis of Drosophila sensory neuron dendrites. Genes Dev. 2006;20:820–835. doi: 10.1101/gad.1391006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:947–957. doi: 10.1038/nrn2513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perfetti CA, Liu Y, Fiez J, Nelson J, Bolger DJ, Tan LH. Reading in two writing systems: Accommodation and assimilation of the brain’s reading network. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 2007;10:131–146. [Google Scholar]
- Petanjek Z, Judas M, Simic G, Rasin MR, Uylings HB, Rakic P, Kostovic I. Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:13281–13286. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105108108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pozo K, Goda Y. Unraveling mechanisms of homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Neuron. 2010;66:337–351. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prince DA, Parada I, Scalise K, Graber K, Jin X, Shen F. Epilepsy following cortical injury: cellular and molecular mechanisms as targets for potential prophylaxis. Epilepsia. 2009;50(Suppl 2):30–40. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2008.02008.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pugh P, Adlaf E, Zhao CS, Markwardt S, Gavin C, Wadiche J, Overstreet-Wadiche L. Enhanced integration of newborn neurons after neonatal insults. Front Neurosci. 2011;5:45. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2011.00045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pullela R, Raber J, Pfankuch T, Ferriero DM, Claus CP, Koh SE, Yamauchi T, Rola R, Fike JR, Noble-Haeusslein LJ. Traumatic injury to the immature brain results in progressive neuronal loss, hyperactivity and delayed cognitive impairments. Dev Neurosci. 2006;28:396–409. doi: 10.1159/000094166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radcliffe J, Bunin GR, Sutton LN, Goldwein JW, Phillips PC. Cognitive deficits in long-term survivors of childhood medulloblastoma and other noncortical tumors: age-dependent effects of whole brain radiation. Int J Dev Neurosci. 1994;12:327–334. doi: 10.1016/0736-5748(94)90081-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rae C, Joy P, Harasty J, Kemp A, Kuan S, Christodoulou J, Cowell CT, Coltheart M. Enlarged temporal lobes in Turner syndrome: an X-chromosome effect? Cereb Cortex. 2004;14:156–164. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramocki MB, Zoghbi HY. Failure of neuronal homeostasis results in common neuropsychiatric phenotypes. Nature. 2008;455:912–918. doi: 10.1038/nature07457. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raznahan A, Cutter W, Lalonde F, Robertson D, Daly E, Conway GS, Skuse DH, Ross J, Lerch JP, Giedd JN, Murphy DD. Cortical anatomy in human X monosomy. Neuroimage. 2010;49:2915–2923. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raznahan A, Shaw P, Lalonde F, Stockman M, Wallace GL, Greenstein D, Clasen L, Gogtay N, Giedd JN. How does your cortex grow? J Neurosci. 2011;31:7174–7177. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0054-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rees S, Harding R, Walker D. The biological basis of injury and neuroprotection in the fetal and neonatal brain. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2011;29:551–563. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2011.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rekate HL, Grubb RL, Aram DM, Hahn JF, Ratcheson RA. Muteness of cerebellar origin. Arch Neurol. 1985;42:697–698. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1985.04060070091023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rezaie R, Simos PG, Fletcher JM, Cirino PT, Vaughn S, Papanicolaou AC. Temporo-parietal Brain Activity as a Longitudinal Predictor of Response to Educational Interventions among Middle School Struggling Readers. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2011;17:875–885. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000890. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts PD, Leen TK. Anti-hebbian spike-timing-dependent plasticity and adaptive sensory processing. Front Comput Neurosci. 2010;4:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2010.00156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Juda M, Kantermann T, Allebrandt K, Gordijn M, Merrow M. Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11:429–438. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms. 2003;18:80–90. doi: 10.1177/0748730402239679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell VA, Oades RD, Tannock R, Killeen PR, Auerbach JG, Johansen EB, Sagvolden T. Response variability in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: a neuronal and glial energetics hypothesis. Behav Brain Funct. 2006;2:30–54. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-2-30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sacks O. In: The mind’s eye. Knopf Alfred A., editor. London: Picador, New York; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Sakamoto M, Imayoshi I, Ohtsuka T, Yamaguchi M, Mori K, Kageyama R. Continuous neurogenesis in the adult forebrain is required for innate olfactory responses. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:8479–8484. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018782108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sale A, Berardi N, Maffei L. Enrich the environment to empower the brain. Trends Neurosci. 2009;32:233–239. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salman MS, Dennis M, Sharpe JA. The cerebellar dysplasia of Chiari II malformation as revealed by eye movements. Can J Neurol Sci. 2009;36:713–724. doi: 10.1017/s0317167100008325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandovici I, Smith NH, Nitert MD, Ackers-Johnson M, Uribe-Lewis S, Ito Y, Jones RH, Marquez VE, Cairns W, Tadayyon M, O’Neill LP, Murrell A, Ling C, Constancia M, Ozanne SE. Maternal diet and aging alter the epigenetic control of a promoter-enhancer interaction at the Hnf4a gene in rat pancreatic islets. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:5449–5454. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1019007108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saxena S, Caroni P. Mechanisms of axon degeneration: from development to disease. Prog Neurobiol. 2007;83:174–191. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scaglione A, Moxon KA, Aguilar J, Foffani G. Trial-to-trial variability in the responses of neurons carries information about stimulus location in the rat whisker thalamus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:14956–14961. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1103168108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schachar R, Levin HS, Max JE, Purvis K, Chen S. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms and response inhibition after closed head injury in children: do preinjury behavior and injury severity predict outcome? Dev Neuropsychol. 2004;25:179–198. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2004.9651927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaer M, Debbane M, Bach Cuadra M, Ottet MC, Glaser B, Thiran JP, Eliez S. Deviant trajectories of cortical maturation in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome (22q11DS): a cross-sectional and longitudinal study. Schizophr Res. 2009;115:182–190. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schipul SE, Keller TA, Just MA. Inter-regional brain communication and its disturbance in autism. Front Syst Neurosci. 2011;5:10. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlaug G, Marchina S, Norton A. Evidence for plasticity in white-matter tracts of patients with chronic Broca’s aphasia undergoing intense intonation-based speech therapy. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1169:385–394. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04587.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD. The role of the cerebellum in cognition and emotion: personal reflections since 1982 on the dysmetria of thought hypothesis, and its historical evolution from theory to therapy. Neuropsychol Rev. 2010;20:236–260. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9142-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt-Hieber C, Jonas P, Bischofberger J. Enhanced synaptic plasticity in newly generated granule cells of the adult hippocampus. Nature. 2004;429:184–187. doi: 10.1038/nature02553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider GE, Jhaveri SR. Neuroanatomical correlations of spared or altered function after brain lesions in the newborn hamster. In: Stein DG, Rosen JJ, Butters N, editors. Plasticity and Recovery of Function in the Central Nervous System. Academic Press; New York: 1974. pp. 65–109. [Google Scholar]
- Shaw P, Kabani NJ, Lerch JP, Eckstrand K, Lenroot R, Gogtay N, Greenstein D, Clasen L, Evans A, Rapoport JL, Giedd JN, Wise SP. Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J Neurosci. 2008;28:3586–3594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, Blachman BA, Pugh KR, Fulbright RK, Skudlarski P, Mencl WE, Constable RT, Holahan JM, Marchione KE, Fletcher JM, Lyon GR, Gore JC. Development of left occipitotemporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically- based intervention. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:926–933. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sierra A, Abiega1 O, Shahraz A, Neumann H. Janus-faced microglia: beneficial and detrimental consequences of microglial phagocytosis. Front Cellular Neurosci. 2013;7:1–22. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2013.00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simos PG, Fletcher JM, Bergman E, Breier JI, Foorman BR, Castillo EM, Davis RN, Fitzgerald M, Papanicolaou AC. Dyslexia-specific brain activation profile becomes normal following successful remedial training. Neurology. 2002;58:1203–1213. doi: 10.1212/wnl.58.8.1203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sowell ER, Mattson SN, Kan E, Thompson PM, Riley EP, Toga AW. Abnormal cortical thickness and brain-behavior correlation patterns in individuals with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:136–144. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:309–315. doi: 10.1038/nn1008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spironelli C, Penolazzi B, Vio C, Angrilli A. Cortical reorganization in dyslexic children after phonological training: evidence from early evoked potentials. Brain. 2010;133:3385–3395. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spolidoro M, Sale A, Berardi N, Maffei L. Plasticity in the adult brain: lessons from the visual system. Exp Brain Res. 2009;192:335–341. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1509-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stahnisch FW, Nitsch R. Santiago Ramon y Cajal’s concept of neuronal plasticity: the ambiguity lives on. Trends Neurosci. 2002;25:589–591. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(02)02251-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staudt M. Reorganization after pre- and perinatal brain lesions. J Anat. 2010;217:469–474. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2010.01262.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steen RG, Hamer RM, Lieberman JA. Measurement of brain metabolites by 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy in patients with schizophrenia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:1949–1962. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein M, Federspiel A, Koenig T, Wirth M, Strik W, Wiest R, Brandeis D, Dierks T. Structural plasticity in the language system related to increased second language proficiency. Cortex. 2012;48:458–465. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stiles J, Jernigan TL. The basics of brain development. Neuropsychol Rev. 2010;20:327–348. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9148-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Subramaniam B, Naidu S, Reiss AL. Neuroanatomy in Rett syndrome: cerebral cortex and posterior fossa. Neurology. 1997;48:399–407. doi: 10.1212/wnl.48.2.399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamura Y, Ueki Y, Lin P, Vorbach S, Mima T, Kakigi R, Hallett M. Disordered plasticity in the primary somatosensory cortex in focal hand dystonia. Brain. 2009;132:749–755. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan WH, Bacino CA, Skinner SA, Anselm I, Barbieri-Welge R, Bauer-Carlin A, Beaudet AL, Bichell TJ, Gentile JK, Glaze DG, Horowitz LT, Kothare SV, Lee HS, Nespeca MP, Peters SU, Sahoo T, Sarco D, Waisbren SE, Bird LM. Angelman syndrome: Mutations influence features in early childhood. Am J Med Genet A. 2011;155A:81–90. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.33775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor HG, Alden J. Age-related differences in outcomes following childhood brain insults: an introduction and overview. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1997;3:555–567. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tetzlaff C, Okujeni S, Egert U, Wörgötter F, Butz M. Self-organized criticality in developing neuronal networks. PLoS Comput Biol. 2010;6:e1001013. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1001013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson PM, Lee AD, Dutton RA, Geaga JA, Hayashi KM, Eckert MA, Bellugi U, Galaburda AM, Korenberg JR, Mills DL, Toga AW, Reiss AL. Abnormal cortical complexity and thickness profiles mapped in Williams syndrome. J Neurosci. 2005;25:4146–4158. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0165-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Timofeev I, Bazhenov M, Avramescu S, Nita DA. Posttraumatic epilepsy: the roles of synaptic plasticity. Neuroscientist. 2010;16:19–27. doi: 10.1177/1073858409333545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trainor LJ, Shahin AJ, Roberts LE. Understanding the benefits of musical training: effects on oscillatory brain activity. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1169:133–142. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04589.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trasande CA, Ramirez JM. Activity deprivation leads to seizures in hippocampal slice cultures: is epilepsy the consequence of homeostatic plasticity? J Clin Neurophysiol. 2007;24:154–164. doi: 10.1097/WNP.0b013e318033787f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Traynor BJ, Singleton AB. Nature versus nurture: death of a dogma, and the road ahead. Neuron. 2010;68:196–200. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treble A, Juranek J, Steubing KA, Dennis M, Fletcher J. Cortical reorganization in spina bifida myelomeningocele: Relations with motor and cognitive function. Cereb Cortex. 2012 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turrigiano G. Too many cooks? Intrinsic and synaptic homeostatic mechanisms in cortical circuit refinement. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2011;34:89–103. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turrigiano GG, Nelson SB. Homeostatic plasticity in the developing nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:97–107. doi: 10.1038/nrn1327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vachha B, Adams RC, Rollins NK. Limbic tract anomalies in pediatric myelomeningocele and Chiari II malformation: anatomic correlations with memory and learning--initial investigation. Radiology. 2006;240:194–202. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2401050674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Heuvel MP, Mandl RC, Kahn RS, Hulshoff Pol HE. Functionally linked resting-state networks reflect the underlying structural connectivity architecture of the human brain. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:3127–3141. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O. Rich-club organization of the human connectome. J Neurosci. 2011;31:15775–15786. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3539-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Dongen HR, Catsman-Berrevoets CE, van Mourik M. The syndrome of ‘cerebellar’ mutism and subsequent dysarthria. Neurology. 1994;44:2040–2046. doi: 10.1212/wnl.44.11.2040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Mourik M, Catsman-Berrevoets CE, Yousef-Bak E, Paquier PF, van Dongen HR. Dysarthria in children with cerebellar or brainstem tumors. Pediatr Neurol. 1998;18:411–414. doi: 10.1016/s0887-8994(97)00232-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanderhaeghen P, Cheng HJ. Guidance molecules in axon pruning and cell death. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol. 2010;2:a001859. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a001859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voets NL, Bernhardt BC, Kim H, Yoon U, Bernasconi N. Increased temporolimbic cortical folding complexity in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology. 2011;76:138–144. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318205d521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogel AC, Power JD, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. Development of the brain’s functional network architecture. Neuropsychol Rev. 2010;20:362–375. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9145-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volman V, Bazhenov M, Sejnowski TJ. Pattern of trauma determines the threshold for epileptic activity in a model of cortical deafferentation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011a;108:15402–15407. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112066108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volman V, Sejnowski TJ, Bazhenov M. Topological basis of epileptogenesis in a model of severe cortical trauma. J Neurophysiol. 2011b;106:1933–1942. doi: 10.1152/jn.00458.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang L, Conner JM, Rickert J, Tuszynski MH. Structural plasticity within highly specific neuronal populations identifies a unique parcellation of motor learning in the adult brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:2545–2550. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1014335108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang X, Pal R, Chen XW, Kumar KN, Kim OJ, Michaelis EK. Genome-wide transcriptome profiling of region-specific vulnerability to oxidative stress in the hippocampus. Genomics. 2007;90:201–212. doi: 10.1016/j.ygeno.2007.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang X, Michaelis EK. Selective neuronal vulnerability to oxidative stress in the brain. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:1–13. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wass S. Distortions and disconnections: disrupted brain connectivity in autism. Brain Cogn. 2011;75:18–28. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Webb C, Rose FD, Johnson DA, Attree EA. Age and recovery from brain injury: clinical opinions and experimental evidence. Brain Inj. 1996;10:303–310. doi: 10.1080/026990596124476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wegiel J, Kuchna I, Nowicki K, Imaki H, Marchi E, Ma SY, Chauhan A, Chauhan V, Bobrowicz TW, de Leon M, Louis LA, Cohen IL, London E, Brown WT, Wisniewski T. The neuropathology of autism: defects of neurogenesis and neuronal migration, and dysplastic changes. Acta Neuropathol. 2010;119:755–770. doi: 10.1007/s00401-010-0655-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weis S, Weber G, Neuhold A, Rett A. Down syndrome: MR quantification of brain structures and comparison with normal control subjects. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 1991;12:1207–1211. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White T, Andreasen NC, Nopoulos P, Magnotta V. Gyrification abnormalities in childhood- and adolescent-onset schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:418–426. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00065-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White T, Hilgetag CC. Gyrification and neural connectivity in schizophrenia. Dev Psychopathol. 2011;23:339–352. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White T, Su S, Schmidt M, Kao CY, Sapiro G. The development of gyrification in childhood and adolescence. Brain Cogn. 2010;72:36–45. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilde EA, Hunter JV, Bigler ED. Pediatric traumatic brain injury: neuroimaging and neurorehabilitation outcome. NeuroRehabilitation. 2012;31:245–260. doi: 10.3233/NRE-2012-0794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilde EA, Hunter JV, Newsome MR, Scheibel RS, Bigler ED, Johnson JL, Fearing MA, Cleavinger HB, Li X, Swank PR, Pedroza C, Roberson GS, Bachevalier J, Levin HS. Frontal and temporal morphometric findings on MRI in children after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma. 2005;22:333–344. doi: 10.1089/neu.2005.22.333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Will B, Dalrymple-Alford J, Wolff M, Cassel JC. The concept of brain plasticity--Paillard’s systemic analysis and emphasis on structure and function (followed by the translation of a seminal paper by Paillard on plasticity) Behav Brain Res. 2008;192:7–11. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.11.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wobrock T, Gruber O, McIntosh AM, Kraft S, Klinghardt A, Scherk H, Reith W, Schneider-Axmann T, Lawrie SM, Falkai P, Moorhead TW. Reduced prefrontal gyrification in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2010;260:455–464. doi: 10.1007/s00406-009-0096-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolosin SM, Richardson ME, Hennessey JG, Denckla MB, Mostofsky SH. Abnormal cerebral cortex structure in children with ADHD. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:175–184. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu TC, Wilde EA, Bigler ED, Li X, Merkley TL, Yallampalli R, McCauley SR, Schnelle KP, Vasquez AC, Chu Z, Hanten G, Hunter JV, Levin HS. Longitudinal changes in the corpus callosum following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Dev Neurosci. 2010;32:361–373. doi: 10.1159/000317058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yakovlev PI, Lecours AR. The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain. In: Minkowski A, editor. Regional development of the brain in early life. Blackwell; Oxford: 1967. pp. 3–70. [Google Scholar]
- Yang S, Weiner BD, Zhang LS, Cho SJ, Bao S. Homeostatic plasticity drives tinnitus perception in an animal model. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:14974–14979. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1107998108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yizhar O, Fenno LE, Prigge M, Schneider F, Davidson TJ, O’Shea DJ, Sohal VS, Goshen I, Finkelstein J, Paz JT, Stehfest K, Fudim R, Ramakrishnan C, Huguenard JR, Hegemann P, Deisseroth K. Neocortical excitation/inhibition balance in information processing and social dysfunction. Nature. 2011;477:171–178. doi: 10.1038/nature10360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang Y, Zhou Y, Yu C, Lin L, Li C, Jiang T. Reduced cortical folding in mental retardation. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2010;31:1063–1067. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A1984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]