Significance
The hypothalamic orexin (hypocretin) system controls survival-related processes such as food intake, arousal, and stress. Here we show that orexins also play an important role in learning about stimuli that predict harm. We demonstrate that blocking orexin activity in the noradrenergic locus coeruelus (LC) reduces, whereas increasing its activity enhances, threat learning in a Pavlovian auditory threat conditioning paradigm. Moreover, we demonstrate a direct functional connection between orexin enhancement of LC activity and amygdala-dependent memory processes. Strong, aversive memories can lead to fear and anxiety disorders that have a negative impact on individuals and their quality of life. The orexin system may represent a unique treatment target for these disorders.
Keywords: norepinephrine, fear conditioning, channelrhodopsin-2
Abstract
Survival in a dangerous environment requires learning about stimuli that predict harm. Although recent work has focused on the amygdala as the locus of aversive memory formation, the hypothalamus has long been implicated in emotional regulation, and the hypothalamic neuropeptide orexin (hypocretin) is involved in anxiety states and arousal. Nevertheless, little is known about the role of orexin in aversive memory formation. Using a combination of behavioral pharmacology, slice physiology, and optogenetic techniques, we show that orexin acts upstream of the amygdala via the noradrenergic locus coeruleus to enable threat (fear) learning, specifically during the aversive event. Our results are consistent with clinical studies linking orexin levels to aversive learning and anxiety in humans and dysregulation of the orexin system may contribute to the etiology of fear and anxiety disorders.
Hess and Akert demonstrated that electrical stimulation of the perifornical (PFH) region of the hypothalamus elicits defensive or aggressive responses in cats (1). Others showed that hypothalamic stimulation can serve as the aversive unconditioned stimulus (US) (2), indicating that the hypothalamus processes threat information important for aversive learning. One possibility is that orexin neurons, which populate these hypothalamic areas, may mediate these observed responses, as these neurons project to and modulate brain areas critical for threat processing, reward, and memory.
Orexins are neuropeptides produced in the PFH and lateral regions of the hypothalamus (LH) (3, 4). Two orexin peptides (Orexin-A and Orexin-B) are processed from one peptide precursor (prepro-orexin) and bind two distinct G protein–coupled receptors (OrxR1 and OrxR2) in the brain (3, 4). Activation of either receptor commonly increases excitability in target neurons by reducing potassium channel conductance, enhancing presynaptic glutamate release, or increasing postsynaptic NMDA receptor (NMDAR) conductance (5, 6). Orexin receptors are differentially distributed in the brain and may serve differing roles in stress, arousal, vigilance, feeding, reward processing, and drug addiction (7–10). Evidence suggests that, in general, OrxR2 is involved in maintenance of arousal or wakefulness (11, 12), whereas OrxR1 mediates responses to environmental stimuli (13, 14).
Recent reports point to a role for the orexin system in emotional regulation. Overactivity in orexin neurons can exacerbate panic-like episodes and lead to an anxiety-like phenotype in rats (15, 16). Conversely, administration of the dual orexin receptor antagonist almorexant blunts autonomic and behavioral responses affiliated with heightened stress levels (17, 18). Although orexin system activity is linked to general states of hyperarousal, the precise role of orexin in these and other aversive states remains unknown.
Hypothalamic orexin neurons send a dense output to the locus coeruelus (LC) and depolarize neurons in vitro and in vivo (19–21). In line with their connectivity, LC neurons respond to phasic stimuli in a manner comparable to orexin neurons (22), suggesting that orexin neurons modulate LC responses to salient sensory events. Interestingly, orexin and LC neurons are both activated by aversive stimuli such as shock (23, 24). Thus, orexin could contribute to aversive learning by way of LC, given the importance of norepinephrine to aversive memory processes in amygdala (25–27).
Pavlovian threat (fear) conditioning is a well-established behavioral paradigm to assess the formation, storage, and expression of aversive memories (28). During training, animals learn to associate an aversive US, such as a footshock, with a neutral conditioned stimulus (CS), such as a tone, when both occur in close temporal proximity. Here, we tested the hypothesis that orexin neurons phasically activate locus coeruleus neurons during an aversive event to enable threat learning. Using a combination of behavioral pharmacology, electrophysiology, and optogenetic approaches, we show that orexin neurons, via activation of OrxR1 in the LC, facilitate the acquisition of amygdala-dependent threat memory.
Results
Regulation of Threat Learning by the Orexin System.
To test the hypothesis that orexin carries information important for aversive memory formation, we blocked OrxR1 activity at various phases of an auditory threat conditioning paradigm. We administered the OrxR1 antagonist SB 334867 by intracerebroventricular (ICV) infusion before threat conditioning and examined conditioned freezing behavior, a general reaction to perceived threat (29), in a long-term memory (LTM) test (Fig. 1A). When SB 334867 infusions were made before training, freezing was impaired during LTM [Fig. 1B, Left, n = 7–16/group, 5 μg/side, 64.3 ± 6% vs. 39.1 ± 4% freezing, Student t Test, t(20) = 3.429, P = 0.003], demonstrating that OrxR1 activation is required for normal formation of threat memories. To rule out potential drug effects during the consolidation phase, because the drug was administered before training, we infused SB 334867 immediately following training and found no effect on LTM freezing behavior [Fig. 1B, Center, n = 5–7/group, 5 μg/side, 66 ± 6% vs. 64 ± 6% freezing, Student t test, t(10) = 0.199, P = 0.85]. Similarly, SB 334867 infusion before the LTM test did not significantly influence expression of conditioned freezing [Fig. 1B, Right, n = 8/group, 5 μg/side, 55 ± 3% vs. 56.7 ± 5% freezing, Student t test, t(14) = 0.238, P = 0.82]. These data show that OrxR1 activation, specifically during the training phase, is required for normal threat memory formation.
We hypothesized that OrxR1 activation is responsible for the effect of SB 334867 on acquisition of threat memory but could not rule out a nonselective effect of the drug on OrxR2 receptors. To test whether OrxR2 contributes to aversive learning, animals were infused with the selective OrxR2 antagonist TCS-OX2-29 at an effective dose before training (30). We found no effect on learning, suggesting that OrxR2 signaling is not involved in the acquisition or consolidation of threat-conditioned memories [Fig. 1C; n = 8/group, 5 μg/side, 64 ± 6% vs. 63 ± 5%, Student t test, t(14) = 0.131, P = 0.89]. Taken together, these data suggest that central signaling by orexin via OrxR1 is critical for the learning but not for the consolidation or expression of aversive memories.
Orexin Signaling in the LC Mediates Threat Learning.
Behavioral pharmacology experiments revealed no direct role for OrxR1 signaling in lateral nucleus of the amygdala (LA) in the acquisition of threat memories [n = 8/group, 1 μg/side, 69.5 ± 5% vs. 69.2 ± 5%, Student t test, t(14) = 0.038, P = 0.970], consistent with findings showing sparse orexin projections to the LA and minimal receptor expression (10, 31). Because orexin projections and OrxR1s are dense in the LC, orexin positively modulates LC neurons (19, 20), and the LC, is a major source of the catecholamine norepinephrine, which is strongly implicated in aversive memory formation (25, 26, 32), we hypothesized that local blockade of OrxR1 in LC exclusively during the training phase would impair threat memory formation. Consistent with this prediction, pharmacological blockade of OrxR1 signaling in LC before conditioning [Fig. 2B; n = 5–10/group, 300 ng or 1 μg/side; vehicle: 73.1 ± 4% vs. 300 ng: 73.2 ± 4% vs. 1 μg: 47.9 ± 3% freezing, one-way ANOVA, F(2,23) = 19.79, Tukey’s multiple comparison test, P < 0.001], but not immediately after conditioning [Fig. 2C; n = 5/group, 1 μg/side, 72 ± 6% vs. 79 ± 5%, t(7) = 0.883, P = 0.41], reduced threat memory formation, suggesting that OrxR1 signaling in LC is required during the training phase. Importantly, these results were not due to shock sensitivity (Fig. S1), and off-site infusions of SB 334867 did not produce this behavioral effect [Fig. S2A; n = 14/group, 63 ± 5% vs. 64 ± 5% freezing, Student t test, t(26) = 0.129, P = 0.89], thus confirming the behavioral specificity, site specificity, and, to an extent, the dose used in the observed results. To further corroborate these findings, we next assessed short-term-memory (STM) before consolidation (33). If OrxR1 signaling is required for acquisition, pretraining blockade of OrxR1 should influence STM in addition to LTM. Indeed, we found that SB 334867 infusion before training reduced STM freezing [Fig. 2D; n = 14–20/group, 1 μg/side, 81 ± 3% vs. 61 ± 3% freezing, Student t test, t(32) = 4.786, P < 0.001], further supporting the hypothesis that OrxR1 signaling is important during the learning phase of aversive memory formation.
Optogenetic Activation of Orexin Fibers Causes Rapid, Direct Excitation of Cells in the LC.
Studies have described a direct functional connection between orexin neurons and the LC, although the behavioral consequences of orexin neuron manipulations have not been explored until recently. We selectively expressed channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) (34) in orexin neurons and measured the effects of locally activating orexin terminals in the LC. We targeted the medial and PFH orexin fields due to evidence that these populations may be involved in aversive processing (13, 35), observing an infection efficiency of 45.06 ± 3.82% (n = 12 animals). After confirming viral expression in PFH orexin neurons (Fig. 3B and Fig. S3A), we ensured that we could obtain consistent photo-activation of ChR2-expressing orexin neurons by recording light-evoked action potentials from these cells in acute coronal brain slices. ChR2-expressing cells were clearly visible under wide-field fluorescence in acute slices (Fig. S3A). Optical activation of orexin cells elicited consistent and robust spiking in ChR2-expressing neurons (three of three cells; Fig. S3B), whereas no response was observed in any of the mCherry-expressing cells (n = 4), even at maximal light power.
As expected, we also observed fibers extending from infected orexin neurons to the LC (Fig. 3C). We used photo-activation of these ChR2-expressing axon terminals to elicit and characterize synaptic transmission from these fibers onto projection cells of the LC. We obtained whole-cell current-clamp recordings from LC cell somata while optically stimulating the ChR2-infected axon terminals using a truncated protocol similar to that used in vivo (20-Hz train, 10-ms pulse duration, 10 pulses total) (36). Using optical stimulation alone, we were able to observe robust fast synaptic responses in a number of LC cells accompanied by a more long-lasting depolarization (Fig. 3E).
The fast component of the light-evoked synaptic response was measured by the maximal amplitude of the first excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP; SI Materials and Methods), and the slow component was measured by the area under the synaptic depolarization (Fig. 3F). Application of blockers of AMPA receptors and orexin receptors significantly decreased both the fast (one-way ANOVA, F = 9.13, P < 0.001) and slow (one-way ANOVA, F = 8.84, P < 0.001) components of depolarization (n = 4 cells from four animals). As expected based on previous work (37), a large component (79 ± 4%) of the fast EPSP was blocked after 15 min of incubation in 6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (CNQX) (10 μM) and therefore mediated by AMPA-type glutamate receptors (two-tailed paired comparison, P = 0.004). The area under the light-induced depolarization was also significantly reduced by CNQX (77 ± 5%), indicating significant summation of AMPA receptor-mediated EPSPs (two-tailed paired comparison, P = 0.004). Surprisingly, in these same cells, when the OrxR1 antagonist SB 334867 (10 μM) was bath-applied in conjunction with CNQX for 10 min, the remaining component (93 ± 4%) of the fast synaptic depolarization was largely abolished (two-tailed paired comparison with CNQX, P = 0.045). The addition of SB 334867 with CNQX completely abolished the long-lasting depolarization (99 ± 3%, two-tailed paired comparison with CNQX, P = 0.02), consistent with a prolonged, volume transmission-type effect of optically induced orexin release. These results demonstrate that orexin expressing cells of the PFH corelease both orexin and glutamate to mediate direct, rapid, and robust depolarization of LC cells.
In a separate set of experiments, we examined the effect of SB 334867 alone on similar light-evoked responses (Fig. S4). We observed that OrxR1 blockade in the absence of CNQX did not significantly change EPSP amplitude, although we observed a negative trend (Fig. S4A; EPSP amplitude = 75 ± 16% of baseline, P = 0.11, two-tailed paired comparison). We did not observe any trend when measuring the effect of SB alone on the area under the cumulative synaptic response (Fig. S4B; area = 103 ± 16% of baseline, P = 0.85, two-tailed paired comparison). The lack of effect due to SB alone could be explained by the variability inherent in the glutamatergic responses riding on the oscillating membrane potentials of LC neurons. To explore this possibility, we calculated the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) of the predicted orexin contribution to the synaptic responses. As anticipated, the SNR of the orexin-mediated depolarization was near 1:1 in the presence of intact glutamatergic transmission (Fig. S4C). In the presence of CNQX, however, SNR increased to 1.94 ± 0.5 for the EPSP amplitude and 3.88 ± 1.2 for the area under the response. Thus, although the synaptic signature of orexin alone is small relative to the AMPA receptor–mediated response, the modest depolarizing bias provided by orexin could increase LC cell firing in response to synaptic inputs. The robust depolarization of LC cells provided by combined activation of glutamate and orexin receptors could significantly enhance learning in a threat conditioning paradigm.
Optogenetic Activation of Orexin Fibers in LC During Training Enhances Threat Learning.
We next assessed the effect of optically activating ChR2-expressing orexin fibers in vivo. First, we photoactivated orexin fibers in LC and used c-Fos immunohistochemistry as a marker for neuronal activity (Fig. 4 A and B). We targeted the medial and PFH orexin fields due to evidence that these populations may be involved in aversive processing (13, 35), observing an infection efficiency of 45 ± 4% (n = 12 animals). Increased c-Fos expression was detected in dopamine beta hydroxylase (DBH)-immunopositive neurons of ChR2-mCherry–expressing animals relative to mCherry-alone controls [Fig. 4C; n = 6/group, two-way ANOVA, interaction: F(1,20) = 3.82, P = 0.06, main effect of virus: F(1,20) = 5.22, P = 0.03; main effect of side: F(1,20) = 12.75, P = 0.002; Bonferonni posttest, P < 0.05 for ipsilateral side between ChR2-mCherry and mCherry animals]. These results, in combination with the slice electrophysiology data, demonstrate that optical stimulation of orexinergic axons evoke robust LC activation, allowing us to temporally regulate transmitter release from orexin terminals during threat conditioning trials.
To determine whether orexin fiber stimulation facilitates memory, we used a weak threat conditioning paradigm that produces lower levels of freezing than normal conditioning procedures (36). In the experimental group, brief optical stimulation of orexin expressing axons in LC co-occurred with tone-shock presentation at the end of each trial (20-Hz train, 10-ms pulse duration for 2 s; Fig. 4 D and E). Optically stimulated ChR2-expressing animals froze significantly more than both the animals expressing mCherry alone and no-light controls during LTM (78 ± 5% vs. 51 ± 3% vs. 55 ± 5%, respectively), suggesting that orexin fiber stimulation served to enhance threat memory formation [Fig. 4D; n = 5–7/group, one-way ANOVA, F(2,17) = 8.79, P = 0.003]. We observed no significant difference between the two control groups (ChR2 without light and mCherry with light) but a significant difference between controls and the experimental group (Tukey’s multiple comparison test, mCherry-light vs. ChR2-light and ChR2-no light vs. ChR2; both P < 0.01). Therefore, only the combination of ChR2 and photo-activation lead to enhanced freezing during LTM. To confirm that orexin signaling through OrxR1 is responsible for our observations, ChR2-expressing animals were infused with either SB 334867 or vehicle in LC before training with blue light stimulation (Fig. 4E). OrxR1 antagonism significantly decreased the effects of optical stimulation [n = 7/group, 1 μg ipsilateral infusion, 76 ± 4% vs., 55 ± 7% freezing, Student t test, t(12) = 2.771, P = 0.02], suggesting that our behavioral results are due in large part to evoked orexin release in the LC.
Functional Disconnection of a Hypothalamic-LC-LA Circuit Impairs Threat Learning.
To test whether the orexin-to-LC circuit influences aversive learning via downstream effects on the amygdala, we used a strategy whereby the hypothalamus-LC-LA circuit is pharmacologically disconnected at different nodes of the circuit during behavioral training (Fig. 5 A and B). In these experiments, activity in LC was unilaterally antagonized with SB 334867, whereas norepinephrine signaling was blocked unilaterally in LA with the β-adrenergic receptor (βAR) antagonist propranolol at an effective dose (25, 26). LC projections to LA are ipsilateral (38), and therefore unilateral LC manipulations should not affect the contralateral LA. Indeed, simultaneous ipsilateral drug infusions in LC and LA had no effect on LTM (Fig. 5 A and C; one-way ANOVA and Tukey’s multiple comparison test, vehicle vs. ipsilateral drug treatment, not significant), indicating that an intact unilateral LC-LA projection is sufficient for normal threat learning.
In contrast, contralateral drug-treated animals in which the circuit is disrupted in both hemispheres (at the level of LC on one side and LA on the other) displayed a significant decrease in LTM freezing compared with ipsilateral and contralateral vehicle-treated animals and ipsilateral drug-infused animals [Fig. 5 B and C; one-way ANOVA, F(2,27) = 6.24, P = 0.006, Tukey’s multiple comparison test, contralateral drug treatment vs. vehicle, P < 0.05, contralateral drug treatment vs. ipsilateral drug treatment, P < 0.05]. These data indicate that that a functional hypothalamus (orexin)-LC-LA circuit is necessary for normal threat memory formation and that the orexin-to-LC projection enhances aversive learning by evoking norepinephrine release in the LA.
Discussion
Here we uncover a serial circuit between the hypothalamus, LC, and the amygdala and demonstrate that orexin activity in the LC acts as a key signal for emotional memory formation. Our data show that orexin fibers originating in the perifornical region of the hypothalamus directly depolarize LC neurons through rapid corelease of glutamate and orexin and that the orexin component, likely via activation of OrxR1, modulates downstream circuit elements to enhance threat conditioning. We also demonstrate that orexin activity in the LC can drive norepinephrine signaling through βARs in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala to enhance threat memory formation.
Our finding of a critical role for orexin action in LC during threat conditioning complements previous reports showing that orexin neurons are activated in response to salient or arousing stimuli (38–40). Recent work using brief optical stimulation of orexin neurons provides functional evidence that orexin mediates sleep-to-wake transitions by activating LC neurons and that this modulation occurs through OrxR1 (40, 41). However, what does this mean in terms of the current results? In light of the work presented here, the orexin-to-LC circuit may represent a saliency signal that, in a natural context, arouses the animal during exposure to threatening or painful stimuli and primes neural circuits underlying threat conditioning. By increasing LC activity, OrxR1 signaling can increase norepinephrine release and induce activation of βARs in the amygdala, thereby enhancing plasticity in lateral amygdala neurons (27).
In slices, we demonstrate that optical stimulation of orexin fibers directly triggers both an AMPA receptor–mediated glutamatergic response, which is consistent with other findings (37), and an OrxR1-dependent depolarization in LC cells (5, 20, 21). Although synaptic glutamate release is likely critical for hypothalamus–LC communication, we show that OrxR1 activation is likely to play a key role by enhancing synaptic depolarization in LC cells.
Many neuromodulatory cells corelease both glutamate and a modulator. The modulatory component is often more subtle and prolonged, as is the case here, and is consistent with a volume transmission-type mechanism (42). The subtle, OrxR1-mediated depolarization that we observe in vitro could enhance synaptic summation and firing in LC cells in vivo, thereby increasing norepinephrine release in the LA. Consistent with this idea, previous work has shown that OrxA infusion into the LC triggers norepinephrine release and synaptic enhancement in the hippocampal dentate gyrus in vivo (43). A similar mechanism in the amygdala may be responsible for our behavioral results.
It is important to consider if orexin release in LC would, in the absence of a US, be sufficient to enhance memory formation. Threat conditioning is thought to occur by way of Hebbian processes in the amygdala, whereby the US strongly depolarizes LA neurons and alters synaptic transmission at coactive CS input synapses in the LA (28). Therefore, it is unlikely that BAR activation in the absence of an aversive stimulus would be sufficient for plasticity and learning.
Our results suggest that OrxR1 activity in LC during the learning episode is required for threat memory acquisition, although we cannot exclude the possibility that activation of OrxR1 also engages signaling cascades that may be required for later consolidation processes as has been suggested recently (44). A previous study demonstrated that βAR activation in LA enhances threat conditioning in a way that mirrors the effect of OrxR1 activity in LC; i.e., receptor activation is required during, but not following the learning episode (25). Together, these findings support a critical role for the hypothalamus:LC:LA circuit during the learning episode.
Our findings are consistent with studies in human orexin-compromised narcoleptics, who are impaired in acquiring a conditioned threat response and show reduced amygdala activity relative to controls when exposed to aversively conditioned stimuli (45, 46). This impairment is effectively the opposite of what is observed in disorders such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), which involves states of hyperarousal, a hyperactive amygdala, and the inability to control fear responses (47, 48). Indeed, cerbrospinal fluid (CSF) orexin levels are altered in patients with PTSD (49), as are norepinephrine levels (50), and a recent study in humans also showed that orexin-A levels in the amygdala positively correlate with emotional state (51). Dysregulation of the orexin system might explain susceptibility to PTSD or occur as a result of the disease, and thus might provide a specific target for treatment in these individuals. Orexin receptor antagonists have been used in clinical trials for other purposes (52), but OrxR1 antagonism may provide relief for chronic stress and related disorders.
In sum, the current work describes a unique circuit mechanism linking stress, arousal and threat learning mediated by the orexin system. Dysregulation of the orexin system or downstream noradrenergic mechanisms could serve as both a diagnostic measure and a treatment target for fear and anxiety disorders in susceptible individuals.
Materials and Methods
Detailed methods can be found in SI Materials and Methods.
Subjects.
Adult male Sprague–Dawley rats were used. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Experimental Animals and were approved by the New York University Animal Care and Use Committee.
Stereotaxic Surgery.
Procedures for stereotaxic surgery have been previously described (25). For amygdala experiments, cannulae were aimed at the LA; for ICV experiments, rats were implanted with single guide cannulae aimed at the right lateral ventricle; for LC experiments, double guide cannulae were aimed at LC; and for disconnection experiments, rats were implanted with one guide targeting the LC and the other targeting LA.
For optogenetic experiments, virus was unilaterally injected into the PFH. After 2–4 wk, animals were handled and subjected to behavioral conditioning or euthanized for slice physiology as described below.
Drug Preparation, Microinfusion, and Behavior.
SB 334867 (Tocris) was prepared for three dose groups: 300 ng, 1.0 μg, or 5 μg/0.3 μL/side (5 μg/5 μL for ICV). TCS OX2 29 (Tocris) was used at 5 μg/5 μL ICV. For disconnection experiments propranolol (±propranolol hydrochloride; Sigma-Aldrich) was administered at 1 μg/0.3 μL. Rats were infused at 0.1 or 2.5 μL/min for ICV infusions and were allowed to move freely in their home cage during infusions. Afterward, cannulae were left in place for an additional 1–2 min to allow drug diffusion away from the cannula tip. For all experiments, animals were infused only once (no within-subject experiments were performed). Animals were handled 1 d before training to minimize the stress of infusion. Drug infusions occurred 15–20 min before training, immediately after training (for testing consolidation), or 15–20 min before expression test. For disconnection experiments, propranolol was infused into LA simultaneously with SB 334867 infusion in LC. Vehicle-infused controls were included for both contralateral and ipsilateral animals and were combined for analysis due to no significant difference between groups (P = 0.77). Freezing data were scored offline and analyzed using GraphPad Prism (GraphPad Software).
Channelrhodopsin Experiments.
Procedures are as described previously (36). Orexin neuron-specific lentivirus constructs pLV-Hcrt::ChR2-mCherry and pLV-Hcrt:: mCherry have been described (34), and were produced by the University of North Carolina Gene Therapy Center (Vector Core Services). Before training, a fiber optic cable was inserted ∼0.5 mm above the dorsal tip of the LC. A three CS–US pairing protocol was used where each CS consisted of a series of auditory pips and the US was blue light laser stimulation combined with a weak, ∼0.5-mA footshock that coterminated with the last 2 s of the CS. A weak training protocol was used to obtain lower baseline freezing levels (∼50%) and thus avoid ceiling effects on freezing levels. In another set of ChR2-expressing animals, pretraining infusion of SB 334867 (1 μg/0.3 μg) or vehicle preceded conditioning by 20 min. Twenty-four hours following training, animals were placed in a novel context (same LTM context as all other experiments), and following a 3-min acclimation period, animals were presented with five CSs at a random intertrial interval no longer than 5 min.
For c-Fos detection, animals were handled for 3 d before light stimulation to reduce baseline c-Fos levels. Ninety minutes following stimulation, animals were perfused for histological processing.
Histology and Immunohistochemistry.
Histology for cannula targeting was performed as described previously (25). To verify targeting area and cell specificity of viral expression, neuronal activity in the LC immunohistochemistry was performed to detect mCherry (1:500; Clontech Laboratories), orexin-A (1:500; R&D Systems), c-Fos (1:5,000; Calbiochem), and DBH (1:2,000; EMD).
Slice Preparation, Whole-Cell Recordings, and Data Acquisition.
We prepared 260- to 280-µm-thick acute coronal slices of hypothalamus and horizontal slices containing LC from adult rats. Animals were anesthetized and transcardially perfused with ice-cold oxygenated slicing solution. Brains were removed immediately and immersed in ice-cold, oxygenated slicing solution. Slices were prepared, transferred to a 32–33 °C chamber, and maintained in continuously oxygenated artificial CSF (aCSF). After 30–45 min, slices were kept at room temperature and until recording at 32 ± 0.5 °C.
Whole-cell current clamp recordings were conducted in orexin projection cells of the medial PFH (identified by fluorescence) or from principal cells of the LC. Photo-activation in slices was achieved using a 470-nm high-power LED (Thorlabs) coupled to the microscope using a customized Siskiyou beamsplitter cube. Data were acquired and analyzed using PClamp software (Molecular Devices). Signals were acquired using an AxoClamp 2B amplifier, digitized through a Digidata 1440A at a sampling rate of 25 kHz, and filtered online at 10 kHz. We conducted statistical analyses in Matlab (Mathworks) using standard resampling methods, including the Matlab Resampling Package.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank M. Hou, B. T. Elie, and E. C. Andrade for technical assistance and C. K. Cain for editing the manuscript. Research reported in this publication was supported by National Institutes of Health Award F32MH094062.
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1320325110/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- 1.Hess WR, Akert K. Experimental data on role of hypothalamus in mechanism of emotional behavior. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1955;73(2):127–129. doi: 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1955.02330080005003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Nakao H. Emotional behavior produced by hypothalamic stimulation. Am J Physiol. 1958;194(2):411–418. doi: 10.1152/ajplegacy.1958.194.2.411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.de Lecea L, et al. The hypocretins: Hypothalamus-specific peptides with neuroexcitatory activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95(1):322–327. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.1.322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Sakurai T, et al. Orexins and orexin receptors: A family of hypothalamic neuropeptides and G protein-coupled receptors that regulate feeding behavior. Cell. 1998;92(4):573–585. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)80949-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ivanov A, Aston-Jones G. Hypocretin/orexin depolarizes and decreases potassium conductance in locus coeruleus neurons. Neuroreport. 2000;11(8):1755–1758. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200006050-00031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Borgland SL, Storm E, Bonci A. Orexin B/hypocretin 2 increases glutamatergic transmission to ventral tegmental area neurons. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28(8):1545–1556. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06397.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Winsky-Sommerer R, Boutrel B, de Lecea L. Stress and arousal: The corticotrophin-releasing factor/hypocretin circuitry. Mol Neurobiol. 2005;32(3):285–294. doi: 10.1385/MN:32:3:285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Aston-Jones G. Brain structures and receptors involved in alertness. Sleep Med. 2005;6(Suppl 1):S3–S7. doi: 10.1016/s1389-9457(05)80002-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Aston-Jones G, et al. Lateral hypothalamic orexin/hypocretin neurons: A role in reward-seeking and addiction. Brain Res. 2010;1314:74–90. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Marcus JN, et al. Differential expression of orexin receptors 1 and 2 in the rat brain. J Comp Neurol. 2001;435(1):6–25. doi: 10.1002/cne.1190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wu MF, Nienhuis R, Maidment N, Lam HA, Siegel JM. Role of the hypocretin (orexin) receptor 2 (Hcrt-r2) in the regulation of hypocretin level and cataplexy. J Neurosci. 2011;31(17):6305–6310. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0365-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Mang GM, et al. The dual orexin receptor antagonist almorexant induces sleep and decreases orexin-induced locomotion by blocking orexin 2 receptors. Sleep. 2012;35(12):1625–1635. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Harris GC, Wimmer M, Aston-Jones G. A role for lateral hypothalamic orexin neurons in reward seeking. Nature. 2005;437(7058):556–559. doi: 10.1038/nature04071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Smith RJ, See RE, Aston-Jones G. Orexin/hypocretin signaling at the orexin 1 receptor regulates cue-elicited cocaine-seeking. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;30(3):493–503. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06844.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Johnson PL, et al. Activation of the orexin 1 receptor is a critical component of CO2-mediated anxiety and hypertension but not bradycardia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37(8):1911–1922. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lungwitz EA, et al. Orexin-A induces anxiety-like behavior through interactions with glutamatergic receptors in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis of rats. Physiol Behav. 2012;107(5):726–732. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.05.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Furlong TM, Vianna DM, Liu L, Carrive P. Hypocretin/orexin contributes to the expression of some but not all forms of stress and arousal. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;30(8):1603–1614. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06952.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Steiner MA, Lecourt H, Jenck F. The brain orexin system and almorexant in fear-conditioned startle reactions in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012;223(4):465–475. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2736-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.van den Pol AN, et al. Hypocretin (orexin) enhances neuron activity and cell synchrony in developing mouse GFP-expressing locus coeruleus. J Physiol. 2002;541(Pt 1):169–185. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.017426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Horvath TL, et al. Hypocretin (orexin) activation and synaptic innervation of the locus coeruleus noradrenergic system. J Comp Neurol. 1999;415(2):145–159. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Soffin EM, et al. SB-334867-A antagonises orexin mediated excitation in the locus coeruleus. Neuropharmacology. 2002;42(1):127–133. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(01)00156-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mileykovskiy BY, Kiyashchenko LI, Siegel JM. Behavioral correlates of activity in identified hypocretin/orexin neurons. Neuron. 2005;46(5):787–798. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Chen FJ, Sara SJ. Locus coeruleus activation by foot shock or electrical stimulation inhibits amygdala neurons. Neuroscience. 2007;144(2):472–481. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.09.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Winsky-Sommerer R, et al. Interaction between the corticotropin-releasing factor system and hypocretins (orexins): A novel circuit mediating stress response. J Neurosci. 2004;24(50):11439–11448. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3459-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bush DE, Caparosa EM, Gekker A, Ledoux J. Beta-adrenergic receptors in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala contribute to the acquisition but not the consolidation of auditory fear conditioning. Front Behav Neurosci. 2010;4:154. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Debiec J, Ledoux JE. Disruption of reconsolidation but not consolidation of auditory fear conditioning by noradrenergic blockade in the amygdala. Neuroscience. 2004;129(2):267–272. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Tully K, Bolshakov VY. Emotional enhancement of memory: How norepinephrine enables synaptic plasticity. Mol Brain. 2010;3:15. doi: 10.1186/1756-6606-3-15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:155–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fanselow MS. Conditioned and unconditional components of post-shock freezing. Pavlov J Biol Sci. 1980;15(4):177–182. doi: 10.1007/BF03001163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Huang SC, Dai YW, Lee YH, Chiou LC, Hwang LL. Orexins depolarize rostral ventrolateral medulla neurons and increase arterial pressure and heart rate in rats mainly via orexin 2 receptors. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2010;334(2):522–529. doi: 10.1124/jpet.110.167791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Peyron C, et al. Neurons containing hypocretin (orexin) project to multiple neuronal systems. J Neurosci. 1998;18(23):9996–10015. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-23-09996.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lee HJ, Berger SY, Stiedl O, Spiess J, Kim JJ. Post-training injections of catecholaminergic drugs do not modulate fear conditioning in rats and mice. Neurosci Lett. 2001;303(2):123–126. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(01)01733-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. Memory consolidation of auditory pavlovian fear conditioning requires protein synthesis and protein kinase A in the amygdala. J Neurosci. 2000;20(18):RC96. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-18-j0003.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Adamantidis AR, Zhang F, Aravanis AM, Deisseroth K, de Lecea L. Neural substrates of awakening probed with optogenetic control of hypocretin neurons. Nature. 2007;450(7168):420–424. doi: 10.1038/nature06310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Johnson PL, et al. A key role for orexin in panic anxiety. Nat Med. 2010;16(1):111–115. doi: 10.1038/nm.2075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Johansen JP, et al. Optical activation of lateral amygdala pyramidal cells instructs associative fear learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107(28):12692–12697. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1002418107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Schöne C, et al. Optogenetic probing of fast glutamatergic transmission from hypocretin/orexin to histamine neurons in situ. J Neurosci. 2012;32(36):12437–12443. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0706-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Jones BE, Moore RY. Ascending projections of the locus coeruleus in the rat. II. Autoradiographic study. Brain Res. 1977;127(1):25–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Gompf HS, Aston-Jones G. Role of orexin input in the diurnal rhythm of locus coeruleus impulse activity. Brain Res. 2008;1224:43–52. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.05.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Carter ME, et al. Tuning arousal with optogenetic modulation of locus coeruleus neurons. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(12):1526–1533. doi: 10.1038/nn.2682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Adamantidis A, Carter MC, de Lecea L. Optogenetic deconstruction of sleep-wake circuitry in the brain. Front Mol Neurosci. 2010;2:31. doi: 10.3389/neuro.02.031.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Rice ME, Cragg SJ. Dopamine spillover after quantal release: Rethinking dopamine transmission in the nigrostriatal pathway. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2008;58(2):303–313. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Walling SG, Nutt DJ, Lalies MD, Harley CW. Orexin-A infusion in the locus ceruleus triggers norepinephrine (NE) release and NE-induced long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus. J Neurosci. 2004;24(34):7421–7426. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1587-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Soya S, et al. Orexin receptor-1 in the locus coeruleus plays an important role in cue-dependent fear memory consolidation. J Neurosci. 2013;33(36):14549–14557. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1130-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Khatami R, Birkmann S, Bassetti CL. Amygdala dysfunction in narcolepsy-cataplexy. J Sleep Res. 2007;16(2):226–229. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2007.00587.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ponz A, et al. Reduced amygdala activity during aversive conditioning in human narcolepsy. Ann Neurol. 2010;67(3):394–398. doi: 10.1002/ana.21881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Milad MR, et al. Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(12):1075–1082. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Jovanovic T, Ressler KJ. How the neurocircuitry and genetics of fear inhibition may inform our understanding of PTSD. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(6):648–662. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09071074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Strawn JR, Geracioti TD., Jr Noradrenergic dysfunction and the psychopharmacology of posttraumatic stress disorder. Depress Anxiety. 2008;25(3):260–271. doi: 10.1002/da.20292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Geracioti TD, Jr, et al. CSF norepinephrine concentrations in posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(8):1227–1230. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Blouin AM, et al. Human hypocretin and melanin-concentrating hormone levels are linked to emotion and social interaction. Nat Commun. 2013;4:1547. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Coleman PJ, Renger JJ. Orexin receptor antagonists: a review of promising compounds patented since 2006. Expert Opin Ther Pat. 2010;20(3):307–324. doi: 10.1517/13543770903567085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.