Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychology logoLink to Frontiers in Psychology
. 2013 Dec 13;4:928. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00928

The impact of expertise in olfaction

Jean-Pierre Royet 1,*, Jane Plailly 1, Anne-Lise Saive 1, Alexandra Veyrac 1, Chantal Delon-Martin 2,3
PMCID: PMC3861696  PMID: 24379793

Abstract

Olfactory expertise remains poorly understood, most likely because experts in odor, such as perfumers, sommeliers, and oenologists, are much rarer than experts in other modalities, such as musicians or sportsmen. In this review, we address the specificities of odor expertise in both odor experts and in a priori untrained individuals who have undergone specific olfactory training in the frame of an experiment, such as repeated exposure to odors or associative learning. Until the 21st century, only the behavioral effects of olfactory training of untrained control individuals had been reported, revealing an improvement of olfactory performance in terms of sensitivity, discrimination, memory, and identification. Behavioral studies of odor experts have been scarce, with inconsistent or inconclusive results. Recently, the development of cerebral imaging techniques has enabled the identification of brain areas and neural networks involved in odor processing, revealing functional and structural modifications as a function of experience. The behavioral approach to odor expertise has also evolved. Researchers have particularly focused on odor mental imagery, which is characteristic of odor experts, because this ability is absent in the average person but is part of a perfumer’s professional practice. This review summarizes behavioral, functional, and structural findings on odor expertise. These data are compared with those obtained using animals subjected to prolonged olfactory exposure or to olfactory-enriched environments and are discussed in the context of functional and structural plasticity.

Keywords: odor expert, perfumer, oenologist, mental imagery, perceptual learning, functional and structural reorganization, brain plasticity, neurogenesis

INTRODUCTION

Grenouille, who had phenomenal olfactory ability, was able to remember the olfactory imprint of a person and to instantly discern his mood. As a perfumer’s apprentice in 18th-century France, Grenouille attempted to create the ultimate, love-inspiring perfume. However, Grenouille was only a fictional character in a story written by the German writer Süskind (1986). Other testimonies of individuals with a noteworthy sense of smell have been reported in the literature. Bedichek (1960, p. 57), who was a writer, teacher, and naturalist, reported in a posthumously published book that there are “notable noses,” people who are exceptionally sensitive to odors. For instance, he explained that Helen Keller (1908a,b), who described her experience in The Century Magazine, was able to “recognize an old-fashioned country house because it has several layers of odors, left by a succession of families, of plants, perfumes and draperies.Bedichek (1960, p. 57) further highlighted that “She disentangles and identifies odors by their respective ages, a discrimination I have not found claimed by any nose except that of the bee which one observer declares identifies passage of time by displacement of antennae in flight.” More recently, Engen (1982), an eminent scientific authority in sensory perception, described an example of experienced noses used in the Vietnam War to detect the whereabouts of machinery and other items. In his famous book, Sachs (1985), a British-American neurologist, also reported the clinical case of a young student, D. Stephen, who experimented with drugs (cocaine, amphetamine). One night, Stephen vividly dreamt that he was a dog, experiencing a world unimaginably rich and significant in smells. On waking, he found that he actually retained this amazingly acute olfactory ability. As emphasized by Engen (1982), one problem with notable noses is that information about them is always anecdotal and is obtained from indirect testimonies, which are not experimentally verifiable. What can we say about the olfactory performances of these noses?

OLFACTORY PERFORMANCE IN TRAINED INDIVIDUALS AND ODOR EXPERTS

The concept of perceptual learning refers to a phenomenon whereby sensory experience induces changes in behavior and brain function (Gibson, 1991; Goldstone, 1998; Gilbert et al., 2001; Fahle and Poggio, 2002). However, Gawel (1997, p. 268) indicated that the literature does not always clearly delineate what constitutes training and what is experience: “following training, a panelist can be said to be more experienced, but he can also obtain experience without any formal training.Gawel (1997) suggested that, in the first case, better performances result from a uniform and directed program of instruction, whereas in the second case, experience relates to passive exposure to a wide variety of stimuli, which makes them more familiar. He specifies (p. 268) that “thought may be molded by discussion with others with more or less experience, but always in an unstructured way.

In this review, we shall focus on two aspects of perceptual learning by examining data from a priori untrained subjects who improved their performance by specific olfactory training (in the frame of an experiment) and from odor experts whose performance is the result of both learning and experience. These experts are mainly perfumers, oenologists, and sommeliers. Surprisingly, most behavioral studies dedicated to evaluating the performance of odor experts have examined wine experts1. To the best of our knowledge, only three studies have been devoted to perfumers (Livermore and Laing, 1996; Gilbert et al., 1998; Zarzo and Stanton, 2009). Therefore, when we present expert performances, most of the studies described will concern wine professionals (oenologists and sommeliers). Interestingly, wine discrimination has been used as an example of perceptual learning since the end of the 19th century (James, 1890; Gibson, 1953; Gibson and Gibson, 1955). It is further important to emphasize that wine experts use not only their olfactory system but also their gustatory and trigeminal functions to form a unitary perceptual experience (Small and Prescott, 2005). Wine experts also employ visual perception when identifying a wine (Panghorn et al., 1963; Morrot et al., 2001).

ODOR SENSITIVITY

In the olfactory domain, the repeated presentation of an odor (within the perithreshold concentration range) in untrained subjects results in the lowering of thresholds and the enhancement of signal detection sensitivity measures (Engen, 1960; Doty et al., 1981; Rabin and Cain, 1986; Dalton et al., 2002). Similar results are observed for volatile substances such as androstenone2, for which an individual is conspicuously anosmic but is able to detect with training (Wysocki et al., 1989; Mainland et al., 2002). These data suggest that odor experts who are trained daily can acquire better olfactory sensitivity. However, surprisingly, when the performances of wine experts were compared with those of wine novices or controls, no difference in olfactory sensitivity was revealed for either wine-related components such as tannin or alcohol or non-wine-related components such as n-butyl-alcohol (Berg et al., 1955; Bende and Nordin, 1997; Parr et al., 2002; Brand and Brisson, 2012). Bende and Nordin (1997) explained that the non-superiority in detection of wine tasters was due to their professional inexperience with a detection task per se. It is also possible that these results were due to the inadequacy of the experimental procedures used in studies.

Several authors state that the plasticity that underpins the emergence of better detection following repeated exposure to odors originates in the central components of the olfactory system, although they do not rule a contribution from peripheral components (Rabin and Cain, 1986; Mainland et al., 2002). In this context, repeated exposure to an odorant (e.g., androstenone, amyl acetate, isovaleric acid, or phenyl ethyl alcohol) can increase olfactory sensitivity to the odorant in mice (Yee and Wysocki, 2001) and rats (Doty and Ferguson-Segall, 1989) and can also increase the sensitivity of the olfactory receptor cells to that odorant in genetically anosmic mice (Wang et al., 1993) and in salmon (Nevitt et al., 1994). Thus, these data provide evidence for stimulus-induced plasticity in sensory receptor cells and suggest that the ability of olfactory cells to exhibit plasticity may be related to their continual turnover (Wang et al., 1993; Huart et al., 2013).

ODOR DISCRIMINATION

Stimulus “differentiation” also represents an important mechanism of perceptual learning in which experience refines sensory perception through the differentiation of stimulus features, dimensions, or categories (Gibson, 1991; Goldstone, 1998; Schyns et al., 1998). In olfaction, the discrimination task usually consists of comparing two odors in order to determine if they are identical or not3. Since it has been claimed that an expert can distinguish as many as 10,000 or even 15,000 odors, not including mixtures (Wright, 1964, 1972), the ability to discriminate between odors could be considered as an area of competence of odor experts. Several studies have shown that wine or beer experts have better discrimination or memory abilities than novices (Walk, 1966; Owen and Machamer, 1979; Peron and Allen, 1988; Solomon, 1990; Bende and Nordin, 1997; Parr et al., 2002; Hughson and Boakes, 2009; Zucco et al., 2011). For instance, Bende and Nordin (1997) reported that sommeliers have greater abilities to discriminate odors of eugenol and citral in a mixture than untrained subjects, although they reported only occasionally experiencing these two odors in their profession. The authors claimed that perceptual learning in odor discrimination can be generalized to other odors as well. Peron and Allen (1988) also demonstrated that novice drinkers of beer improve their ability to discriminate beer flavors with experience.

Rather than evaluating discrimination abilities between two odors, some studies have aimed to determine the maximum number of components that an individual can distinguish within a mixture. Untrained subjects can distinguish only three or four components within a mixture (Laing and Francis, 1989; Schab and Cain, 1992). Using a trained panel of 10 women and an expert panel of 8 male professional perfumers and flavorists, Livermore and Laing (1996) observed that the number of components that experts can discriminate and identify is not higher than that of untrained subjects. Nevertheless, when mixtures of two and three components only were used, experts recorded significantly more hits and fewer false alarms4 than did trained non-experts. Livermore and Laing (1996) suggested that the inability of participants to discriminate more than three of four stimuli is a physiologically imposed limit that could be related to the overlap of the odorants’ perceptual or cognitive representations. Thus, when odors are not sufficiently separated in multidimensional perceptual space, the addition of other odorants to the mixture can increase the chance of their representations overlapping, increasing the possibility of perceptual confusion and reducing the ability of the subjects to identify odors. Nevertheless, given that descriptions of wine by sommeliers are usually rich in vocabulary, Hughson and Boakes (2001) suggested that these experts might distinguish more components in a mixture than perfumers or flavorists.

ODOR MEMORY

A wide variety of tests are used to evaluate odor recognition memory (Doty, 1991). One test assesses short-term recognition memory and is similar to the discrimination procedure described above, except that a delay of a few seconds to several tens of seconds separates the two odors of a pair (Engen et al., 1973; Jehl et al., 1994). To our knowledge, only a single study with naïve subjects has investigated the impact of training on odor memory by passive exposure to stimuli (Jehl et al., 1995). The authors demonstrated that familiarization by repeated presentation of target or distractor odors improved discrimination performance by reducing the number of false alarms5, that is, incorrect recognition (Figure 1). More recently, Hughson and Boakes (2009) evaluated wine drinkers using a different procedure and demonstrated that experience can improve short-term wine recognition (4 min) by passive perceptual learning.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Effect of familiarization. Number of incorrect recognitions (false alarm scores) as a function of the number of familiarization sessions (0, 1, 2, and 3) and of the type of odor (target, distractor, or both target and distractor) to which subjects were familiarized. Vertical bars, standard errors of the mean (modified from Jehl et al., 1995).

To investigate long-term odor recognition memory, the procedure typically consists of using a set of odors for inspection, followed by the presentation of a second set of odors, including equal numbers of previously presented odors (old) and new odors, in a later testing session (Walk and Johns, 1984). For each item, subjects then indicate whether they have previously smelt the odor or not. Using such a memory test, Rabin and Cain (1984) observed that recognition performances increased with odor familiarity rated at inspection, but they did not specifically examine the influence of repeated presentation of stimuli.

ODOR IDENTIFICATION

Smell is likely the most difficult sensory modality to verbalize (Wippich et al., 1989). Human beings possess an excellent odor detection and discrimination abilities but typically have great difficulty in identifying specific odorants (Richardson and Zucco, 1989). The fact that there are no specific terms to describe odor and that odors are identified in terms of idiosyncratic personal experience can explain this difficulty. It has been hypothesized that odor information processing shares some of the cortical resources used in language processing and that these two types of processing can interfere with each other (Lorig, 1999).

Correlating with these observations, the human ability to identify and to name6 odors is extremely limited (Engen, 1987; Richardson and Zucco, 1989). Estimates vary from approximately 6 to 22 odors when subjects are tested for the first time (Engen, 1960; Sumner, 1962; Desor and Beauchamp, 1974; Lawless and Engen, 1977; Cain, 1979). However, all investigations in naïve subjects have consistently shown that identification performance improves with practice (Desor and Beauchamp, 1974; Cain and Krause, 1979; Cain, 1982). This result is observed as well when subjects must use only labels generated during the first exposure as when they have the option to change labels (Cain, 1979).

IMPACT OF VERBALIZATION ON OLFACTORY PERFORMANCE

Cain (1979) suggested that experts such as perfumers, flavor chemists, food technologists, and wine tasters must verbalize their olfactory experiences and thus identify odors better than untrained persons. To facilitate the description of complex mixtures of stimuli and the classification of sensations, experts are trained to use descriptors of odors, aromas, and flavors. Accordingly, specific terminologies are employed to describe and classify perfumes (Figure 2; Zarzo and Stanton, 2009), wines (Noble et al., 1987), Brandies (Jolly and Hattingh, 2001), or certain alimentary products such as cereals or Cheddar cheese (Chambers and Smith, 1993; Roberts and Vickers, 1994; Drake et al., 2001). Correlatively, it is natural to observe that experts (e.g., trained panelists) better characterize or describe wines (Lawless, 1984; Solomon, 1990; Gawel, 1997; Solomon, 1997; Chollet and Valentin, 2000; Hughson and Boakes, 2001), beers (Clapperton and Piggott, 1979), fishes (Cardello et al., 1982), and perfumes (Lawless, 1988) than non-experts. Consistent with these data, perfumers (or wine professionals) are less prone to classify odors in terms of their hedonic quality than non-experts, suggesting that they are able to discern (or label) perceptual qualities not available to untrained individuals (Yoshida, 1964; Ballester et al., 2008). Chollet and Valentin (2000) suggested that the perceptual representation of wine is similar in experts and novices but the verbalization of this representation varies with the level of expertise. Experts use analytical terms, whereas non-experts use holistic terms (Schab, 1991; Chollet and Valentin, 2000). Gawel (1997) even hypothesized that superior sensorial knowledge in trained panelists not only leads to the search for descriptors but also facilitates the expectation of prototypical characters, which can result in a higher probability of the detection of components.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Fragrance wheel. Fourteen perfume categories (within circles) are depicted. For the purposes of comparison, the odor effects diagram (inner square, letters in italics) proposed by Calkin and Jellinek (1994) is also illustrated (with permission from Zarzo and Stanton, 2009).

Discrimination and recognition memory performances of odors and aromas, as described above (see Odor Discrimination and Odor Memory), were evaluated in perceptual terms only. However, except for two studies in which the authors knowingly used unfamiliar odors (Jehl et al., 1994, 1995), semantic impact was likely largely present but not considered in these studies. In addition, it was demonstrated, in an experimental frame, that discrimination and memory performances can partly be improved by verbalization of the stimuli or the knowledge of their names. Such results have been observed in wine experts (Solomon, 1990; Melcher and Schooler, 1996) and in naïve subjects (Lawless and Engen, 1977; Rabin, 1988; Jehl et al., 1997). For instance, Rabin (1988) reported that naïve subjects trained to label specific odors significantly enhanced their ability to discriminate them one day later. According to Rabin (1988, p. 539), “endowing a layperson with a perfumer’s experience would make subtle mixture components more salient stimuli.

In short, it emerges from these data that perceptual (via passive exposure) and cognitive (label learning, development of classification schemas) changes accompany the development of wine expertise (Solomon, 1997; Hughson and Boakes, 2001, 2002; Zucco et al., 2011). However, if perceptual learning of wine, which depends on the frequency and diversity of exposure to stimuli, is rapid and passive, cognitive expertise (semantic) is slower and difficult to develop and requires many years of practice (Zucco et al., 2011). Similar changes are likely associated with the development of expertise in perfumers or flavorists (Jones, 1968; Schab and Cain, 1992). With time, the expert can then acquire perceptual abilities incredibly superior to that of an untrained person (Schab and Cain, 1992).

ODOR MENTAL IMAGERY

The review of the literature described above shows that it is difficult to propose a test to reveal the higher sensory capacities of odor experts compared to naïve subjects. Data are often conflicting, and it is difficult to decide what is sensory and what is semantic in these tasks. The mental imagery task can satisfy these requirements.

With regards to olfaction, the widespread assertion is that it is very difficult for the average person to mentally imagine odors, in contrast to our ability to mentally imagine images, sounds, or music (Stevenson and Case, 2005; Stevenson et al., 2007). Despite behavioral and psychophysical studies demonstrating the existence of odor imagery (Lyman and McDaniel, 1990; Algom and Cain, 1991; Algom et al., 1993; Carrasco and Ridout, 1993; Ahsen, 1995; Djordjevic et al., 2004a,b, 2005), several authors have even claimed that recalling physically absent odors is not possible (Engen, 1991; Crowder and Schab, 1995; Herz, 2000). However, odor experts do not appear to have difficulty in mentally smelling odors. When perfumers are questioned, they claim that they are quite able to do this and that these images provide the same sensations as the olfactory experiences evoked by odorous stimuli themselves. Gilbert et al. (1998) were the first to investigate olfactory imagery abilities in fragrance experts and to provide evidence that they are better than in non-expert controls. Importantly, they did not observe a difference between the visual mental imagery abilities of the expert and non-expert groups.

BRAIN REORGANIZATION WITH OLFACTORY PERFORMANCE

The Polish neuroscientist Jerzy Konorski (1948) is regarded as being the first to introduce the term neuroplasticity (also referred to as brain plasticity, cortical plasticity, or cortical re-mapping) to the scientific literature (Jancke, 2009). Konorski presented one of the earliest comprehensive theories of associative learning as a result of long-term neuronal plasticity and also proposed the idea that synapses strengthen with use. The advent of modern brain imaging methods has boosted the study of cortical plasticity in healthy human subjects in the last 20 years (Jancke, 2009). These techniques have enabled the investigation of functional as well as structural plasticity7 in experts such as musicians or sportsmen. What about olfactory expertise?

FUNCTIONAL AND STRUCTURAL DATA IN NON-EXPERTS

A few recent studies suggest that, even in the absence of specific learning, everyday olfactory experience improves olfactory performance and simultaneously shapes olfactory bran regions in the average person (Buschhuter et al., 2008; Frasnelli et al., 2010; Seubert et al., 2013). For instance, the volumes of the olfactory bulb, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and insula are positively correlated with the composite measure of olfactory threshold, discrimination, and identification scores (Frasnelli et al., 2010). Moreover, to compensate for their lack of vision, it is well established that blind subjects develop enhanced abilities in the use of their remaining senses. Accordingly, Rombaux et al. (2010) observed that blind subjects have better olfactory performance than sighted control subjects and correlatively have higher olfactory bulb volumes. Congenital or early blind subjects also activate olfactory areas (amygdala, OFC, hippocampus) and occipital areas more strongly than sighted control subjects during an olfactory task (Kupers et al., 2011; Renier et al., 2013), providing evidence that blind individuals undergo adaptive neuroplastic changes.

Other studies demonstrate that changes in brain activity can be observed in healthy control subjects after training. Li et al. (2008) demonstrated that odor aversive learning enhances the perceptual discrimination of initially indistinguishable odor enantiomers and that these results parallel the spatial divergence of ensemble activity patterns in the primary olfactory cortex (piriform cortex). These results indicate that aversive learning updates odor quality representations in the piriform cortex or, in other terms, emphasizes a spatial reorganization of odor coding. The same team also demonstrated that prolonged exposure (3.5 min) to a floral-smelling odorant is sufficient to enhance perceptual differentiation of novel odorants that are related in odor quality or functional groups (Figure 3; Li et al., 2006). This finding indicates that subjects become floral “experts.” This effect is paralleled by increased responses in both the posterior piriform cortex and the medial OFC. The authors of this older work speculated that this learning-induced plasticity could reflect two neuronal mechanisms: an enlargement of cortical receptive fields that results in the recruitment of more neurons (spatial summation), or, alternatively, a synchronization of neuronal activity (temporal summation; Gilbert et al., 2001).

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3

Experience-induced neural plasticity in the OFC predicts olfactory perceptual learning. (A) The scatterplot demonstrates a strong correlation between the level of learning-induced OFC signal and the behavioral magnitude of perceptual learning. (B) Activation is superimposed on a mean T1-weighted coronal section and displays the area in OFC exhibiting this correlation. OFC, orbitofrontal cortex (modified with permission from Li et al., 2006).

The results of Li et al. (2006) are echoed by electrophysiological data reported by Wilson (2000, 2003) using anesthetized rats. The authors suggested that perceptual learning via prolonged odorant exposure (habituation) can modify odor-evoked activity in the piriform cortex independently of the responses in the olfactory bulb. These data suggest that adequate sensory experience favors the formation of novel odor representations in the piriform cortex, which could promote olfactory differentiation at both the behavioral (Cleland et al., 2002; Fletcher and Wilson, 2002; Johnson et al., 2002) and neural (Wilson, 2000, 2003) levels.

FUNCTIONAL AND STRUCTURAL DATA IN ODOR EXPERTS

The first study to investigate brain changes related to odor-taste expertise was reported in 2005. Castriota-Scanderbeg et al. (2005) found that, in contrast to naïve drinkers of wine, who activate the primary gustatory cortex and brain areas implicated in emotional processing (e.g., the amygdala), sommeliers activate more brain regions involved in high-level cognitive processes such as working memory and selection of behavioral strategies (the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) when they taste wine than when they taste glucose.

The second study was performed in perfumers (Plailly et al., 2012). The authors postulated that, in contrast to laymen, perfumers learn to form olfactory sensory representations through daily practice and extensive training. Because they claim to have the ability to produce perceptual images of smells in the total absence of odorants, we estimated that the ability to form odor mental images is a crucial component of a perfumer’s expertise (Royet et al., 2013). Finally, as for other sensory modalities (Kosslyn et al., 2001), we hypothesized that similar neural networks are activated during mental imagery and the actual perception of odorous sensory stimuli.

As in two studies performed in untrained subjects (Djordjevic et al., 2005; Bensafi et al., 2007), we observed that the piriform cortex is activated when perfumers mentally imagine odors. We further revealed that, during the creation of mental images of odors, expertise influences not only this primary olfactory area but also the OFC and the hippocampus, regions that are involved in memory and the formation of complex sensory associations, respectively. In these areas, the magnitude of activation was negatively correlated with experience: the greater the level of expertise, the lower the activation of these key regions (Figure 4). We explained these results in terms of improvements of perceptual capacity and, consequently, gains in performance. Perfumers require less effort to mentally imagine odors than novices. The evocation of mental images is more spontaneous, almost instantaneous, and do not need to rely on high-level cognitive processes to gather information. These abilities, acquired with time and experience, are essential for perfumers because they allow them to devote all of their cognitive resources to the artistic activity that is the creation of novel fragrances.

FIGURE 4.

FIGURE 4

Functional reorganization in perfumers. Significant negative correlations between the length of expertise in professional experts and the level of activation (amplitude) in (A) the posterior piriform cortices and (B) the left hippocampus (modified from Plailly et al., 2012).

Many studies have shown brain anatomical modifications as a result of learning and training. In experts with enhanced visual, auditory, or motor skills, such as musicians and athletes, greater performances are associated with structural brain changes in modality-specific brain areas. In olfaction, studies indicating structural modifications have only been performed in patients suffering from anosmia, hyposmia, or neurological disease (e.g., Abolmaali et al., 2002; Mueller et al., 2005; Rupp et al., 2005; Rombaux et al., 2006, 2009a,b; Wattendorf et al., 2009; Bitter et al., 2010). Therefore, these studies focus on alterations of olfactory processes associated with atrophy in olfactory-related areas. Recently, we studied structural modifications in the brains of perfumers (Delon-Martin et al., 2013). Using voxel-based morphometry and all possible methodological improvements to reduce false positives, we detected an increase in gray-matter volume in the bilateral gyrus rectus/medial orbital gyrus (GR/MOG), an orbitofrontal area that surrounds the olfactory sulcus, in perfumers. In addition, the gray-matter volumes in the anterior piriform cortex and left GR/MOG were positively correlated with experience in professional perfumers but negatively correlated with age in control subjects (Figure 5), suggesting that training counteracts the effects of aging.

FIGURE 5.

FIGURE 5

Structural reorganization in perfumers. Relationship between structural modifications and years of age. The regression lines between the gray-matter volume and years of age (from 20 to 60 years old) show a positive slope in older experts (OE, green) and a negative slope in older controls (OC, blue) for (A) the left GR/MOG and (B) the right anterior piriform cortex. GR/MOG, gyrus rectus/medial orbital gyrus (modified from Delon-Martin et al., 2013).

Our data are the first to demonstrate the functional and structural impact of long-term odor training. What characterizes odor experts compared with other types of experts? Professional musicians practice several hours a day; their practice begins early in life and continues intensively throughout their lives. Sportsmen such as gymnasts or swimmers also begin early in life, but their careers end more rapidly than those of musicians, at approximately 30–35 years of age, when their physical performance does not allow them to be competitive. In contrast to musicians and sportsmen, odor experts such as perfumers and flavorists begin their training only in early adulthood, at the beginning of their working life or when they join a specialized school. They then live in an enriched olfactory environment in which they learn to characterize and recognize numerous stimuli daily and to learn to discriminate minute differences between odors. They can continue their training into old age. Olfactory performance is usually reported to decrease with age in the layman (e.g., Doty et al., 1984; Stevens et al., 1990; Murphy et al., 1991), and these deficits are partly due to both degenerative processes within the olfactory epithelium (Doty et al., 1984; Welge-Lussen, 2009) and changes in central olfactory structures (e.g., Tomlinson and Henderson, 1976). However, our functional and structural data demonstrate that perfumers can improve their performance throughout their lives and that intensive olfactory training can also counteract the effects of age. The volume of several brain regions involved in odor processing increases in perfumers but decreases in laymen. Thus, the metaphor “use it or lose it” used by Jancke (2009, p. 535) in reference to brain plasticity can also be applied to the olfactory modality. Furthermore, even if a peripheral dysfunction is observed in elderly odor experts, our findings further suggest that elderly perfumers would still be able to mentally imagine perfumes, just as deaf professional musicians are still able to continue to compose and conduct by mentally imagining music.

NEURONAL AND CELLULAR MECHANISMS RELATED TO OLFACTORY LEARNING

In the frame of our functional study in which perfumers were asked to generate mental images of odors (Plailly et al., 2012), a decrease in the amplitude of brain activation with the level of expertise could be due to greater selectivity of neurons resulting from the decorrelation of neuronal activity (Gilbert et al., 2001). Similar mechanisms have been observed in the antennal lobe of honeybees that are trained on one odorant. The sensorial representation of that odorant becomes smaller, more compact, and non-overlapping with representations of other odorants (Faber et al., 1999). This effect has also been observed in rats that are trained to discriminate highly overlapping odorous mixtures (Chapuis and Wilson, 2012).

The nature of the cellular events that underlie structural changes in the human brain is still unknown (May, 2011), although it is widely assumed that gray matter loss in neurodegeneration corresponds to neural loss (Baron et al., 2001; Thieben et al., 2002). Several mechanisms have been proposed to explain increases in gray matter: neurogenesis, gliogenesis, synaptogenesis, and vascular changes (Figure 6; Zatorre et al., 2012). We will discuss only the two main mechanisms related to neuronal activity-dependent changes in gray matter.

FIGURE 6.

FIGURE 6

Candidate cellular mechanisms for gray matter plasticity. Cellular events in gray matter regions underlying changes detected by magnetic resonance imaging during learning include axon sprouting, dendritic branching, and synaptogenesis, neurogenesis, changes in glial number and morphology, and angiogenesis (image courtesy of Marina Corral; modified with permission from Zatorre et al., 2012).

First, gray matter increases can be explained by fast morphological changes in the intracortical axonal architecture, including the formation of new connections by dendritic spine growth (i.e., synaptogenesis) and changes in the strength of existing connections (Trachtenberg et al., 2002). These changes have been implicated in experience-related morphological modifications in the rat hippocampus (Moser et al., 1994; Geinisman et al., 2000; O’Malley et al., 2000) and have been suggested as a mechanism (long-term potentiation) underlying long-term memory (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993; Luscher et al., 2000). A 3-day olfactory learning in rats is accompanied by a dendritic spine density increase (15%) along apical dendrites of pyramidal neurons in the piriform cortex, suggesting an increased number of excitatory synapses (Knafo et al., 2001). As activity-induced dendritic morphogenesis in the hippocampus can occur within tens of minutes (Maletic-Savatic et al., 1999), the perceptual learning observed by Li et al. (2006) could be associated with such modifications.

Second, gray matter increases can be related to slow mechanisms, such as adult neurogenesis, which has been reported in the olfactory bulbs of rodents and primates, including humans (Bonfanti and Peretto, 2011; Curtis et al., 2011; Ming and Song, 2011; Huart et al., 2013; Lazarov and Marr, 2013). Although the functional impact of the addition of new olfactory neurons to mature circuits remains an outstanding question, many recent investigations have highlighted the role of network activity in shaping ongoing neurogenesis and, in turn, how the integration of new neurons refines pre-existing network functions and, consequently, olfactory behavior. To date, olfactory adult neurogenesis was associated with an improvement in short-term olfactory memory when mice were exposed daily to a novel but not familiar enriched olfactory environment (Rochefort et al., 2002; Bovetti et al., 2009; Veyrac et al., 2009). It was also demonstrated that olfactory perceptual learning both increases and requires adult neurogenesis (Moreno et al., 2009). Interestingly, constitutive neurogenesis has been described in the adult piriform cortex in several mammalian species (Bernier et al., 2002; Shapiro et al., 2007). Here, we suggest that the gray matter volume increase in the piriform cortex of perfumers could result from a fast remodeling of the intracortical neuronal network, but genesis of new neurons in this brain area cannot be excluded.

CONCLUSION

This review of the literature presents the findings of studies in which odor experts were subjects. In contrast to other domains of expertise, odor expertise has been rarely studied (Ericsson and Lehmann, 1996; Vicente and Wang, 1998; De Beni et al., 2007). In 1998, Vicente and Wang wrote that there were at least 51 studies of the effects of expertise in at least 19 different domains, including music (e.g., piano), sport (e.g., skating, baseball), games (e.g., bridge, go, chess), computer programming, medical diagnosis, maps, algebra, and circuit diagrams. The model of expertise research is the chess player because experts can reach very high levels of competence and the ability of participants is measurable and can be rated in a laboratory (De Beni et al., 2007). In all cases, studies of expertise emphasize the role of long-term working memory on performance (Ericsson and Kintsch, 1995) and highlight that “memory recall performance on meaningful stimuli has almost always been found to be correlated with domain expertise” (Vicente, 1988; Vicente and Wang, 1998, p. 33).

The extremely high performance of experts begs the fundamental question of whether their faculties are innate or acquired with training. In 1869, Francis Galton claimed that, because the limits on height and body size are genetically determined, innate mechanisms must also determine mental capacities (see Galton, 1979). Ericsson and Lehmann (1996) suggested that the influence of innate, domain-specific basic capacities (talent) on expert performance is small, possibly even negligible. However, more recent studies indicate that characteristics that distinguish experts from naïve subjects are mainly the result of adaptation. High expertise is typically associated with prolonged and maintained practice lasting many years and involving daily exercises (De Beni et al., 2007). The apparent emergence of early talent then depends on factors “such as motivation, parental support, and access to the best training environments and teachers” (Ericsson et al., 2009, p. 199).

In the context of odor experts, it is likely that expertise is acquired with training and experience rather than acquired innately, thus confirming a previous report that the notable nose is bred rather than born (Bedichek, 1960, p. 61; Engen, 1982, p. 5). Our work in cerebral imaging has led us to the same conclusions. Olfactory mental imagery capacities develop with practice and do not result from innate skill (Plailly et al., 2012). The structural modifications observed in the brain after intensive practice of an activity are not stable and rapidly disappear when this activity stops (Jancke, 2009). However, an exception that deserves to be noted is the case of synesthetes, who possess faculties to perceive a given sensory stimulus via another or several other sensory modalities. Synesthesia is a rare phenomenon that can have a genetic origin, which could explain the exceptional performances of experts such as mental calculators. Although relatively less frequent, examples of synesthesia involving olfactory sensation have been described in the literature (Day, 2005).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS) and the LABEX Cortex (NR-11-LABX-0042) of Université de Lyon within the program “Investissements d’Avenir” (ANR-11-IDEX-0007) operated by the French National Research Agency (ANR). Alexandra Veyrac was funded by LABEX Cortex.

Footnotes

1

We identified approximately 50 studies devoted to wine expertise (without taking into account expertise of other types of alcohol such as beer or brandy). This number is not huge but is much higher than the three behavioral studies that have been devoted to perfumers. Whereas the number of perfumers in the world is approximately 500 (120 in France and Switzerland), the number of oenologists (without sommeliers) can be estimated at more than 150,000 (of which 9,500 live in France) in 44 wine-producing countries.

2

Androstenone is a pheromone that has been identified in pigs. Although this steroid is also found in sweat and urine of both human male and female, and that gender-specific differences in olfactory sensitivity to this odor have been demonstrated (see, e.g., Dalton etal., 2002), it has not yet been recognized as being a human pheromone. Androstadienone, that is a compound closely-related to androstenone, has also been suggested to be a human pheromonal substance.

3

Other types of discrimination tasks are used, such as the triangle test, in which three samples, two of which are identical, are presented to participants. The task consists of determining which stimulus is different (Amerine etal., 1965). Another task asks subjects to rank samples along a sensory dimension. In the case of wine, the sensory dimension can be attributes of odor (e.g., alcohol, fruit) or taste such as sugared or astringency (produced by tannin; Solomon, 1990).

4

In such a discrimination task, a hit is defined when the subject correctly identifies a component that is present; a false alarm is defined when the subject incorrectly identifies a component as being present.

5

In the short-term recognition task, the subject must indicate whether the two odors of a pair are identical or different. A hit is defined when the two odors are identical and are so declared by the subject. A false alarm is defined when the two odors are different but are declared as identical by the subject.

6

In a typical multiple-choice identification test, the subject has a list of labels when the olfactory stimulus is presented. One of the labels is veridical (e.g., strawberry). A second label is an alternative name and evokes a similar odor (a near miss, such as raspberry). Other names are more distinct alternatives (far misses, such as tar). The number of names can vary from three to four to several dozen. In a naming test, only the odor is presented to the subject. This test is therefore more difficult than the multiple-choice test. The results can be analyzed in terms of response accuracy (veridical label, near and far misses; see, e.g., Rabin and Cain, 1984; Lyman and McDaniel, 1986).

7

The concept of “functional brain plasticity” refers to modifications of brain activity, whereas “structural brain plasticity” refers to changes at the anatomical level.

REFERENCES

  1. Abolmaali N. D., Hietschold V., Vogl T. J., Huttenbrink K. B., Hummel T. (2002). MR evaluation in patients with isolated anosmia since birth or early childhood. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 23 157–164 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ahsen A. (1995). Self-report questionnaires: new directions for imagery research. J. Ment. Imag. 19 107–123 [Google Scholar]
  3. Algom D., Cain W. S. (1991). Remembered odors and mental mixtures: tapping reservoirs of olfactory knowledge. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 17 1104–1119 10.1037/0096-1523.17.4.1104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Algom D., Marks L. E., Cain W. S. (1993). Memory psychophysics for chemosensation: perceptual and mental mixtures of odor and taste. Chem. Senses 18 151–160 10.1093/chemse/18.2.151 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Amerine M., Panghorn R., Roessler E. (1965). Principles of Sensory Evaluation of Food. New York: Academy Press [Google Scholar]
  6. Ballester J., Patris B., Symonaux R., Valentin D. (2008). Conceptual vs. perceptual wine spaces: does expertise matter? Food Qual. Pref. 19 267–276 10.1016/j.foodqual.2007.08.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baron J. C., Chetelat G., Desgranges B., Perchey G., Landeau B., De La Sayette V., et al. (2001). In vivo mapping of gray matter loss with voxel-based morphometry in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage 14 298–309 10.1006/nimg.2001.0848 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bedichek R. (1960). The Sense of Smell. New York: Doubleday [Google Scholar]
  9. Bende M., Nordin S. (1997). Perceptual learning in olfaction: professional wine tasters versus controls. Physiol. Behav. 62 1065–1070 10.1016/S0031-9384(97)00251-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bensafi M., Sobel N., Khan R. M. (2007). Hedonic-specific activity in piriform cortex during odor imagery mimics that during odor perception. J. Neurophysiol. 98 3254–3262 10.1152/jn.00349.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Berg H., Filipello F., Hinreiner E., Webb A. (1955). Evaluation of thresholds and minimum difference concentrations for various constituents of wine. Food Technol. 9 23–26 [Google Scholar]
  12. Bernier P. J., Bedard A., Vinet J., Levesque M., Parent A. (2002). Newly generated neurons in the amygdala and adjoining cortex of adult primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99 11464–11469 10.1073/pnas.172403999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bitter T., Bruderle J., Gudziol H., Burmeister H. P., Gaser C., Guntinas-Lichius O. (2010). Gray and white matter reduction in hyposmic subjects – a voxel-based morphometry study. Brain Res. 1347 42–47 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bliss T. V., Collingridge G. L. (1993). A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 361 31–39 10.1038/361031a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bonfanti L., Peretto P. (2011). Adult neurogenesis in mammals – a theme with many variations. Eur. J. Neurosci. 34 930–950 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07832.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bovetti S., Veyrac A., Peretto P., Fasolo A, De Marchis S. (2009). Olfactory enrichment influences adult neurogenesis modulating GAD67 and plasticity-related molecules expression in newborn cells of the olfactory bulb. PLoS ONE 4:e6359 10.1371/journal.pone.0006359 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brand G., Brisson R. (2012). Lateralisation in wine olfactory threshold detection: comparison between experts and novices. Laterality 17 583–596 10.1080/1357650X.2011.595955 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Buschhuter D., Smitka M., Puschmann S., Gerber J. C., Witt M., Abolmaali N. D., et al. (2008). Correlation between olfactory bulb volume and olfactory function. Neuroimage 42 498–502 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cain W. S. (1979). To know with the nose: keys to odor identification. Science 203 467–470 10.1126/science.760202 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cain W. S. (1982). Odor identification by males and females:predictions vs. performance. Chem. Senses 7 129–142 10.1093/chemse/7.2.129 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cain W. S., Krause R. J. (1979). Olfactory testing: rules for odor identification. Neurol. Res. 1 1–9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Calkin R. R., Jellinek J. S. (1994). Perfumery: Practice and Principles 1st Edn New York: Wiley [Google Scholar]
  23. Cardello A. V., Mealler O., Kapsalis J. G., Segars R. A., Sawyer F. M., Murphy C., et al. (1982). Perception of texture by trained and consumer panels. J. Food Sci. 47 1186–1197 10.1111/j.1365-2621.1982.tb07646.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Carrasco M., Ridout J. B. (1993). Olfactory perception and olfactory imagery: a multidimensional analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 19 287–301 10.1037/0096-1523.19.2.287 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Castriota-Scanderbeg A., Hagberg G. E., Cerasa A., Committeri G., Galati G., Patria F., et al. (2005). The appreciation of wine by sommeliers: a functional magnetic resonance study of sensory integration. Neuroimage 25 570–578 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Chambers E., Smith E. A. (1993). Effects of testing experience on performance of trained sensory panelists. J. Sens. Stud. 8 155–166 10.1111/j.1745-459X.1993.tb00210.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Chapuis J., Wilson D. A. (2012). Bidirectional plasticity of cortical pattern recognition and behavioral sensory acuity. Nat. Neurosci. 15 155–161 10.1038/nn.2966 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Chollet S., Valentin D. (2000). Le degré d’expertise a-t-il une influence sur la perception olfactive? Quelques éléments de réponse dans le domaine du vin. Ann. Psychol. 100 11–36 10.3406/psy.2000.28625 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Clapperton J. F., Piggott J. R. (1979). Flavour characterization by trained and untrained assessors. J. Inst. Brew. 85 275–277 10.1002/j.2050-0416.1979.tb03922.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Cleland T. A., Morse A., Yue E. L., Linster C. (2002). Behavioral models of odor similarity. Behav. Neurosci. 116 222–231 10.1037/0735-7044.116.2.222 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Crowder R. G., Schab F. R. (1995). “Imagery for odors,” in Memory for Odors eds Crowder R. G., Schab F. R. (Hilsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; ) 93–107 [Google Scholar]
  32. Curtis M. A., Kam M., Faull R. L. (2011). Neurogenesis in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33 1170–1174 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07616.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dalton P., Doolittle N., Breslin P. A. (2002). Gender-specific induction of enhanced sensitivity to odors. Nat. Neurosci. 5 199–200 10.1038/nn803 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Day S. (2005). “Some demographic and socio-cultural aspects of synesthesia,” in Synesthesia eds Robertson L. C., Sagiv N. (Oxford: University Press; ) 11–33 [Google Scholar]
  35. De Beni R., Cornoldi C., Larsson M., Magnussen S., Rönnberg J. (2007). “Memory experts: visual learning, wine tasting, orienteering and speech-reading,” in Everydday Memory eds Helstrup T., Magnussen S. (Hove: Psychological Press; ) 201–227 [Google Scholar]
  36. Delon-Martin C., Plailly J., Fonlupt P., Veyrac A., Royet J. P. (2013). Perfumers’ expertise induces structural reorganization in olfactory brain regions. Neuroimage 68C 55–62 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Desor J. A., Beauchamp G. K. (1974). The human capacity to transmit olfactory information. Percept. Psychophys. 16 551–556 10.3758/BF03198586 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Djordjevic J., Zatorre R. J., Jones-Gotman M. (2004a). Effects of perceived and imagined odors on taste detection. Chem. Senses 29 199–208 10.1093/chemse/bjh022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Djordjevic J., Zatorre R. J., Petrides M., Jones-Gotman M. (2004b). The mind’s nose: effects of odor and visual imagery on odor detection. Psychol. Sci. 15 143–148 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503001.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Djordjevic J., Zatorre R. J., Petrides M., Boyle J. A., Jones-Gotman M. (2005). Functional neuroimaging of odor imagery. Neuroimage 24 791–801 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Doty R. L. (1991). “Psychophysical measurement of odor perception in humans,” in The Human Sense of Smell eds Laing D. G., Doty R. L., Breipohl W. (Berlin: Springer-Verlag; ) 95–134 [Google Scholar]
  42. Doty R. L., Ferguson-Segall M. (1989). Influence of adult castration on the olfactory sensitivity of the male rat: a signal detection analysis. Behav. Neurosci. 103 691–694 10.1037/0735-7044.103.3.691 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Doty R. L., Shaman P., Applebaum S. L., Giberson R., Siksorski L., Rosenberg L. (1984). Smell identification ability: changes with age. Science 226 1441–1443 10.1126/science.6505700 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Doty R. L., Snyder P. J., Huggins G. R., Lowry L. D. (1981). Endocrine, cardiovascular, and psychological correlated of olfactory sensitivity changes during the human menstrual cycle. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 95 45–60 10.1037/h0077755 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Drake M. A., McIngvale S. C., Cadwallader K. R., Civille G. V. (2001). Development of a descriptive language for Cheddar cheese. J. Food Sci. 66 1422–1427 10.1111/j.1365-2621.2001.tb15225.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Engen T. (1960). Effect of practice and instruction on olfactory thresholds. Percept. Motor Skills 10 195–198 10.2466/pms.1960.10.3.195 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Engen T. (1982). The Perception of Odors. New York: Academic Press [Google Scholar]
  48. Engen T. (1987). Remembering odors and their names. Am. Scientist 75 497–503 [Google Scholar]
  49. Engen T. (1991). Odor Sensation and Memory. New York: Praeger [Google Scholar]
  50. Engen T., Kuisma J. E., Eimas P. D. (1973). Short-term memory of odors. J. Exp. Psychol. 99 222–225 10.1037/h0034645 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Ericsson K. A., Kintsch W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychol. Rev. 102 211–245 10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Ericsson K. A., Lehmann A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 47 273–305 10.1146/annurev.psych.47.1.273 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ericsson K. A., Nandagopal K., Roring R. W. (2009). Toward a science of exceptional achievement: attaining superior performance through deliberate practice. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1172 199–217 10.1196/annals.1393.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Faber T., Joerges J., Menzel R. (1999). Associative learning modifies neural representations of odors in the insect brain. Nat. Neurosci. 2 74–78 10.1038/4576 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Fahle M., Poggio T. (2002). Perceptual Learning. Cambridge MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
  56. Fletcher M. L., Wilson D. A. (2002). Experience modifies olfactory acuity: acetylcholine-dependent learning decreases behavioral generalization between similar odorants. J. Neurosci. 22 RC201 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Frasnelli J., Lundstrom J. N., Boyle J. A., Djordjevic J., Zatorre R. J., Jones-Gotman M. (2010). Neuroanatomical correlates of olfactory performance. Exp. Brain Res. 201 1–11 10.1007/s00221-009-1999-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Galton F. (1979). Hereditary Genius: An Inquiry into its Laws and Consequences (First published in 1869). London: Friedman [Google Scholar]
  59. Gawel R. (1997). The use of language by trained and untrained experienced wine tasters. J. Sens. Stud. 12 267–284 10.1111/j.1745-459X.1997.tb00067.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Geinisman Y., Disterhoft J. F., Gundersen H. J., McEchron M. D., Persina I. S., Power J. M., et al. (2000). Remodeling of hippocampal synapses after hippocampus-dependent associative learning. J. Comp. Neurol. 417 49–59 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Gibson E. J. (1953). Improvement in perceptual judgments as a function of controlled pratice or training. Psychol. Bull. 50 401–431 10.1037/h0055517 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Gibson E. J. (1991). An Odyssey in Learning and Perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
  63. Gibson J. J., Gibson E. J. (1955>). Perceptual learning: differentiation or enrichment? Psychol. Rev. 62 32–41 10.1037/h0048826 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Gilbert A. N., Crouch M., Kemp S. E. (1998). Olfactory and visual mental imagery. J. Ment. Imag. 22 137–146 [Google Scholar]
  65. Gilbert C. D., Sigman M., Crist R. E. (2001). The neural basis of perceptual learning. Neuron 31 681–697 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00424-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Goldstone R. L. (1998). Perceptual learning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 49 585–612 10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.585 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Herz R. S. (2000). Verbal coding in olfactory versus nonolfactory cognition. Mem. Cogn. 28 957–964 10.3758/BF03209343 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Huart C., Rombaux P., Hummel T. (2013). Plasticity of the human olfactory system: the olfactory bulb. Molecules 18 11586–11600 10.3390/molecules180911586 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Hughson A. L., Boakes R. A. (2001). Perceptual and cognitive aspects of wine expertise. Aust. J. Psychol. 53 103–108 10.1080/00049530108255130 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. Hughson A. L., Boakes R. A. (2002). The knowing nose: the role of knowledge in wine expertise. Food Qual. Pref. 13 463–472 10.1016/S0950-3293(02)00051-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  71. Hughson A. L., Boakes R. A. (2009). Passive perceptual learning in relation to wine: short-term recognition and verbal description. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (Hove) 62 1–8 10.1080/17470210802214890 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. James W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. New York: Holt [Google Scholar]
  73. Jancke L. (2009). The plastic human brain. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 27 521–538 10.3233/RNN-2009-0519 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Jehl C., Royet J. P., Holley A. (1994). Very short term recognition memory for odors. Percept. Psychophys. 56 658–668 10.3758/BF03208359 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Jehl C., Royet J. P., Holley A. (1995). Odor discrimination and recognition memory as a function of familiarization. Percept. Psychophys. 57 1002–1011 10.3758/BF03205459 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Jehl C., Royet J. P., Holley A. (1997). Role of verbal encoding in short- and long-term odor recognition. Percept. Psychophys. 59 100–110 10.3758/BF03206852 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Johnson B. A., Ho S. L., Xu Z., Yihan J. S., Yip S., Hingco E. E., et al. (2002). Functional mapping of the rat olfactory bulb using diverse odorants reveals modular responses to functional groups and hydrocarbon structural features. J. Comp. Neurol. 449 180–194 10.1002/cne.10284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Jolly N. P., Hattingh S. (2001). A Brandy Aroma Wheel for South African brandy. S. Afr. J. Enol. Vitic. 22 1–6 [Google Scholar]
  79. Jones F. N. (1968). “Information content of olfactory quality,” in Theories of Odors and Odor Measurement ed. Tanoyloac N. (Bebek: Robert College Center; ) 297 [Google Scholar]
  80. Keller H. (1908a). “Sense and Sensibility,” in The Century Magazine New York: The Century Company [Google Scholar]
  81. Keller H. (1908b). “Sense of Sensibility,” in The Century Magazine New York: The Century Company [Google Scholar]
  82. Knafo S., Grossman Y., Barkai E., Benshalom G. (2001). Olfactory learning is associated with increased spine density along apical dendrites of pyramidal neurons in the rat piriform cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13 633–638 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2001.01422.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Konorski J. (1948). Conditioned reflexes and neuron organization. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press [Google Scholar]
  84. Kosslyn S. M., Ganis G., Thompson W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2 635–642 10.1038/35090055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Kupers R., Beaulieu-Lefebvre M., Schneider F. C., Kassuba T., Paulson O. B., Siebner H. R., et al. (2011). Neural correlates of olfactory processing in congenital blindness. Neuropsychologia 49 2037–2044 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.03.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Laing D. G., Francis G. W. (1989). The capacity of humans to identify odors in mixtures. Physiol. Behav. 46 809–814 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90041-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Lawless H., Engen T. (1977). Associations to odors: interference, mnemonics, and verbal labeling. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Learn. 3 52–59 10.1037/0278-7393.3.1.52 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Lawless H. T. (1984). Flavor description of white wine by “expert” and nonexpert wine consumers. J. Food Sci. 49 120–123 10.1111/j.1365-2621.1984.tb13686.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  89. Lawless H. T. (1988). “Odour description and odour classification revisited,” in Food Acceptability ed. Thompson D. M. H. (London: Elsevier Applied Science; ) 27–40 [Google Scholar]
  90. Lazarov O., Marr R. A. (2013). Of mice and men: neurogenesis, cognition and Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5:43 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Li W., Howard J. D., Parrish T. B., Gottfried J. A. (2008). Aversive learning enhances perceptual and cortical discrimination of indiscriminable odor cues. Science 319 1842–1845 10.1126/science.1152837 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Li W., Luxenberg E., Parrish T., Gottfried J. A. (2006). Learning to smell the roses: experience-dependent neural plasticity in human piriform and orbitofrontal cortices. Neuron 52 1097–1108 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Livermore A., Laing D. G. (1996). Influence of training and experience on the perception of multicomponent odor mixtures. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 22 267–277 10.1037/0096-1523.22.2.267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Lorig T. S. (1999). On the similarity of odor and language perception. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23 391–408 10.1016/S0149-7634(98)00041-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Luscher C., Nicoll R. A., Malenka R. C., Muller D. (2000). Synaptic plasticity and dynamic modulation of the postsynaptic membrane. Nat. Neurosci. 3 545–550 10.1038/75714 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Lyman B. J., McDaniel M. A. (1986). Effects of encoding strategy on long-term memory for odours. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 38 753–765 10.1080/14640748608401624 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  97. Lyman B. J., McDaniel M. A. (1990). Memory for odors and odor names: modalities of elaborating and imagery. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 16 656–664 10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.656 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Mainland J. D., Bremner E. A., Young N., Johnson B. N., Khan R. M., Bensafi M., et al. (2002). Olfactory plasticity: one nostril knows what the other learns. Nature 419 802 10.1038/419802a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Maletic-Savatic M., Malinow R., Svoboda K. (1999). Rapid dendritic morphogenesis in CA1 hippocampal dendrites induced by synaptic activity. Science 283 1923–1927 10.1126/science.283.5409.1923 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. May A. (2011). Experience-dependent structural plasticity in the adult human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 15 475–482 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Melcher J. M., Schooler J. W. (1996). The misrembrance of wines past: verbal and perceptual expertise differentially mediate verbal overshadowing of taste memory. J. Mem. Lang. 35 231–245 10.1006/jmla.1996.0013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  102. Ming G. L., Song H. (2011). Adult neurogenesis in the mammalian brain: significant answers and significant questions. Neuron 70 687–702 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Moreno M. M., Linster C., Escanilla O., Sacquet J., Didier A., Mandairon N. (2009). Olfactory perceptual learning requires adult neurogenesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106 17980–17985 10.1073/pnas.0907063106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Morrot G., Brochet F., Dubourdieu D. (2001). The color of odors. Brain Lang. 79 309–320 10.1006/brln.2001.2493 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Moser M. B., Trommald M., Andersen P. (1994). An increase in dendritic spine density on hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells following spatial learning in adult rats suggests the formation of new synapses. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91 12673–12675 10.1073/pnas.91.26.12673 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Mueller A., Rodewald A., Reden J., Gerber J., Von Kummer R., Hummel T. (2005). Reduced olfactory bulb volume in post-traumatic and post-infectious olfactory dysfunction. Neuroreport 16 475–478 10.1097/00001756-200504040-00011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Murphy C., Cain W. S., Gilmore M. M., Skinner R. B. (1991). Sensory and semantic factors in recognition memory for odors and graphic stimuli: elderly versus young persons. Am. J. Psychol. 104 161–192 10.2307/1423153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Nevitt G. A., Dittman A. H., Quinn T. P., Moody W. J., Jr (1994). Evidence for a peripheral olfactory memory in imprinted salmon. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91 4288–4292 10.1073/pnas.91.10.4288 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Noble A. C., Arnold R. A., Buechsenstein J., Leach E. J., Schmidt J. O., Stern P. M. (1987). Modification of a standardized system of wine aroma terminology. Am. J. Enol. Vitic. 38 143–146 [Google Scholar]
  110. O’Malley A., O’Connell C., Murphy K. J., Regan C. M. (2000). Transient spine density increases in the mid-molecular layer of hippocampal dentate gyrus accompany consolidation of a spatial learning task in the rodent. Neuroscience 99 229–232 10.1016/S0306-4522(00)00182-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Owen D. H., Machamer P. K. (1979). Bias-free improvement in wine discrimination. Perception 8 199–209 10.1068/p080199 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Panghorn R., Berg H., Hansen B. (1963). The influence of color on discrimination of sweetness in dry table-wine. Am. J. Psychol. 76 492–495 10.2307/1419795 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  113. Parr W. V., Heatherbell D., White K. G. (2002). Demystifying wine expertise: olfactory threshold, perceptual skill and semantic memory in expert and novice wine judges. Chem. Senses 27 747–755 10.1093/chemse/27.8.747 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Peron R. M., Allen G. L. (1988). Attempts to train novices for beer flavor discrimination: a matter of taste. J. Gen. Psychol. 115 403–418 10.1080/00221309.1988.9710577 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Plailly J., Delon-Martin C., Royet J. P. (2012). Experience induces functional reorganization in brain regions involved in odor imagery in perfumers. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33 224–234 10.1002/hbm.21207 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Rabin M. D. (1988). Experience facilitates olfactory quality discrimination. Percept. Psychophys. 44 532–540 10.3758/BF03207487 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Rabin M. D., Cain W. S. (1984). Odor recognition: familiarity, identifiability, and encoding consistency. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 10 316–325 10.1037/0278-7393.10.2.316 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Rabin M. D., Cain W. S. (1986). Determinants of measured olfactory sensitivity. Percept. Psychophys. 39 281–286 10.3758/BF03204936 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Renier L., Cuevas I., Grandin C. B., Dricot L., Plaza P., Lerens E., et al. (2013). Right occipital cortex activation correlates with superior odor processing performance in the early blind. PLoS ONE 8:e71907 10.1371/journal.pone.0071907 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Richardson J. T., Zucco G. M. (1989). Cognition and olfaction: a review. Psychol. Bull. 105 352–360 10.1037/0033-2909.105.3.352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Roberts A. K., Vickers Z. M. (1994). A comparison of trained and untrained judges’ evaluation of sensory attribute intensities and liking of cheddar cheeses. J. Sens. Stud. 9 1–20 10.1111/j.1745-459X.1994.tb00226.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  122. Rochefort C., Gheusi G., Vincent J. D., Lledo P. M. (2002). Enriched odor exposure increases the number of newborn neurons in the adult olfactory bulb and improves odor memory. J. Neurosci. 22 2679–2689 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Rombaux P., Grandin C., Duprez T. (2009a). How to measure olfactory bulb volume and olfactory sulcus depth? B-ENT 5(Suppl. 13) 53–60 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Rombaux P., Martinage S., Huart C., Collet S. (2009b). Post-infectious olfactory loss: a cohort study and update. B-ENT 5(Suppl. 13) 89–95 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Rombaux P., Huart C., De Volder A. G., Cuevas I., Renier L., Duprez T., et al. (2010). Increased olfactory bulb volume and olfactory function in early blind subjects. Neuroreport 21 1069–1073 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32833fcb8a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Rombaux P., Weitz H., Mouraux A., Nicolas G., Bertrand B., Duprez T., et al. (2006). Olfactory function assessed with orthonasal and retronasal testing, olfactory bulb volume, and chemosensory event-related potentials. Arch. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 132 1346–1351 10.1001/archotol.132.12.1346 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Royet J. P., Delon-Martin C., Plailly J. (2013). Odor mental imagery in non-experts in odors: a paradox? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:87 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Rupp C. I., Fleischhacker W. W., Kemmler G., Oberbauer H., Scholtz A. W., Wanko C., et al. (2005). Various bilateral olfactory deficits in male patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 31 155–165 10.1093/schbul/sbi018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Sachs O. (1985). The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat. London: Gerald Duckworth & Co [Google Scholar]
  130. Schab F. R. (1991). Odor memory – taking stock. Psychol. Bull. 109 242–251 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.242 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Schab F. R., Cain W. S. (1992). “Memory for odors,” in The Human Sense of Smell eds Laing D. G., Doty R. L., Breipohl W. (Berlin: Springer-Verlag; ) 217–240 [Google Scholar]
  132. Schyns P. G., Goldstone R. L., Thibaut J. P. (1998). The development of features in object concepts. Behav. Brain Sci. 21 1–17; discussion 17–54. 10.1017/S0140525X98000107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Seubert J., Freiherr J., Frasnelli J., Hummel T., Lundstrom J. N. (2013). Orbitofrontal cortex and olfactory bulb volume predict distinct aspects of olfactory performance in healthy subjects. Cereb. Cortex 23 2448–2456 10.1093/cercor/bhs230 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Shapiro L. A., Ng K. L., Kinyamu R., Whitaker-Azmitia P., Geisert E. E., Blurton-Jones M., et al. (2007). Origin, migration and fate of newly generated neurons in the adult rodent piriform cortex. Brain Struct. Funct. 212 133–148 10.1007/s00429-007-0151-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Small D. M., Prescott J. (2005). Odor/taste integration and the perception of flavor. Exp. Brain Res. 166 345–357 10.1007/s00221-005-2376-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Solomon G. E. A. (1990). Psychology of novice and expert wine talk. Am. J. Psychol. 103 495–517 10.2307/1423321 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  137. Solomon G. E. A. (1997). Conceptual change and wine expertise. J. Learn. Sci. 6 41–60 10.1207/s15327809jls0601_3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  138. Stevens J. C., Cain W. C., Demarque A. (1990). Memory and identification of simulated odors in elderly and young persons. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 28 293–296 10.3758/BF03334025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  139. Stevenson R. J., Case T. I. (2005). Olfactory imagery: a review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12 244–264 10.3758/BF03196369 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Stevenson R. J., Case T. I., Mahmut M. (2007). Difficulty in evoking odor images: the role of odor naming. Mem. Cognit. 35 578–589 10.3758/BF03193296 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Sumner D. (1962). On testing the sense of smell. Lancet 2 895–897 10.1016/S0140-6736(62)90679-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Süskind P. (1986). Le Parfum. Paris: Editions Fayard [Google Scholar]
  143. Thieben M. J., Duggins A. J., Good C. D., Gomes L., Mahant N., Richards F., et al. (2002). The distribution of structural neuropathology in pre-clinical Huntington’s disease. Brain 125 1815–1828 10.1093/brain/awf179 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Tomlinson B. E., Henderson G. (1976). “Observations on the brains of demented old people,” in Neurobiology of Aging eds Terry R. D., Gershon S. (New York: Raven; ) 88–94 [Google Scholar]
  145. Trachtenberg J. T., Chen B. E., Knott G. W., Feng G., Sanes J. R., Welker E., et al. (2002). Long-term in vivo imaging of experience-dependent synaptic plasticity in adult cortex. Nature 420 788–794 10.1038/nature01273 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Veyrac A., Sacquet J., Nguyen V., Marien M., Jourdan F., Didier A. (2009). Novelty determines the effects of olfactory enrichment on memory and neurogenesis through noradrenergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 34 786–795 10.1038/npp.2008.191 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Vicente K. J. (1988). Adapting the memory recall paradigm to evaluate interfaces. Acta Psychol. 69 249–278 10.1016/0001-6918(88)90035-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  148. Vicente K. J., Wang J. H. (1998). An ecological theory of expertise effects in memory recall. Psychol. Rev. 105 33–57 10.1037/0033-295X.105.1.33 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Walk H. A., Johns E. E. (1984). Interference and facilitation in short-term memory for odors. Percept. Psychophys. 36 508–514 10.3758/BF03207510 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Walk R. D. (1966). Perceptual learning and discrimination of wine. Psychon. Sci. 5 57–58 10.3758/BF03328278 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  151. Wang H. W., Wysocki C. J., Gold G. H. (1993). Induction of olfactory receptor sensitivity in mice. Science 260 998–1000 10.1126/science.8493539 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Wattendorf E., Welge-Lussen A., Fiedler K., Bilecen D., Wolfensberger M., Fuhr P., et al. (2009). Olfactory impairment predicts brain atrophy in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurosci. 29 15410–15413 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1909-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Welge-Lussen A. (2009). Ageing, neurodegeneration, and olfactory and gustatory loss. B-ENT 5(Suppl. 13) 129–132 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Wilson D. A. (2000). Comparison of odor receptive field plasticity in the rat olfactory bulb and anterior piriform cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 84 3036–3042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Wilson D. A. (2003). Rapid, experience-induced enhancement in odorant discrimination by anterior piriform cortex neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 90 65–72 10.1152/jn.00133.2003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Wippich W., Mecklenbrauker S., Trouet J. (1989). Implicit and explicit memories of odors. Arch. Psychol. (Frankf.) 141 195–211 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Wright R. H. (1964). Odor and molecular vibration: the far infrared spectra of some perfume chemicals. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 116 552–558 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1964.tb45084.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Wright R. H. (1972). Stereochemical and vibrational theories of odour. Nature 239 226 10.1038/239226a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Wysocki C. J., Dorries K. M., Beauchamp G. K. (1989). Ability to perceive androstenone can be acquired by ostensibly anosmic people. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 86 7976–7978 10.1073/pnas.86.20.7976 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Yee K. K., Wysocki C. J. (2001). Odorant exposure increases olfactory sensitivity: olfactory epithelium is implicated. Physiol. Behav. 72 705–711 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00428-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Yoshida M. (1964). Studies of psychometric classification of odors. Jpn. Psychol. Res. 6 145–154 [Google Scholar]
  162. Zarzo M., Stanton D. T. (2009). Understanding the underlying dimensions in perfumers’ odor perception space as a basis for developing meaningful odor maps. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 71 225–247 10.3758/APP.71.2.225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Zatorre R. J., Fields R. D., Johansen-Berg H. (2012). Plasticity in gray and white: neuroimaging changes in brain structure during learning. Nat. Neurosci. 15 528–536 10.1038/nn.3045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Zucco G. M., Carassai A., Baroni M. R., Stevenson R. J. (2011). Labeling, identification, and recognition of wine-relevant odorants in expert sommeliers, intermediates, and untrained wine drinkers. Perception 40 598–607 10.1068/p6972 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Psychology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES