Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2013 Dec 9;110(52):21177–21182. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1316885110

Restoration of function after brain damage using a neural prosthesis

David J Guggenmos a,b,1, Meysam Azin c,2, Scott Barbay a,b, Jonathan D Mahnken d, Caleb Dunham a,b, Pedram Mohseni c, Randolph J Nudo a,b,3
PMCID: PMC3876197  PMID: 24324155

Significance

Closed-loop systems, or brain–machine–brain interfaces (BMBIs), have not been widely developed for brain repair. In this study, we targeted spared motor and somatosensory regions of the rat brain after traumatic brain injury for establishment of a functional bridge using a battery-powered microdevice. The results show that by using discriminated action potentials as a trigger for stimulating a distant cortical location, rapid recovery of fine motor skills is facilitated. This study provides strong evidence that BMBIs can be used to bridge damaged neural pathways functionally and promote recovery after brain injury. Although this study is restricted to a rodent model of TBI, it is likely that the approach will also be applicable to other types of acquired brain injuries.

Keywords: brain–machine–brain interface, neural plasticity, traumatic brain injury, closed-loop, long-term potentiation

Abstract

Neural interface systems are becoming increasingly more feasible for brain repair strategies. This paper tests the hypothesis that recovery after brain injury can be facilitated by a neural prosthesis serving as a communication link between distant locations in the cerebral cortex. The primary motor area in the cerebral cortex was injured in a rat model of focal brain injury, disrupting communication between motor and somatosensory areas and resulting in impaired reaching and grasping abilities. After implantation of microelectrodes in cerebral cortex, a neural prosthesis discriminated action potentials (spikes) in premotor cortex that triggered electrical stimulation in somatosensory cortex continuously over subsequent weeks. Within 1 wk, while receiving spike-triggered stimulation, rats showed substantially improved reaching and grasping functions that were indistinguishable from prelesion levels by 2 wk. Post hoc analysis of the spikes evoked by the stimulation provides compelling evidence that the neural prosthesis enhanced functional connectivity between the two target areas. This proof-of-concept study demonstrates that neural interface systems can be used effectively to bridge damaged neural pathways functionally and promote recovery after brain injury.


The view of the brain as a collection of independent anatomical modules, each with discrete functions, is currently undergoing radical change. New evidence from neurophysiological and neuroanatomical experiments in animals, as well as neuroimaging studies in humans, now suggests that normal brain function can be best appreciated in the context of the complex arrangements of functional and structural interconnections among brain areas. Although mechanistic details are still under refinement, synchronous discharge of neurons in widespread areas of the cerebral cortex appears to be an emergent property of neuronal networks that functionally couple remote locations (1). It is now recognized that not only are discrete regions of the brain damaged in injury or disease but, perhaps more importantly, the interconnections among uninjured areas are disrupted, potentially leading to many of the functional impairments that persist after brain injury (2). Likewise, plasticity of brain interconnections may partially underlie recovery of function after injury (3).

Technological efforts to restore brain function after injury have focused primarily on modulating the excitability of focal regions in uninjured parts of the brain (4). Purportedly, increasing the excitability of neurons involved in adaptive plasticity expands the neural substrate potentially involved in functional recovery. However, no methods are yet available to alter the functional connectivity between spared brain regions directly, with the intent to restore normal communication patterns. The present paper tests the hypothesis that an artificial communication link between uninjured regions of the cerebral cortex can restore function in a rodent model of traumatic brain injury (TBI). Development of such neuroprosthetic approaches to brain repair may have important implications for the millions of individuals who are left with permanent motor and cognitive impairments after acquired brain injury, as occurs in stroke and trauma.

For the present experiment, we used a rodent model of focal brain injury to the caudal forelimb area (CFA), a region that is part of the cortical sensorimotor system. This area in the frontal cortex shares many properties with primary motor cortex (M1) of primates; injury to M1 results in long-term impairment in reaching and grasping functions (5). Traditionally, it has been thought that impairment occurs because M1 provides substantial outputs to the motor apparatus in the spinal cord, thus directly affecting motor output function. However, M1 also has important interconnections with the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) located in the parietal lobe (Fig. 1A). Long-range corticocortical fibers from S1 provide critical information to M1 about the position of the limb in space. Thus, injury to M1 results in impaired motor performance due, at least in part, to disruption in communication between the somatosensory and motor cortex (6).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Theoretical model of neuroprosthetic treatment approach after brain injury. (A) Normal connectivity of M1, S1, and PM. Both M1 (CFA in rat) and PM (RFA in rat) send substantial outputs to the spinal cord via the corticospinal tract. Also, extensive reciprocal connections exist between M1 and PM, as well as between M1 and S1. (B) Effects of focal M1 injury on brain connectivity and the hypothetical effect of a BMBI to restore somatosensory-motor communication. An injury to M1, as might occur in stroke or brain trauma, results in a focal area of necrosis, as well as loss of M1 outputs to the spinal cord. Corticocortical communication between M1 and S1 (and between M1 and PM) is also disrupted, further contributing to functional impairment. Because the uninjured PM also contains corticospinal neurons, it might have the ability to serve in a vicarious role. The dotted line indicates enhanced functional connection between PM and S1 that we propose is established after treatment with a BMBI. (C) Location of target areas in rat cerebral cortex. A topographic map of the somatosensory representation in S1 is superimposed on the cortex.

To test our hypothesis that functional recovery can be facilitated by creating an artificial communication link between spared somatosensory and motor regions of the brain, we focused on the rat’s premotor cortex (PM). The rostral forelimb area (RFA) is a premotor area in the rodent’s frontal cortex that shares many properties with PM of primates and is thought to participate in recovery of function after injury to M1 (5, 79). PM areas are so-named because the principal target of their output fibers is M1 (10). PM areas also have long-range corticocortical connections with somatosensory areas, but at least in intact animals, they appear to be relatively weak compared with M1’s connections with the somatosensory cortex (9, 11, 12).

Our approach was to link the neural activity of the PM forelimb area (RFA) functionally with activation of the S1 forelimb area following a controlled cortical impact (CCI) to M1 (Fig. 1 B and C). To this end, a microdevice was developed with the ability to deliver activity-dependent stimulation (ADS) through recording and digitizing extracellular neural activity from an implanted microelectrode, discriminating individual action potentials (spikes), and delivering small amounts of electrical current to another microelectrode implanted in a distant population of neurons (13, 14). This closed-loop system was similar, in principle, to the “Neurochip” used previously by other investigators to demonstrate the effects of local ADS in intact animals (15), but it was miniaturized for head-mounted, wireless operation (Fig. 2A and Fig. S1). By linking the activity of one area of the cortex with that of a distant area of the cortex, a closed-loop brain–machine–brain interface (BMBI) for artificial corticocortical communication between PM and S1 was created.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

ADS protocol. After injury to the CFA, a recording microelectrode was placed in the RFA, whereas a stimulating microelectrode was placed in the distal forelimb field of S1. A BMBI discriminated action potentials in the RFA, and after a 7.5-ms delay, it delivered a low-level electrical current pulse to S1 (13). (A) Sketch of a rat retrieving a food pellet with a BMBI attached to the skull. (B) Sample traces of recordings from the RFA showing action potentials and stimulus artifacts from an ICMS current delivered to S1. Time-amplitude window discriminators are indicated by red boxes. A total of 100 superimposed traces are shown.

Individual spikes were detected in PM, and subsequent stimulation was delivered to S1 after a 7.5-ms delay (Fig. 2B). (Because connections between distant cortical areas are commonly reciprocal, enhanced communication theoretically could be established by ADS in either direction.) After the M1 injury, rats were implanted with microelectrodes connected to the BMBI microdevice (Fig. 2A). The microdevice delivered ADS 24 h per day up to 28 d postinjury, except for brief motor assessment sessions on predetermined days. Behavioral recovery in ADS rats was compared with recovery in rats with open-loop stimulation (OLS), in which S1 stimulation was uncorrelated with spikes in PM, and with control rats that had no microdevice implanted.

Results

Testing Motor Skill After Brain Injury.

The primary behavioral assay for determining whether ADS resulted in functional improvement after brain injury was a skilled reaching task. This widely used task is a particularly sensitive measure of forelimb motor function after M1 lesions in both rodents and primates. Rats were pretrained to achieve a minimum criterion score of >70% successful pellet retrievals. After the lesion was created, rats were tested on the task during assessment sessions on postlesion days 3, 5, 8, 14, 21, and 28. During each postlesion assessment session, rats were tested under two conditions: first with the microdevice stimulation function turned OFF and then with the stimulation function turned ON. Rats in each of the three groups demonstrated a severe deficit on the skilled reaching task in the first few days after the injury (Fig. 3). On postlesion days 3 and 5, there were no significant differences in motor performance between the groups (global comparisons: P = 0.5265 and P = 0.0945, respectively). Rats in the control group (with a lesion but no microdevice) continued to demonstrate a profound deficit that plateaued at only about 25% successful retrievals. In striking contrast, by postlesion day 8, group performance was significantly different (global comparison: P = 0.0044). Rats in the ADS group showed a substantial and statistically significant behavioral improvement in reaching success compared with rats in the other groups in the ON condition (pairwise comparisons: P = 0.0418 for ADS vs. OLS, P = 0.0012 for ADS vs. control, and P = 0.2110 for OLS vs. control; Fig. 3 and Movies S1 and S2). By postlesion day 14, the performance of the rats in the ADS group was approximately at prelesion levels and significantly higher than that of rats in the other groups. The difference between the OLS group and the control group approached significance on day 14 (global comparison: P = 0.0004; pairwise comparisons: P = 0.0284 for ADS vs. OLS, P < 0.0001 for ADS vs. control, and P = 0.0555 for OLS vs. control). By postlesion day 21, performance in the ADS group remained high and statistically different from that of the control group. Performance was not significantly different in the ADS group between days 14 and 21 (P = 0.576). However, by day 21, the OLS group had improved further, so that the difference between the two groups was not significant (global comparison: P = 0.0007; pairwise comparisons: P = 0.0891 for ADS vs. OLS, P = 0.0002 for ADS vs. control, and P = 0.0278 for OLS vs. control). Although the mean performance of the ADS group was higher than that of the OLS group even in the OFF condition, differences were not statistically significant on any postlesion day (Fig. S2).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Performance of rats on a skilled reaching task after injury to M1 (ON condition). The ADS group is shown in red, the OLS group is shown in blue, and the control group is shown in black. The dotted line indicates the average prelesion performance of all animals in the study. The bounded area indicates the 95% confidence interval. Regression lines are based on an LMM (43). Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. *P < 0.05 (pairwise difference between the ADS and OLS groups). Because the statistical analysis was an intent-to-treat model, rats were included in the analysis even if the microdevice was no longer functional. Only one rat in the ADS group had a microdevice that was functional by postlesion day 28; thus, figures are presented through postlesion day 21 (SI Results). Diamonds, squares, and triangles represent individual animal data points. #, microdevice not functional (Tables S1 and S2).

Immediate Effects Within Single Sessions.

Rats in the ADS group often showed substantially improved performance within a single day’s session when the microdevice was switched from the OFF to the ON condition. One particularly salient example can be seen in a video of a rat in the ADS group on postlesion day 8 (Movie S2). In the OFF condition, this rat made many attempts to reach through the opening in the Plexiglas but was rarely able to do so accurately. Large trajectory errors were made, and relatively few retrievals were completed successfully. Following completion of trials in the OFF condition, the microdevice was programmed to the ON state, a process that required 2–3 min. As soon as the microdevice was turned ON, the rat began to retrieve pellets with noticeably enhanced success. Movements tended to be slower and seemingly more deliberate, and fewer errors were made. A statistical analysis of the ADS group between the OFF and ON conditions revealed significantly better performance in the ON condition on postlesion day 3 (P = 0.0003), postlesion day 5 (P = 0.0005), and postlesion day 8 (P = 0.0019) and marginally better performance on postlesion day 14 (P = 0.0666). The same analysis for the OLS group revealed significantly worse performance in the ON condition on postlesion day 3 (P = 0.0471) and marginally worse performance on postlesion day 5 (P = 0.0554) and postlesion day 8 (P = 0.0781) (Fig. S3). These effects tended to dissipate over time, so that no differences were detected between OFF and ON conditions in either group by postlesion day 21. These within-day differences through postlesion day 8 suggest that the timing of the S1 stimulus pulse is critical. Behavioral performance was significantly better when the S1 stimulus pulse was delivered contingent upon an action potential in the RFA (i.e., in the ADS group).

Effects of ADS on Functional Connectivity.

To explore possible neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the behavioral effects of the ADS treatment on postinjury motor performance, we performed post hoc analysis of spike events in the RFA that were discriminated in the 28 ms after each S1 stimulus pulse. This time window represented our imposed blanking period during which additional S1 stimulus pulses could not occur. Poststimulus spike histograms were compared with 28-ms periods chosen from data acquired in the OFF condition 7.5 ms after each RFA spike event. The results show that substantially more spikes in the RFA occurred following S1 stimulation in the ADS group, with peak activity occurring ∼4–6 ms after the S1 stimulus pulse (Fig. 4A). Spike rates were nearly threefold higher averaged across the 28-ms period compared with a comparable period in the OFF condition. Spike rates in the OLS group were slightly lower than in the ADS group in the OFF condition but were significantly lower than in the ADS group in the ON condition. These data suggest that ADS substantially reinforced network interactions between S1 and the RFA, whereas OLS did not.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Comparison of spike activity in the RFA in the ADS and OLS groups. Data represent spikes discriminated in the RFA over a 28-ms period. In the ON condition, the trigger for the data acquisition was the S1 stimulus pulse. In the OFF condition, the trigger for the data acquisition was 7.5 ms after a spike event in the RFA. (A) Composite posttrigger spiking histograms derived from neural recordings in the RFA compiled from days 1, 5, 8, 14, and 21 (±1 d). Histograms portray the mean spike counts per trigger event within each time bin (also Fig. S4). Spike counts were based on an average of over 22,000 trigger events per animal per day. Poststimulus firing rates were substantially higher in the ADS ON condition (33.1 Hz), compared with the ADS OFF (12.5 Hz), OLS ON (6.6 Hz), or OLS OFF (10.1 Hz) condition. (B) Average spike firing rates throughout the 28-ms window for each day. Error bars represent between-subject variation on each day (plus 1 SD). LMMs detected higher firing rates in the ADS group compared with the OLS group with stimulation ON (P < 0.0001). Firing rates did not differ statistically between groups in the OFF condition (P > 0.05). Posttrigger spiking histograms for each day are shown in Fig. S4.

Subdividing the spike histograms by day reveals that enhanced spike activity in the ADS ON condition is evident even on the first day that the microdevice was activated (Fig. 4B and Fig. S4). There is also a trend toward further increases in spike discharge between the first (days 1 and 5) and second (days 8 and 14) weeks in the ADS group, corresponding to the time period when behavioral performance approached normal levels.

Whether behavioral performance and enhanced functional connectivity persist following the end of treatment cannot be addressed fully based on the current results (SI Discussion). However, it is noteworthy that there was a significant decrease in mean performance in the ADS group between postinjury days 21 and 28 (Fig. S5). During this time period, microelectrode-microdevice connection failures prevented normal operation of the microdevice in most of the ADS rats. This phenomenon of reduced behavioral performance after deactivation provides further support for the notion that the behavioral improvements were mediated by closed-loop operation. It also suggests that either a longer duration of operation (i.e., beyond 21 d) is required for persistent effects or that closed-loop stimulation enhances the rate, but not the extent, of recovery compared with OLS. Nonetheless, the present data provide persuasive evidence that targeted closed-loop stimulation approaches are feasible as brain repair strategies. Rapid behavioral recovery parallels the development of increased functional connectivity between spared somatosensory and motor regions of the cortex.

Discussion

This proof-of-concept study indicates that a closed-loop neuroprosthetic microdevice can enhance functional connectivity between distant cortical locations and generate rapid improvement in motor function after cortical injury, at least in rats with M1 damage. A closed-loop device with similar functionality induced neurophysiological changes when applied over a short distance within M1 of intact monkeys (15). More recently, spike-triggered stimulation was used to demonstrate increased potentiation between neurons in the sensorimotor cortex of rats. The spike-stimulation delay was important, because 5 ms resulted in robust increases, whereas 100 or 500 ms resulted in no potentiation (16). The present study demonstrates that the extension of the ADS approach to injured brains has demonstrable effects on recovery and establishes functional communication that is qualitatively different compared with uncorrelated stimulation. The current implementation of the system architecture, using a lightweight, battery-powered, wireless, miniaturized microdevice for spike-triggered intracortical microstimulation (ICMS), represents an important step in the process of developing implantable BMBIs for neural repair in clinical populations.

Differential Mechanisms Underlying the Effects of OLS and ADS on Behavioral Recovery.

The mechanisms underlying the therapeutic effects of OLS and ADS after injury in the present model of TBI are still somewhat speculative. In the 1940s, Donald Hebb (17) postulated that “When one cell repeatedly assists in firing another, the axon of the first cell develops synaptic knobs… in contact with the soma of the second cell.” This hypothesis has morphed into the modern maxim “Cells that fire together, wire together,” a phrase made popular by neuroscientist, Carla Shatz (18). A large literature has grown from these initial hypotheses, and a neurophysiological phenomenon widely known as “Hebbian plasticity” has formed the basis for many neuroscientific models of learning and memory. Previous studies in intact primates and rodents using ADS or paired-pulse stimulation show the ability for such coactivation to alter output properties of cortical neurons (15, 16, 19). Presumably, the stimulation causes Hebbian-like plasticity to alter existing connectivity within a cortical area.

Although significant behavioral recovery occurred in both the ADS and OLS groups compared with control rats, the ADS group improved substantially more rapidly. Also, in the early postlesion period, the ADS group demonstrated a qualitatively different ON vs. OFF performance compared with the OLS group. These behavioral results alone suggest that different mechanisms underlie recovery in ADS and OLS groups. Although the results of ICMS on behavioral outcomes in animal models of brain injury have not been reported previously, several studies have examined the therapeutic effects of surface stimulation in either human stroke survivors or animal stroke models. For example, an invasive technology using epidural stimulation to provide low-level current pulses over uninjured cortical areas during the execution of rehabilitative training resulted in behavioral improvement in rodent and nonhuman primate models of cortical ischemic injury (20, 21). Although initial results in clinical stroke populations were promising, the therapeutic effect of open-loop epidural stimulation was not demonstrated in a randomized clinical trial (22). Nonetheless, noninvasive cortical stimulation approaches (transcranial magnetic stimulation and transcranial direct-current stimulation) continue to attract substantial interest due to positive results in small groups of stroke survivors (23).

Evidence to support specific mechanisms underlying the effects of open-loop electrical stimulation of the cortex on recovery is largely correlative but includes motor map reorganization, increased dendritic length and spine density, cell proliferation and cell migration in the subventricular zone, receptor subunit expression, activation of antiapoptotic cascades, increased neurotrophic factors, enhanced angiogenesis, and proliferation of inflammatory cells (20, 21, 2428). Because the number of stimulus pulses was similar in the ADS and OLS groups in the present study, it is reasonable to conclude that if electrical stimulation promoted proliferative processes, the effects were the same in the two groups.

In addition, various OLS protocols produce alterations in synaptic efficacy. These data are particularly relevant because of the qualitative differences in functional connectivity observed between ADS and OLS groups. Long-term potentiation (LTP), an experimental phenomenon first discovered in the hippocampus of anesthetized rabbits over 40 y ago (29), is expressed in both excitatory and inhibitory synapses throughout the mammalian brain (30). Although many experimental protocols have been developed to optimize synaptic potentiation in various model systems, the sign and magnitude of synaptic potentiation are heavily dependent upon the frequency and pattern of stimulation (31, 32).

Despite comparable mean stimulation frequency between the two groups, the temporal structure of stimulus pulses differed between the ADS and OLS groups. Interstimulus intervals spanned approximately the same range, but the intrinsic temporal firing pattern observed in the ADS group resulted in a greater number of short interstimulus intervals (Fig. S6A). Thus, ADS stimulation occasionally consisted of stimulus pulses at higher frequency, somewhat analogous to theta-burst stimulation, in which train bursts of high-frequency pulses (e.g., four to eight pulses at 100–300 Hz) are delivered at about 6–7 Hz (i.e., within the theta-rhythm frequency). Theta-burst stimulation is often used to optimize generation of LTP, especially in the neocortex of awake animals, where LTP has traditionally been more difficult to generate (33). In a study in the neocortex of freely moving rats, theta-burst stimulation, using parameters similar to those used in the hippocampus, evoked LTP, but the effects required at least 5 d to develop and plateaued at about 15 d (34). In the present study, although enhanced, short-latency spike discharge was evident with ADS even on the first day of stimulation, the time course of the behavioral effects was remarkably similar to the slowly developing LTP found in the rat neocortex study.

Theta-burst timing protocols vary considerably depending upon the particular model system. However, a recent study in a mouse brain slice preparation in the dorsal striatum suggests that the optimal theta-burst patterns are those that best match intrinsic neural activity patterns (35). Further, “burstiness” was critical to inducing LTP. Simply reducing the interburst pause from 35 ms to 20 ms eliminated the induction of LTP. It is possible that our imposed 28-ms blanking period further contributed to the neurophysiological and behavioral effects. We propose that by using a closed-loop stimulation paradigm, the intrinsic stimulation patterns that optimally drive synaptic potentiation in the corticocortical pathways were used. (The feasibility of using optimal theta-burst parameters in an open-loop mode of stimulation is discussed in SI Discussion).

In summary, OLS and ADS may both contribute to behavioral recovery but by somewhat different mechanisms. Electrical stimulation, in general, is likely to modulate neuronal growth processes, leading to adaptive plasticity that could account for at least part of the behavioral improvement. In the closed-loop (ADS) condition, however, the intrinsic firing pattern drives synaptic potentiation in a manner similar to that observed in theta-burst protocols. Although potentiation builds rapidly (within 1 d), we propose that chronic ADS results in a behaviorally relevant, functional connection between S1 and PM.

Future Applications of Closed-Loop Neuroprostheses for Treating Neurological Disorders.

A closed-loop neuroprosthesis applying ADS across distant cortical areas is a vastly different approach to brain repair than has been achieved to date. Therapeutic closed-loop stimulation in the brain is still uncommon. However, analogous approaches are already being tested for epilepsy, and an expanded role for closed-loop systems for deep brain stimulation in Parkinson disease is now being considered (36, 37). Further, closed-loop approaches are under development in animal models of spinal cord injury (38, 39). Other investigators have proposed a closed-loop approach for a cognitive prosthesis that has shown promise in animal models (40). Other potential clinical applications based on the current model include stroke, focal TBI, and surgical resections. Finally, a variety of neurological syndromes that are thought to be related to disruption of cortical communication may be amenable to ADS. In the 1960s, Norman Geschwind identified several disorders collectively called “disconnection syndromes,” revolutionizing the field of behavioral neurology (41). The consideration of closed-loop approaches to repair of cortical disconnection syndromes may open treatment options for a variety of conditions in which neural communication is disrupted, whether due to disease, injury, or idiopathic causes.

Materials and Methods

Animals.

Adult, male Long–Evans hooded rats (n = 16, weight: 350–450 g; Harlan) were procured at 4 mo of age. Protocols for animal use were approved by the Kansas University Medical Center Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and adhered to the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (42). Each rat was singly housed in a transparent cage and provided with food and water ad libitum. The room was kept on a 12-h:12-h light/dark cycle, and ambient temperature was maintained at 22 °C.

Rats were assigned to three groups: the ADS group, the OLS group, and the control group. Rats in all three groups received a CCI injury over the M1 forelimb area (5). Postmortem histological analysis confirmed that lesion size was comparable across groups (SI Results). The surgical procedures (e.g., burr holes, skull screws, dura resection) were identical in all three groups. Microelectrode implantation and microdevice attachment were identical in the ADS and OLS groups. In both the ADS and OLS groups, one single-shank microelectrode array was inserted into the S1 forelimb area. A second single-shank microelectrode array was inserted into the RFA (depths are provided in SI Materials and Methods). In the ADS group, stimulation in S1 was contingent upon spike activity in the RFA; that is, time-amplitude window discriminators determined when action potentials were recorded from the RFA microelectrode. Discrimination of an individual action potential triggered delivery of a brief pulse of electrical current to the microelectrode implanted in S1. In the OLS group, the stimulation was delivered arbitrarily at a frequency approximately the same as that in the ADS group but with the timing of stimulation uncorrelated with the discriminated action potentials (SI Materials and Methods). The wireless, battery-powered microdevice, mounted to the freely moving rat’s skull, operated 24 h per day (Fig. 2A and Fig. S1).

CCI Procedure.

In each rat, the skull over the CFA was removed while leaving the dura intact. A 3-mm diameter rod with a flat tip was placed into a commercial impactor device (Leica Microsystems), centered over the target location (SI Materials and Methods), and then lowered until the surface of the tip was in contact with the dura, as indicated by an audible signal triggered by a feedback sensor. The rod was then retracted and armed. An impact was delivered with an excursion of 2 mm below the surface of the dura. This protocol leads to reproducible lesions that damage all cortical layers within the CFA with minimal superficial damage to underlying white matter tracts and limited or no damage to adjacent cortical areas (5).

Microdevice Programming.

ADS programming.

To determine discrimination parameters for ADS, the channel with the best signal-to-noise ratio was chosen. This same channel was later used during microdevice operation to determine spike events that triggered stimulation. Using a custom MATLAB (MathWorks) script, action potentials were discriminated offline by thresholding and two user-adjustable time-amplitude windows, with the intent of maximizing discrimination of observed spikes while minimizing noise and/or stimulus artifacts. Stimulation parameters were set to deliver a 60-μA, 192-μs, pseudobiphasic current pulse with a 7.5-ms delay following spike discrimination (Fig. 2B). A blanking interval following each spike discrimination prevented additional stimulus pulses for 28 ms. The spike discrimination, timing, and stimulation parameters were then uploaded to the microdevice for online spike discrimination. Thus, during device operation in the ADS group, each discriminated spike in PM triggered a stimulation pulse in S1, constrained by the blanking interval.

The 7.5-ms delay was based on previous studies of the effective delay within local networks, analysis of spike-stimulus delays in pilot data, as well as constraints in the current microdevice architecture. The 28-ms blanking interval was also based on analysis of spike-stimulus delays in pilot data and was set to reduce the possibility of producing a positive-feedback loop, in which S1 stimulation might drive action potentials in PM, retriggering stimulation of S1.

OLS programming.

Stimulation parameters were the same in the OLS group as for the ADS group. However, the stimulation was not contingent upon recorded neural activity. Instead, the stimulation was set to occur arbitrarily with interstimulus intervals ranging from 35 to 200 ms (randomized equally across the range), closely approximating the stimulus rate for the ADS group (SI Materials and Methods, SI Results, and Fig. S6A).

Signal monitoring and maintenance.

The neural activity and stimulation rates were monitored daily throughout the study via a wireless connection. The microdevice ran continuously, delivering ADS or OLS 24 h a day during the experiment, except for brief periods required for behavioral assessment, changing the battery, and adjusting the window discriminator parameters.

Bandpass-filtered neural data (∼500 Hz to 5 kHz) were recorded at ∼35.7 kHz per channel from either one or four channels (wireless or wired connection, respectively) during all signal monitoring and behavioral trials using LabVIEW software (National Instruments). In addition, all animals had multiple sessions during which data were recorded during home cage behavior. The raw signal recording duration of any single monitoring period was software-limited to ∼45 min, but the stimulus trigger signal could be recorded for up to 24 h. The neural signal data were converted from a LabVIEW file to a text file and analyzed using custom MATLAB software.

Behavioral Training and Assessment.

Skilled reaching task.

Each rat was tested in a 30-cm × 30-cm × 52-cm Plexiglas reaching chamber. For each trial, a single food pellet (45 mg; Bioserv) was placed into a shallow well 2 cm from the front wall on an external shelf positioned 3 cm from the bottom of the chamber. The rat was required to reach through a narrow slot to retrieve the pellet with its forepaw (Fig. 2A). After forelimb preference was determined, a removable Plexiglas wall was used to force the animal to use only the preferred forelimb (5). Trials were recorded with a digital camcorder for playback and analysis. The percentage of success was measured as the percentage of trials in which the rat grasped, retrieved, and brought the pellet to the mouth (60 trials per day). Before entry into the remainder of the study, the rat was required to reach and retrieve food pellets above 70% success for 3 consecutive days. Following the injury (see below), behavioral probing sessions were conducted on postlesion days 3, 5, 8, 14, 21, and 28. Testing on postlesion days 1 and 2 was not practical due to the effects of surgical recovery and postsurgical analgesics on behavioral performance. Probing sessions consisted of 20 trials with the microdevice stimulation function turned OFF and then 20 trials with the microdevice stimulation function turned ON.

Foot-fault task.

Rats were also assessed on a foot-fault task to determine the effects of the injury on a locomotion task. In general, although there was an effect of the injury on this task on postlesion day 3, no lesion effects were observed on subsequent days. Also, there were no differences between groups at any time points. This result was not unexpected, because the foot-fault task is less sensitive, and spontaneous recovery is common with lesions restricted to the forelimb motor cortex.

Statistical Analysis of Behavioral Performance.

Initially, animals were randomly assigned to an ADS (n = 6) or control (n = 5) group. A subsequent OLS group (n = 5) was studied after group randomization. This was necessary to use neurophysiological data from the ADS group to determine the stimulation protocol for the OLS group.

Linear mixed models (LMMs) (43) were generated via restricted maximum likelihood estimation using SAS version 9.2 PROC GLIMMIX (SAS Institute, Inc.) to model performance on the skilled reaching task for each animal over time. Results are presented to mirror a series of one-way ANOVA models because the LMM provides analogous results. For animals in the ADS and OLS groups, the difference between the OFF and ON conditions was studied as an outcome. Models included fixed effects for treatment group, time, and their interaction.

Time was treated as a continuous measure to generate estimates of a polynomial relationship for recovery profiles in each treatment group over time up to a (treatment group-specific) quadratic relationship. Animal-specific effects were introduced by allowing for random intercepts in these models; thus, the models allowed for estimation of normally distributed error terms both for between- and within-animal effects. Backward elimination was used to determine the functional form of these relationships with F test P values <0.05 for effects to remain in the models. All lower ordered terms were retained in models in the presence of higher level interaction effects, regardless of statistical significance. Models were evaluated by visual inspection of observed vs. predicted values for each animal to assess model fit, observed vs. residuals plots to assess constant variance assumptions, and histograms of the residuals and quantile-quantile plots to assess the assumption of normally distributed random effects. Residuals included both those for the random intercept coefficients (for between-animal error terms) and overall residuals (for within-animal error terms).

Linear contrasts of model estimates were used to test for treatment group differences on postlesion days 3, 5, 8, 14, 21, and 28 using F tests, with day 28 serving as the a priori time point of interest for the comparison of ADS vs. OLS. Other pairwise comparisons at each time point were also tested (SI Materials and Methods, Protocol Deviations). Given the single, a priori primary comparison, no further adjustments for multiple comparisons were made. Linear contrasts were used to generate 95% confidence intervals for each treatment group for those specific days and, within the ADS and OLS groups, to test for differences in the OFF vs. ON conditions. Two-sided P values were used for presentation of results.

Supplementary Material

Supporting Information

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Andrea Pack and Alexandra Brown for technical assistance, Cam Teskey for invaluable comments on the discussion, and Casey Wood for artistic contributions. This research was funded by the Department of Defense Traumatic Brain Injury-Investigator-Initiated Research Award Program under Awards W81XWH-10-1-0741/0742 (to P.M. and R.J.N.) and the American Heart Association under Award 09BGIA2280495 (to P.M.). The fabrication costs for the chip in the microdevice were generously supported by the Advanced Platform Technology Center—A Veterans Affairs Research Center of Excellence (Cleveland, OH).

Footnotes

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

*This Direct Submission article had a prearranged editor.

This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1316885110/-/DCSupplemental.

References

  • 1.Vicente R, Gollo LL, Mirasso CR, Fischer I, Pipa G. Dynamical relaying can yield zero time lag neuronal synchrony despite long conduction delays. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105(44):17157–17162. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809353105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Carter AR, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Why use a connectivity-based approach to study stroke and recovery of function? Neuroimage. 2012;62(4):2271–2280. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Nudo RJ. Neural bases of recovery after brain injury. J Commun Disord. 2011;44(5):515–520. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Webster BR, Celnik PA, Cohen LG. Noninvasive brain stimulation in stroke rehabilitation. NeuroRx. 2006;3(4):474–481. doi: 10.1016/j.nurx.2006.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Nishibe M, Barbay S, Guggenmos D, Nudo RJ. Reorganization of motor cortex after controlled cortical impact in rats and implications for functional recovery. J Neurotrauma. 2010;27(12):2221–2232. doi: 10.1089/neu.2010.1456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Friel KM, et al. Dissociation of sensorimotor deficits after rostral versus caudal lesions in the primary motor cortex hand representation. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94(2):1312–1324. doi: 10.1152/jn.01251.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Conner JM, Chiba AA, Tuszynski MH. The basal forebrain cholinergic system is essential for cortical plasticity and functional recovery following brain injury. Neuron. 2005;46(2):173–179. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Nudo RJ, Frost SB. The evolution of motor cortex and motor systems. In: Kaas JH, editor. Evolutionary Neuroscience. Oxford: Academic; 2009. pp. 727–756. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Rouiller EM, Moret V, Liang F. Comparison of the connectional properties of the two forelimb areas of the rat sensorimotor cortex: Support for the presence of a premotor or supplementary motor cortical area. Somatosens Mot Res. 1993;10(3):269–289. doi: 10.3109/08990229309028837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dum RP, Strick PL. Motor areas in the frontal lobe of the primate. Physiol Behav. 2002;77(4-5):677–682. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00929-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Dancause N, et al. Extensive cortical rewiring after brain injury. J Neurosci. 2005;25(44):10167–10179. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3256-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fang PC, Stepniewska I, Kaas JH. Ipsilateral cortical connections of motor, premotor, frontal eye, and posterior parietal fields in a prosimian primate, Otolemur garnetti. J Comp Neurol. 2005;490(3):305–333. doi: 10.1002/cne.20665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Azin M, Guggenmos DJ, Barbay S, Nudo RJ, Mohseni P. A miniaturized system for spike-triggered intracortical microstimulation in an ambulatory rat. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2011;58(9):2589–2597. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2011.2159603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Azin M, Guggenmos DJ, Barbay S, Nudo RJ, Mohseni P. A battery-powered activity-dependent intracortical microstimulation IC for brain-machine-brain interface. IEEE J Solid-State Circuits. 2011;46(4):731–745. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Jackson A, Mavoori J, Fetz EE. Long-term motor cortex plasticity induced by an electronic neural implant. Nature. 2006;444(7115):56–60. doi: 10.1038/nature05226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Rebesco JM, Miller LE. Stimulus-driven changes in sensorimotor behavior and neuronal functional connectivity application to brain-machine interfaces and neurorehabilitation. Prog Brain Res. 2011;192:83–102. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53355-5.00006-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hebb DO. The Organization of Behavior. New York: Wiley; 1949. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Shatz CJ. The developing brain. Sci Am. 1992;267(3):60–67. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0992-60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Rebesco JM, Stevenson IH, Körding KP, Solla SA, Miller LE. Rewiring neural interactions by micro-stimulation. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010;4:39. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Adkins-Muir DL, Jones TA. Cortical electrical stimulation combined with rehabilitative training: Enhanced functional recovery and dendritic plasticity following focal cortical ischemia in rats. Neurol Res. 2003;25(8):780–788. doi: 10.1179/016164103771953853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Plautz EJ, et al. Post-infarct cortical plasticity and behavioral recovery using concurrent cortical stimulation and rehabilitative training: A feasibility study in primates. Neurol Res. 2003;25(8):801–810. doi: 10.1179/016164103771953880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Plow EB, Carey JR, Nudo RJ, Pascual-Leone A. Invasive cortical stimulation to promote recovery of function after stroke: A critical appraisal. Stroke. 2009;40(5):1926–1931. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.540823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Perez MA, Cohen LG. The corticospinal system and transcranial magnetic stimulation in stroke. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2009;16(4):254–269. doi: 10.1310/tsr1604-254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Adkins DL, Hsu JE, Jones TA. Motor cortical stimulation promotes synaptic plasticity and behavioral improvements following sensorimotor cortex lesions. Exp Neurol. 2008;212(1):14–28. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2008.01.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Baba T, et al. Electrical stimulation of the cerebral cortex exerts antiapoptotic, angiogenic, and anti-inflammatory effects in ischemic stroke rats through phosphoinositide 3-kinase/Akt signaling pathway. Stroke. 2009;40(11):e598–e605. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.563627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Henderson AK, Pittman QJ, Teskey GC. High frequency stimulation alters motor maps, impairs skilled reaching performance and is accompanied by an upregulation of specific GABA, glutamate and NMDA receptor subunits. Neuroscience. 2012;215:98–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.04.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Jahanshahi A, et al. Electrical stimulation of the motor cortex enhances progenitor cell migration in the adult rat brain. Exp Brain Res. 2013;231(2):165–177. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3680-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Edwardson MA, Lucas TH, Carey JR, Fetz EE. New modalities of brain stimulation for stroke rehabilitation. Exp Brain Res. 2013;224(3):335–358. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3315-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bliss TV, Lomo T. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol. 1973;232(2):331–356. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Malenka RC, Bear MF. LTP and LTD: An embarrassment of riches. Neuron. 2004;44(1):5–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Feldman DE. Synaptic mechanisms for plasticity in neocortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2009;32:33–55. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Feldman DE. The spike-timing dependence of plasticity. Neuron. 2012;75(4):556–571. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Trepel C, Racine RJ. Long-term potentiation in the neocortex of the adult, freely moving rat. Cereb Cortex. 1998;8(8):719–729. doi: 10.1093/cercor/8.8.719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Racine RJ, Chapman CA, Trepel C, Teskey GC, Milgram NW. Post-activation potentiation in the neocortex. IV. Multiple sessions required for induction of long-term potentiation in the chronic preparation. Brain Res. 1995;702(1-2):87–93. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01025-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hawes SL, Gillani F, Evans RC, Benkert EA, Blackwell KT. Sensitivity to theta-burst timing permits LTP in dorsal striatal adult brain slice. J Neurophysiol. 2013;110(9):2027–2036. doi: 10.1152/jn.00115.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Skarpaas TL, Morrell MJ. Intracranial stimulation therapy for epilepsy. Neurotherapeutics. 2009;6(2):238–243. doi: 10.1016/j.nurt.2009.01.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Santos FJ, Costa RM, Tecuapetla F. Stimulation on demand: Closing the loop on deep brain stimulation. Neuron. 2011;72(2):197–198. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Jackson A, Zimmermann JB. Neural interfaces for the brain and spinal cord—Restoring motor function. Nat Rev Neurol. 2012;8(12):690–699. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Nishimura Y, Perlmutter SI, Fetz EE. Restoration of upper limb movement via artificial corticospinal and musculospinal connections in a monkey with spinal cord injury. Front Neural Circuits. 2013;7:57. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Berger TW, et al. A hippocampal cognitive prosthesis: Multi-input, multi-output nonlinear modeling and VLSI implementation. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 2012;20(2):198–211. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2012.2189133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Catani M, Mesulam M. What is a disconnection syndrome? Cortex. 2008;44(8):911–913. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Committee for the Update of the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (2011) Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (The National Academies Press, Washington, DC) [Google Scholar]
  • 43.McCulloch CE, Searle SR. Generalized, Linear, and Mixed Models. New York: Wiley; 2001. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supporting Information
Download video file (8MB, mov)
Download video file (9MB, mov)

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES