Abstract
Objectives
To test the hypothesis that emotional withdrawal is an early indicator of affective disorder in infants heavily exposed prenatally to alcohol, which is independent of alcohol-related effects on mother-infant interaction and temperament and discriminated between children later diagnosed with fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS) and partial FAS (PFAS) and predicted cognitive and affective outcomes at 5 and 9 years.
Methods
The sample consisted of Cape Coloured (mixed ancestry) infants, whose mothers were interviewed during pregnancy regarding their alcohol consumption using a timeline follow-back approach. Infant emotional withdrawal (n = 85) was assessed on the Alarm Distress Baby Scale at 6.5 months. Mother-infant interaction was evaluated from video recordings during free play and infant feeding at 6.5 months (n = 127). Infant temperament was assessed by maternal report on the EAS Temperament Survey at 13 months (n = 119). Socio-demographic and psychological correlates of maternal alcohol use and infant iron deficiency were examined as potential confounders. The children were diagnosed for FAS/PFAS by expert dysmorphologists at 5 years; cognitive and affective function, at 5 and 9 years.
Results
Prenatal alcohol exposure was associated with increased infant emotional withdrawal and decreased activity, but unrelated to mother-infant interaction or any other temperament measures. Children later diagnosed with FAS and PFAS at 5 years exhibited more emotional withdrawal and less responsivity and activity as infants. Infant withdrawal, responsivity, quality of interaction, and maternal sensitivity also predicted poorer IQ and affective response at 5 and 9 years. When all four infant affective measures were examined simultaneously in a regression analysis, only infant emotional withdrawal persisted as a significant predictor of 9-year IQ.
Conclusions
This study is the first to document a direct effect of fetal alcohol exposure on emotional withdrawal in infancy. These data link prenatal alcohol to a specific aspect of infant affective function not attributable to mother-infant interaction, infant temperament, or other socioemotional aspects of the infant’s environment and identify infant emotional withdrawal as an early indicator of affective disturbance, particularly in children later diagnosed with FAS and PFAS.
Keywords: infant emotional withdrawal, prenatal alcohol exposure, infant temperament, fetal alcohol syndrome, mother-infant interaction, iron deficiency, IQ, Draw-A-Person Test
Introduction
Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD) range from fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS), which entails growth retardation, microcephaly, and craniofacial dysmorphology, to alcohol-related neurodevelopmental disorder, in which the distinctive FAS dysmorphology is lacking and neurobehavioral deficits are generally less severe. Although most attention has been focused on the cognitive deficits that characterize FASD, socioemotional development is also significantly affected in children and adolescents (e.g., Carmichael Olson et al., 1997; S. Jacobson et al., 2006; O’Connor et al., 2002; Sood et al., 2001). Some investigators have suggested that the socioemotional features of FASD seen in adolescence and adulthood are “secondary disabilities” resulting primarily from poorer executive function (attention problems, impulse control, affective organization; Streissguth et al., 1996). However, given that prenatal alcohol exposure causes diffuse neurological damage, it may impact directly on affective response in ways that may be evident already in infancy, long before executive function emerges.
The diagnostic classificatory system for infants and toddlers DC: 0-3 (Zero to Three) recognized the importance of impaired affect in infancy by including a category of depression. Although withdrawal has long been recognized as a key symptom of infant depression (Spitz, 1946), the concept of infant withdrawal remains ill-defined. Brief withdrawal is normal in the mother-infant interaction when observed at a microanalytic level (e.g., Weinberg and Tronick, 1994). Sustained infant withdrawal has been described clinically, often related to maternal non-contingent responsiveness, which is observed in maternal depression (e.g., Field, 1984). Severe withdrawal is also a key feature of non-organic failure-to-thrive (Powell and Bettes, 1992) and may be seen in clinical conditions, including central nervous system disease and visual and auditory sensory impairment (Behrman et al., 1983). In addition, emotional withdrawal occurs in severe forms of malnutrition, and its disappearance is a reliable sign of recovery (Guedeney, 1997). Depressed affect has also been found in infants with iron deficiency (Lozoff et al., 1998; Carter et al., 2010), and infant withdrawal has been recognized as an element of disorganized attachment (Lyons-Ruth et al., 1993; Main and Solomon, 1986). The Alarm Baby Distress (ADBB) Scale (Guedeney and Fermanian, 2001), a measure of infant emotional withdrawal designed to assess infants who experience organic and/or social environmental insults (Matthey et al., 2005), has not previously been used to assess effects of teratogenic exposures.
This study examines the relation between prenatal alcohol exposure and three sets of measures of infant affective function—emotional withdrawal, mother-infant interaction, and infant temperament. We hypothesize that (1) infant emotional withdrawal is an early indicator of affective disorder in infants exposed prenatally to alcohol; (2) alcohol-related emotional withdrawal is independent of effects of prenatal alcohol exposure on mother-infant interaction and infant temperamental characteristics; (3) alcohol-related infant emotional withdrawal is seen over and above effects of maternal depression, anxiety, life stress, exposure to smoking or other drugs, or infant iron deficiency; (4) children diagnosed with FAS will have been emotionally withdrawn as infants; and (5) emotional withdrawal is predictive of affective and cognitive outcomes at 5 and 9 years independent of the effects of other aspects of infant affective function.
Methods
Sample
The sample consists of 144 infants (77 males and 67 females) for whom data were available on infant withdrawal (n=85), mother-infant interaction (n=127), or infant temperament (n=119). The infants are from the Cape Coloured (mixed ancestry) community in Cape Town, South Africa, and are participating in a prospective, longitudinal study on the effects of heavy prenatal alcohol exposure on development (Jacobson et al., 2008; Molteno et al., 2010). A very high incidence of FAS has been documented in this population (May et al., 2000; 2007; J. Jacobson et al., 2006), which is comprised of descendants of the Khoi and San people, the original inhabitants of the region, with an admixture of white European settlers, slaves from Malaysia, and black Africans. Due to the tradition of very heavy alcohol use in this community, there is an unusually high prevalence of heavy maternal drinking during pregnancy.
Women were recruited from an antenatal clinic that serves an economically disadvantaged Cape Coloured population. Each mother was interviewed antenatally regarding her alcohol consumption both at time of recruitment and at conception, using a timeline follow-back interview (Jacobson et al., 2002). Any woman averaging at least 1.0 oz absolute alcohol (AA)/day, the equivalent of 2 standard drinks, or reporting at least 2 binge drinking episodes (at least 5 standard drinks/occasion) during the first trimester of pregnancy was invited to participate. Women initiating antenatal care at this clinic who drank <0.5 oz AA/day and did not binge drink (52 abstainers and 8 light drinkers) were also invited to participate.
Excluded were women <18 years of age and those with diabetes, epilepsy, or cardiac problems requiring treatment, as well as religiously observant Moslem women, whose religious practices prohibit alcohol consumption. Infant exclusionary criteria were major chromosomal anomalies, neural tube defects, multiple births, and seizures. Informed consent was obtained at recruitment and at the 6.5-, 12-, and 13-month and 5- and 9-year visits; oral consent, from the child at 9 years. Approval was obtained from the Wayne State University and University of Cape Town ethics committees. All women who reported drinking during pregnancy were advised to stop or reduce their intake and were offered referrals for treatment.
Alcohol and Drug Use
Each mother was interviewed about her alcohol consumption twice during pregnancy—at recruitment (M±SD=24.6±6.8 wk gestational age (GA) and later in pregnancy (33.5±6.7 GA) using timeline follow-back procedures (Jacobson et al., 2002). In the recruitment interview, the mother was asked about her drinking on a day-by-day basis during a typical 2-week period around time of conception, with recall linked to specific times of day and activities. If her drinking had changed since conception, she was also asked about her drinking during the past 2 weeks and when her drinking had changed. At the later antenatal visit, the mother was again interviewed about her drinking during the previous 2 weeks. At 1-month postpartum, the mother was asked about her drinking during a typical 2-week period during the latter part of pregnancy after the previous alcohol interview. Volume was recorded for each type of alcohol beverage consumed each day and converted to oz of AA using multipliers proposed by Bowman et al. (1975; liquor—0.4, beer—0.04, wine—0.2).
Six summary measures were constructed: oz AA/day, AA/drinking day (quantity/occasion), and frequency of drinking at conception and averaged across pregnancy. Alcohol abuse and/or dependence were diagnosed based on DSM-IV criteria using the alcohol module of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS). Each mother was also asked how many cigarettes she smoked per day and how often she used drugs, including cocaine, marijuana, and methaqualone (“mandrax”) during pregnancy. Postpartum maternal drinking was assessed at the 12-month follow-up visit using a timeline follow-back approach to assess drinking during a typical 2-week period.
Behavioral Assessments
Infant emotional withdrawal was assessed at 6.5 months for 85 infants during the maternal interview and infant examination by an examiner blind with respect to maternal alcohol history. The infant was undressed, weighed, and measured and his/her social responsiveness was noted. The ADBB scale consists of eight items: facial expression, eye contact, general level of activity, self-stimulating gestures, vocalisations, briskness of response to stimulation, capacity to engage a relationship, and infant’s capacity to attract attention relating to social behavior. Each item is rated on a 5-point scale (0 absolutely normal to 4 obviously pathological). The items are summed to provide a total score. The ADBB has been shown to have good psychometric properties and inter-rater reliability (Guedeney and Fermanian, 2001; Lopes, 2004). A cutoff score of 5 or more, suggested to identify infants at risk for suboptimal social behavior, has been validated in two different cultures (Lopes, 2004; Puura, 2004). C.M. and the late Andrea Hay (A.H.), M.A., a child clinical psychologist, both completed training using tapes supplied by Guedeney and independently scored 26 of the assessments, with an inter-observer reliability of r=0.92.
Mother-infant interaction was assessed at 6.5 months for 127 infants from videotaped recordings in two situations: free play (10 min) and infant feeding (about 10 min). The mother was asked to interact with her baby as she does at home. Using a method developed for an intervention program in an indigent Cape Town peri-urban community (Molteno and Tomlinson, 1997), the play interactions were coded for maternal sensitivity, infant responsivity, and overall quality of the interaction. Each of these variables was rated for the entire 10 minutes on a scale from 0-3 (0=none, 1=a little, 2=moderate, 3=a lot). Maternal sensitivity involved the mother’s showing affection, warmth, and empathy by making eye-contact, smiling or vocalizing, or gently holding or rocking the infant. Intrusive behavior included attempting interaction while the infant cried or turned away, or if the mother shook or handled the infant roughly. Infant responsivity was scored when the infant responded to the mother’s showing affection by making eye-contact, smiling, or vocalizing. Quality of interaction was scored when there was active communication, positive vocalizations, and general synchrony in joint activities. C.M. and A.H. rated 22 interactions independently; inter-observer reliabilities were r=0.65 for free play and r=0.95 for feeding.
Temperament was assessed by maternal report at 13 months and at 5 years, using the EAS Temperament Survey (Buss and Plomin, 1984), a 20-item questionnaire, which assesses emotionality, activity, shyness, and sociability. Each of these dimensions of temperament is measured by five items (e.g., tends to be shy, cries easily, likes to be with people), each of which is rated on a 5-point scale. EAS ratings were available for 119 infants at 13 months and 116 children at 5 years. Intercorrelations of the maternal ratings at these two ages were low to moderate for three of the dimensions of temperament—emotionality, activity, and shyness (see Table 5 below).
Table 5.
Relation of infant affective measures with 5- and 9-year affective outcomes and IQ
5-Year EAS |
IQ |
||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Emotionality | Activity | Sociability | Shyness | Draw-a-Person | 5-year | 9-year | |||||
N | r | r | r | r | N | r | N | r | N | r | |
Infant withdrawal | 60 | .18 | −.20 | −.08 | −.02 | 74 | −.26* | 80 | −.20† | 75 | −.26* |
Mother-infant interaction | |||||||||||
During free play | |||||||||||
Maternal sensitivity | 101 | .06 | −.10 | −.08 | .02 | 112 | .21* | 120 | .16† | 113 | .16 |
Infant responsivity | 101 | −.18† | −.04 | −.06 | −.04 | 112 | .15 | 120 | .19* | 113 | .20* |
Quality of interaction | 101 | .01 | −.11 | −.07 | −.03 | 112 | .18* | 120 | .18* | 113 | .17* |
During feeding | |||||||||||
Maternal sensitivity | 100 | −.03 | −.06 | .02 | .01 | 112 | .06 | 120 | .15† | 112 | .20* |
Infant EAS temperament | |||||||||||
Emotionality | 112 | .33*** | −.06 | .03 | .17† | 104 | .08 | 113 | −.12 | 105 | −.12 |
Activity | 112 | −.06 | .24 | .14 | −.17† | 104 | .03 | 113 | .13 | 105 | .20* |
Sociability | 112 | −.33*** | .07 | .11 | −.23* | 104 | .02 | 113 | .03 | 105 | .13 |
Shyness | 112 | .23* | −.06 | −.00 | −.28** | 104 | −.07 | 113 | −.01 | 105 | −.05 |
pp< .10
pp< .05
pp< .01
pp< .00
Iron status
A blood sample was collected from a finger-stick by a trained nurse at 6.5 or 12 months. A complete blood count was obtained, including hemoglobin (Hb), mean corpuscular volume (MCV), mean corpuscular hemoglobin (MCH), and red cell distribution width (RDW). Infants were determined to have iron deficiency anemia (IDA) if they had Hb of <10.9g/L and RDW of >15% plus MCV of <70.0 fL or MCH of <23.0 pg (Carter et al., 2007). A continuous measure of degree of iron deficiency was constructed by averaging z-scores for the four indicators of iron deficiency, which loaded together on the first component of a principal components analysis. The signs for Hb, MCV, and MCH were reversed in constructing this composite measure so that a higher score indicated a greater degree of deficiency.
Child Assessments
In September 2005, we organized a clinic at which each child was examined for growth and FAS anomalies using a standard protocol (Hoyme et al., 2005) independently by two expert FAS dysmorphologists, who subsequently reached agreement regarding FAS diagnosis. There was substantial agreement between the two U.S.-based dysmorphologists on their assessments of all dysmorphic features, including palpebral fissure length and philtrum and vermilion ratings based on the Astley and Clarren (2001) rating scales (r values = 0.80, 0.84, and 0.77, respectively), and between them and the Cape Town-based dysmorphologist (median r=0.78), who evaluated seven children who missed the clinic.
At 5 years, the children were administered the Junior South African Individual Scales (JSAIS; Madge et al., 1981). The JSAIS is a standardized IQ assessment, available in Afrikaans and English that has been normed for South African children. The Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children--Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) was administered at the 9-year follow-up assessment. In addition to Full Scale IQ, the WISC-IV provides four index scores: Verbal Comprehension, Perceptual Reasoning, Working Memory, and Processing Speed. IQ scores on the 5-year JSAIS were strongly correlated with the 9-year WISC-IV scores, r=0.67 p<.001. Testing was conducted in Afrikaans or English, depending on the primary language of instruction in the child’s school.
In addition, we asked the child to draw a person (see Draw-A-Person Test (DAP); Harris, 1963). Following Tharinger and Stark (1990), the drawings were scored for the presence of a happy (smiling), sad (frowning/unhappy), neutral, or incomplete face (missing facial features including the mouth), or as unable to be scored. Tharinger and Stark found that children with mood disorders (not including anxiety) produced significantly fewer happy faces than controls, leading them to suggest that the absence of a happy face is likely indicative of depression. In our Detroit Longitudinal Cohort, we found that teachers reported higher Internalizing scores on the Teacher Report Form (Achenbach, 1991) for children who drew fewer happy faces, confirming that this assessment may provide a good measure of depression in young children (Corobana et al., 2003). We also found that prenatal alcohol exposure was associated with fewer happy faces in that cohort.
Control Variables
Eighteen control variables were assessed to adjust for potential confounding: (1) four sociodemographic measures collected prenatally—socioeconomic status (SES) assessed on the Hollingshead (1975) Four Factor Index and maternal age at delivery, education, and non-verbal cognitive competence assessed on the Raven Progressive Matrices (Raven et al., 1996); (2) six measures of maternal emotional function—perceived life stress on the Life Events Scale (Holmes and Rahe, 1967) collected at the 12-month visit; the mean of maternal depression data collected on the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al., 1996) prenatally and at 6.5- and 12-month visits, and the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS; Cox et al., 1987) and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) Major Depression Module, both administered at 6.5-month visit; and trait and state anxiety on the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Scale (1983) at prenatal visit; (3) maternal marijuana (days/week) and smoking during pregnancy (cigarettes/day) collected at prenatal visits, and postpartum alcohol use, collected at 12-month visit; and (4) infant gender, age at assessment, birth weight, GA, and iron deficiency. Because only four women reported using methaqualone during pregnancy and only one reported cocaine use, there were too few to control for these exposures in multivariate analyses; instead we reran the analyses without their infants to see if the alcohol effects continued to be evident.
Data Analysis
All variables were checked for normality of distribution. AA/day at conception and during pregnancy were positively skewed (skew >3.0) and normalized by means of log (X +1) transformation. Pearson correlation was used to examine the intercorrelations among the measures of infant affective function—infant withdrawal, mother-infant interaction, and infant temperament—and the relation of those measures to the sociodemographic and maternal control variables. Multiple regression was used to examine the relation of prenatal alcohol exposure to the infant withdrawal, mother-infant interaction, and infant temperament measures. All control variables even weakly related to each of these outcomes (at p<.10) were controlled statistically in the regression analyses examining effects on those outcomes. Because the iron assessments were missing for 7 children on infant withdrawal and 32 for the mother-infant measures, separate regression analyses were conducted to control for iron deficiency together with the other potential confounders. Analysis of variance was used to examine the degree to which infant withdrawal predicted 5-year FAS diagnoses. Pearson correlations were used to examine the relation of the infant affective measures to 5- and 9-year IQ and child affective function assessed on the DAP.
Results
Sample
The mothers were poorly educated. Only 17.4% completed high school; two had no formal education (Table 1). One mother who was 17.1 years of age at delivery was inadvertently included in the sample. Virtually all (90.3%) were in the two lowest Hollingshead SES tiers. More than one-third (39.3%) met criteria for moderate to severe depression on the BDI, and 22.9% met criteria for major depression on the SCID.
Table 1.
Sample characteristics (N = 144)
M or % | SD | Range | |
---|---|---|---|
Maternal characteristics | |||
Socioeconomic statusa | 20.1 | 8.5 | 8 - 46 |
Age at delivery | 26.8 | 6.2 | 17.1 - 43.8 |
Parity | 2.2 | 1.3 | 1 - 8 |
Years of school completed | 8.6 | 2.6 | 0 - 12 |
Percent married | 31.3 | --- | --- |
Raven scoreb | 28.2 | 10.5 | 6 - 55 |
Depression | |||
BDIc | 15.3 | 11.6 | 0 - 53 |
SCIDd (n = 134) | 22.9 | --- | --- |
EPDSe (n = 133) | 15.7 | 3.4 | 7 - 22 |
Spielberger anxietyf | |||
State | 34.6 | 9.1 | 20 - 75 |
Trait | 44.1 | 8.9 | 25 - 67 |
Perceived life stressg (n = 117) | 17.8 | 18.2 | 0 - 102 |
Daily average (oz AA)h | |||
At conception (n = 79) | 1.5 | 1.6 | 0.02 - 11.6 |
During pregnancy (n = 85) | 0.9 | 1.0 | 0.01 - 7.4 |
Average drinks/occasion (oz AA)h | |||
At conception (n = 79) | 4.3 | 2.7 | 0.3 - 15.4 |
During pregnancy (n = 85) | 3.9 | 2.5 | 0.6 - 15.4 |
Number of drinking days/weekh | |||
At conception (n = 79) | 2.3 | 1.5 | 0.1 - 7.0 |
During pregnancy (n = 85) | 1.5 | 1.1 | 0.03 - 6.7 |
Marijuana use (days/week, n = 14)h | 2.4 | 2.0 | 0.03 - 7.0 |
Cigarettes/day (n = 101)h | 7.9 | 6.1 | 0.4 - 40.0 |
Infant characteristics | |||
Gender (% male) | 53.5 | --- | --- |
Gestational age (wk) | 38.7 | 2.4 | 29 - 43 |
Birthweight (g) | 2878 | 576 | 1130 - 4240 |
Head circumference (cm) (n = 143) | 32.8 | 1.9 | 23 - 36 |
Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of Social Status (Hollingshead, 1975) assessed prenatally. Maternal demographic data also collected at prenatal visit.
Raven Progressive Matrices (Raven et al., 1996) administered at prenatal visit.
Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1996) data collected at prenatal and 6.5- and 12-month visits from the mother were averaged for the three assessments.
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) Major Depression Module administered at 6.5 months.
Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (Cox et al., 1987) administered at 6.5 months.
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger et al., 1983) administered at prenatal visit.
Adapted from Holmes and Rahe (1967) administered at 12-month visit.
Consumers only; AA=absolute alcohol.
Although 54.9% of the mothers drank heavily at time of conception, these women did not drink on a daily basis but concentrated their drinking on 2.3 days/week at conception and 1.5 days/week across pregnancy. Among the drinkers, more than half were alcohol abusing or dependent; 12.9% met DSM-IV criteria for alcohol abuse, 38.8%, for alcohol dependence. Most (95.8%) of the 14 women reporting marijuana used it <3 days/month. A large majority smoked cigarettes, with 9.0% smoking an average of 10 or more cigarettes/day.
Relation of Infant Withdrawal to Other Measures of Affective Function and Potential Confounders
Mean (±SD) score on infant withdrawal was 3.4±3.2 (range=0-18), with 23 infants (27.1%) scoring above the threshold of 5 or more, placing them at risk for affective problems. Table 2 shows that, as expected, greater infant withdrawal was related to poorer mother-infant interaction, particularly maternal sensitivity and infant responsivity. The strongest association was the negative relation between withdrawal and infant responsivity. With regard to temperament, activity was positively related to quality of mother-infant interaction and negatively to infant withdrawal. The negative relation with withdrawal is not surprising given that reduced mobility is a feature of the withdrawal response. Withdrawal and mother-infant interaction were unrelated to the three other EAS measures of infant temperament—emotionality, sociability, and shyness. Nor were infant withdrawal or mother-interaction related to any of the temperament measures at 5 years.
Table 2.
Associations among infant withdrawal, mother-infant interaction, and infant temperament
Mother-infant interaction |
|||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
During free play |
During feeding |
Infant EAS temperament |
|||||||
Infant withdrawal | Maternal sensitivity |
Infant responsivity |
Quality of interaction |
Maternal sensitivity |
Emotionality | Activity | Sociability | Shyness | |
Infant withdrawal | -- | ||||||||
Mother-infant interaction | |||||||||
During free play | |||||||||
Maternal sensitivity | −.47*** (84) | -- | |||||||
Infant responsivity | −.56*** (84) | .66*** (127) | -- | ||||||
Quality of interaction | −.34** (84) | .71*** (127) | .71*** (127) | -- | |||||
During feeding | |||||||||
Maternal sensitivity | −.36*** (83) | .40*** (125) | .34*** (125) | .44*** (125) | -- | ||||
Infant EAS temperament | |||||||||
Emotionality | .08 (63) | −.07 (103) | −.04 (103) | .01 (103) | −.04 (102) | -- | |||
Activity | −.29* (63) | .29** (103) | .21* (103) | .12 (103) | .15 (102) | −.09 (119) | -- | ||
Sociability | −.12 (63) | .12 (103) | .15 (103) | .01 (103) | .02 (102) | −.22* (119) | .29** (119) | -- | |
Shyness | −.07 (63) | .06 (103) | −.01 (103) | .03 (103) | −.04 (102) | .47*** (119) | −.16† (119) | −.46*** (119) | -- |
Values are Pearson rs.
p<.10
p<.05
p<.01
p<.001
Infant withdrawal was not related to any of the sociodemographic measures except maternal education (Table 3). Nor was withdrawal related to maternal cognitive performance, depression, anxiety, life stress, or smoking or drug use during pregnancy. By contrast, quality of mother-infant interaction and EAS temperament measures were related to numerous socioenvironmental measures, including maternal SES, education, cognitive ability, depression, anxiety, and life stress. Regarding infant characteristics, withdrawal was related to lower birthweight and GA; sociability, to higher birthweight. Shyness was greater in girls; activity, in boys. Although iron deficiency anemia was not related to any of the infant affective outcomes, the continuous measure of iron deficiency was related to increased withdrawal and poorer maternal sensitivity, infant responsivity, and quality of interaction.
Table 3.
Relation of infant withdrawal, mother-infant interaction, and infant temperament with sociodemographic and other maternal and infant characteristics
Mother-infant interaction |
|||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
During free play |
During feeding |
Infant EAS temperament |
|||||||||||
Infant withdrawal | Maternal sensitivity |
Infant responsivity |
Quality of interaction |
Maternal sensitivity |
Emotionality | Activity | Sociability | Shyness | |||||
N | r | N | r | r | r | N | r | N | r | r | r | r | |
Maternal characteristics | |||||||||||||
Socioeconomic statusa |
85 | −.12 | 127 | .25** | .24** | .30*** | 127 | .14 | 119 | −.17† | .21* | .07 | −.11 |
Age at delivery | 85 | .02 | 127 | −.05 | −.04 | −.02 | 127 | .05 | 119 | .05 | −.12 | −.09 | .03 |
Parity | 85 | .04 | 127 | .01 | −.03 | −.05 | 127 | .04 | 119 | .09 | −.02 | −.06 | −.04 |
Education (yr) | 85 | −.20† | 127 | .19* | .15† | .18* | 127 | .11 | 119 | −.13 | .22* | .19* | −.14 |
Marital status | 85 | −.08 | 127 | .10 | .10 | .14 | 127 | .24** | 119 | −.09 | .01 | .10 | −.09 |
Raven scoreb | 85 | −.18† | 127 | .29*** | .30*** | .32*** | 127 | .20* | 119 | −.18† | .30*** | .19* | −.12 |
Depression | |||||||||||||
Beck depressionc | 85 | .16 | 127 | −.05 | −.09 | −.15† | 127 | −.17† | 119 | .23* | −.10 | −.21* | .06 |
Major depressiond | 85 | .001 | 127 | −.05 | .06 | −.004 | 127 | −.11 | 109 | .16 | .08 | −.06 | .02 |
Postpartum | |||||||||||||
Depressione | 85 | .02 | 126 | .07 | .002 | .07 | 126 | .06 | 108 | −.26** | −.06 | .03 | −.02 |
Anxiety | |||||||||||||
State anxietyf | 85 | .06 | 127 | −.13 | −.12 | −.14 | 127 | −.20* | 119 | .15 | −.09 | −.06 | .16† |
Trait anxietyf | 85 | .10 | 127 | .05 | −.09 | .000 | 127 | −.03 | 119 | .29** | −.01 | −.14 | .12 |
Life stressg | 62 | .12 | 102 | .06 | −.07 | .02 | 101 | −.02 | 116 | .25** | .09 | −.10 | −.02 |
Marijuana use (days/week) |
85 | .001 | 127 | −.05 | −.003 | −.01 | 127 | −.01 | 119 | .03 | .06 | .004 | .14 |
Cigarettes/day | 85 | .15 | 127 | .05 | −.04 | .11 | 127 | .03 | 119 | .13 | .05 | −.19* | .06 |
Infant characteristics | |||||||||||||
Genderh | 85 | .10 | 127 | .03 | .05 | .10 | 127 | .01 | 119 | .10 | −.16† | .03 | .19* |
Birth weight | 85 | −.22* | 127 | .06 | .07 | .02 | 127 | .13 | 119 | .05 | .07 | .15* | .05 |
Gestational age | 85 | −.21† | 127 | .13 | .06 | .12 | 127 | .15 | 119 | .12 | .09 | .15 | .02 |
Age at visit | 85 | −.06 | 127 | .03 | .02 | −.07 | 127 | −.12 | 119 | −.13 | −.15 | −.08 | −.03 |
Iron deficiency anemiai |
59 | .16 | 71 | −.17 | −.14 | −.10 | 70 | −.04 | 56 | .13 | −.13 | .17 | −.10 |
Iron deficiencJ | 78 | .37** | 95 | −.25* | −.25* | −.21* | 94 | −.10 | 73 | .19 | −.13 | .13 | .03 |
Values are Pearson rs.
p<.10
p<.05
p<.01
p<.001
Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of Social Status (Hollingshead, 1975).
Raven Progressive Matrices (Raven et al., 1996).
Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1996).
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) Major Depression Module.
Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (Cox et al., 1987).
State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger et al., 1983).
Adapted from Holmes and Rahe (1967).
1=male, 2=female.
Iron deficiency anemia diagnosed if the infant had hemoglobin (Hb) <10.9g/L and red cell distribution width (RDW) >15% plus mean corpuscular volume (MCV) <70.0 fL or mean corpuscular hemoglobin (MCH) <23.0 pg.
A continuous measure of degree of iron deficiency constructed by averaging z-scores for four indicators of iron deficiency (Hb, MCV, MCH, and RDW). The signs for Hg, MCV, and MCH were reversed so that a higher score indicates a greater degree of deficiency
Effects of Prenatal Alcohol Exposure on Infant Affective Outcomes
Fetal alcohol-exposed infants exhibited more withdrawal than controls, M±SD=4.2±3.6, compared to 2.6±2.5, respectively, t(83)=−2.33, p=.02. One-third (33.3%) of the exposed infants scored above the cut-point for risk for affective problems on infant withdrawal, compared with 18.6% of controls.
More frequent drinking at conception and during pregnancy was related to increased infant withdrawal after control for confounders (Table 4). By contrast, the relation of frequency of maternal postpartum alcohol use to withdrawal was not significant, ß=.19, p>.10, confirming that the alcohol effect on infant withdrawal is attributable to exposure in utero rather than a consequence of being raised by a drinking mother. After control for confounders, prenatal alcohol exposure was not related to any of the mother-infant interaction measures and was related only to decreased infant activity on the EAS. When the regressions were rerun omitting the five infants exposed to cocaine or methamphetamine, the effects were essentially unchanged.
Table 4.
Relation of prenatal alcohol exposure to infant withdrawal, mother-infant interaction, and infant temperament
Average alcohol/day |
Drinks/occasion |
Frequency |
|||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
At conception |
Across pregnancy |
At conception |
Across pregnancy |
At conception |
Across pregnancy |
||||||||
N | r | β | r | β | r | β | r | β | r | β | r | β | |
Infant withdrawala | 85 | .18† | .15 | .19† | .16 | .09 | .05 | .12 | .07 | .29** | .26* | .29** | .26* |
Mother-infant interaction | |||||||||||||
During free play | |||||||||||||
Maternal sensitivityb | 127 | −.11 | −.02 | −.12† | −.03 | −.12† | −.03 | −.15* | −.10 | −.10 | −.04 | −.12† | −.03 |
Infant responsivityb | 127 | −.15 | −.08* | −.19 | −.12* | −.20 | −.13* | −.16* | −.14 | −.11 | −.07 | −.17 | −.10* |
Quality of interactionc | 127 | −.07 | .06 | −.08 | .07 | −.11 | .01 | −.09 | −.03 | −.03 | .06 | −.07 | .07 |
During feeding | |||||||||||||
Maternal sensitivityd | 127 | −.19* | −.08 | −.19* | −.09 | −.17* | −.08 | −.08 | .02 | −.17* | −.08 | −.20* | −.10 |
Infant EAS Temperament | |||||||||||||
Emotionalitye | 105 | .16† | .03 | .20* | .07 | .18* | .07 | .16* | .06 | .14† | .04 | .14† | .03 |
Activityf | 119 | −.26** | −.16† | −.24 | −.13 | −.28** | −.20* | −.28** | −.23* | −.20* | −.12 | −.22 | −.12 |
Sociabilityg | 119 | −.09 | .07 | −.11 | .09 | −.12 | −.01 | −.16* | −.05 | −.09 | .05 | −.10 | .10 |
Shynessh | 119 | .04 | −.01 | .05 | .02 | .09 | .05 | .05 | .02 | .02 | −.03 | .01 | −.02 |
p < .10
p < .05
p < .01
Confounders: Mother’s education, Raven score, birthweight, gestational age.
Confounders: Mother’s education, socioeconomic status, Raven score.
Confounders: Mother’s education, socioeconomic status, Raven score, Beck depression score.
Confounders: Raven score, Beck depression score, state anxiety, marital status.
Confounders: Socioeconomic status, Raven score, life stress, Beck depression score, postpartum depression, trait anxiety.
Confounders: Mother’s education, socioeconomic status, Raven score, gender.
Confounders: Mother’s education, Raven score, Beck depression score, pregnancy smoking, birthweight.
Confounders: State anxiety, gender.
Additional analyses were conducted to assess whether the effect of alcohol on withdrawal was mediated by its effects on iron deficiency. When frequency of maternal drinking during pregnancy and degree of iron deficiency were entered simultaneously in a multiple regression analysis, controlling for confounders, the effects of these exposures were independent: maternal drinking, ß=.30, p<.01; iron deficiency, ß=.36, p<.01, indicating that both prenatal alcohol exposure and iron deficiency were related to infant withdrawal. The same effects were seen in relation to alcohol use at time of conception.
Relation of Infant Measures to FAS Diagnosis and Childhood Cognitive and Affective Outcomes
Except in the most severe cases, it is difficult to diagnose FAS or PFAS during infancy. We, therefore, examined the degree to which infant withdrawal is already apparent in alcohol-exposed infants in an analysis of variance, comparing three groups diagnosed at 5 years— FAS/PFAS (n=18), heavily exposed (HE) non-syndromal (n=24), and non- or minimally exposed controls (n=38). There was a main effect of diagnostic group on infant withdrawal, F(2,77)=3.34, p<.05. In post hoc tests, the FAS/PFAS group scored higher than both the HE group (p=.06) and controls, p<.01, suggesting that affective withdrawal can already be seen in infancy in the more severely affected children. There were no differences on infant withdrawal between the HE and control groups, p>.20.
Among the mother-infant interaction ratings, only infant responsivity was related to FASD diagnosis, F(2,117)=7.41, p<.001, with the FAS/PFAS group (n=30) showing less responsivity than the HE (n=41) and controls (n=49), ps<.002 and .001, respectively. There were main effects of FASD diagnostic group on 13-month EAS infant activity and sociability, F(2,113)=3.78 and F(2,113)=8.49, ps<.05 and .001, respectively, and an association that fell short of significance for emotionality, F(2,113)=2.36, p<.10. Infants with FAS/PFAS (n=32) were more irritable (p<.05) and less active (p<.05) and sociable (p<.01) than controls (n=39). No differences were found for HE (n=43) vs. controls, all ps>.15. The combination of infant outcomes that best predicted an FAS/PFAS diagnosis was examined in a multiple regression analysis with FAS/PFAS vs. control group as the outcome measure. Infant withdrawal, responsivity, and sociability jointly explained 23.6% of the variance in this diagnosis, F(3,37)=3.80, p=.018.
Infant withdrawal predicted fewer happy faces on the DAP (Table 5). The only other predictor of happy faces was greater maternal sensitivity during free play. When infant withdrawal and maternal sensitivity were entered simultaneously in a regression predicting happy faces, only the withdrawal effect was significant, β=−.26, p<.05; the sensitivity effect was not, β=.04, p>.20. None of the 10 children diagnosed with FAS drew a happy face, confirming a similar finding of an association between prenatal alcohol use and fewer happy faces in our Detroit Longitudinal Cohort at 7.5 years (Corobana et al., 2003).
Infant emotional withdrawal also predicted poorer IQ on the WISC-IV at 5 and 9 years; infant responsivity, quality of interaction, and maternal sensitivity were related to higher IQ at both ages; and more infant activity predicted higher 9-year IQ. When all four infant affective measures were included together in a multiple regression analysis, only the relation with infant emotional withdrawal was significant, β=−.34, t=−2.05, p<.05; none of the other βs were significant, all ps>.20.
Discussion
This study is the first to identify infant withdrawal as an affective endpoint seen early in development that is sensitive to prenatal alcohol exposure, even after control for demographic measures and maternal emotional function, smoking, and postpartum drinking. Although poorer maternal sensitivity during mother-infant interaction was also associated with increased infant withdrawal, fetal alcohol exposure contributed independently to infant withdrawal over and above the contribution of maternal sensitivity. Our findings are consistent with previous studies showing a relation between maternal non-contingent responsiveness and more sustained infant withdrawal (Cohn and Tronick, 1983; Field, 1984). The finding that infant emotional withdrawal was even more strongly related to infant responsivity than to maternal sensitivity suggests that fetal alcohol-related withdrawal may also contribute to reduced sensitivity by the caregiver.
By contrast to mother-infant interaction and the temperament measures, infant withdrawal was largely unrelated to socioenvironmental factors, including maternal depression, anxiety, and life stress. In most populations, maternal depression is associated with poorer cognitive and socioemotional development in children (e.g., Murray and Cooper, 1997). The absence of a link between maternal depression and infant withdrawal in the current study is consistent with reports from two recent studies of socially disadvantaged Australian infants (Bookless, 2004; Matthey et al., 2005), in which maternal postpartum depression was also prevalent. The absence of this association may be attributable to the social interaction provided to infants by extended family members, especially in poor families living together. Alternatively, the absence of this association in this sample suggests that infant withdrawal may reflect a neurological impairment that is independent of the postpartum social environment and, in this case, attributable to fetal alcohol exposure.
Infant emotional withdrawal was negatively related to interactions during both free play and feeding, confirming the expected relation between mother-infant interaction and infant withdrawal. Nevertheless, none of the mother-infant relationship measures were predicted by prenatal alcohol exposure. The principal aspect of temperament associated with prenatal alcohol exposure was decreased activity level. Lower activity in the more heavily exposed infants reported by the mothers at 13 months are consistent with the blunted affective response seen in alcohol-exposed newborns (e.g., Oberlander et al., 2010; Streissguth et al., 1983). Moreover, the potential utility of infant activity as an early indicator of later temperament was demonstrated by its moderate association with EAS activity at 5 years.
The finding that infant emotional withdrawal is sensitive to iron deficiency provides further evidence of the validity of this measure since iron deficiency has been shown to be related to depressed infant affect (Lozoff et al., 1998; Carter et al., 2010). Consistent with our findings of increased infant withdrawal, iron deficient infants in Chile were less likely to show positive affect, interact socially, or to check caregiver reactions (Lozoff et al., 2003). Similarly, in a Detroit inner-city study conducted in our laboratory, iron deficient infants exhibited lower levels of positive affect and social engagement (Lozoff et al., 2008), poorer attention to photos of their mothers and other women (Burden et al., 2007), and poorer orientation and engagement when administered a cognitive task (Carter et al., 2010). As noted above, we found that prenatal alcohol exposure and iron deficiency were each independently associated with higher levels of infant withdrawal.
It is noteworthy that infant affective measures discriminated between infants subsequently diagnosed with FAS or PFAS, indicating that the affective components of these disorders are already manifest early in development. Moreover, although withdrawal, responsivity, quality of interaction, and maternal sensitivity were each predictive of higher 9-year IQ, emotional withdrawal was the strongest predictor, underscoring the importance of this feature of FASD.
In conclusion, this is the first study to link fetal alcohol exposure directly to an affective endpoint that emerges early in development, well before higher cognitive or executive function. This finding suggests that affective problems that have been described in later childhood, adolescence, and adulthood in FASD are not secondary to deficits in attention or higher order cognition. DAP, an innovative measure of childhood depression or mood disorder (Tharinger and Stark, 1990) was also predicted by the infant withdrawal measure, further supporting the hypothesis that infant withdrawal provides an early indicator of fetal alcohol-related affective disturbance. It is noteworthy that not a single child diagnosed with FAS drew a happy face at the 5-year assessment. Our analysis of the fetal alcohol diagnostic data suggests that infant affective withdrawal is particularly prevalent among the most severely affected children and predictive of later affective and cognitive outcomes. Moreover, the link between prenatal alcohol exposure and infant withdrawal was not attributable to postpartum maternal drinking, quality of mother-infant interaction, or maternal life stress, anxiety, or depression. Given the apparently direct link between exposure to alcohol in utero and infant withdrawal, these data suggest that affective withdrawal may constitute an early manifestation of an important aspect of the extensive socioemotional problems that characterize fetal alcohol-related disorders.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Recruitment of the Cape Town longitudinal cohort was funded by two administrative supplements to NIAAA R01 AA09524 (SWJ), the NIH Office of Research on Minority Health, and the Foundation for Alcohol Related Research, Cape Town, South Africa. The 5-year follow-up assessments of the Cape Town longitudinal cohort were funded by U01 AA014790 to SWJ in conjunction with the NIAAA Collaborative Initiative on Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder (CIFASD). The 2005 dysmorphology assessments were funded, in part, by U01 AA014790 (SWJ) and U24 AA014815 (KL Jones) from CIFASD. This research received supplemental funding from the Joseph Young, Sr., Fund from the State of Michigan (SWJ and JLJ). Portions of this research were presented at the 2012 meetings of the World Association on Infant Mental Health, Cape Town, South Africa, and the Research Society on Alcoholism, San Francisco, CA. We acknowledge the major contribution of the late Andrea Hay during the infant phase of this study. We thank Denis Viljoen, Maggie September, Julie Croxford, Anna Susan Marais, and Renee Sun, for their contributions to this study, and the three dysmorphologists, H. Eugene Hoyme, Luther Robinson, and Nathaniel Khaole, who examined the children. We also thank the mothers and children in the UCT longitudinal cohort for their continuing participation and contribution to the study.
Footnotes
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
References
- Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile. University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; Burlington, VT: 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Astley SJ, Clarren SK. Measuring the facial phenotype of individuals with prenatal alcohol exposure: correlations with brain dysfunction. Alcohol Alcohol. 2001;36:147–159. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/36.2.147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck Depression Inventory 11. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Behrman RE, Vaughan VC, Nelson WE. Nelson Textbook of Pediatrics. 13th edition WB Saunders; Philadelphia: 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Bookless C. A longitudinal study examining factors associated with withdrawal in infants, in The use of the Alarm Distress Baby Scale (ADBB) (Guedeney A chair); Symposium conducted at the meeting of the World Association for Infant Mental Health; Melbourne, Australia. 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Bowman RS, Stein LI, Newton JR. Measurement and interpretation of drinking behavior. Q J Stud Alcohol. 1975;36:1154–1172. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1975.36.1154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burden MJ, Westerlund AJ, Armony-Sivan R, Nelson CA, Jacobson SW, Lozoff B, Angelilli ML, Jacobson JL. An event-related potential study of maternal face recognition in infants with iron deficiency anemia. Pediatr. 2007;120:e336–e345. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-2525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buss AH, Plomin R. Temperament: Early Developing Personality Traits. Lawrence Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Carmichael Olson H, Streissguth AP, Sampson PD, Barr H, Bookstein FL, Thiede K. Association of prenatal alcohol exposure with behavioral and learning problems in early adolescence. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr. 1997;36:1187–1194. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199709000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter RC, Jacobson SW, Molteno CD, Jacobson JL. Fetal alcohol exposure, iron deficiency, and infant growth. Pediatr. 2007;120:559–567. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-0151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter RC, Jacobson JL, Burden MJ, Armony-Sivan R, Dodge NC, Angelilli ML, Lozoff B, Jacobson SW. Iron deficiency and cognitive function in infancy. Pediatr. 2010;126:e427–434. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-2097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohn JF, Tronick EZ. Three months old infant’s reaction to simulated maternal depression. Child Dev. 1983;54:334–335. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corobana R, Quigley C, Yumoto C, Dietrich R, Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW. Impact of maternal alcohol use on child depression at 7.5 years. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2003;27:137A. [Google Scholar]
- Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression: Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression scale. Br J Psychiatr. 1987;160:782–786. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Field TM. Early interactions between infants and their postpartum depressed mothers. Infant Behav Dev. 1984;7:517–522. [Google Scholar]
- Guedeney A. From early withdrawal reaction to infant depression. A baby alone does exist. Infant Ment Health J. 1997;18:339–349. [Google Scholar]
- Guedeney A, Fermanian J. A validity and reliability study of assessment and screening for sustained withdrawal reaction in infancy: The Alarm Distress Baby Scale. Infant Ment Health J. 2001;22:559–575. [Google Scholar]
- Harris DB. Children’s drawings as measures of intellectual maturity. Harcourt, Brace & World, Inc.; New York: 1963. [Google Scholar]
- Hollingshead AB. Four-factor Index of Social Status. Yale University; New Haven, CT: 1975. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Holmes TH, Rahe RH. The Social Readjustment Rating Scale. J Psychosomatic Res. 1967;11:213–218. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(67)90010-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoyme HE, May PA, Kalberg WO, Kodituwakku P, Gossage JP, Trujillo PM, Buckley DG, Miller JH, Aragon AS, Khaole N, Viljoen DL, Jones KL, Robinson LK. A practical clinical approach to diagnosis of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: Clarification of the 1996 Institute of Medicine criteria. Pediatr. 2005;115:39–47. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, Molteno CD, Odendaal H. A prospective examination of the incidence of heavy drinking during pregnancy among Cape Colored South African women. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2006;30:233A. [Google Scholar]
- Jacobson SW, Chiodo LM, Sokol RJ, Jacobson JL. Validity of maternal report of prenatal alcohol, cocaine, and smoking in relation to neurobehavioral outcome. Pediatr. 2002;109:815–825. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.5.815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobson SW, Carr LG, Croxford J, Sokol RJ, Li T-K, Jacobson JL. Protective effects of the alcohol dehydrogenase-ADH1B allele in African American children exposed to alcohol during pregnancy. J Pediatr. 2006;148:30–37. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2005.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobson SW, Stanton ME, Molteno CD, Burden MJ, Fuller DS, Hoyme HE, Robinson LK, Khaole N, Jacobson JL. Impaired eyeblink conditioning in children with fetal alcohol syndrome. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32:365–372. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00585.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopes S. The Distress Alarm Baby Scale: A validity and reliability study, in The use of the Alarm Distress Baby Scale (ADBB) (Guedeney A, chair); Symposium conducted at the meeting of the World Association for Infant Mental Health; Melbourne, Australia. 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Lozoff B, Klein NK, Nelson EC, McClish DK, Manuel M, Chacon ME. Behavior of infants with iron-deficiency anemia. Child Dev. 1998;69:24–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lozoff B, De Andraca I, Castillo M, Smith JB, Walter T, Pino P. Behavioral and developmental effects of preventing iron-deficiency anemia in healthy full-term infants. Pediatr. 2003;112:846–854. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lozoff B, Clark KM, Jing Y, Armony-Sivan R, Angelilli ML, Jacobson SW. Dose-response relations between iron deficiency with or without anemia and infant social-emotional behavior. J Pediatr. 2008;152:696–702. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.09.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyons-Ruth K, Alpern L, Repacholi B. Disorganized infant attachment classification and maternal psychosocial problems as predictors of hostile-aggressive behavior in the preschool classroom. Child Dev. 1993;64:572–585. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02929.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madge EM, van den Berg AR, Robinson M, Landman J. Junior South African Individual Scales. Human Sciences Research Council; Pretoria: 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Main M, Solomon J. Discovery of an insecure disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern: Procedures, findings and implications for the classification of behavior. In: Yogman M, Brazelton TB, editors. Affective development in infancy. Ablex; Norwood, NJ: 1986. pp. 95–124. [Google Scholar]
- Matthey S, Guedeney A, Starakis N, Barnett B. Assessing the social behavior of infants: Use of the ADBB scale and relationship to mother’s mood. Infant Ment Health J. 2005;26:442–458. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- May PA, Brooke L, Gossage JP, Croxford J, Adnams C, Jones KL, Robinson L, Viljoen D. Epidemiology of fetal alcohol syndrome in a South African community in the Western Cape Province. Am J Pub Health. 2000;90:1905–1912. doi: 10.2105/ajph.90.12.1905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- May PA, Gossage JP, Marais AS, Adnams C, Hoyme HE, Jones KL, Robinson LK, Khaole N, Snell C, Kalberg WO, Hendricks L, Brooke L, Stellavato C, Viljoen D. The epidemiology of fetal alcohol syndrome and partial FAS in a South African community. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;88:259–271. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molteno CD, Tomlinson M. A coding scheme for mother-infant interaction. 1997. Unpublished. [Google Scholar]
- Molteno CD, Jacobson SW, Carter RC, Jacobson JL. Infant symbolic play as an early indicator of fetal alcohol-related deficit. Infancy. 2010;15:586–607. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00031.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray L, Cooper PJ. Postpartum depression and child development. Psychol Med. 1997;27:253–260. doi: 10.1017/s0033291796004564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oberlander TF, Jacobson SW, Weinberg J, Grunau RE, Molteno CD, Jacobson JL. Prenatal alcohol exposure alters biobehavioral reactivity to pain in newborns. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2010;34:681–692. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01137.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor MJ, Shah B, Whaley S, Cronin P, Gunderson B, Graham J. Psychiatric illness in a clinical sample of children with prenatal alcohol exposure. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2002;28:743–754. doi: 10.1081/ada-120015880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powell GF, Bettes BA. Infantile depression, non organic failure to thrive and DSM-II-R: A different perspective. Child Psychiatr Human Dev. 1992;22:185–198. doi: 10.1007/BF00705891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puura K. The Alarm Baby Distress Scale in assessment of two-month-old infants, in The use of the Alarm Baby Distress Scale (ADBB) (Guedeney A chair); Symposium conducted at the meetings of the World Association for Infant Mental health; Melbourne, Australia. 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Raven JC. Raven Progressive Matrices. Oxford Psychologist’s Press; Oxford: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Sood B, Delaney-Black V, Covington C, Nordstrom-Klee B, Ager J, Templin T, Janisse J, Martier S, Sokol RJ. Prenatal alcohol exposure and childhood behavior at age 6 to 7 years: I. Dose-response effect. Pediatr. 2001;108:e34. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.2.e34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press; Palo Alto, CA: 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Spitz RA. Anaclitic depression. Psychoanalytic Stud Child. 1946;2:313–341. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Streissguth AP, Barr HM, Kogan J, Bookstein FL. Final Report: Understanding the Occurrence of Secondary Disabilities in Clients with Fetal Alcohol Syndrome (FAS) and Fetal Alcohol Effects (FAE) University of Washington Publication Services; Seattle, WA: 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Streissguth AP, Barr HM, Martin DC. Maternal alcohol use and neonatal habituation assessed with the Brazelton scale. Child Dev. 1983;54:1109–1118. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tharinger DI, Stark K. A qualitative versus quantitative approach to evaluating the Draw-A-Person and Kinetic Family Drawing: A study of mood- and anxiety-disorder children. Psychol Assessment: J Consulting Clin Psychol. 1990;2:365–375. [Google Scholar]
- Weinberg MK, Tronick EZ. Beyond the face: An empirical study of infant affective configurations of facial, vocal, gestural and regulatory behaviors. Child Dev. 1994;65:1503–1515. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00832.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]