See corresponding article on page 122.
In this issue of the Journal, Alderman et al (1) report on the long-term follow-up of a cohort of individuals whose mothers participated in a randomized nutrition supplementation trial. One group of mothers received a protein-energy supplement, in the form of baked biscuits, from week 20 of gestation through parturition, whereas the control group received the same biscuits for 6 mo starting at delivery. The intervention was effective at increasing mean birth weight. In previous research, no longer-term benefits of prenatal supplementation on growth, body composition, or cardiometabolic risk were seen; and in this study, no differences were seen between the groups in several measures of cognitive development measured at the mean age of 19 y. The usual concerns of differential attrition, masking/confounding, and power are all addressed adequately and cannot explain the null result. What we are left with is the conclusion that the 2 groups indeed did not differ.
As discussed by the authors, the primary concern with this design is that it is not possible to distinguish between 2 conclusions: 1) the 2 interventions were each equally effective or 2) neither of the interventions was effective.
The former possibility is at least partially addressed by Maluccio et al (2) [and in related work by Stein et al (3)], who analyzed the impact of nutrition supplementation provided throughout pregnancy and the first 2 y of postnatal life in a cohort of rural Guatemalans. They reported substantial positive impacts on schooling (in girls, among whom schooling levels were lower than those of boys) and on performance on cognitive tests (in both males and females) as adults, even accounting for the differences in schooling, compared with those who were in a comparison village or who were exposed to the study intervention at other ages. So we can conclude (subject to the usual caveat that this is a single study conducted in one context) that interventions sustained over this entire age range indeed have positive benefits to the children.
The second concern is addressed in the seminal studies of the Dutch famine by Zena Stein et al (4). The circumstances of the Dutch famine, a 5-mo period in which food rations fell below 1000 kcal/d, facilitated the differentiation of the consequences of exposure to acute food shortages in various stages of pregnancy (5). Exposure in late gestation (but not exposure in early gestation) was associated with a large (∼250 g) reduction in mean birth weights, compared with those without famine exposure. Stein et al used the Dutch military conscription database to relate performance at age 18 y on a variant of the Ravens Progressive Matrices to place and month of birth, a proxy for maternal exposure to famine, and found no impact of the famine on performance, after adjusting for the employment class of the recruit’s father. (Of note, the famine did result in a marked decrease in fertility, especially among the working classes, so failure to adjust for paternal employment status results in an apparent increase in mental performance among males exposed to famine in early pregnancy, a group in which children from the professional classes are overrepresented.) Subsequent work has followed 2 independent subsets of individuals exposed to the Dutch famine through to later adulthood, again finding little or no suggestion of any impact of exposure to famine on cognitive functioning (6, 7).
So what can we learn from these studies? The putative benefits of prenatal supplementation with specific nutrients on brain development are as yet not fully defined. There is ongoing work related to DHA, a major constituent of brain tissue (8). By and large, the results suggest little impact of prenatal supplementation; where impacts have been shown, the studies have continued the supplementation postnatally, whether by supplementing the mother or, for children who were not breastfed, by fortifying formula. The timing of prenatal interventions, most of which were implemented in the second half of pregnancy, may be suboptimal: data from the Dutch famine suggest that epigenetic changes consequent to undernutrition are apparent only when exposures occur periconceptionally (9). It is unlikely that we will identify a “magic bullet” of a single intervention at a single stage of development that will help all children reach their genetically endowed potential. The critical window of efficacy is likely to be nutrient- and context-specific, because a nutrient that is rate-limiting in one setting might not be so in another.
The benefits of early cognitive stimulation are often stressed (10, 11). It is here that nutritional interventions may have their role—in ensuring that the developing child has the strength and health to explore his or her environment and benefit from this stimulation. Child growth in the first 2 y predicts schooling attainment (12), and stunting at age 2 is related to a wide range of measures of human capital in adulthood (13). Thus, ensuring adequate child growth in early childhood remains the challenge. Supplementation of the mother may provide her with the energy she needs to interact with her child, and supplementation of the child provides energy for growth and exploration and micronutrients to enhance the immune response to the inevitable infections. Sustained, balanced, optimal nutrition of the mother before and during the pregnancy and through lactation, and of the child once breast milk is no longer adequate at ∼6 mo, is likely to provide maximal benefit. This message is not new, and of course the challenges of delivery remain.
Acknowledgments
The author declared no conflicts of interest.
REFERENCES
- 1.Alderman H, Hawkesworth S, Lundberg M, Tasneem A, Mark H, Moore SE. Supplemental feeding during pregnancy compared with maternal supplementation during lactation does not affect schooling and cognitive development through late adolescence. Am J Clin Nutr 2014;99:122–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Maluccio JA, Hoddinott J, Behrman JR, Martorell R, Quisumbing AR, Stein AD. The impact of improving nutrition during early childhood on education among Guatemalan adults. Econ J 2009;119:734–63. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Stein AD, Wang M, DiGirolamo A, Grajeda R, Ramakrishnan U, Ramirez-Zea M, Yount K, Martorell R. Nutritional supplementation in early childhood, schooling, and intellectual functioning in adulthood: a prospective study in Guatemala. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2008;162:612–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Stein Z, Susser M, Saenger G, Marolla F. Nutrition and mental performance. Science 1972;178:708–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lumey LH, Stein AD, Kahn HS, van der Pal-de Bruin KM, Blauw GJ, Zybert PA, Susser ES. Cohort profile: the Dutch Hunger Winter Families Study. Int J Epidemiol 2007;36:1196–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.de Rooij SR, Wouters H, Yonker JE, Painter RC, Roseboom TJ. Prenatal undernutrition and cognitive function in late adulthood. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2010;107:16881–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.de Groot RHM, Stein AD, Jolles J, van Boxtel MPJ, Blauw GJ, van de Bor M, Lumey LH. Gestational exposure to the Dutch famine in 1944-45 and cognitive performance at age 59 years. Int J Epidemiol 2011;40:327–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gould JF, Smithers LG, Makrides M. The effect of maternal omega-3 (n–3) LCPUFA supplementation during pregnancy on early childhood cognitive and visual development: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Am J Clin Nutr 2013;97:531–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Heijmans BT, Tobi EW, Stein AD, Putter H, Blauw G-J, Susser ES, Slagboom PE, Lumey LH. Persistent epigenetic differences associated with prenatal exposure to famine in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2008;105:17046–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA, Simonoff E, Grantham-McGregor SM. Early childhood stunting is associated with poor psychological functioning in late adolescence and effects are reduced by psychosocial stimulation. J Nutr 2007;137:2464–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Engle PL, Fernald LC, Alderman H, Behrman J, O′Gara C, Yousafzai A, de Mello MC, Hidrobo M, Ulkuer N, Ertem I, et al. Global Child Development Steering Group. Strategies for reducing inequalities and improving developmental outcomes for young children in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet 2011;378:1339–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Martorell R, Horta BL, Adair LS, Stein AD, Richter L, Fall CHD, Bhargava SK, Biswas SKD, Perez L, Barros FC, et al. Consortium on Health Orientated Research in Transitional Societies Group. Weight gain in the first two years of life is an important predictor of schooling outcomes in pooled analyses from five birth cohorts from low- and middle-income countries. J Nutr 2010;140:348–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hoddinott J, Behrman JR, Maluccio JA, Melgar P, Quisumbing AR, Ramirez-Zea M, Stein AD, Yount KM, Martorell R. Adult consequences of growth failure in early childhood. Am J Clin Nutr 2013;98:1170–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]