1. Introduction
Bloodstein and Bernstein Ratner (2008) suggested that “there is a rather pronounced tendency of stutterers to have functional difficulties of articulation…‘immature’ speech and the like” (p. 226). As shown in Table 1, this suggestion supports findings from early (1920 – late 1960’s), informal or nonstandardized assessments indicating that children who stutter (CWS) exhibit poorer articulation abilities than children who do not stutter (CWNS). However, Table 1 also shows that more recent (1980’s - present), formal or standardized assessments have not always reported such between-group differences. Taken together, results from extant empirical studies appear equivocal regarding the association between childhood stuttering and articulation (Nippold, 2002).
Table 1.
Early empirical investigations employing informal or non-standardized measures of articulation
| ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Study | Method of Articulation Assessment | Participants
|
Findings | |||||
Inclusion Criteria: Articulation Abilities | N | Age (years;months) | ||||||
| ||||||||
Total CWS (n girls) | Total CWNS (n girls) | Overall Range | Mean (SD)
|
|||||
CWS | CWNS | |||||||
McDowell (1928) | Sentence repetition task | Freely varying | 331 | 331 | 7–12 years | N/A | N/A | CWS exhibited a greater number of incorrect (articulatory) responses than CWNS |
Morley (1957) | Single word productions | Freely varying | 37 | 114 | Each child examined at ages 3;9, 4;9, and 6;6 years | N/A | N/A | CWS produced more articulation errors than CWNS at 6;6 years of age, but not at the younger ages. |
Schindler (1955) | Spontaneous responses on the “speech articulation tests”2 | N/A | 126 | 252 | 1st–12th grade | N/A3 | N/A3 | Significantly more CWS than CWNS “made some type of articulation error on the speech articulation tests” (Schindler, 1955, p. 355) |
Williams & Silverman (1968) | Story-telling, sentence imitation, and reading tasks | N/A | 1154 | 1154 | Kindergarten-9th grade | N/A | N/A | More CWS than CWNS exhibited at least one consistent articulation error, particularly in the younger grades. No statistical analyses were performed to determine significance. |
Recent empirical investigations employing formal or standardized measures of articulation
| ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Study | Method of Articulation Assessment | Participants
|
Findings | |||||
Inclusion Criteria: Articulation Abilities | N | Age (years;months) | ||||||
| ||||||||
Total CWS (n girls) | Total CWNS (n girls) | Overall Range | Mean (SD)
|
|||||
CWS | CWNS | |||||||
Anderson & Conture (2000) | GFTA | All scored WNL | 20 (4 girls) | 20 (4 girls) | 3;0–5;5 years | 3;10 (9.8 mos)5 | 3;11 (9.6 mos)5 | CWS exhibited significantly poorer GFTA scores than CWNS |
Anderson & Conture (2004) | GFTA-2 | All scored WNL | 16 (4 girls) | 16 (4 girls) | 3;3–5;5 years | 4;5 (N/A) | 4;4 (N/A) | No significant group difference |
Anderson, Pellowski, & Conture (2005) | GFTA-2 | CWS freely varied; CWNS scored WNL | 45 (16 girls) | 45 (16 girls) | 3;0–5;11 years | 4;1 (N/A) | 4;1 (N/A) | No significant group difference |
Arnold, Conture, & Ohde (2005) | GFTA | All scored WNL | 9 (4 girls) | 9 (4 girls) | 3;0–5;11 years 3-year-old age group |
N/A3 | N/A3 | No significant group difference |
Byrd, Conture, & Ohde (2007) | GFTA | All scored WNL | 266 (N/A) | 266 (N/A) | 5-year-old age group | N/A3 | N/A3 | No significant group difference (3- and 5-year-old age groups were combined so that all CWS were compared to all CWNS) |
Coulter, Anderson, & Conture (2009)7 | GFTA-2 | CWS freely varied; CWNS scored WNL | 40 (15 girls) | 40 (15 girls) | 3;0–5;8 years | 3;11 (9.7 mos) | 4;0 (9.4 mos) | No significant group difference |
Pellowski & Conture (2005) | GFTA-2 | All scored WNL | 23 (2 girls) | 23 (5 girls) | 3;0–5;11 years | 4;6 (9 mos) | 4;6 (9 mos) | No significant group difference |
Pellowski, Conture, Anderson, & Ohde (2001) | GFTA | All scored WNL | 25 (5 girls) | 25 (5 girls) | 3;0–6;3 years | N/A3 | N/A3 | CWS exhibited significantly poorer GFTA scores than CWNS |
Ryan (1992) | AAPS | N/A | 20 (5 girls) | 20 (5 girls) | 2;10–5;9 years | 4;4 (8.7 mos) Moderate 12;7 (N/A) |
4;1 (9.1 mos) | No significant group difference |
St. Louis & Hinzman (1988) | GFTA-Experimental; data obtained from a national survey | Unclear | 48 (10 girls) | 24 (12 girls) | 6–20 years | Severe 12;5 (N/A) | 12;5 (N/A) | CWS produced significantly more articulation errors than CWNS |
CWS and CWNS participants were matched for chronological and mental age, intelligence, sex, language and racial background.
The exact title and nature of the speech articulation tests were not specified in the report.
Participants were matched for age. However, M age and SD for each talker-group was not provided.
Gender ratio was reported to be approximately 4:1 males to females in each talker group. Participants were analyzed in four groups: kindergarten through first grade (N=25), second through third grade (N=32), fourth through sixth grade (N=34), and seventh through ninth grade (N=24).
Standard deviations (SD) were calculated by the present authors based on the data reported by Anderson and Conture (2000, Table 1).
Each talker group consisted of 13 3-year-olds and 13 5-year-olds. Participants were matched for gender. However, specific gender information was not provided.
Coulter, Anderson, and Conture (2009) reported Study 1 and Study 2 results. However, because Study 2 includes participants from Anderson, Pellowski, and Conture (2005), we only reported results for Study 1 in the present table.
Note: For many of the above studies, analyses of children’s articulation were done as part of a larger, overall purpose (e.g., phonological or lexical priming). Arizona Articulation Proficiency Scale (AAPS; Barker, 1973); Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-Experimental (GFTA-experimental; Goldman & Fristoe, 1968); Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation (GFTA; Goldman & Fristoe, 1986); Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2nd edition (GFTA-2; Goldman & Fristoe, 2000).
In attempts to better understand and interpret extant literature, we examine four salient issues related to this body of knowledge. The first issue relates to two terms sometimes interchangeably used to describe CWS’ speech sound abilities— articulation and phonology. The second issue relates to the apparent motivation for past investigations of the association between childhood stuttering and articulation. Third, as suggested above, is the issue of methodological differences among empirical studies that compared the articulation abilities of CWS and CWNS. Fourth, is the issue of the relative paucity of information regarding the association between CWS’ articulation abilities and their frequency, severity and type of stuttering.
1.1 Definition of speech sound abilities: Articulation versus phonology
Speech sound abilities can be categorized as articulatory (i.e., phonetic) or phonological (i.e., phonemic) in nature. Specifically, articulation refers to “motor processes involved in the planning and execution of sequences of overlapping gestures that result in speech” (Bauman-Waengler, 2004, p. 2). In contrast, phonology refers to cognitive/linguistic processes involved in how speech sound information is represented/organized, stored, planned and retrieved (Bauman-Waengler, 2004). It is possible for children to exhibit both articulatory and phonological speech sound errors (Bauman-Waengler, 2004).
Some have broadly used the term “phonology,” referring to both articulatory (i.e. motoric) and phonological (i.e., cognitive/linguistic) elements of speech production (Gierut, 1998; Shriberg & Kwiatkowski, 1982). Similarly, articulation and phonology have not always been differentiated when discussed in association with childhood stuttering (e.g., Arndt & Healey, 2001; Bloodstein & Bernstein Ratner, 2008; Coulter, Anderson, & Conture, 2009). However, differentiation between these two processes would seem important because studies of each involve varying methodologies, with results having differing implications (i.e., for a comprehensive discussion of this topic, see Bauman-Waengler [2004] and Kamhi [1992]). Similarly, it seems important to distinguish, where possible, between empirical studies of the association between stuttering and articulation and those of the association between stuttering and phonology. Thus, the present paper focused on the association between childhood stuttering and speech sound articulation1—recognizing that others have made significant contributions to our understanding of the association between childhood stuttering and phonology (e.g., Blood, Ridenour, Qualls & Hammer, 2003; Paden, Ambrose & Yairi, 2002; Paden, Yairi, & Ambrose, 1999).
1.2 Past studies of the association between articulation and stuttering
1.2.1 Motivation
Review of previous empirical investigations of the association between articulation and childhood stuttering suggests that such studies were motivated by: (1) general interest in speech-language variables possibly associated with childhood stuttering (e.g., St. Louis & Hinzman, 1988); (2) specific interest in whether articulation errors or disorders may be associated with childhood stuttering (Pellowski, Conture, Anderson, & Ohde, 2001); or (3) the possibility that articulation errors or disorders may represent a competing account for the association between stuttering and other variables (e.g., language abilities; Anderson & Conture, 2000). Regardless of the precise motivation, previous researchers have generally attempted to determine whether CWS, compared to CWNS, exhibit statistically or clinically significant differences in their speech sound development. Findings of statistically significant differences in articulation suggest that there are subtle to not-so-subtle articulation differences between CWS and CWNS (Pellowski et al., 2001), regardless of whether these differences represent frank or clinically significant articulation disorders. Findings of clinically significant differences in articulation suggest that articulation disorders are more prevalent among CWS than CWNS (e.g., Blood et al., 2003). Such between-group articulation differences—statistical or clinical—have been suggested to contribute to CWS’ challenges establishing normally fluent speech.
1.2.2 Varying sample and methodological characteristics
As noted above, there have been equivocal findings regarding the articulation abilities of CWS versus those of CWNS. In general, such equivocation seems to relate, at least in part, to between-study differences in sample characteristics and methodologies (Table 1).
Regarding sample characteristics, varying sample sizes, age-ranges, and inclusion/diagnostic criteria make it challenging to compare findings across various investigations. Specifically, sample sizes range from nine (Arnold, Conture, & Ohde, 2005) to 126 CWS (Schindler, 1955). The relatively small samples assessed by some studies raise the question of statistical power and generalizability to the population of CWS. However, studies assessing larger samples (Schindler, 1955; Williams & Silverman, 1968) have sometimes included wide age-ranges (e.g., 1st through 12th grade children). Given the developmental nature of childhood stuttering, which suggests that the disorder changes over time, it is difficult to extrapolate findings from older children (e.g., St. Louis & Hinzman, 1988), whose articulation abilities are relatively well established, to those of preschool-age children (e.g., Anderson & Conture, 2000), whose articulation abilities are less well established. Additionally, given that articulation difficulties have been shown to co-occur with stuttering more frequently than most other speech, language, or related disorders (e.g., Blood et al., 2003), it seems likely that varying diagnostic criteria across studies (i.e., including children with/without articulation disorders) may also have affected reported findings. Indeed, Table 1 shows that findings of studies employing relatively rigorous diagnostic criteria have not always been consistent with those whose diagnostic criteria were less clear or not reported.
Regarding methodological differences, articulation could be studied from various perspectives (e.g., acoustic, perceptual, kinematic and physiological). As shown in Table 1, some empirical studies assessing articulation employed informal/non-standardized perceptual methods (e.g., Williams & Silverman, 1968) whereas others employed formal/standardized perceptual methods (e.g., Coulter et al., 2009). Undoubtedly, both assessment methods provide pertinent information regarding children’s articulation abilities. However, it is possible that equivocal findings across studies relate to the fact that informal measures of articulation may consist of different tasks or stimuli than formal measures of articulation. For example, informal methods may involve relatively unstructured conversation that might not elicit all target sounds, whereas formal methods using single-word picture naming tasks require elicitation of all target sounds. Indeed, Morrison and Shriberg (1992) found significant differences between children’s articulatory performance on standardized measures versus during running/conversational speech. Additionally, children’s performances on standardized measures are compared against norms of the general population. In contrast, there are generally no norms against which to compare children’s performance on informal/non-standardized assessment methods.
Taken together, given the varying sample characteristics and methodologies across studies, it remains unclear how the articulation abilities of CWS, particularly those close to onset of the disorder (i.e., preschool-age), compare to those of CWNS. Thus, further empirical investigation of this topic seems warranted, especially one involving (1) a relatively large sample of participants, (2) a more circumscribed chronological age range, (3) more replicable inclusion criteria, and (4) widely-used standardized means of measuring speech sound articulation.2
1.2.3 Association between articulation and stuttering frequency, type, and severity
Relatively few published studies have reported findings of the association between young CWS’ articulation and their stuttering frequency, type, and severity. Ryan (1992, 2001) reported no significant correlation between preschool-age CWS’ stuttering frequency and their articulation abilities. St. Louis and Hinzman (1988) reported that school-age CWS with severe stuttering exhibited significantly more articulation errors than those with moderate stuttering severity, a finding that did not replicate in a later study (St. Louis, 1991). In a related study of the relation between speech disfluencies and articulation disorders, Ragsdale and Sisterhen (1984) reported a greater frequency of speech disfluencies, particularly repetitions, exhibited by 5- to 6-year-old children with versus those without articulation disorders. Suffice it to say, the association between children’s articulation abilities and their frequency, severity, and type of speech disfluencies is still uncertain
1.3 Present Study
1.3.1 Motivation
As the preceding review suggests, it is challenging to readily interpret the association between perceptual measures of speech sound articulation and childhood stuttering based on extant findings. This is at least partially due to the fact that such findings were based on different sample characteristics and methodologies. Therefore, motivated to improve our ability to interpret the association between articulation and stuttering, the present authors addressed these sample and methodological concerns by (1) studying a relatively large sample of participants (N=277); (2) employing a widely used standardized measure of speech sound production (i.e., the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2 [GFTA-2], Goldman & Fristoe, 2000); (3) explicating replicable inclusion criteria; and (4) a circumscribed chronological age range of preschool-age children (i.e., 3- to 5-year-old children). Studying the articulation abilities of CWS and CWNS within a restricted age range, particularly preschool-age children, is important because: (1) this is the time period when most children begin to stutter; (2) this is a time period when children continuously develop and refine their speech sound systems; and (3) relatively little empirical attention has been paid to the association between the articulation abilities of preschool-age CWS and their stuttering frequency, type, and severity.
1.3.2 Purpose and hypotheses
Thus, it was the purpose of the present study to attempt to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the association between articulation and childhood stuttering. In doing so, we examined two major issues regarding this association, while concurrently addressing the aforementioned sample and methodological concerns. The first issue relates to the association between articulation and the diagnosis of stuttering (i.e., CWS vs. CWNS); that is, whether there are statistically significant differences between preschool-age CWS’ and CWNS’ performance on a standardized, perceptual measure of articulation (i.e., GFTA-2 scores). We hypothesized that CWS’ GFTA-2 standard scores would be significantly lower than those of CWNS, and that these differences would be impacted by age and gender. The second issue relates to the association between articulation and stuttering behaviors; that is, whether preschool-age CWS’ articulation abilities (as indicated by their performance on a standardized, perceptual measure of articulation) are associated with various measures of their stuttering behaviors (i.e., frequency and severity of stuttering, as well as the sound prolongation index ([SPI]; Schwartz & Conture, 1988). We hypothesized that for CWS, GFTA-2 standard scores would be inversely related to stuttering frequency, severity, and SPI (i.e., the number of sound prolongations per stuttered disfluencies).
2. Methods
2.1 Participants
Participants included 277 monolingual, English speaking preschool-age children (3;0–5;11 years of age), 128 of whom stutter (CWS; 101 males, M = 48.26 months, SD = 9.01) and 149 who do not stutter (CWNS; 76 males, M = 51.23 months, SD =9.58). As will be discussed in the Data Analysis section below, variables such as chronological age and gender were included as covariates in the statistical models to reflect competing explanations for possible between-group differences in GFTA-2 scores.
These participants’ data were previously collected as part of a large-scale empirical investigation of linguistic and emotional associates of childhood stuttering (e.g., Arnold, Conture, Key, & Walden, 2011; Choi, Conture, Walden, Lambert, & Tumanova, 2013; Johnson, Walden, Conture, & Karrass, 2010; Richels, Buhr, Conture, & Ntourou, 2010; Walden et al., 2012). All were paid volunteers whose parents either learned of the study from an advertisement in a free monthly parent magazine circulated throughout Middle Tennessee, were contacted from Tennessee State birth records, or were referred to the Vanderbilt Bill Wilkerson Hearing and Speech Center for an evaluation. All parents signed an informed consent, and all children assented.
2.2 Classification and Inclusion Criteria
Participants were classified as CWS if they (a) exhibited three or more stuttered disfluencies ([SD] i.e., sound/syllable repetitions, sound prolongations or single-syllable whole-word repetitions) per 100 words of conversational speech (Conture, 2001; Yaruss, 1998), and (b) scored 11 or greater (i.e., severity of at least “mild”) on the Stuttering Severity Instrument-3 (SSI-3; Riley, 1994). In contrast, participants were classified as CWNS if they (a) exhibited two or fewer SD per 100 words of conversational speech, and (b) scored 10 or lower on the SSI-3 (i.e., severity of less than “mild”).3 Children’s speech fluency behaviors were considered “ambiguous”—rendering them unclassifiable—based on the following criteria (either [a] or [b]): (a) if the child exhibited two or fewer SDs per 100 words and scored 11 or greater on the SSI-3; OR (b) if the child exhibited three or more SD per 100 words and scored 10 or lower on the SSI-3. Children were required to meet all of the criteria listed above to be considered a CWS, CWNS, or unclassifiable.4
Participants were only included in the present study if they were classified as either CWS or CWNS based on both their stuttering frequency and total SSI-3 scores (see criteria [a] and [b] above); unclassifiable children (criteria [c] above) were excluded from participation. Additionally, included children were required to have no known or reported hearing, neurological, developmental, or intellectual disorders. Included participants were further required to have complete data for all standardized tests but were allowed to freely vary in their scores on the articulation and language measures.5
2.3 Final Data Corpus
The initial cohort consisted of 301 children, seven of whom were removed from the study because it was not possible to determine their talker group classification (i.e., their frequency of SD and SSI-3 scores placed them in the unclassifiable category). Of the remaining 294 children, five were excluded from the study because they did not fall within the target age range (i.e., <3;0 or >5;11 years of age), and 12 were excluded from further consideration because either one or more of their standardized speech or language data were missing. The removal of the abovementioned 24 children resulted in the final 277 participants (128 CWS, 149 CWNS) who were analyzed in the present study.
2.4 Measure of Speech Sound Articulation Abilities
Participants’ standard scores on the norm-referenced “Sounds in Words” subtest of the GFTA-2 were assessed to determine their speech sound articulation abilities. More specifically, GFTA-2 standard scores reflect examiners’ perceptual judgment of children’s (in)correct speech sound production of consonants at the word level. A greater standard score indicates that the child has better articulation abilities. As described by Anderson, Pellowski, & Conture (2005):
The GFTA-2 examines an individual’s articulation of consonant sounds in Standard American English via spontaneous single-word elicitation in response to pictures.6 The GFTA-2 was standardized on a normative sample of 2350 participants aged 2;0 to 21;11 and has a median coefficient alpha reliability of .94 and .96 for males and females, respectively, and a median test-retest reliability of .98 for initial, medial, and final sounds. (pp. 226–227)
2.5 Measurement of Speech Fluency
Participants’ speech fluency was measured with respect to frequency, type, and severity of stuttering, to be described in further detail below. These values were derived from a 300-word conversational speech sample—obtained through child-examiner free-play—using a disfluency count sheet (Conture, 2001) in conjunction with the SSI-3.
2.5.1 Types of Disfluencies
Participants’ speech disfluencies were categorized as either stuttered or non-stuttered. As with similar, published studies of preschool-age children by the present research group (e.g., Coulter et al., 2009; Richels et al., 2010), stuttered disfluencies included sound/syllable repetitions ([SSR] e.g., “s-s-s-sorry”), single-syllable whole-word repetitions ([WWR] e.g., “the-the-the”),7 and sound prolongations ([SP] e.g., “ssssorry”). Nonstuttered disfluencies included interjections ([INT] e.g., “um”), phrase repetitions ([PR] e.g., “I want to I want to”), and revisions ([REV] e.g., “I’m going to the store the restaurant”).
2.5.2 Frequency of Total, Stuttered and Non-stuttered Disfluencies
Frequency of total disfluencies (TD) was calculated by dividing the total number of all speech disfluencies (stuttered + non-stuttered) by the total number of words produced (i.e., TD/TW). Frequency of stuttered disfluencies (SD) was measured by dividing the total number of stuttered disfluencies by the total number of words spoken (i.e., SD/TW). Frequency of non-stuttered disfluencies (NSD) was measured by dividing the total number of non-stuttered disfluenices per total words (NSD/TW).
2.5.3 Stuttering Severity
Participants’ stuttering severity was determined by their overall score on the SSI-3 (Riley, 1994)—a criterion-referenced measure assessing stuttering frequency, duration, and physical concomitants.8
2.6 Sound Prolongation Index (SPI)
The sound prolongation index (SPI), a measure empirically shown by Schwartz and Conture (1988) to significantly differentiate among preschool-age CWS, was calculated by dividing the total number of sound prolongations by the total number of stuttered disfluencies (SP/SD) produced during a 300-word conversational sample.
2.7 Procedures
2.7.1 Parent Interview
Data collection for all participants consisted of a parent interview, wherein information was obtained regarding the family’s history of speech-language and fluency disorders, as well as caregivers’ concerns about their children’s speech-language abilities (for further detail pertaining to this interview process, see Conture, 2001). Additionally, information regarding participants’ socioeconomic status (SES) was gathered. SES data was classified using the Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of Social Position (Hollingshead, 1975), a protocol assessing SES based on the United States Census. This index takes into account both parents’ educational levels, occupation, gender, and marital status. Consistent with Hollingshead’s (1975) descriptions for data handling, computed SES scores range on a continuum from eight to 66,9 with a higher score indicating a higher socioeconomic status. Specifically, a score of eight reflects the lowest possible level of occupational status (e.g., dishwashers) and education (less than 7th grade), whereas a score of 66 reflects the highest level of occupational status (e.g., aeronautical engineer) and educational level (graduate education).
2.8.2 Child Testing
Testing was conducted in a controlled laboratory environment as part of a pre-experimental diagnosis/screening to determine inclusion/exclusion for subsequent experimental research (e.g., Byrd, Conture, & Ohde, 2007; Coulter et al., 2009; Johnson et al., 2010). While one examiner conducted the parent interview, another examiner engaged the child in conversation during free-play, from which measures of speech fluency were obtained (see Measurement of Speech Fluency section above). Participants were then administered a series of standardized speech and language tests in the following, fixed order: the “Sounds in Words” subtest of the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2 (GFTA-2; Goldman & Fristoe, 2000), the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Edition (PPVT-III; Dunn & Dunn, 1997), the Expressive Vocabulary Test (EVT; Williams, 1997), and the Test of Early Language Development-3 (TELD-3; Hresko, Reid, & Hamill, 1999). These standardized tests assessed children’s articulation abilities, receptive and expressive vocabulary, as well as receptive and expressive language skills, respectively. Examiners adhered to the administrative procedures stipulated in the manuals of the abovementioned standardized speech-language measures.
Standardized testing was followed by the administration of bilateral, pure tone and tympanometric hearing screenings; all audiometric equipment was routinely calibrated. Although testing procedures might have introduced an element of fatigue to some of the later administered tests (e.g., TELD-3), this procedure was a constant one for all participants in both talker groups. Furthermore, the present authors have found that the above procedures maximize the chances that the greatest number of preschool-age children will successfully complete all standardized speech-language testing.
2.9 Data Analyses
2.9.1 Talker Group Characteristics
Speech fluency variables
Prior to testing the present study’s main hypotheses, generalized linear models (GLM; Nelder & Wedderburn, 1972)10 were performed to assess between-group differences (i.e., CWS vs. CWNS) in speech fluency (i.e., SSI scores, as well as frequency of stuttered, non-stuttered, and total disfluencies). Given that the speech disfluency data followed a negative binomial distribution,11 GLM was chosen because it allows for analysis of count data that do not follow a normal distribution. Interested readers are referred to Gardner, Mulvey, and Shaw (1995) for more detailed statistical illustrations/explanations of GLM and negative binomial distributions.
Demographic and language variables
A series of statistical analyses were performed to better understand the age, gender, SES, language, and vocabulary characteristics of our CWS and CWNS samples. Characteristics that significantly differed between the talker groups were included as covariates in subsequent statistical models to account for competing explanations for possible between-group differences in GFTA-2 scores.
With the exception of gender, all the sample characteristics followed normal distributions and allowed for inferential parametric assessment. A chi-square (χ2) was performed to assess between-group gender differences given the non-normal categorical nature of the data. A series of analyses of variance (ANOVA) were performed to assess possible between-group differences regarding the other sample characteristics (i.e., age, SES, TELD-3, etc…). Because multiple significance tests may yield false (i.e., “significant”) results by chance, we employed a bootstrap re-sampling with replacement procedure (Efron, 1993) for multiple tests with a family wise false discovery rate of p <.05 (Hochberg, 1988; Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995). Re-sampling makes no assumptions about normality or independence. This was done using SAS PROC MULTTEST (Westfall, Tobias et al. 1999).
2.9.2 Hypotheses
To test the present study’s first hypothesis, generalized estimating equations (GEE)12 were employed to assess whether there are overall differences between the talker groups’ GFTA-2 scores. As will be described below, several covariates were included in the models to prevent misattributing variance in GFTA-2 scores to other talker group differences. To further assess articulation differences between talker groups relative to age, participants were divided into three separate age groups (3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds). Separate GEEs were performed for each age group, with each model containing gender as a covariate and a unique set of additional covariates pertinent to the specified age group. To test the second hypothesis, Spearman’s Rho correlations assessed the relation between CWS’ speech sound abilities and stuttering frequency, severity, and SPI.
Present findings were considered significant if their associated p-values were .05 or less. Estimates of effect size (ES) were expressed in partial eta squares (ηp2), Spearman’s rho (ρ), beta weights (β) or w (Cohen, 1988), depending on the statistical/analytical procedure employed. Traditional or recommended interpretations for these effect sizes were assumed (e.g., ηp2 = .01/.06/.14 ~ small/medium/large effects; w =.10/.30/.50 ~ small/medium/large effects [Cohen, 1973, 1988; UCLA: Statistical Consulting Group; Ferguson, 2009; Volker, 2006]).
2.9.3 Statistical power
We performed a Cohen-based power analysis (Cohen, 1988, 1992)—using PASS software (Hintze, 2008)—for a (1) one-way ANOVA with two groups (i.e., the first hypothesis); and (2) within-group correlational analysis (i.e., the second hypothesis). Power was evaluated by estimating the minimum detectable effect size (MDES; Kraemer, Mintz, Noda, Tinklenberg, & Yesavage, 2006). We assumed traditional criteria: p < .05 two-tailed, power=80%, and Cohen’s effect size guidelines (e.g., d = .2/.5/.8 ~ small/med/large effects; r = .1/.3/.5 ~ small/medium/large effects).
Relative to the first hypothesis (i.e., between-group GFTA-2 differences), a one-way ANOVA with two groups (N=277; 128 CWS + 149 CWNS) using a standardized outcome (mean = 0, std = 1) could detect effects as small as Cohen’s d = 0.34 SDs with 80% power. Relative to the second hypothesis (i.e., within-group correlation between GFTA-2 and stuttering behaviors), a correlational analysis with N=128 could detect effects as small as r = 0.24 with 80% power. Given the above MDESs, we concluded that the present study is sufficiently powered to detect small to medium effects (Cohen, 1992). However, it should be noted that non-significant effects might occur if the reported ESs are too weak.
2.10 Inter- and Intra-judge Reliability for Measurement of Speech Disfluencies
Intra-class correlation coefficients (ICC; McGraw & Wong, 1996; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979) using the absolute agreement criterion were calculated to assess inter- and intra-judge reliability for the measurement of stuttered (SD), non-stuttered (NSD), and total disfluencies (TD). Four examiners, trained in assessing stuttering, measured participants’ disfluencies in real time while watching video-recorded speech samples (obtained during child-clinician conversations in free-play).
Approximately 12% (n=32; 14 CWNS and 18 CWS) of participants’ video-recorded speech samples were randomly selected to assess inter-judge reliability. ICCs ranged from .95 to .97 (M=.96), with average measures of .989, p<.001, for identification of SD; from .82 to .89 (M=.86), with average measures of .955, p<.001, for identification of NSD; and from .94 to .97 (M=.96), with average measures of .987, p<.001, for identification of TD. Intra-judge reliability was collected for 11 participants (M=6 CWS; M=5 CWNS). At least 3 months passed between the first and second disfluency counts. ICCs ranged from .95 to .99 (M=.97) for identification of SD, from .88 to .96 (M=.93) for identification of NSD, and from .97 to .98 (M=.97) for identification of TD. The above ICC reliability values exceed the popular criterion of .7 (Yoder & Symons, 2010).
3. Results
3.1 Talker Group Characteristics
Table 2 shows participants’ demographic, speech fluency, and language characteristics. It should be noted that these various characteristics were not dependent variables of the present study’s main hypotheses, but were assessed to better understand the CWS and CWNS samples.
Table 2.
Mean (Standard Deviation) | F(df) | Wald χ2(df) | p (bootstrapped)c | ηp2 | β | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
CWS | CWNS | ||||||
Demographic Information
| |||||||
Chronological Age (in months) | 48.26 (9.01) | 51.23 (9.58) | 6.99 (1, 275) | N/A | .009 (.045) | .025 | N/A |
Gendera | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | < .001 | N/A | N/A |
SESb | 43.19 (11.69) | 46.34 (10.45) | 3.68 (1, 246) | N/A | .056 (.248) | .015 | N/A |
| |||||||
Speech Fluency Measures
| |||||||
Total Disfluencies (%TD) | 12.28 (5.86) | 4.21 (2.16) | N/A | 356.63 (1, 274) | < .001 | N/A | −1.084 |
Stuttered Disfluencies (%SD) | 8.41 (5.52) | 1.29 (.71) | N/A | 641.24 (1, 274) | < .001 | N/A | −1.889 |
Non-Stuttered Disfluencies (%NSD) | 3.85 (2.41) | 2.88 (1.86) | N/A | 15.1 (1, 274) | < .001 | N/A | −.293 |
SSI-3 Total Score | 18.32 (5.73) | 7.01 (1.58) | N/A | 630.03 (1, 274) | < .001 | N/A | −.960 |
| |||||||
Language Measures
| |||||||
PPVT-III | 105.5 (14.23) | 108.66 (13.68) | 3.55 (1, 275) | N/A | .061 (.264) | .013 | N/A |
EVT | 108.77 (12.84) | 112.38 (13.57) | 5.12 (1, 275) | N/A | .024 (.118) | .018 | N/A |
TELD – 3 Receptive | 108.74 (17.34) | 114.09 (14.54) | 7.81 (1, 275) | N/A | .006 (.03) | .028 | N/A |
TELD – 3 Expressive | 103.67 (14.37) | 105.07 (13.61) | 0.69 (1, 275) | N/A | .408 (.922) | .002 | N/A |
Note. As described in the Data Analyses section of the text (section 2.9), ANOVAs were performed to assess between-group differences in chronological age, SES and standardized measures of language (e.g., TELD-3, PPVT-III, EVT); a chi-square was performed to assess between-group gender differences. Therefore, Wald χ2 and β values were N/A for these measures. Additionally, GLM assessed between-group differences on the speech fluency measures (i.e., SSI-3 scores, as well as frequency of stuttered, non-stuttered, and total disfluencies). For these measures, F and ηp2 values are N/A.
A chi-square analysis assessed between-group gender differences, which provided frequencies of boys and girls per talker group, rather than M, SD, or F. Thus, results of the chi-square were not included in Table 2. As discussed in the Methods and Results sections, chi-square results indicated that the present sample consisted of more boys than girls who stutter (CWS=27 females and 101 males; CWNS=73 females and 76 males), χ2 (1)=23.233, p< .001, w =.29. Such findings are expected, given the gender differences in childhood stuttering (i.e., more boys than girls stutter).
SES information was available for 248 of the 277 total participants (132 CWNS, 116 CWS).
As described in the Methods, a bootstrap re-sampling procedure was employed when appropriate to control for false discovery rates.
Speech fluency variables
As would be expected based on talker group classification, preschool-age CWS, when compared to preschool-age CWNS, exhibited (1) a significantly greater mean percentage of total disfluencies (TD), Wald χ2 (1, 274)=356.63, p < 0.001, β =−1.084; (2) significantly more stuttered disfluencies (SD) per 100 words, Wald χ2 (1, 274)=641.24, p < 0.001, β=−1.889; and (3) and significantly more non-stuttered disfluencies (NSD) per 100 words, Wald χ2 (1, 274)=15.1, p < 0.001, β =−.293. Consistent with these findings, CWS exhibited significantly higher mean scores on the SSI-3, Wald χ2 (1, 274)=630.03, p < 0.001, β=−.96 (Table 2). All of the above β values (an estimate of effect size) indicated strong effects, with the exception of NSD whose β was “minimum [but] ‘practically’ significant…for social science data” (Ferguson, 2009, Table 1). For CWS, the sound prolongation index (SPI) ranged from 0% to 89.19%, with a mean of 19.13% (SD=20.7).
Demographic and language variables
Significant between-group differences were found for chronological age, F (1, 275)=6.986, p=.009 (p=.045, bootstrapped), ηp2=.025, gender, χ2 (1)=23.233, p< .001, w =.29.13 EVT, F(1, 275)=5.12, p=.024 (p=.118, bootstrapped), ηp2 =.018, and the receptive subtest of the TELD-3, F(1, 275)=7.81, p=.006 (p=.03, bootstrapped), ηp2 =.028. The effect sizes (ES) for these variables were small to medium. To control for possible effects of these between-group differences on GFTA scores, these factors (i.e., age, gender, EVT, TELD-3 receptive) were entered in the statistical model as covariates. Further consideration for variables that did not significantly differ between the talker groups (e.g., SES; see Table 2) did not appear warranted.
3.1.2 Hypothesis 1: Overall between-group differences in articulation abilities
Table 3 provides results of the GEE, which was the statistical model used to assess whether CWS score significantly lower on the GFTA-2 than CWNS (hypothesis 1). Given that the talker-groups significantly differed on a number of demographic and language variables (see Table 2), the GEE model included several covariates and two interaction terms to account for other possible explanations of variation in GFTA-2 scores. Covariates included chronological age, gender, EVT and TELD-3 receptive subscale scores; interactions included talker group X gender and talker group X chronological age.
Table 3.
Overall Between-Group Differences | Between-Group Differences Stratified by Age | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
3 year-olds | 4 year-olds | 5 year-olds | ||||||
Talker Group | CWS | CWNS | CWS | CWNS | CWS | CWNS | CWS | CWNS |
N | 128 | 149 | 62 | 57 | 51 | 58 | 15 | 34 |
Gender | ||||||||
Male | 101 | 76 | 45 | 27 | 43 | 28 | 13 | 21 |
Female | 27 | 73 | 17 | 30 | 8 | 30 | 2 | 13 |
GFTA-2 Standard Score | ||||||||
Descriptive Statistics | ||||||||
M (SD) | 105.06 (11.45) | 106.32 (12.92) | 108.34 (9.64) | 107.6 (13.86) | 102.27 (12.63) | 107.72 (12.28) | 101 (11.02) | 101.79 (11.59) |
Inferential Statistics | ||||||||
EM (SEE) | 105.61 (1.06) | 105.87 (1.06) | 108.38 (1.30) | 107.59 (1.82) | 104.63 (1.77) | 106.60 (1.59) | 104.31 (2.77) | 102.77 (1.71) |
Wald χ2 (df) | 1.02 (1) | .12 (1) | .666 (1) | .20 (1) | ||||
p | .312 | .729 | .415 | .655 | ||||
β | −.06 | −.007 | .019 | −.015 |
As shown in Table 3, results of the GEE indicated no significant differences between the speech sound articulation abilities of preschool-age CWS and CWNS, p = .312, β=−.06. Of the abovementioned covariates and interactions included in this model, a significant effect was only found for the TELD-3 receptive, Wald χ2 (1) =4.241, p = .039, β =.001; all other variables (age, gender, EVT and the interactions) were non-significant (p values ranged from .484 to .06; β ranged from .001 to −.019). Given the small β, further consideration regarding the significant TELD-3 receptive effect did not seem warranted (i.e., as GFTA-2 scores increase by 1 point, TELD-3 receptive scores tend to increase by .001 points, rendering its impact on GFTA-2 performance negligible). For participants in the present study, β values were small for all independent variables and covariates (Ferguson, 2009).
Age-related between-group differences in articulation abilities
To further assess the possible effects of age and gender on GFTA scores, participants were divided into three age groups: a 3-year-old (57 CWNS, 62 CWS), 4-year-old (58 CWNS, 51 CWS), and 5-year-old age group (34 CWNS, 15 CWS). ANOVAs assessed possible between-group differences, within each age group, on the various standardized language tests (e.g., TELD-3, PPVT-III, EVT). Those found to be significant were included as covariates, in addition to gender, in subsequent statistical analyses.14
As shown in Table 3, no significant between-group differences were found for the GFTA-2 for the 3-year-old, 4-year-old, or 5-year-old age groups. Furthermore, no significant gender effects were found for the 3- or 4-year-old groups (p =.943 and .086, respectively; β =.007 and .019, respectively). A significant but small gender effect (as indicated by β; Ferguson, 2009) was found for the 5-year-old age group, with males (EM=99.02, SEE=1.99) scoring lower on the GFTA-2 than females (EM=108.25, SEE=2.19), Wald χ2 (1) =10.304, p = .001, β =−.089. Specifically, 5-year-old CWNS males scored significantly poorer on the GFTA-2 (EM=98.29, SEE=2.42) than both CWS females (EM=109.06, SEE=3.52; p=.037) and CWNS females (EM=107.45, SEE=1.96; p=.001). Likewise, CWS males scored significantly poorer on the GFTA-2 (EM=99.76, SEE=2.74) than both CWS females (p=.002) and CWNS females (p=.038). However, as previously mentioned, β was quite small for this gender effect (Ferguson, 2009).
3.1.3 Hypothesis 2: Association between CWS’ articulation abilities and stuttering behaviors
Spearman’s rho (ρ) correlation was conducted for CWS to test whether an inverse relation exists between their GFTA-2 standard scores and stuttering behaviors. Findings showed no correlations between CWS’ GFTA-2 standard scores and TD, SD, and NSD, nor between GFTA-2 standard scores and SSI3 scores or SPI. Rho ranged from .003 to .102, with associated p values ranging from .251 to .975. Effect sizes were small for each of these correlations, as indicated by ρ < .2 (Ferguson, 2009).
4. Discussion
4.1 Overall Findings and Implications
The present study resulted in two main findings. First, contrary to hypothesis one, there was no overall significant difference between the articulation abilities of preschool-age CWS and CWNS. Second, contrary to hypothesis two, preschool-age CWS’ articulation abilities did not correlate with their stuttering behaviors (i.e., stuttering frequency, severity, and SPI). These findings suggest that for this sample of preschool-age children, there is no appreciable association between the diagnosis or behavioral characteristics of stuttering and speech sound articulation, at least when the latter is perceptually measured by a standardized assessment of speech sound articulation.
These findings are curious in light of the relatively high percentage of CWS reported to have co-occurring articulation difficulties (e.g., Blood et al., 2003). However, there are two salient differences between the present study and those reporting the prevalence of co-occurring articulation disorders in CWS. First, whereas the present study assessed the articulation abilities of preschool-age children between 3;0 to 5;11 years of age, some previous studies in this area did not include younger preschool-age children (i.e., Blood et al., 2003; Blood & Seider, 1981). Others studied a wider age-range of CWS, including preschool-age CWS, but reported results for all participants without explicating the prevalence of speech sound disorders within particular age groups (e.g., Arndt & Healey, 2001). Second, most of the preschool-age CWS in the present study exhibited within-normal articulation abilities (WNL; based on GFTA-2 scores), whereas other studies (i.e., Arndt & Healey, 2001; Blood et al., 2003; Blood & Seider, 1981) assessed CWS with identified articulation disorders. Thus, although present findings help clarify the association between articulation abilities and childhood stuttering—in otherwise typically developing preschool-age children—the prevalence of articulation disorders exhibited by preschool-age CWS, when compared to their CWNS peers, remains an open empirical question.
Given the above, we propose three possible accounts of the present findings: First, there may be a different association between preschool-age children’s stuttering and their articulatory (motoric) versus phonological (linguistic) abilities. Second, there may be a different association between preschool-age children’s stuttering and the speed versus accuracy of their speech planning or production. Third, there may be differences in the speech sound abilities of school-age versus those of preschool-age children. These possibilities are addressed below.
4.1.1 Articulation versus Phonology
As mentioned above, speech sound articulation is often considered to be a motoric process involved in the execution or production of speech, whereas phonology is a cognitive/linguistic process involved in how speech sound information is represented/organized, stored, planned and retrieved (Bauman-Waengler, 2004; Gierut, 1998). One might consider that even if there is no relation between preschool-age CWS’ speech fluency and their perceived number of articulation errors (when the latter is perceptually measured using the GFTA-2), the possibility remains that there is an association between preschool-age children’s speech fluency and the quantity or quality of their phonological processes (i.e., typical or atypical patterns of rule-based speech sound errors, such as cluster reduction, gliding, and stopping). To date, however, there are equivocal findings regarding between-group differences in the number or types of phonological processes.
For instance, some have reported that preschool-age CWS exhibit a greater number of disordered or atypical phonological processes than preschool-age CWNS (e.g., Louko, Edwards, & Conture, 1990), whereas others reported no such differences (e.g., Yaruss, LaSalle, & Conture, 1998). Additionally, some have found no significant relations between the number or types of CWS’ phonological processes and their stuttering behaviors (e.g., stuttering frequency and duration, SPI; Louko et al., 1990; Yaruss et al., 1998), whereas others reported that preschool-age CWS with disordered phonology, compared to those without, exhibited significantly more sound prolongations (Wolk, Edwards, & Conture, 1993). Taken together, further empirical study is warranted to better understand between-group differences in phonological versus articulatory abilities, particularly in preschool-age CWS versus their CWNS peers. Implications of such studies should provide insight into the association between childhood stuttering and linguistic versus motoric processes associated with speech-language planning and production.
4.1.2 Accuracy versus Speed of Speech Processing, Planning, or Production
Perhaps differences between the articulation abilities of preschool-age CWS and CWNS are not apparent with respect to accuracy but rather in speed or temporal aspects of speech processing, planning or production. In other words, even if GFTA-2 scores (essentially a measure of articulatory accuracy) of preschool-age CWS do not differ from those of CWNS, the speed with which the talker groups’ process, plan and produce speech sounds might differ. This possibility could not be investigated using the present study’s methodology. However, such speculation is consistent with extant reports of between-group differences in (non)speech reaction times (RT) in children and adults who do versus those who do not stutter (e.g., Jones, Fox, & Jacewicz, 2012; Melnick, Conture, & Ohde, 2003; Weber-Fox, Spencer, Spruill, & Smith, 2004).
For instance, in a phonological priming study examining preschool-age children exhibiting WNL articulation abilities, Melnick et al. (2003) found that preschool-age CWNS with higher GFTA scores exhibited faster speech reaction times (SRT) than those with lower GFTA scores. For preschool-age CWS, however, no relations were found between GFTA scores and reaction times. These findings led the authors to conclude that “even after removing children with apparent and/or clinically significant articulatory difficulties, it would appear that the articulatory systems of CWS are less well-developed or organized than those of their normally fluent peers” (p. 1439).
Similarly, several studies examined the phonological processing abilities of adults who do (AWS) and do not stutter (AWNS) during non-speech tasks (e.g., rhyme judgment tasks). Findings indicated that compared to AWNS, AWS demonstrated longer delays and slower RTs especially during cognitively loaded (non)speech tasks (Jones et al., 2012; Weber-Fox et al., 2004). These results were taken to suggest that “the phonological processing system of AWS, compared to AWNS, are slower and more vulnerable to delays…especially when the cognitive load increased” (Jones et al., 2012).
The above suggests that temporal aspects of speech processing, planning or production may moderate/mediate the association between speech sound articulation and childhood stuttering. However, support or refutation for such possibilities must await future empirical investigation employing different methodologies than those used in the present study.
4.1.3 Articulation Abilities in Preschool- Versus School-age Children
It is also possible that the association between childhood stuttering and articulation is more apparent in school-age rather than in preschool-age children. Such speculations seem reasonable since children typically continue to develop and refine their speech sound productions until 6 to 9 years of age (Shriberg, Gruberg, Kwiatkowski, 1994; Shriberg, Kwiatkowski, & Gruber, 1994; Table 7, Smit, Hand, Freilinger, Bernthal, & Bird, 1990). Thus, it is fairly common for preschool-age children to exhibit speech sound errors that would be considered typical/acceptable. However, errors that persist beyond the typical age of acquisition would be considered more problematic. Additionally, there have been reports of typically developing children exhibiting reversals of previously acquired speech sounds, particularly of /s/ and /z/ productions (e.g., Smit et al., 1990). In other words, there are children who have acquired correct production of speech sounds “early in development and then adopt an error variant for a time before reverting to an acceptable production” (e.g., Smit et al., 1990, p.791).
Related to the above, Ryan (1992, 2001) found no significant differences between the articulation scores of preschool-age CWS and CWNS. However, he reported that five of the CWS (25% of the CWS sample), but none of the CWNS, later required therapy to correct “residual” speech sound errors. Residual errors are typically considered to be distortions of fricatives or liquids (e.g., /s/, /r/, and /l/) that persist in children ages 9 years and above (Shriberg & Kwiatkowski, 1994). Taken together, future research in this area may benefit from longitudinal studies of articulation in preschool-age children as they progress into school-age years, or replication and extension of the present study using both preschool- and school-age CWS and CWNS.
4.2 Ancillary Findings: Articulation Abilities Relative to Gender
A gender effect for the GFTA-2 was found for the 5-year-old age group, with girls generally outperforming boys, regardless of talker group. However, inferences regarding gender should be made with caution, given the relatively small effect size and sample of female CWS in this study, particularly in the 5-year-old age group. With that caveat in mind, present findings suggest that in the general population, older preschool-age girls tend to exhibit stronger articulation abilities than older preschool-age boys, a finding consistent with those indicating that typically developing girls generally exhibit better articulation abilities than boys (Kenney & Prather, 1986; Kenney, Prather, Mooney, & Jeruzal, 1984; Smit et al., 1990; Templin, 1957). Similarly, Blood et al. (2003) reported that 5- to 18-year-old CWS males exhibited a greater percentage of co-occurring articulation disorders than 5- to 18-year-old CWS females. However, it should be noted that the above studies did not report gender differences relative to specific age groups. Thus, further investigations, employing large sample sizes, are warranted to determine whether preschool-age males present with more articulation disorders/difficulties than preschool-age females—both in the general and stuttering populations—within specific age groups (i.e., 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds).
5. Caveats
One limitation of the present study is that articulation was assessed using a standardized measure eliciting speech sounds in single-word responses. Some children might perform fairly well in single-word responses, but display poor sound production in conversational speech (Morrison & Shriberg, 1992). Perhaps a multi-method approach, including both formal and informal measures of speech sound development across varying speaking contexts, would provide a more comprehensive assessment of the association between articulation abilities and childhood stuttering.
A second limitation is that the GFTA-2 and similar standardized measures of articulation represent but one method of assessing children’s speech sound production—a method involving perceptual judgments of the (in)accuracy with which children produce speech sounds at the word level. Such perceptual, standardized methods are certainly salient to a comprehensive understanding of the association between articulation and stuttering. However, based on the overall results of such methods (e.g., standard scores), it could be difficult to determine whether speech sound errors were phonological/phonemic (i.e., cognitively or linguistically based) or phonetic (i.e., motorically based) in nature (Bauman-Waengler, 2004; Gierut, 1998). Therefore, other available means of measuring children’s speech sound abilities (e.g., acoustic, physiological, kinematic, etc.) should be explored to assess the relation between various facets of articulation and childhood stuttering.
A third limitation is that the vast majority (94%) of participants scored within (WNL) or above the normal limits (ANL) on the GFTA-2 (e.g., 122 CWS scored W/ANL, 6 scored BNL). Thus, our findings might not be generalizable to populations of preschool-age children exhibiting disordered articulation. Certainly, our findings do not rule out the possibility of a subgroup of preschool-age CWS with clinically significant or frank articulation disorders (Blood & Seider, 1981; Van Riper, 1971), for whom results might differ. For this subgroup, it is possible that a stronger relation exists between articulation and characteristics of their stuttering (e.g., frequency and severity).
6. Conclusion
Equivocal findings have been reported regarding differences between the articulation abilities of CWS and CWNS. Such equivocation relates, at least in part, to between-study differences in sample characteristics and methodologies. In the present investigation, we addressed these sample and methodological concerns by studying a relatively large sample of preschool-age children using standardized, replicable methodology. In doing so, we examined (1) between-group articulation differences, and (2) the association between articulation and stuttering behaviors within CWS.
Present findings indicated that preschool-age CWS do not appear to differ from their non-stuttering peers, at least on the basis of one standardized test of speech sound articulation (i.e., GFTA-2). Furthermore, results showed no significant correlation between preschool-age CWS’ articulation abilities and their stuttering frequency and severity, or SPI. We, therefore, concluded that for this sample of preschool-age children, there is no apparent association between childhood stuttering and speech sound articulation abilities when the latter is assessed by the GFTA-2 (a standardized, perceptual measure of consonant production at the word level).
Given the developmental nature of stuttering and the possibility that chronological age impacts speech sound articulation (the latter indicated by Shriberg et al. [1994] and others), one might suggest that both articulation abilities and childhood stuttering change over time. These changes may contribute to different interactions among chronological age, gender, speech sound articulation and childhood stuttering across the preschool- and school-age years. Therefore, such interactions should be further considered in subsequent theoretical accounts, narrative and meta-analytical reviews, as well as empirical studies of the association between speech sound articulation and childhood stuttering.
Highlights.
The GFTA-2 assessed preschool-age CWS’ and CWNS’ articulation abilities.
No articulation differences were found between preschool-age CWS and CWNS.
CWS’ articulation scores did not correlate with measures of stuttering.
Articulation—measured by the GFTA-2—and stuttering are not related in preschoolers.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported in part by NIDCD/NIH research Grants, R01 DC000523-16 and R01 DC006477-01A2, the National Center for Research Resources, a CTSA grant (1 UL1 RR024975) to Vanderbilt University, and a Vanderbilt University Discovery Grant. The research reported herein does not reflect the views of the NIH, NCHD, or Vanderbilt University. The authors would like to thank Dr. Ralph Ohde for reviews of earlier drafts of this manuscript, as well as Dahye Choi, Victoria Tumanova, and Hatun Zengin-Bolatkale for their assistance from a distance. We would also like to extend our sincere appreciation to the participants who made this study possible.
Biographies
Chagit Edery Clark, M.S., CCC-SLP, is a speech-language pathologist currently pursuing a Ph.D. at the Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences at Vanderbilt University. Her main research interests relate to speech, language, and emotional contributions to childhood stuttering.
Edward G. Conture, Ph.D., CCC-SLP, is a professor at the Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences at Vanderbilt University. Dr. Conture is an ASHA Fellow and has received the Honors of the Association. His main research interests relate to the role of emotional and linguistic variables to the onset and development of childhood stuttering.
Tedra A. Walden, Ph.D., is a Professor of Psychology at Peabody College at Vanderbilt University and a Senior Fellow at the Institute for Public Policy Studies. Dr. Walden’s research focuses on the early socio-emotional development and the role of emotions in childhood stuttering.
Warren E. Lambert, Ph.D., is an assistant director of the Statistics and Methodology Core at Vanderbilt Kennedy Center, and a senior associate in Biostatistics at Vanderbilt University Medical Center and Special Education at Peabody College. While licensed as a clinical psychologist in Tennessee, he now works with Kennedy Center members as a statistical consultant.
Footnotes
Various means may be used to assess speech sound articulation, including but not limited to those which are acoustic, kinematic, physiological, and perceptual in nature. In the current study, we focus on a standardized, perceptual measure of children’s (in)correct speech sound production of consonant sounds at the word level (i.e., the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2 [GFTA-2], Goldman & Fristoe, 2000). For ease of readability, and given historical and current, conventional uses of the GFTA-2, we will henceforth refer to this measure as an index of “articulation,” avoiding the more neutral/specific yet wordy descriptions of the GFTA-2 (i.e., “perceptual (in)correctness of speech sound production”).
The present authors recognize that employing standardized measures of articulation represents but one line of evidence regarding the perceived accuracy with which CWS and CWNS reach broad articulatory targets. This approach does, however, allow one to compare children’s performance against normative data, thus providing insight into the relative development of both talker groups’ speech sound abilities.
It should be noted that the SSI-3 does not include a “no stuttering” category. Given that the lowest stuttering severity category on the SSI-3 is “very mild,” which corresponds with a total overall score of 10 or below, there could be some overlap between CWS and CWNS who fall under this category. To minimize such potential overlap, only children who scored 11 or above on the SSI-3 and exhibited 3 or more stuttered disfluencies (SD) per 100 words were classified as CWS. Similarly, only children who scored 10 or below on the SSI-3 and exhibited below 3 SDs per 100 words were classified as CWNS. The present authors acknowledge that there will always be an error term or potential overlap between the talker groups, wehtehr using the present or other stuttering classification schemas.
See Howell, Bailey, and Kothari (2010) as well as Jiang, Lu, Peng, Zhu, and Howell (2012) for reviews of other classification schemes that have been used to diagnose stuttering in older individuals (i.e., school-age children and adults) who stutter.
As previously mentioned, all participants (N=277) were analyzed regardless of whether their GFTA-2 scores were within- (n=260; 122 CWS, 138 CWNS) or below-normal limits (n=17; 6 male CWS, 11 CWNS). The present authors separately analyzed preschool-age participants (1) with freely varying GFTA-2 scores (N=277) and (2) who scored with-normal limits (n=260). Given the substantial overlap between the two samples, which resulted in similar findings, results herein are only reported for the total 277 participants with freely varying GFTA-2 scores.
For children exhibiting difficulty spontaneously responding to pictures, examiners adhered to the administrative procedures (e.g., providing verbal cues, modeling, assessing stimulability etc..) stipulated in the GFTA-2 manual to elicit production of target sounds.
Single-syllable whole-word repetitions produced “without tension are not counted as stuttering. Repetition of one-syllable words may be stuttering if the word sounds abnormal (shortened, prolonged, staccato, tense, etc.); however, when these single-syllable words are repeated but are otherwise spoken normally, they do not qualify as stuttering using the definition just stated” (Riley, 1994, p. 4). Thus, in accordance with the above quotation from the SSI-3 manual, only perceptually “abnormal (shortened, prolonged, staccato, tense, etc.” single-syllable whole-word repetitions were counted as stuttered disfluencies. In the present study, perceptually effortless, non-tense repetitions of single-syllable whole words—such as those produced for emphasis (e.g., the child says, “it was a big, big dog,” while gesturing how large the dog was)—were not counted as stuttered or nonstuttered disfluencies. These non-effortful, non-tense repetitions of single-syllable whole words were excluded from the fluency data used to (1) determine talker group classification, and (2) assess the association between children’s articulation scores and frequency of (non)stuttered speech disfluencies. Other stuttering classification schemes—some of which exclude WWRs from the SD category—have been used particularly when assessing older, school-age and adults who stutter (e.g., Howell et al., 2010; Jiang et al., 2012).
See Howell (2013) for a thorough description and assessment of the SSI-3.
Weighted Family SES scores are calculated by multiplying the occupation scale score by a weight of five and the education scale score by a weight of three, as per Hollingshead’s protocol.
“Generalized” linear models allow one to analyze dependent variables that follow various distributions (e.g., binary, Poisson, or negative binomial), including those which are non-normal (Nelder & Wedderburn, 1972). “The GLM should not be confused with the general linear model [i.e., ANOVA] described by Cohen (1968)… The latter is a generalization of multivariate and univariate regression with normally distributed errors” (Gardner, Mulvey, & Shaw, 1995, p. 395).
Non-normality of distribution was determined by graphical descriptive analysis of the data (i.e., histogram) as well as results of the Shapiro-Wilk test of normality (p<.001 for all disfluency measures). “Negative binomial” is a type of a Poisson regression with overdispersion (e.g., a long right-hand tail).
GEE were used to assess between-group GFTA-2 differences given the Poisson-like (i.e., non-normal) distribution of participants’ speech sound articulation scores.
As expected, given gender differences in childhood stuttering (i.e., more boys than girls stutter), the sample presently studied consisted of more males than females who stutter (CWS=27 females and 101 males; CWNS=73 females and 76 males).
Only the 4-year-old age group required additional covariates, which included the PPVT, EVT, as well as the TELD-3 receptive and expressive subscale scores. However, no significant effects were found for these variables on the GFTA-2 (p values ranged from .342 to .456).
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Contributor Information
Chagit E. Clark, Email: chagit.edery.clark@vanderbilt.edu.
Edward G. Conture, Email: edward.g.conture@vanderbilt.edu.
Tedra A. Walden, Email: tedra.walden@vanderbilt.edu.
Warren E. Lambert, Email: warren.lambert@vanderbilt.edu.
References
- Ambrose NG, Yairi E. Normative disfluency data for early childhood stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 1999;42(4):895–909. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4204.895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JD, Conture EG. Language abilities of children who stutter: A preliminary study. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 2000;25:283–304. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson J, Conture E. Sentence-structure priming in young children who do and do not stutter. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2004;47:552–571. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/043). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JD, Pellowski MW, Conture EG. Childhood stuttering and dissociations across linguistic domains. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 2005;30(3):219–253. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2005.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnold HS, Conture EG, Key APF, Walden T. Emotional reactivity, regulation and childhood stuttering: A behavioral and electrophysiological study. Journal of Communication Disorders. 2011;44:276–293. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnold H, Conture E, Ohde R. Influence of phonological density on picture naming responses of young children who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 2005;30:125–148. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2005.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arndt J, Healey EC. Concomitant Disorders in School-Age Children Who Stutter. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools. 2001;32(2):68–78. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2001/006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barker J. Arizona Articulation Proficiency Scale. Western Psychological Association; Los Angeles, CA: 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Bauman-Waengler J. Articulation and phonological impairments: A clinical focus. Boston: Allyn & Bacon; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society B. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
- Blood GW, Ridenour VJ, Qualls CD, Hammer CS. Co-occurring disorders in children who stutter. Journal of Communication Disorders. 2003;36(6):427–448. doi: 10.1016/s0021-9924(03)00023-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blood GW, Seider R. The concomitant problems of young stutterers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1981;46:31–33. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4601.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bloodstein O, Bernstein Ratner N. A handbook on stuttering. 6. Clifton Park, NY: Delmar Learning; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Byrd CT, Conture EG, Ohde RN. Phonological Priming in Young Children Who Stutter: Holistic Versus Incremental Processing. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology. 2007;16(1):43–53. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2007/006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi D, Conture EG, Walden TA, Lambert W, Tumanova V. Behavioral inhibition and childhood stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 2013;38:171–183. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2013.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Multiple regression as a general data-analytic system. Psychological Bulletin. 1968;70:426–443. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Eta-squared and partial eta-squared in fixed factor ANOVA designs. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 1973;33:107–112. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. A power primer. Psychological bulletin. 1992;112(1):155–159. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.112.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conture EG. Stuttering: Its nature, diagnosis, and treatment. Boston, MA: Needham Heights: Allyn and Bacon; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Coulter CE, Anderson JD, Conture EG. Childhood stuttering and dissociations across linguistic domains: A replication and extension. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 2009;34:257–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2009.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunn L, Dunn L. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III. 3. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Efron BTR. An Introduction to the Bootstrap. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson CJ. An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice. 2009;40(5):532. [Google Scholar]
- Gardner W, Mulvey EP, Shaw EC. Regression analyses of counts and rates: Poisson, overdispersed Poisson, and negative binomial models. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;118(3):392–404. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.3.392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gierut JA. Treatment efficacy: Functional phonological disorders in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1998;41:S85–S100. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4101.s85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldman R, Fristoe M. Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation (experimental edition) Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University; 1968. [Google Scholar]
- Goldman R, Fristoe M. Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Goldman R, Fristoe M. Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-2. 2. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services, Inc; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Hintze J. PASS 2008. NCSS, LLC; Kaysville, Utah, USA: 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Hochberg Y. A sharper Bonferroni procedure for multiple significance testing. Biometrika. 1988;75:800–803. [Google Scholar]
- Hollingshead A. Unpublished manuscript. Yale University; New Haven, CT: 1975. Four factor index of social status. [Google Scholar]
- Howell P. Screening school-aged children for risk of stuttering. Journal of Fluency. 2013;38(2):102–123. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2012.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howell P, Bailey E, Kothari N. Changes in the pattern of stuttering over development for children who recover or persist. Clinical linguistics & phonetics. 2010;24(7):556–575. doi: 10.3109/02699200903581034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hresko W, Reid D, Hammill D. Test of Early Language Development-3. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Jiang J, Lu C, Peng D, Zhu C, Howell P. Classification of types of stuttering symptoms based on brain activity. PloS one. 2012;7(6):e39747. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson KN, Karrass J, Conture EG, Walden T. Influence of stuttering variation on talker group classification in preschool children: Preliminary findings. Journal of communication disorders. 2009;42(3):195–210. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2008.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson KN, Walden TA, Conture EG, Karrass J. Spontaneous Regulation of Emotions in Preschool Children Who Stutter: Preliminary Findings. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2010;53(6):1478–1495. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/08-0150). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones RM, Fox RA, Jacewicz E. The effects of concurrent cognitive load on phonological processing in adults who stutter. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2012;55:1862–1875. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/12-0014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamhi AG. The need for a broadbased model of phonological disorders. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools. 1992;23:261–268. [Google Scholar]
- Kenney K, Prather E. Articulation development in preschool children: Consistency of productions. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1986;29:29–36. doi: 10.1044/jshr.2901.29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenney K, Prather E, Mooney M, Jeruzal N. Comparison among three articulation sampling procedures with preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1984;27:226–231. doi: 10.1044/jshr.2702.231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraemer HC, Mintz J, Noda A, Tinklenberg J, Yesavage JA. Caution regarding the use of pilot studies to guide power calculations for study proposals. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2006;63(5):484. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.5.484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Louko LJ, Edwards ML, Conture EG. Phonological characteristics of young stutterers and their normally fluent peers: Preliminary observations. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 1990;15(4):191–210. [Google Scholar]
- McDowell ED. Educational and emotional adjustments of stuttering children. New York: Columbia University Teachers College; 1928. [Google Scholar]
- McGraw KO, Wong SP. Forming inferences about some intraclass correlation coefficients. Psychological methods. 1996;1(1):30–46. [Google Scholar]
- Melnick KS, Conture EG, Ohde RN. Phonological Priming in Picture Naming of Young Children Who Stutter. Journal Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2003;46(6):1428–1443. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/111). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morley ME. The development and disorders of speech in childhood. Edinburgh, Scotland: Livingstone; 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Morrison JA, Shriberg LD. Articulation testing versus conversational speech sampling. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1992;35:259–273. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3502.259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelder JA, Wedderburn RWM. Generalized linear models. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, A. 1972;135 (3):370–384. [Google Scholar]
- Nippold MA. Stuttering and phonology: Is there an interaction? American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2002;11:99–110. [Google Scholar]
- Paden EP, Ambrose NG, Yairi E. Phonological progress during the first 2 years of stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2002;45(2):256. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paden EP, Yairi E, Ambrose NG. Early Childhood Stuttering II: Initial Status of Phonological Abilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1999;42:1113–1124. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4205.1113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellowski MW, Conture EG. Lexical priming in picture naming of young children who do and do not stutter. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2005;48:278–294. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellowski MW, Conture EG, Anderson JD, Ohde RN. Articulatory and phonological assessment of children who stutter. Paper presented at the The third world congress on fluency disorders; Nyborg, Denmark. 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Ragsdale JD, Sisterhen DH. Hesitation phenomena in the spontaneous speech of normal and articulatory- defective children. Language and Speech. 1984;27:235–244. doi: 10.1177/002383098402700303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richels C, Buhr A, Conture E, Ntourou K. Utterance complexity and stuttering on function words in preschool-age children who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 2010;35(3):314–331. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2010.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riley GD. Stuttering severity instrument for children and adults. 3. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan B. Articulation, language, rate and fluency characteristics of 20 stuttering and nonstuttering preschool children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1992;35:333–342. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3502.333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan B. A longitudinal study of articulation, language, rate, and fluency of 22 preschool children. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 2001;26:107–127. [Google Scholar]
- Schindler MA. A study of educational adjustments of stuttering and nonstuttering children. In: Johnson W, Leutenegger R, editors. Stuttering in children and adults. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press; 1955. pp. 348–357. [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz HD, Conture EG. Subgrouping Young Stutterers: Preliminary Behavioral Observations. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1988;31(1):62–71. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3101.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shriberg Gruberg, Kwiatkowski Long-Term Normalization in Developmental Phonological Disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1994;37:1151–1177. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3705.1151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shriberg LD, Kwiatkowski J. Phonological disorders I: A diagnostic classification system. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1982;47:226–241. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4703.226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shriberg LD, Kwiatkowski J. Developmental phonological disorders I: A clinical profile. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 1994;37(5):1100. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3705.1100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shriberg LD, Kwiatkowski J, Gruber FA. Developmental phonological disorders II: Short-term speech sound normalization. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1994;37:1127–1150. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3705.1127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shrout PE, Fleiss JL. Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol Bull. 1979;86(2):420–428. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.86.2.420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smit A, Hand L, Freilinger J, Bernthal J, Bird A. The Iowa articulation norms project and its Nebraska replication. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1990;55:779–798. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5504.779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- St Louis KO. The stuttering/articulation disorders connection. In: Peters HFM, Hulstijn W, Starkweather CW, editors. Speech motor control and stuttering. Amsterdam: Elsevier/Excerpta Medica; 1991. pp. 393–400. [Google Scholar]
- St Louis KO, Hinzman AR. A descriptive study of speech, language, and hearing characteristics of school-aged stutterers. Journal of Fluency Disorders. 1988;13(5):331–355. [Google Scholar]
- Templin MC. Certain language skills in children. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, The Institute of Child Welfare; 1957. (Monograph Series No. 26) [Google Scholar]
- UCLA: Statistical Consulting Group. [accessed April 26, 2013];Introduction to SAS. Retrieved from http://www.ats.ucla.edu/stat/sas/notes2/
- Van Riper C. The nature of stuttering. 1. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall; 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Volker MA. Reporting effect size estimates in school psychology research. Psychology in the Schools. 2006;43(6):653–672. [Google Scholar]
- Walden T, Frankel C, Buhr A, Johnson K, Conture E, Karrass J. Dual diathesis-stressor model of emotional and linguistic contributions to developmental stuttering. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2012;40(4):633–644. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9581-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weber-Fox C, Spencer RMC, Spruil JE, III, Smith A. Phonological processing in adults who stutter: Electrophysiological and behavioral evidence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2004;47:1244–1258. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/094). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westfall P, Tobias R, et al. Multiple Comparisions and Multiple Tests Using the SAS System. Cary, NC: SAS Publishing; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Williams KT. Expressive vocabulary test (EVT) Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, Inc; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Williams DE, Silverman FH. Note concerning articulation of school-age stutterers. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1968;27:713–714. doi: 10.2466/pms.1968.27.3.713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolk L, Edwards ML, Conture EG. Coexistence of stuttering and disordered phonology in young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 1993;36:906–917. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3605.906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yaruss JS. Real-Time analysis of speech fluency: Procedures and reliability training. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 1998;7:25–37. [Google Scholar]
- Yaruss JS, LaSalle LR, Conture EG. Evaluating Stuttering in Young Children: Diagnostic Data. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology. 1998;7(4):62–76. [Google Scholar]
- Yoder P, Symons F. Observational measurement of behavior. New York: Springer; 2010. [Google Scholar]