Abstract
Some evidence suggests that genetic polymorphisms in oxytocin pathway genes influence various social behaviors, but findings thus far have been mixed. Many studies have been based in small samples and there is possibility of publication bias. Using data from 2 large U.S. prospective cohorts with over 11,000 individuals, we investigated 88 SNPs in OXTR, AVPR1A, and CD38, in relation to social integration (measured as social connectedness in both binary and continuous forms and being continuously married). After correction for multiple testing only one SNP in CD38 (rs12644506) was significantly associated with social integration and that SNP predicted when using a dichotomized indicator of social connectedness (adjusted p=0.02), but not a continuous measure of social connectedness or the continuously married outcome. A significant gender-heterogeneous effect was identified in one OXTR SNP on dichotomized social connectedness; specifically, rs4686302 T allele was nominally associated with social connectedness in men, whereas the association direction was opposite in women (adjusted gender heterogeneity p=0.02). Furthermore, the rs53576 A allele was significantly associated with social connectedness only in women, and the effect magnitude was stronger in a dominant genetic model (adjusted p=0.003). In summary, our findings suggested that common genetic variants of OXTR, CD38, and AVPR1A are not associated with social integration as measured in this study using the simplified Berkman-Syme Social Network Index, but these findings and other work hint that effects may be modified by gender or other social experiences. Further work considering genetic pathways in relation to social integration may be more fruitful if these additional factors can be more comprehensively evaluated.
Keywords: OXTR, CD38, AVPR1A, social integration, sex-specific, candidate gene
BACKGROUND
The importance of social relationships in human health has long been recognized. For example, social integration, defined as the presence of close personal and social relationships, has been shown to be associated with multiple physical and psychological health outcomes such as depression, cardiovascular disease, and mortality (Kawachi and Berkman, 2001; Kawachi et al., 1996). However, the biological mechanisms underlying behaviors related to being socially connected are not well understood. The neuropeptides oxytocin (OXT) and arginine vasopressin (AVP) are two of the most-studied brain signaling molecules encoding proteins relevant to social behavior. This OXT-AVP neural pathway has received the most attention to date in research considering the underlying biology of social behavior in humans given prior work on the function and plausible effects of these neuropeptides (Insel, 2010; Lee et al., 2009a; Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2011). OXT and AVP have been implicated in shaping social behaviors in mammals ranging from rodents to humans (Heinrichs et al., 2009), including attachment (Insel and Young, 2001), parent-infant bonding (Gordon et al., 2010), social recognition (Winslow and Insel, 2002, 2004), and aggression (Bosch et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2009b). This neuropeptide system also plays a role in pathological human behaviors that involve social deficits, such as autism (Hammock and Young, 2006).
Studies of intranasally-administered OXT and AVP [summarized in (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2011)] as well as correlation studies between peripheral levels of OXT and AVP and behaviors such as trust, physical contact with a partner, stressor response, and social memory (Grewen et al., 2005; Rimmele et al., 2009; Taylor et al., 2006; Zak et al., 2005) provide accumulating evidence that these neuropeptide molecules are involved in modulating a spectrum of social behaviors. Both OXT and AVP are sexually dimorphic and mediate action of sex hormones in the regulation of social cognition (Gabor et al., 2012). Because of the high degree of preservation of neuropeptide system across mammals and the heritability of social behaviors in humans (Maher et al., 2011; Scourfield et al., 1999), greater understanding of variation in genes that encode neuropeptides may shed light on the nature of social relationships individuals form. Research investigating the effect of genetic variation in the genes encoding OXT and AVP on social behaviors has not provided conclusive evidence, but additional evidence now implicates the genes that encode their receptors (Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2011). The oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene encodes a protein that belongs to the class I G protein-coupled receptor family. The neuroimaging study supported the implication of OXTR in hypothalamic-limbic circuits for emotional regulation and sociality (Tost et al., 2010). The AVP receptor 1A (AVPR1A) gene encodes the AVP receptor in the human brain that differs in structure with OXT by just two amino acids.
Recently, the transmembrane protein CD38 has also received attention as it is involved in oxytocinergic neural transmission (Jin et al., 2007). CD38−/− knockout mice had both decreased plasma OXT level and significant social impairments, including poorer maternal nurturing and less effective social behaviors (Jin et al., 2007). Research investigating association between CD38 genetic polymorphisms or expression levels in relation to autism also suggests a role for CD38 in regulating OXT release and contributing to disorders characterized by social deficits (Ebstein et al., 2011; Lerer et al., 2010; Munesue et al., 2010; Riebold et al., 2011).
Studies have provided some evidence for an association of genetic polymorphisms in OXTR (3p25), AVPR1A (12q14-q15), or CD38 (4p15) genes with behaviors related to forming social relationships, including social recognition and empathy in humans. However, most of the studies conducted to date used small samples, sometimes with fewer than 100 individuals. Studies have shown that behavioral traits are complex, and multiple genetic loci are involved in their variation, each with a small to moderate effect (McGuffin et al., 2001). Given that social integration is a complex trait, a study trying to assess its genetic contributions with small sample size is likely to be underpowered for detection of a true association. Furthermore, to our knowledge there is no study that has examined the effects of these genes specifically on social integration. However, using the most intensively studied SNP in the OXTR gene, rs53576 as an example, even when we reviewed the literature that examines the effects of this particular SNP on phenotypes broadly related to social integration, we identified relatively inconsistent findings (Table S1). To try to resolve some of the inconsistencies in prior work, and to consider effects more directly in relation to social integration, in the present study, we examined the associations between genetic polymorphisms in OXTR, AVPR1A, and CD38 genes, in relation to social integration measured across multiple time points in two large prospective cohorts of men and women. Given the important role of these genes in various social behaviors, we hypothesized that genetic polymorphisms in each of these genes would be associated with social integration. Because behaviors are relatively stable across time (Huesmann et al., 1984; Weisbuch et al., 2010), we used an average social integration score across repeated measures over time to reduce extraneous (i.e., non-genetic) variability in the phenotype and increase power to detect genetic determinants. In addition, we explore whether there is a sex-dependent mechanism impacting the association in the present study given the evidence of sex-dependent action of OXT and AVP (Carter, 1998; Gabor et al., 2012; Wu et al., 2012).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Study Sample
The study includes participants from 7 genome-wide association (GWA) case-control studies nested within the Nurses’ Health Study (NHS) and Health Professionals Follow-up Study (HPFS) cohorts. More description of the cohort has previously been published (Colditz and Hankinson, 2005) and specific details on how participants were selected into each of these GWA studies have also been published elsewhere (Cornelis et al., 2011).
Briefly, the NHS recruited 121,700 female US-registered nurses ages 30-55 years old residing in 11 large US states who completed and returned an initial self-administered questionnaire on their medical histories and baseline health-related exposures in 1976. Since then, biennial questionnaires with prospective collection of exposure information on risk factors and follow-up of health outcomes have been collected. The response completion rate is more than 90% in over 20 years of follow-up. The NHS participants included in the current study are from the blood subcohort that comprised 32,826 women in the NHS who gave blood specimens between 1989 and 1990.
The HPFS recruited 51,529 men from all 50 US states in health professions (dentists, pharmacists, optometrists, osteopath physicians, podiatrists, and veterinarians) aged 40-75, who at the start of the study answered a detailed questionnaire by mail. Similar to the NHS, health- and disease-related information was collected via biennial questionnaires in HPFS. The HPFS participants included in the current study are from the blood subcohort of 18,159 men who provided blood samples between 1993 and 1994.
The analytic sample for the analyses presented here was from a subsample (n = 6,992) of the Nurses’ Health Study and a subsample (n = 4,177) of the Health Professional Follow-up Study with genome-wide single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) information and at least one of the social integration phenotypes available.
Measure of social integration
Measures of social integration were developed using items from a commonly-used simplified Berkman-Syme Social Network Index (BSSNI) (Berkman and Syme, 1979). The items were assessed at multiple time points in both NHS and HPFS, 5 times from 1992 to 2008 at intervals of 4 years in the NHS and in 1988, 1996, and 2008 in the HPFS, and included four types of social connections: (1) marital status (married/having a domestic partnership, separated/divorced, widowed, or single) (2) contact with close friends (none, 1-2, 3-5, 6-9, 10+ friends), (3) regular attendance at religious services (never or almost never attend, twice a month to once a year, once/week, >once/week), and (4) participation in any community or volunteer groups or other organizations (such as church-connected groups, self-help group, charity, and public service) (none, 1-2, 3-5, 6-10, 11-15, 16+ hours/week). In the HPFS, the marital status was also assessed in additional questionnaire waves, i.e., examined every two years from 1986 to 2008.
In this study, the phenotype of social integration was measured in three ways: (1) being continuously married (yes/no), (2) being socially connected (dichotomized: yes/no), and (3) level of social connectedness (continuous). We used being continuously married to capture the ability to maintain a long-term stable intimate relationship. Being continuously married was derived from the marital status item of the BSSNI, and the dichotomized and continuous measures of social connectedness were derived from all four items of the BSSNI. For each item of the BSSNI, we carried forward the information reported in the prior questionnaire cycle if it was missing in the current cycle.
For the continuously married phenotype, an individual was scored as 1 if s/he was married, widowed, or living with a partner at all waves up until they died and 0 otherwise. The total number waves at which marital status was assessed were 5 for the NHS (marital status assessed every 4 years from 1992 to 2008) and 12 for the HPFS (marital status assessed every 2 years from 1986 to 2008). Participants were included in the analysis in they provided information on marital status in all waves during the follow-up period. To derive the dichotomized definition of social connectedness phenotype, at each time point each component was defined in the Table 1.
Table 1.
Item | Score of 1 if: | Score of 0 if: |
---|---|---|
Marital status | Married, living with a partner | Widowed, separated, divorced |
Religious service attendance | Regularly with varying frequency | Never |
Contact with close friends | At least one | None |
Group participation | At least one hour per week | No participation in any groups |
At each wave, individuals had to have at least 2 non-missing values out of four components to be scored. To avoid unfairly penalizing individuals missing scores in one or another category, we took an average across four social integration components, ranging from 0 (socially isolated) to 1 (socially connected). The dichotomized definition of social connectedness (summarizing over all time points) was scored 0 if subjects had an average score below or equal to 0.25 in any given wave (ever socially isolated) and 1 if subjects had an average score above 0.25 at all waves (socially connected). Among the NHS participants who had genotype data, 2.4% had missing information on the “continuously married” variable and 0.9% had missing information on the “socially connected” variable. Among the HPFS participants with genotype data, 0.7% and 11.9% had missing information on “continuously married” and “socially connected” variable, respectively. A small proportion of NHS participants (0.58%) were excluded from the analytic sample because they did not have any information on the phenotypes; these individuals were on average older and had higher BMI than the participants with information on some or all phenotypes regarding social integration. All participants in the HPFS gave at least some information on social integration.
Social connectedness was also assessed via a continuous scale to maintain the original outcome variation. Instead of assigning a binary value to each social integration component following prior work with this measure (Eng et al., 2002), at each time point we categorized each component according to the component score, as shown in the Table 2. For each wave, we averaged the category scores across the four components to create a continuous social connectedness score. The same carry-forward approach described above was applied for missingness. A final long-term cumulative social connectedness measure was derived by taking the average of the continuous social connectedness score across waves, ranging from 0 to 3.
Table 2.
Item | Score of 3 if: | Score of 2 if: | Score of 1 if: | Score of 0 if: |
---|---|---|---|---|
Marital status | Married, living with a partner | N/A | N/A | Widowed, separated, divorced |
Religious service attendance | > once/week | once/week | <once/week | Never |
Contact with close friends | 10+ | 3-9 | 1-2 | None |
Group participation | 11+ Hours/week | 3-10 Hours/week | 1-2 Hours/week | None |
Because a great deal of research has suggested that involvement in religious institutions provides people a sense of belongingness and general social identity, religious service attendance has frequently been included in social network measures in prior literature. However, people also attend religious services regularly for religious reasons. As a result a measure of religious service attendance may not accurately capture the aspects of social integration of interest in this study. To address this concern, we also conducted a secondary analysis excluding religious service attendance from the social integration phenotypes.
Selection and analysis of single nucleotide polymorphisms
Genotypes for the current analysis were derived from seven nested case-control genome-wide association substudies in the Nurses’ Health Study and Health Professional Follow-Up Study, initially designed to assess female breast cancer (BrCa), and coronary heart disease (CHD), kidney stone (KS), and type 2 diabetes (T2D) in men and women. 10,905 individuals with genotype data and information on social integration phenotypes were included in the study. Population structure was investigated by principal component analysis using the genome-wide data. Due to very few people with genetically inferred non-Caucasian ancestry, these individuals were excluded from our analysis. Genotyping and quality control have been detailed elsewhere (Cornelis et al., 2011). SNP imputation was performed using MACH software (v1.0.16) with HapMap CEU phased II data (Release 22) as the reference panel. Imputation probabilities rounded to most likely genotypes were used or analysis.
For the current targeted analysis, based on prior theory and empirical findings we selected 98 SNPs located in the chromosomal regions containing AVPR1A (8 SNPs), OXTR (29 SNPs), and CD38 (61 SNPs). 7 SNPs (1 in OXTR and 6 in CD38) with minor allele frequency less than 1% and 3 additional SNPs (1 in OXTR and 2 in CD38) with imputation quality of less than 0.5 in one or more of seven NHS/HPFS GWA substudies were further removed, leaving a set of 88 SNPs to be included in the final analysis. A number of studies have identified OXTR rs53576 as being associated with loneliness, empathy, and trust behavior (Krueger et al., 2012; Lucht et al., 2009; Rodrigues et al., 2009). Unfortunately this was only available (also directly genotyped) in two of our 7 NHS/HPFS substudies. Because it has been one of the most extensively assessed SNPs in the literature, we additionally examined rs53576 in the two substudies (N=1,043; 553 men and 490 women) which included this SNP in relation to social integration.
Statistical Analyses
Logistic regression models were used to estimate the effect magnitude [odds ratio (OR) and 95 % confidence interval (95% CI)] of genetic markers on dichotomous outcomes (i.e., being socially connected, or being continuously married). Linear regression models were used to estimate the effect of each polymorphism on the continuous social connectedness score. Initially, analyses were conducted in each of the NHS and HPFS substudies separately using PLINK toolset, adjusted for the disease status of each initial GWA substudy and the top three or four eigenvectors. Fixed-effect meta-analyses of the overall SNP effects were then used to combine results across 7 NHS/HPFS substudies using the GWAMA program. Gender-specific effects were also examined in GWAMA to assess potential heterogeneous SNP effects by gender. Associations were examined under the additive mode of inheritance. In the analysis with rs53576 only, we further considered both the dominant and recessive models because both models have been suggested in the literature.
To correct for multiple testing of single markers within each gene, the effective number of independent tests was determined using the method proposed by Nyholt (Nyholt, 2004) and modified by Li and Ji (Li and Ji, 2005), as implemented in the SNP Spectral Decomposition (SNPSpD) (http://gump.qimr.edu.au/general/daleN/SNPSpD/). Following this procedure, the Bonferroni-adjusted p-value was computed, so the overall type I error was maintained at 0.05 for each gene. The adjusted p-value was set to 1 if the value exceeded 1. We did not further adjust for number of phenotypes examined because the three phenotypes are nested in / associated with each other.
In addition to SNP analyses, gene-based analyses were also performed on all SNP-level results from the meta-analysis of all three phenotypes using VEGAS (Liu et al., 2010). The software applies a test that incorporates information from a set of markers within a gene and accounts for LD between markers by using simulations from the multivariate normal distribution. A Bonferroni-corrected threshold of p <0.017, which equaled to 0.05/3 (number of genes tested), was used to indicate a significant gene-based association.
RESULTS
Table 3 describes the characteristics of the sample. Of 11,169 participants with information on social connectedness over time or having a stable relationship, 63% were female. The participants had an average age over 55 years old when the first social integration measurement was administered. The majority of the participants were continuously married over the follow-up period. Women reported almost twice the prevalence of social isolation as men in our study. The Pearson correlation coefficient of continuous social connectedness score between the first and last assessment across 16 years in the NHS was 0.62; the corresponding correlation across 20 years in the HPFS was 0.72, suggesting that social integration was relatively stable over time.
Table 3.
NHS | HPFS | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean/Frequency | %/standard deviation (range) | Mean/Frequency | %/standard deviation (range) | |
Age at first social integration measurement (years) | 60.2 | 6.7 | 57.6 | 9.7 |
Continuously married | 6313 | 91.9 | 3655 | 88.1 |
Dichotomized social connectedness | 6200 | 88.9 | 3471 | 94.4 |
Continuous social connectedness score | 1.9 | 0.5 (0.1-3) | 1.8 | 0.5 (0-3) |
The number of individuals varies from 6,869 for being continuously married as an indicator of the ability to maintain a stable relationship to 6,973 for binary social connectedness in the NHS and varies from 3,678 for binary social connectedness to 4,147 for being continuously married in the HPFS.
Table 4 presents the top 5 SNP-phenotype associations per gene associated with each phenotype. Overall we found little evidence of significant association of AVPR1A, OXTR, and CD38 genetic polymorphisms with any of the social integration phenotypes measured in our study after accounting for multiple testing. The only exception was for rs12644506 in the intronic region of CD38 in relation to binary social connectedness, in which the T allele was associated with 24% lower odds of being socially connected (adjusted p = 0.02). To address concerns for the presence of allelic heterogeneity that may have reduced our power to observe significant associations at an individual SNP-level (Slager et al., 2000), all SNP-level results from the meta-analysis of the three phenotypes were additionally subject to gene-based analyses using VEGAS (Liu et al., 2010), but still no significant gene associations were observed (all p-values > 0.10). After excluding religious service attendance from the phenotype measures, only two SNPs were nominally associated, including rs1803404 in OXTR with the dichotomized form of social connectedness (nominal p = 0.02) and rs237897 in CD38 with continuous form of social connectedness (nominal p = 0.02). Neither of these SNPs remained statistically significant after correction for multiple testing (data not shown).
Table 4.
SNPs | Phenotypes |
|||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Being continuously married | Binary social connectedness | Continuous social connectedness score | ||||||||||||||||
AVPR1A | SNP | Effect allele | MAF | OR | p 2 | p 3 | SNP | Effect allele | MAF | OR | p 2 | p 3 | SNP | Effect allele | MAF | beta | p 2 | p 3 |
rs11174810 | T | 0.02 | 0.82 | 0.29 | 0.87 | rs1042615 | A | 0.42 | 1.04 | 0.44 | 1.00 | rs11174811 | A | 0.16 | 0.014 | 0.10 | 0.30 | |
rs3741865 | A | 0.02 | 0.82 | 0.29 | 0.87 | rs3021529 | A | 0.16 | 0.97 | 0.64 | 1.00 | rs10877968 | C | 0.18 | 0.008 | 0.32 | 0.96 | |
rs3021528 | C | 0.02 | 0.82 | 0.29 | 0.87 | rs10877968 | C | 0.18 | 0.98 | 0.72 | 1.00 | rs3021529 | A | 0.16 | 0.008 | 0.38 | 1.00 | |
rs11174811 | A | 0.16 | 1.07 | 0.30 | 0.90 | rs3803107 | A | 0.18 | 0.98 | 0.73 | 1.00 | rs3803107 | A | 0.18 | 0.007 | 0.39 | 1.00 | |
rs3021529 | A | 0.16 | 1.07 | 0.31 | 0.93 | rs11174811 | A | 0.16 | 0.99 | 0.90 | 1.00 | rs1042615 | A | 0.42 | 0.004 | 0.53 | 1.00 | |
CD38 | ||||||||||||||||||
rs2286553 | T | 0.04 | 0.80 | 0.05 | 0.51 | rs12644506 | T | 0.06 | 0.76 | 0.002 | 0.02 | rs6449195 | C | 0.10 | −0.023 | 0.02 | 0.20 | |
rs1112243 | T | 0.04 | 0.80 | 0.06 | 0.61 | rs6449195 | C | 0.10 | 0.81 | 0.005 | 0.05 | rs12644506 | T | 0.06 | −0.028 | 0.03 | 0.30 | |
rs10805347 | A | 0.31 | 1.10 | 0.06 | 0.61 | rs6836946 | G | 0.28 | 0.88 | 0.01 | 0.10 | rs6449197 | T | 0.11 | −0.019 | 0.06 | 0.61 | |
rs1112244 | C | 0.04 | 0.81 | 0.07 | 0.71 | rs6841880 | A | 0.28 | 0.88 | 0.01 | 0.10 | rs3796863 | T | 0.30 | −0.012 | 0.08 | 0.81 | |
rs1004124 | G | 0.04 | 0.81 | 0.07 | 0.71 | rs3796867 | A | 0.07 | 0.81 | 0.02 | 0.20 | rs6836946 | G | 0.28 | −0.010 | 0.14 | 1.00 | |
OXTR | ||||||||||||||||||
rs2268490 | T | 0.13 | 1.16 | 0.04 | 0.60 | rs237887 | G | 0.44 | 1.09 | 0.07 | 1.00 | rs237897 | A | 0.38 | −0.013 | 0.05 | 0.75 | |
rs2268491 | T | 0.12 | 1.16 | 0.05 | 0.75 | rs2268490 | T | 0.13 | 1.14 | 0.08 | 1.00 | rs237895 | T | 0.38 | −0.010 | 0.12 | 1.00 | |
rs2254295 | C | 0.12 | 1.16 | 0.05 | 0.75 | rs237885 | T | 0.48 | 1.09 | 0.09 | 1.00 | rs4564970 | C | 0.08 | 0.015 | 0.19 | 1.00 | |
rs2254298 | A | 0.12 | 1.15 | 0.06 | 0.91 | rs2268491 | T | 0.12 | 1.13 | 0.12 | 1.00 | rs2301261 | T | 0.08 | 0.015 | 0.19 | 1.00 | |
rs4564970 | C | 0.08 | 1.16 | 0.10 | 1.00 | rs2254295 | C | 0.12 | 1.12 | 0.14 | 1.00 | rs2268496 | A | 0.22 | 0.009 | 0.24 | 1.00 |
Significant Bonferroni-corrected p-value (adjusted p-value < 0.05) was bold
Unadjusted p-value
Bonferroni-corrected p-value
When considering a potential gender-specific effect, a missense mutation in the OXTR, rs4686302, showed a statistically significant heterogeneous effect by gender (adjusted gender-heterogeneity p = 0.02) (Table 5). The T allele was borderline statistically associated with dichotomized definition of more social connectedness in men after adjusting for multiple testing (OR = 1.72, 95% CI = 1.18-2.51, adjusted p = 0.08) but not in women. A gender-dependent association of rs4686302 with the other two phenotypes was not statistically significant, but the pattern was consistent with what emerged with the binary social connectedness phenotype (i.e., T allele being associated with higher social integration in men but not in women).
Table 5.
Phenotype | Gender heterogeneity p | Overall effect | Male-specific effect | Female-specific effect | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
p 2 | p 3 | OR/beta4 (95% CI) | p 2 | p 3 | OR/beta4 (95% CI) | p 2 | p 3 | OR/beta4 (95% CI) | p 2 | p 3 | |
Being continuous married | N=11015 | N=4146 | N=6869 | ||||||||
0.10 | 1.00 | 1.04 (0.90,1.20) | 0.60 | 1.00 | 1.19 (0.96, 1.49) | 0.12 | 1.00 | 0.94 (0.78, 1.13) | 0.51 | 1.00 | |
Binary social connectedness | N=10650 | N=3677 | N=6973 | ||||||||
0.001 | 0.02 | 0.96 (0.83, 1.11) | 0.57 | 1.00 | 1.72 (1.18, 2.51) | 0.005 | 0.08 | 0.87 (0.74, 1.01) | 0.07 | 1.00 | |
Continuous social connectedness score | N=10651 | N=3679 | N=6972 | ||||||||
0.11 | 1.00 | 0.01 (−0.01, 0.03) | 0.41 | 1.00 | 0.03 (−0.003, 0.06) | 0.07 | 1.00 | −0.003 (−0.03,0.02) | 0.80 | 1.00 |
Significant Bonferroni-corrected p-value (adjusted p-value < 0.05) was bold
Unadjusted p-value
Bonferroni-corrected p-value
For binary outcome, we calculated odds ratio to present the effect magnitude; for continuous outcome, beta was calculated.
In the subset of 1,043 participants with data available (553 men and 490 women), overall there were no statistically significant associations between OXTR rs53576 genotype and any of the three social integration measures. When considering a potential gender-dependent effect, the A allele was significantly associated with the binary social connectedness phenotype in women but not in men, assuming an additive mode of inheritance after correction of multiple testing (Table 6; adjusted p = 0.02 in women). The magnitude of the effect estimate was larger in models specified with a dominant mode of inheritance, in which AA carriers had 3.31-fold odds of reporting social connectedness compared with AG/GG carriers (adjusted p=0.003), with the adjusted p-value of the gender heterogeneity effect of 0.11. When considering the continuous social connectedness phenotype, the significance was diminished but the patterns were consistent. We did not observe rs53576 to be associated with being continuously married in either women or men in the study.
Table 6.
Phenotype/genetic model | Gender heterogeneity p | Overall effect | Male-specific effect | Female-specific effect | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
p 2 | p 3 | OR/beta4 (95% CI) | p 2 | p 3 | OR/beta4 (95% CI) | p 2 | p 3 | OR/beta4 (95% CI) | p 2 | p 3 | |
Being continuously married | N=970 | N=480 | N=490 | ||||||||
additive | 0.88 | 1.00 | 1.16 (0.87, 1.55) | 0.32 | 1.00 | 1.14 (0.78, 1.67) | 0.51 | 1.00 | 1.19 (0.76, 1.87) | 0.45 | 1.00 |
dominant | 0.99 | 1.00 | 1.22 (0.84, 1.76) | 0.3 | 1.00 | 1.22 (0.74, 2.01) | 0.43 | 1.00 | 1.21 (0.70, 2.11) | 0.49 | 1.00 |
recessive | 0.76 | 1.00 | 1.15 (0.60, 2.21) | 0.67 | 1.00 | 1.07 (0.47, 2.41) | 0.88 | 1.00 | 1.32 (0.44, 3.96) | 0.62 | 1.00 |
Binary social connectedness | N=970 | N=480 | N=490 | ||||||||
additive | 0.01 | 0.15 | 1.55 (1.04, 2.31) | 0.03 | 0.45 | 0.91 (0.51, 1.62) | 0.74 | 1.00 | 2.51 (1.45, 4.37) | 0.001 | 0.02 |
dominant | 0.007 | 0.11 | 1.93 (1.17, 3.16) | 0.01 | 0.15 | 0.81 (0.36, 1.80) | 0.61 | 1.00 | 3.31 (1.76, 6.22) | 0.0002 | 0.003 |
recessive | 0.7 | 1.00 | 1.28 (0.53, 3.07) | 0.59 | 1.00 | 1.07 (0.30, 3.76) | 0.92 | 1.00 | 1.51 (0.44, 5.13) | 0.51 | 1.00 |
Continuous social connectedness | N=1,035 | N=548 | N=487 | ||||||||
additive | 0.15 | 1.00 | 0.001 (−0.05, 0.05) | 0.97 | 1.00 | −0.03 (−0.10, 0.03) | 0.33 | 1.00 | 0.04 (−0.03, 0.10) | 0.29 | 1.00 |
dominant | 0.03 | 1.00 | 0.01 (−0.05, 0.07) | 0.72 | 1.00 | −0.06 (−0.15, 0.03) | 0.19 | 1.00 | 0.08 (−0.01, 0.16) | 0.08 | 1.00 |
recessive | 0.58 | 1.00 | −0.03 (−0.13, 0.08) | 0.61 | 1.00 | −0.0003 (−0.14, 0.14) | 1 | 1.00 | −0.06 (−0.22, 0.10) | 0.45 | 1.00 |
Significant Bonferroni-corrected p-value (adjusted p-value < 0.05) was bold
Unadjusted p-value
Bonferroni-corrected p-value
For binary outcome, we calculated odds ratio to present the effect magnitude; for continuous outcome, beta was calculated.
DISCUSSION
In this study we investigated the associations of genetic polymorphisms in OXTR, CD38, and AVPR1A genes with social integration using phenotypes characterized by the ability to maintain long-term stable relationships and being socially connected. From 88 SNPs tested, we observed only one significant association that met multiple-comparisons adjusted criteria for statistical significance between a CD38 intronic SNP rs12644506 and dichotomized definition of social connectedness. This particular SNP has not yet been reported to be associated with any disease or trait examined thus far in the literature. Given rs12644506 is an intronic SNP, the functional importance of this SNP remains to be determined. In addition, we observed a significant gender-specific association in one OXTR SNP with respect to a dichotomized definition of social connectedness; specifically, rs4686302 T allele was nominally associated with social connectedness in men, whereas the association was non-significant but in the opposite direction in women (adjusted gender heterogeneity p=0.02). Furthermore, rs53576 A allele was significantly associated with social connectedness only in women (adjusted p=0.02), and the effect magnitude was stronger in a dominant genetic model (adjusted p=0.003). OXTR, CD38, and AVPR1A were not associated with the phenotype of being continuously married in either main or gender-specific analyses. The gender difference was not evident in relation to the continuous scale of social connectedness, although the direction of gender-stratified associations was consistent to those in the dichotomized form. Because both dichotomized and continuous measures of social connectedness were expected to share some underlying genetics, the discrepant findings suggested that either the estimations were unstable or the associations were likely non-linear, so the power was decreased when modeling the association linearly.
Participating in community, voluntary, and religious organizations provides a sense of social connectedness, which has long been linked with health benefits (Kawachi and Berkman, 2001). Although attending religious meetings or services increases the likelihood of constructing and maintaining interactive ties in social networks, because people also attend religious services for religious rather than social reasons, including religious attendance into social integration measures may result in misclassifying non-believers and nontraditional worshippers into a less socially integrated category.. However, the results similarly suggest that social integration is not correlated with common genetic variations in OXTR, AVPR1A, and CD38 genes whether including or excluding religious attendance in the social integration phenotype measure.
Although this study is exploratory and false positives cannot be ruled out, the gender-dependent effect of OXTR observed merits further investigation. In fact, there has been evidence that oxytocin and vasopressin release and receptor bonding are expressed in a gender-dependent manner, in which the binding capacity was higher in women than in men (Elands et al., 1990). It has also been reported in previous studies that certain genetic polymorphisms in OXTR show a gender-specific effect on specific social behaviors. For example, Walum et al found that rs7632287 G/G genotype was related to partner bonding and affection in women but not in men (Walum et al., 2012), and Wu et al observed a significant rs4686302 genotype by gender interaction (Wu et al., 2012); however, it was not consistent in direction with our results, thus should not be considered as replication. Observed gender differences, if confirmed in future work, could be due partially to the fact that OXT synthesis and OXTR are exceptionally sensitive to gonadal steroids (Smeltzer et al., 2006). It remains to be elucidated in future studies how these OXTR genetic polymorphisms influence social connectedness via a gender-specific manner.
Oxytocin and arginine vasopressin have been shown to regulate a suite of behaviors which support the formation and maintenance of attachment bonds in animals (for review see (Kim et al., 2010b)). Prosocial effects of oxytocin in humans have also been reported (Heinrichs et al., 2009; Israel et al., 2009; Macdonald and Macdonald, 2010). However, genetic studies of a broad range of social behaviors with oxytocin pathway genes including OXTR, CD38, and APVR1A have had mixed results. Our study suggests that effects may be small and therefore difficult to detect. Here, we use the most studied SNP, rs53576, as an example for discussion. There has been intense research on this particular SNP in relation to various social and behavioral phenotypes in the past 3 years, although no prior study specifically examined social integration, the phenotype investigated in the present study. In reviewing the literature considering the association between rs53576 genotype and various phenotypes broadly used to characterize social integration, ranging from partner bonding to generosity (see supplement for summary of literature, Table S1), we observe that approximately half of the studies reported a positive association with rs53576A while the rest reported a null association. It is noteworthy that the two studies with the largest sample size failed to observe a significant association between rs53576A and prosocial behaviors, including pair bonding behaviors, trust, and generosity (Apicella et al., 2010; Walum et al., 2012). Several of the studies reporting a significant association between rs53576A and prosociality were conducted in small samples (for example, study (Kogan et al., 2011) had only 23 participants) or were investigated in a specific disease group (Park et al., 2010). Given the small sample or particular populations considered, positive findings could be due to chance, or may not be applicable to the general population. The phenotypes examined in these various studies and in our study are different but they do share some common features, leading to the hypothesis that the oxytocin might influence the indicators of social integrations we used here. Walum et al examined the effect of rs53576 on romantic relationship quality/partner bonding (Walum et al., 2012), the phenotype that is closest to the phenotype characterized by ability to maintain a stable romantic relationship examined in this study, and both studies reported null associations.
True associations may not be reproducible across different studies for a number of additional reasons, including insufficient statistical power or effect modification. Our sample is substantially larger than previous studies. The study has over 80% power to detect a genetic risk variant with an odds ratio (OR) of 1.25 for α = 0.05, if the risk allele frequency in the population is 0.1, assuming population risk of social isolation of 8%, the additive genetic model, and complete linkage disequilibrium between a genotyped and a causal genetic variant. However, although it is the largest study to date to examine the effects of genetic variation of OXTR, CD38, and AVPR1A genes and social integration, our study is likely underpowered to detect smaller effects. The potentially limited statistical power in prior work along with the fact that the first set of published studies often overestimate effect sizes due to the winner's curse (Zollner and Pritchard, 2007) could explain our failure to find an association observed in the present study. In addition, gene by environment interactions may have an important influence on social functioning (Bradley et al., 2011). Bradley et al did not identify overall associations of rs53576 with adult emotional dysregulation or attachment style phenotypes, but there was a significant interaction of the SNP with childhood maltreatment to predict social traits in adulthood. The findings may suggest that rs53576 A allele carriers are resilient against the effects of severe childhood adversity and protected against adult emotional dysregulation and disorganized attachment that often occurs among people who experience childhood adversity. These are phenotypes associated with pair-bonding relationships and marital quality. In our study, assessments of chronic stress or childhood adverse experiences were not available. If a substantial number of participants experienced severe chronic stress earlier in life, it may have been more difficult to detect an effect of oxytocin-pathway genes on social integration. Future studies are warranted to investigate whether effects of genetic variations in OXTR, CD38, and AVPR1A influence social integration depend on childhood socioemotional experience.
Several potential limitations should be noted. First, participants in the NHS and HPFS cohorts are all health care workers. If such individuals are more socially oriented towards other people than those in the general public, this might result in more limited variation in the phenotype, which could reduce study power to detect significant associations if any. In addition, individuals who are less religious may in fact have strong social bonds that will not be captured by a measure of religious service attendance. For example, Saslow et al. have shown that individuals who are less religious may be as or more likely as those who are more religious to be bound to others by strong emotional connections (Saslow et al., 2013b), and spirituality (with or without religiosity) may give rise to prosociality because it is strongly associated with compassion and altruism (Saslow et al., 2013a). Furthermore, religious service attendance may vary depending on culture and age. Given these possible sources of variation, the observed associations may not be generalizable. To address this potential concern, we compared the level of social integration in NHS/HPFS participants with that among participants in the US National Health Interview Survey (NHIS), a nationally representative sample of adult households. Although NHS and HPFS participants were older and had attained higher levels of education, the degree of social integration was relatively similar to that reported in the NHIS (Barger, 2013). Similar to participants in the NHS and HPFS, participants in the NHIS generally reported having medium to high social integration levels.
Secondly, existing genotype arrays and HapMap data may not have captured the true causal SNPs (if they exist) in the genetic pathways of interest; a limitation applied more broadly in current genome era. In addition, because the study included a sample with a homogenous ethnicity background, it is not capable to assess gene-by-ethnicity or gene-by-culture interaction, especially in the OXTR, as suggested previously (Chen et al., 2011; Kim et al., 2010a). Thirdly, the study was conducted in an older population. Genetic effects may diminish while environmental effects increase as people age, especially for complex traits. For example, Cornelis et al observed a marginally significant OXTR rs53576-age interaction for predicting optimism, where rs53576 had stronger effect in younger people than in older people (Cornelis et al., 2012).
Lastly, in this study, information on social relationships was updated every four years among the women and every two years among the men; therefore, if participants changed their behaviors or even marital status, for example, due to unexpected life events, we were unable to capture such changes occurring between questionnaire cycles. This could lead to non-differential misclassification of the measured outcome. However, because the social behaviors are relatively stable across time and remarriage occurs primarily among younger to middle aged adults (Kreider, 2006), we expect this issue did not have substantial impact. Furthermore, we used marital status as a surrogate to indicate one's ability to maintain a stable romantic relationship and connect to others. Initial hypotheses regarding social benefits of oxytocin revolved around its role in being able to develop social bonds and recognize social cues. While a role for oxytocin in developing high quality social relationships or greater intimacy within social relationships has also been posited, it has not been explored in detail empirically. Walum et al (Walum et al., 2012) recently reported that women carrying a particular SNP allele (rs7632287A) in OXTR have lower levels of partner bonding or affection toward their partners. Thus, it may be that marital quality, rather than marital status may more appropriately capture the fundamental pair bonding feature, and we might have seen stronger relationships with such a measure. However, we did not have a measure of marriage quality. Such misclassification is likely to be non-differential, potentially biasing our effects toward the null. However, this study also has a number of strengths, including phenotype assessments at multiple time points to ensure a more stable and accurate measurement, high-quality genetic data, and participants with a genetically inferred homogeneous ethnic background, which minimizes bias due to population stratification and admixture.
In summary, we investigated variation in 3 genes in the oxytocin pathway – OXTR, CD38, and AVPR1A – in relation to three measures of social integration in a large sample of men and women. We find little evidence of overall effects of any genes, with only 1 CD38 SNP showing a statistically significant result with one dichotomized form of social integration measure with requires replication. However, there was evidence of a gender-specific association of OXTR with social integration. Our findings are based on a sample many times larger than any prior study and suggest that prior work may have over stated the associations, if any, between oxytocin pathway genes and prosocial behaviors. However, the measure of social integration in this study is somewhat limited. If there is a better measure, it is possible that we would have seen something different. Furthermore, as behaviors such as social integration, like all complex traits, are influenced by multiple genes each with a small effect, it may be that there are other genes, gene-by-gene or gene-by-environment interactions that are more strongly involved in social behavior. Our findings and other work hint that the effects may be partially modified by gender or by other social experiences like chronic stress or aspects of childhood upbringing. Further work considering these genetic pathways in relation to social integration may be more fruitful if these additional factors can be more comprehensively assessed and evaluated.
Supplementary Material
ACKLOWLEDGEMENTS
The authors gratefully acknowledge funding from NIH/NIMH (MH092707-01).
Role of the funding source
The study is supported by NIH/NIMH (MH092707-01). The sponsor has no involvement in study design, data collection, analysis, interpretation of data, writing of the report, and decision to submit the paper for submission.
The corresponding author should confirm that she has full access to all the data in the study and has final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
CONTRIBUTORS
Author Shun-Chiao Chang undertook the statistical analysis and wrote the first draft of manuscript. Authors M Maria Glymour and Laura D. Kubzansky designed the study and wrote the protocol. Author Marissa Rewak managed the literature searches of OXTR rs53576 genotype - social relationship associations. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.
The authors do not have conflicts of interest.
REFERENCE
- Apicella CL, Cesarini D, Johannesson M, Dawes CT, Lichtenstein P, Wallace B, Beauchamp J, Westberg L. No association between oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene polymorphisms and experimentally elicited social preferences. PloS one. 2010;5:e11153. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barger SD. Social Integration, Social Support and Mortality in the US National Health Interview Survey. Psychosom Med. 2013 doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318292ad99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berkman LF, Syme SL. Social networks, host resistance, and mortality: a nine-year follow-up study of Alameda County residents. Am J Epidemiol. 1979;109:186–204. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a112674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bosch OJ, Meddle SL, Beiderbeck DI, Douglas AJ, Neumann ID. Brain oxytocin correlates with maternal aggression: link to anxiety. J Neurosci. 2005;25:6807–6815. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1342-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley B, Westen D, Mercer KB, Binder EB, Jovanovic T, Crain D, Wingo A, Heim C. Association between childhood maltreatment and adult emotional dysregulation in a low-income, urban, African American sample: moderation by oxytocin receptor gene. Development and Psychopathology. 2011;23:439–452. doi: 10.1017/S0954579411000162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter CS. Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1998;23:779–818. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(98)00055-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen FS, Barth ME, Johnson SL, Gotlib IH, Johnson SC. Oxytocin Receptor (OXTR) Polymorphisms and Attachment in Human Infants. Front Psychol. 2011;2:200. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colditz GA, Hankinson SE. The Nurses’ Health Study: lifestyle and health among women. Nat Rev Cancer. 2005;5:388–396. doi: 10.1038/nrc1608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cornelis MC, Glymour MM, Chang SC, Tchetgen EJ, Liang L, Koenen KC, Kang JH, Pasquale LR, Rimm EB, Kawachi I, Kubzansky LD. Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) is not associated with optimism in the Nurses’ Health Study. Molecular psychiatry. 2012;17:1157–1159. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cornelis MC, Monda KL, Yu K, Paynter N, Azzato EM, Bennett SN, Berndt SI, Boerwinkle E, Chanock S, Chatterjee N, Couper D, Curhan G, Heiss G, Hu FB, Hunter DJ, Jacobs K, Jensen MK, Kraft P, Landi MT, Nettleton JA, Purdue MP, Rajaraman P, Rimm EB, Rose LM, Rothman N, Silverman D, Stolzenberg-Solomon R, Subar A, Yeager M, Chasman DI, van Dam RM, Caporaso NE. Genome-wide meta-analysis identifies regions on 7p21 (AHR) and 15q24 (CYP1A2) as determinants of habitual caffeine consumption. PLoS genetics. 2011;7:e1002033. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebstein RP, Mankuta D, Yirmiya N, Malavasi F. Are retinoids potential therapeutic agents in disorders of social cognition including autism? FEBS Lett. 2011;585:1529–1536. doi: 10.1016/j.febslet.2011.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elands J, van Woudenberg A, Resink A, de Kloet ER. Vasopressin receptor capacity of human blood peripheral mononuclear cells is sex dependent. Brain Behav Immun. 1990;4:30–38. doi: 10.1016/0889-1591(90)90004-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eng PM, Rimm EB, Fitzmaurice G, Kawachi I. Social ties and change in social ties in relation to subsequent total and cause-specific mortality and coronary heart disease incidence in men. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;155:700–709. doi: 10.1093/aje/155.8.700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabor CS, Phan A, Clipperton-Allen AE, Kavaliers M, Choleris E. Interplay of oxytocin, vasopressin, and sex hormones in the regulation of social recognition. Behav Neurosci. 2012;126:97–109. doi: 10.1037/a0026464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon I, Zagoory-Sharon O, Leckman JF, Feldman R. Oxytocin and the development of parenting in humans. Biological psychiatry. 2010;68:377–382. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grewen KM, Girdler SS, Amico J, Light KC. Effects of partner support on resting oxytocin, cortisol, norepinephrine, and blood pressure before and after warm partner contact. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:531–538. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000170341.88395.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammock EA, Young LJ. Oxytocin, vasopressin and pair bonding: implications for autism. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2006;361:2187–2198. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinrichs M, von Dawans B, Domes G. Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2009;30:548–557. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huesmann LR, Eron LD, Lefkowitz MM, Walder LO. Stability of aggression over time and generations. Developmental Psychology. 1984;20:1120–1134. [Google Scholar]
- Insel TR. The challenge of translation in social neuroscience: a review of oxytocin, vasopressin, and affiliative behavior. Neuron. 2010;65:768–779. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel TR, Young LJ. The neurobiology of attachment. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2:129–136. doi: 10.1038/35053579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Israel S, Lerer E, Shalev I, Uzefovsky F, Riebold M, Laiba E, Bachner-Melman R, Maril A, Bornstein G, Knafo A, Ebstein RP. The oxytocin receptor (OXTR) contributes to prosocial fund allocations in the dictator game and the social value orientations task. PloS one. 2009;4:e5535. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jin D, Liu HX, Hirai H, Torashima T, Nagai T, Lopatina O, Shnayder NA, Yamada K, Noda M, Seike T, Fujita K, Takasawa S, Yokoyama S, Koizumi K, Shiraishi Y, Tanaka S, Hashii M, Yoshihara T, Higashida K, Islam MS, Yamada N, Hayashi K, Noguchi N, Kato I, Okamoto H, Matsushima A, Salmina A, Munesue T, Shimizu N, Mochida S, Asano M, Higashida H. CD38 is critical for social behaviour by regulating oxytocin secretion. Nature. 2007;446:41–45. doi: 10.1038/nature05526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawachi I, Berkman LF. Social ties and mental health. Journal of urban health : bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine. 2001;78:458–467. doi: 10.1093/jurban/78.3.458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawachi I, Colditz GA, Ascherio A, Rimm EB, Giovannucci E, Stampfer MJ, Willett WC. A prospective study of social networks in relation to total mortality and cardiovascular disease in men in the USA. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1996;50:245–251. doi: 10.1136/jech.50.3.245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim HS, Sherman DK, Sasaki JY, Xu J, Chu TQ, Ryu C, Suh EM, Graham K, Taylor SE. Culture, distress, and oxytocin receptor polymorphism (OXTR) interact to influence emotional support seeking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010a;107:15717–15721. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010830107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim K, Brenner CH, Mair VH, Lee KH, Kim JH, Gelegdorj E, Batbold N, Song YC, Yun HW, Chang EJ, Lkhagvasuren G, Bazarragchaa M, Park AJ, Lim I, Hong YP, Kim W, Chung SI, Kim DJ, Chung YH, Kim SS, Lee WB, Kim KY. A western Eurasian male is found in 2000-year-old elite Xiongnu cemetery in Northeast Mongolia. American journal of physical anthropology. 2010b;142:429–440. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.21242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kogan A, Saslow LR, Impett EA, Oveis C, Keltner D, Rodrigues Saturn S. Thin-slicing study of the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene and the evaluation and expression of the prosocial disposition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108:19189–19192. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112658108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreider RM. Remarriage in the United States.. Presented in the annual meeting of the American Sociological Association; Montreal. August 10-14, 2006.2006. [Google Scholar]
- Krueger F, Parasuraman R, Iyengar V, Thornburg M, Weel J, Lin M, Clarke E, McCabe K, Lipsky RH. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation promotes human trust behavior. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6:4. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee HJ, Macbeth AH, Pagani JH, Young WS., 3rd Oxytocin: the great facilitator of life. Prog Neurobiol. 2009a;88:127–151. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee R, Ferris C, Van de Kar LD, Coccaro EF. Cerebrospinal fluid oxytocin, life history of aggression, and personality disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009b;34:1567–1573. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lerer E, Levi S, Israel S, Yaari M, Nemanov L, Mankuta D, Nurit Y, Ebstein RP. Low CD38 expression in lymphoblastoid cells and haplotypes are both associated with autism in a family-based study. Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research. 2010;3:293–302. doi: 10.1002/aur.156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li J, Ji L. Adjusting multiple testing in multilocus analyses using the eigenvalues of a correlation matrix. Heredity. 2005;95:221–227. doi: 10.1038/sj.hdy.6800717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu JZ, McRae AF, Nyholt DR, Medland SE, Wray NR, Brown KM, Hayward NK, Montgomery GW, Visscher PM, Martin NG, Macgregor S. A versatile gene-based test for genome-wide association studies. American journal of human genetics. 2010;87:139–145. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucht MJ, Barnow S, Sonnenfeld C, Rosenberger A, Grabe HJ, Schroeder W, Volzke H, Freyberger HJ, Herrmann FH, Kroemer H, Rosskopf D. Associations between the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and affect, loneliness and intelligence in normal subjects. Progress in neuro-psychopharmacology & biological psychiatry. 2009;33:860–866. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macdonald K, Macdonald TM. The peptide that binds: a systematic review of oxytocin and its prosocial effects in humans. Harvard review of psychiatry. 2010;18:1–21. doi: 10.3109/10673220903523615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maher BS, Vladimirov VI, Latendresse SJ, Thiselton DL, McNamee R, Kang M, Bigdeli TB, Chen X, Riley BP, Hettema JM, Chilcoat H, Heidbreder C, Muglia P, Murrelle EL, Dick DM, Aliev F, Agrawal A, Edenberg HJ, Kramer J, Nurnberger J, Tischfield JA, Devlin B, Ferrell RE, Kirillova GP, Tarter RE, Kendler KS, Vanyukov MM. The AVPR1A gene and substance use disorders: association, replication, and functional evidence. Biological psychiatry. 2011;70:519–527. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuffin P, Riley B, Plomin R. Genomics and behavior. Toward behavioral genomics. Science. 2001;291:1232–1249. doi: 10.1126/science.1057264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer-Lindenberg A, Domes G, Kirsch P, Heinrichs M. Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12:524–538. doi: 10.1038/nrn3044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Munesue T, Yokoyama S, Nakamura K, Anitha A, Yamada K, Hayashi K, Asaka T, Liu HX, Jin D, Koizumi K, Islam MS, Huang JJ, Ma WJ, Kim UH, Kim SJ, Park K, Kim D, Kikuchi M, Ono Y, Nakatani H, Suda S, Miyachi T, Hirai H, Salmina A, Pichugina YA, Soumarokov AA, Takei N, Mori N, Tsujii M, Sugiyama T, Yagi K, Yamagishi M, Sasaki T, Yamasue H, Kato N, Hashimoto R, Taniike M, Hayashi Y, Hamada J, Suzuki S, Ooi A, Noda M, Kamiyama Y, Kido MA, Lopatina O, Hashii M, Amina S, Malavasi F, Huang EJ, Zhang J, Shimizu N, Yoshikawa T, Matsushima A, Minabe Y, Higashida H. Two genetic variants of CD38 in subjects with autism spectrum disorder and controls. Neuroscience research. 2010;67:181–191. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2010.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nyholt DR. A simple correction for multiple testing for single-nucleotide polymorphisms in linkage disequilibrium with each other. American journal of human genetics. 2004;74:765–769. doi: 10.1086/383251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park J, Willmott M, Vetuz G, Toye C, Kirley A, Hawi Z, Brookes KJ, Gill M, Kent L. Evidence that genetic variation in the oxytocin receptor (OXTR) gene influences social cognition in ADHD. Progress in neuro-psychopharmacology & biological psychiatry. 2010;34:697–702. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.03.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riebold M, Mankuta D, Lerer E, Israel S, Zhong S, Nemanov L, Monakhov MV, Levi S, Yirmiya N, Yaari M, Malavasi F, Ebstein RP. All-trans retinoic acid upregulates reduced CD38 transcription in lymphoblastoid cell lines from Autism spectrum disorder. Mol Med. 2011;17:799–806. doi: 10.2119/molmed.2011.00080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rimmele U, Hediger K, Heinrichs M, Klaver P. Oxytocin makes a face in memory familiar. J Neurosci. 2009;29:38–42. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4260-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues SM, Saslow LR, Garcia N, John OP, Keltner D. Oxytocin receptor genetic variation relates to empathy and stress reactivity in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:21437–21441. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909579106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saslow LR, John OP, Piff PK, Willer R, Wong E, Impett EA, Kogan A, Antonenko O, Clark K, Feinberg M, Keltner D, Saturn SR. The social significance of spirituality: New perspectives on the compassion–altruism relationship. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality. 2013a;5:201–218. [Google Scholar]
- Saslow LR, Willer R, Feinberg M, Piff PK, Clark K, Keltner D, Saturnm S R. My Brother's Keeper?: Compassion predicted generosity more among less religious individuals. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2013b;4:31–38. [Google Scholar]
- Scourfield J, Martin N, Lewis G, McGuffin P. Heritability of social cognitive skills in children and adolescents. Br J Psychiatry. 1999;175:559–564. doi: 10.1192/bjp.175.6.559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slager SL, Huang J, Vieland VJ. Effect of allelic heterogeneity on the power of the transmission disequilibrium test. Genet Epidemiol. 2000;18:143–156. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2272(200002)18:2<143::AID-GEPI4>3.0.CO;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smeltzer MD, Curtis JT, Aragona BJ, Wang Z. Dopamine, oxytocin, and vasopressin receptor binding in the medial prefrontal cortex of monogamous and promiscuous voles. Neuroscience letters. 2006;394:146–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SE, Gonzaga GC, Klein LC, Hu P, Greendale GA, Seeman TE. Relation of oxytocin to psychological stress responses and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis activity in older women. Psychosom Med. 2006;68:238–245. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000203242.95990.74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tost H, Kolachana B, Hakimi S, Lemaitre H, Verchinski BA, Mattay VS, Weinberger DR, Meyer-Lindenberg A. A common allele in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) impacts prosocial temperament and human hypothalamic-limbic structure and function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107:13936–13941. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1003296107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walum H, Lichtenstein P, Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Ganiban JM, Spotts EL, Pedersen NL, Anckarsater H, Larsson H, Westberg L. Variation in the oxytocin receptor gene is associated with pair-bonding and social behavior. Biological psychiatry. 2012;71:419–426. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weisbuch M, Slepian ML, Clarke A, Ambady N, Veenstra-Vanderweele J. Behavioral Stability Across Time and Situations: Nonverbal Versus Verbal Consistency. J Nonverbal Behav. 2010;34:43. doi: 10.1007/s10919-009-0079-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winslow JT, Insel TR. The social deficits of the oxytocin knockout mouse. Neuropeptides. 2002;36:221–229. doi: 10.1054/npep.2002.0909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winslow JT, Insel TR. Neuroendocrine basis of social recognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14:248–253. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu N, Li Z, Su Y. The association between oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism (OXTR) and trait empathy. J Affect Disord. 2012;138:468–472. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zak PJ, Kurzban R, Matzner WT. Oxytocin is associated with human trustworthiness. Horm Behav. 2005;48:522–527. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zollner S, Pritchard JK. Overcoming the winner's curse: estimating penetrance parameters from case-control data. American journal of human genetics. 2007;80:605–615. doi: 10.1086/512821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.