Abstract
Introduction
Several mammalian species spontaneously align their body axis with respect to the Earth’s magnetic field (MF) lines in diverse behavioral contexts. Magnetic alignment is a suitable paradigm to scan for the occurrence of magnetosensitivity across animal taxa with the heuristic potential to contribute to the understanding of the mechanism of magnetoreception and identify further functions of magnetosensation apart from navigation. With this in mind we searched for signs of magnetic alignment in dogs. We measured the direction of the body axis in 70 dogs of 37 breeds during defecation (1,893 observations) and urination (5,582 observations) over a two-year period. After complete sampling, we sorted the data according to the geomagnetic conditions prevailing during the respective sampling periods. Relative declination and intensity changes of the MF during the respective dog walks were calculated from daily magnetograms. Directional preferences of dogs under different MF conditions were analyzed and tested by means of circular statistics.
Results
Dogs preferred to excrete with the body being aligned along the North–South axis under calm MF conditions. This directional behavior was abolished under unstable MF. The best predictor of the behavioral switch was the rate of change in declination, i.e., polar orientation of the MF.
Conclusions
It is for the first time that (a) magnetic sensitivity was proved in dogs, (b) a measurable, predictable behavioral reaction upon natural MF fluctuations could be unambiguously proven in a mammal, and (c) high sensitivity to small changes in polarity, rather than in intensity, of MF was identified as biologically meaningful. Our findings open new horizons in magnetoreception research. Since the MF is calm in only about 20% of the daylight period, our findings might provide an explanation why many magnetoreception experiments were hardly replicable and why directional values of records in diverse observations are frequently compromised by scatter.
Keywords: Magnetoreception, Magnetosensitivity, Magnetic field, Magnetic storm, Magnetic alignment, Dog, Canid, Mammal
Introduction
Magnetic alignment, i.e., spontaneous alignment of the body with respect to the magnetic field lines, when other determinants (e.g. wind direction, sun position, curiosity) of the body position are negligible, has been demonstrated in several species of mammals in diverse behavioral contexts: in grazing and resting cattle, roe deer and red deer [1-4] and hunting red foxes [5] as well as in several other mammalian species (under preparation). Magnetic alignment proved to be a suitable paradigm to scan for the occurrence of magnetosensitivity across animal taxa with a heuristic potential to contribute to the understanding of the mechanism of magnetoreception and identify further functions of a magnetic sense apart from navigation [1-9]. With this in mind we decided to look for examples of expression of magnetic alignment in dogs. Expecting magnetoreception in dogs is reasonable given the extraordinary homing abilities of dogs [10] and closely related species like red foxes, coyotes and grey wolves [11-13]. Wolves, as the progenitors of domestic dogs, possess extraordinary large (about 150–200 km2) home ranges [14], suggesting superior orientation abilities. Furthermore, strong indications for magnetosensation in the red fox [5] add on to the growing evidence.
A discovery of magnetoreception in dogs would open totally new horizons for magnetobiological research: Dogs are widely available experimental subjects all over the world and can easily be trained to react on diverse sensory stimuli [15]. In addition, as dogs are still readily used as experimental animals in a wide array of biomedical applications [16], the discovery of a new sense would have far reaching consequences also in this sector.
Having been inspired by our hitherto observations in other animals [1-3,5-8,17], we monitored spontaneous alignment in dogs during diverse activities (resting, feeding and excreting) and eventually focused on excreting (defecation and urination incl. marking) as this activity appeared to be most promising with regard to obtaining large sets of data independent of time and space, and at the same time it seems to be least prone to be affected by the surroundings.
Results
Circular analysis of the distribution of all values of all dogs irrespective of the magnetic field conditions revealed significant but highly scattered axial orientation during defecation (Table 1). This orientation was, however, not confirmed by the grand mean vector (calculated over the dogs’ mean values, Figure 1, Table 1). Since no significant differences between males and females and since no angular preferences during defecation were found (not shown here), we only present the axial analyses combined for both sexes here.
Table 1.
|
Defecation: all records |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | Pooled | Pooled | Means (n > 5) | Means (n > 5) |
Data type |
angular |
axial |
angular |
axial |
Number of observations |
1,893 |
1,893 |
49 |
49 |
Mean vector (μ) |
133° |
157°/337° |
80° |
148°/328° |
Length of mean vector (r) |
0.013 |
0.052 |
0.043 |
0.209 |
Circular standard deviation |
169° |
70° |
144° |
51° |
95% Confidence interval (-/+) for μ |
- |
- |
- |
121°-174° |
99% Confidence interval (-/+) for μ |
- |
- |
- |
112°-183° |
Rayleigh test (Z) |
0.307 |
5.203 |
0.091 |
2.143 |
Rayleigh test (p) | 0.736 | 0.006 | 0.914 | 0.117 |
After the data were sorted according to the magnetic field conditions (specifically, Kp-index, relative changes of magnetic field intensity or of declination) at the time of recording, a differentiated picture emerged. The relative declination change proved to be the best predictor of alignment, i.e.,. sorting the data according to this parameter provided the most significant results. Analysis of pooled recordings as well as of mean vectors of recordings in dogs sampled during calm magnetic field conditions (relative change in declination = 0%; minimum of 5 observations per dog) revealed a highly significant axial preference for North–South alignment during defecation (for 0% declination change: μ = 173°/353° ± 9° (mean vector orientation angle; 95% confidence interval), r = 0.598 (mean vector length), Rayleigh test: n = 43, p = 6.2⋅10-8, Z = 15.353; second order (weighted) statistics: weighted mean vector (WMV): 175°/355°, r = 0.253, Hotelling test: n = 43, p = 1.02⋅10-7, F = 24.463; Tables 2 and 3, Figures 1 and 2). With increasing relative declination changes the distribution of dogs’ body orientations became more scattered and in the category “> 2%” the distribution was random, and no directional preference was apparent (Table 2, Figure 1). The distributions of dogs’ body orientations in the intervals of relative declination change “0.1%-2%” as well as “> 2%” were significantly different from the distribution at 0%, both, when pooled raw data and when means per dog were analyzed (Mardia-Watson-Wheeler test, p < 0.001, Figure 1). The same dependence of the directional preference on the relative changes of the magnetic declination appeared when males and females were treated separately (not shown here).
Table 2.
|
Pooled raw data |
Means per dog (n > 5) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Declination rate | 0% | 0.1-2% | >2% | 0% | 0.1-2% | >2% |
Number of observations |
607 |
542 |
744 |
43 |
36 |
39 |
Mean vector (μ) |
176°/356° |
111°/291° |
109°/289° |
173°/353° |
104°/184° |
96°/276° |
Length of mean vector (r) |
0.216 |
0.106 |
0.03 |
0.598 |
0.283 |
0.194 |
Circular standard deviation |
50° |
61° |
76° |
29° |
45° |
52° |
95% Confidence interval (-/+) for μ |
168°-183° |
- |
- |
164°-182° |
81°-127° |
- |
99% Confidence interval (-/+) for μ |
166°-185° |
- |
- |
161°-185° |
74°-134° |
- |
Rayleigh test (Z) |
28.248 |
6.133 |
0.672 |
15.353 |
2.891 |
1.464 |
Rayleigh test (p) | <10-12 | 0.002 | 0.511 | 6.20⋅10-8 | 0.055 | 0.233 |
Table 3.
Variable | All records 20:01–08:00 | Quiet MF 20:01–08:00 | All records 08:01–12:00 | Quiet MF 08:01–12:00 | All records 12:01–16:00 | Quiet MF 12:01–16:00 | All records 16:01–20:00 | Quiet MF 16:01–20:00 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Number of observations |
442 |
173 |
599 |
188 |
396 |
109 |
455 |
144 |
Mean vector (μ) |
157°/337° |
177°/357° |
152°/332° |
178°/358° |
12°/192° |
170°/350° |
147°/327° |
173°/353° |
Length of mean vector (r) |
0.042 |
0.239 |
0.073 |
0.2 |
0.074 |
0.26 |
0.088 |
0.176 |
Circular standard deviation |
72° |
48° |
65° |
51° |
65° |
47° |
63° |
53° |
95% Confidence interval (-/+) for μ |
112°-202° |
165°-190° |
130°-174° |
163°-192° |
345°-39° |
155°-184° |
126°-168° |
155°-192° |
99% Confidence interval (-/+) for μ |
98°-217° |
161°-193° |
123°-181° |
159°-106° |
337°-48° |
151°-189° |
119°-175° |
149°-198° |
Rayleigh test (Z) |
0.769 |
9.866 |
3.17 |
7.533 |
2.145 |
7.367 |
3.54 |
4.474 |
Rayleigh test (p) | 0.463 | 0.000052 | 0.042 | 0.000535 | 0.117 | 0.000632 | 0.029 | 0.011 |
Columns denoted “quiet MF (magnetic field)” give statistic values based on analysis of those data from the respective column “all values” which were collected under conditions of stable declination (0% change). Due to small sample sizes for single dogs, the data of all observations done in the given period are pooled. Pooling is justified in this case because samples for respective dogs have comparable sizes and because males and females show comparable posture and alignment during defecation. The data were not sorted here according to months but the distribution of observations during respective months of the year is comparable, so that the distribution of the data (and resulting analysis) would not change if winter and summer observations were further separated. Note random circular distribution of the alignment when all data for respective time periods are analyzed, but highly significant preference for the North–South axis when only observations made under quiet magnetic field are considered.
Analysis of the alignment during defecation under conditions of stable magnetic declination (0% changes) revealed no significant effect of sex. There may be a slight effect of age: dogs in the age category 2.5-7 years showed a clearer preference than younger or elder dogs (not shown). The effect of the dog breed could not be tested because of small sample sizes.
Circular analysis of the distribution of the pooled raw data demonstrated a significant deviation from random distribution also in urinating dogs (Table 4). Analyzing this data for males and females separately we found a slight difference in the patterns between sexes: Pooled data (without the dog M07) and mean values of all males with at least 5 observations revealed a significant angular preference for North-West heading during urination (Table 5). The male borzoi M07 contributed approximately one third of the urination data and was analyzed separately (Table 6); the results were similar to the pooled data of all other males. In contrast, females showed an axial preference for approximately the North–South axis during urination (Table 7). As in the case of defecation, sorting the data according to the relative changes of declination revealed a significant effect of this factor and a significant axial North–South alignment only under calm MF conditions (for 0% declination change: μ = 167°/347° ± 16°, r = 0.343, Rayleigh test: n = 49, p = 0.003, Z = 5.766; second order (weighted) statistics: WMV: 173°/353°, r = 0.165, Hotelling test: n = 49, p = 5.08⋅10-4, F = 8.952; Figure 3, Tables 5, 6, 7). The raw data distributions during changing declination were significantly different from the distribution under calm magnetic conditions (Mardia-Watson-Wheeler test, p < 0.05, Figure 3).
Table 4.
|
Urination: (all records) |
|||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Data type |
Angular |
Axial |
||||||||
Pooled |
Means (n ≥ 5) |
Pooled |
Means (n ≥ 5) |
|||||||
Males | M07 | Females | Males | Females | Males | M07 | Females | Males | Females | |
Number of observations |
1,402 |
2,478 |
1,702 |
24 |
35 |
1,402 |
2,478 |
1,702 |
24 |
35 |
Mean vector (μ) |
312° |
298° |
13° |
292° |
331° |
154°/334° |
175°/355° |
5°/185° |
89°/269° |
11°/191° |
Length of mean vector (r) |
0.048 |
0.105 |
0.03 |
0.213 |
0.213 |
0.037 |
0.196 |
0.132 |
0.101 |
0.292 |
Circular standard deviation |
141° |
122° |
152° |
101° |
101° |
74° |
52° |
58° |
61° |
45° |
95% Confidence interval (-/+) for μ |
268°-356° |
283°-313° |
309°-78° |
217°-8° |
269°-34° |
125°-183° |
171°-179° |
358°-12° |
9°-169° |
348°-33° |
99% Confidence interval (-/+) for μ |
254°-10° |
278°-318° |
288°-98° |
193°-31° |
249°-53° |
116°-192° |
169°-180° |
356°-15° |
343°-194° |
341°-40° |
Rayleigh test (Z) |
3.215 |
27.075 |
1.517 |
1.088 |
1.584 |
1.875 |
94.735 |
29.524 |
0.246 |
2.99 |
Rayleigh test (p) |
0.04 |
1.74⋅10-12 |
0.219 |
0.341 |
0.206 |
0.153 |
< 10-12 |
< 10-12 |
0.786 |
0.049 |
Rao’s spacing test (U) |
307.618 |
354.479 |
339.271 |
140.383 |
131.675 |
325.078 |
357.094 |
346.675 |
134.75 |
145.847 |
Rao’s spacing test (p) | <0.01 | <0.01 | <0.01 | >0.1 | >0.1 | <0.01 | <0.01 | <0.01 | >0.1 | >0.1 |
Data for the male dog M07 are presented in a separate column due to large sample size.
Table 5.
|
Pooled raw data |
Means per dog (n ≥ 5) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Declination rate | 0% | 0.1-2% | >2% | 0% | 0.1-2% | >2% |
Number of observations |
491 |
256 |
655 |
22 |
15 |
22 |
Mean vector (μ) |
293° |
12° |
84° |
291° |
355° |
195° |
Length of mean vector (r) |
0.129 |
0.08 |
0.006 |
0.367 |
0.349 |
0.07° |
Circular standard deviation |
116° |
129° |
182° |
81° |
83° |
132° |
95% Conf. interval (-/+) for μ |
265°-321° |
310°-74° |
- |
246°-335° |
290°-61° |
315°-76° |
99% Conf. interval (-/+) for μ |
256°-329° |
290°-94° |
- |
232°-349° |
270°-81° |
239°-151° |
Rayleigh test (Z) |
8.17 |
1.619 |
0.028 |
2.959 |
1.828 |
0.109 |
Rayleigh test (p) |
2.83⋅10-4 |
0.198 |
0.973 |
0.05 |
0.162 |
0.899 |
Rao’s spacing test (U) |
260.285 |
281.656 |
283.053 |
148.258 |
147.139 |
127.188 |
Rao’s spacing test (p) | <0.01 | <0.01 | <0.01 | >0.1 | >0.1 | >0.5 |
Table 6.
Declination rate | 0-1.7% | 1.8-3.3% | ≥3.4% |
---|---|---|---|
Number of observations |
957 |
818 |
703 |
Mean vector (μ) |
310° |
285° |
280° |
Length of mean vector (r) |
0.154 |
0.08 |
0.078 |
Circular standard deviation |
111° |
129° |
130° |
95% Confidence interval (-/+) for μ |
294°-327° |
250°-320° |
241°-318° |
99% Confidence interval (-/+) for μ |
289°-332° |
240°-331° |
229°-331° |
Rayleigh test (Z) |
22.64 |
5.18 |
5.517 |
Rayleigh test (p) |
1.47⋅10-10 |
0.006 |
0.015 |
Rao’s spacing test (U) |
345.705 |
344.156 |
341.565 |
Rao’s spacing test (p) | < 0.01 | < 0.01 | < 0.01 |
Data are sorted into three categories according to the relative changes of magnetic field declination. Limits of the categories were chosen so that sample sizes are comparable.
Table 7.
|
Pooled raw data |
Means per dog (n ≥ 5) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Declination rate | 0% | 0.1-2% | >2% | 0% | 0.1-2% | >2% |
Number of observations |
603 |
396 |
703 |
27 |
20 |
29 |
Mean vector (μ) |
5°/185° |
2°/182° |
11°/191° |
0°/180° |
7°/187° |
23°/203° |
Length of mean vector (r) |
0.208 |
0.131 |
0.068 |
0.434 |
0.159 |
0.134 |
Circular standard deviation |
51° |
58° |
66° |
37° |
55° |
57° |
95% Conf. interval (-/+) for μ |
357°-12° |
347°-17° |
349°-33° |
163°-196° |
312°-63° |
328°-78° |
99% Conf. interval (-/+) for μ |
354°-15° |
342°-22° |
342°-40° |
157°-201° |
294°-80° |
311°-95° |
Rayleigh test (Z) |
26.146 |
6.839 |
3.251 |
5.085 |
0.503 |
0.52 |
Rayleigh test (p) |
4.41⋅10-12 |
0.001 |
0.039 |
0.005 |
0.61 |
0.598 |
Rao’s spacing test (U) |
312.836 |
330 |
339.004 |
156.975 |
135.002 |
129.554 |
Rao’s spacing test (p) | <0.01 | <0.01 | <0.01 | >0.05 | >0.1 | >0.5 |
Discussion
Dogs rely much on their owner, and for many tasks they might anticipate the demanded behavior by reading their owner’s facial expression and make use of unintentional experimenter-given cues [18-20]. This adds a bias-trap to any research relying on behavioral studies and particularly conditioning. However, this is certainly not a confounder in our study because the dogs do not have to fulfill a certain task, but perform everyday routine behavior.
The study was truly blind. Although the observers were acquainted with our previous studies on magnetic alignment in animals and could have consciously or unconsciously biased the results, no one, not even the coordinators of the study, hypothesized that expression of alignment could have been affected by the geomagnetic situation, and particularly by such subtle changes of the magnetic declination. The idea leading to the discovery of the correlation emerged after sampling was closed and the first statistical analyses (with rather negative results, cf. Figure 1) had been performed. Also, the acquisition of data on magnetic declination was carried out without knowledge of heading values on the respective time and date.
We found no differences in alignment of females and males during defecation and of females during urination, which might be related to a similar posture the animals are adopting during defecation (in all dogs) and urination (in females). Urinating males have a slightly different preference to orient their body axis than urinating females (cf. Figure 3); this could be caused by leg lifting during urination in males. Indications of different directional tendencies depending on which leg (left or right) is lifted are currently under study. All recordings were made outside on open fields, and routes of walks were routinely changed to exclude or limit pseudoreplications which would arise when dogs are defecating or urinating at just a few places within their kennel or house yard.
Natural fluctuations of the Earth’s magnetic field [21,22] have previously been suggested to disturb orientation in birds [23-25], bees [26] and whales [27]; and even to affect vegetative functions and behavior in humans [28,29], reviewed also in [22].
In this study, we provide the first clear and simply measurable evidence for influence of geomagnetic field variations on mammal behavior. Furthermore, it is the first demonstration of the effect of the shift of declination, which has to our knowledge never been investigated before. Previous studies of the effect focused mainly on the variations in field intensity. Although intensity and declination changes are mostly concomitant, declination change was a better predictor of dog alignment. Interestingly, the rate and direction of the changes disturb more effectively than absolute values. Here, for the first time the response can be attributed to the rate of magnetic field changes.
Typically, the daily declination comprises westward-shifts in the morning and eastward-shifts in the afternoon, while the magnetic field is rather stable at night [21,22]. This calls for necessity to test whether the dog alignment is not actually influenced primarily by time of the day and most probably by position of the sun on the sky. We can, however, exclude this alternative. First, days when the magnetic field parameters change erratically and unpredictably (i.e., magnetic storms) are quite frequent. These changes have been well studied by others and are described in the literature (cf. [21,22] for reviews). Second, the data collection was not biased to either morning or afternoon (Table 8). Third, periods of sampling under conditions of quiet magnetic field were rather evenly distributed in the course of the day. Fourth, and most importantly, alignment during excreting was apparent under conditions of quiet magnet field, irrespective of the time of day or month. Time of day per se was not a reliable predictor of expression of alignment (Figure 2, Tables 3, 9). Fifth, generally, there are on average 1,450 sunshine hours per year at maximum in the Czech Republic and in Germany, on localities where measurements were done. Even if we would assume that these sunshine hours were evenly distributed over the daylight period and the year (as our observations were), there would only be a probability of 33% that the observation was made when the sun was visible. Hence, with high probability (67%) most walks during the daylight period were made when it was cloudy.
Table 8.
Declination changes (%) | Proportion of observations (%) |
---|---|
0 |
18 |
0.1-1.0 |
6 |
1.1-2.0 |
19 |
2.1-3.0 |
17 |
3.1-4.0 |
16 |
4.1-5.0 |
12 |
5.1-6 |
3 |
6.1-8.0 |
6 |
>8.1 | 3 |
Table 9.
Period (time) | Proportion of all observations (%) | Proportion of observations under quiet MF (%) |
---|---|---|
05:01–07:00 |
2.2 |
1.6 |
07:01–09:00 |
9.9 |
12.2 |
09:01–11:00 |
26 |
18.5 |
11:01–13:00 |
16.8 |
6.3 |
13:01–15:00 |
11.5 |
12.8 |
15:01–17:00 |
13.6 |
20.8 |
17:01–19:00 |
10.3 |
5.8 |
19:01–21:00 |
7.9 |
18 |
21:01–23:00 |
1.3 |
2.8 |
23:01–05:00 | 0.5 | 1.2 |
Last but not least, the argument that the dogs might orient with regard to sun position so that they turn with their back to the sun in order to avoid dazzling by sunshine during such a sensitive and vulnerable act as excretion can be questioned. This argument is not plausible for urine marking, which is a brief act. We doubt that a dog that cares of not being attacked would always make sure to be turned away from the sun. The dog will likely look in that direction from where danger can most probably be expected - and this is for sure not always the direction away from the sun. In contrast to a human, the dog is relying also on its nose and its ears (in some breeds even more than on its eyes) when monitoring its surroundings - so we may expect that the dog heads with its nose and pinnae against the wind or in the direction of interest. Directing the pinnae and the nose may take priority over eyes. One can also often observe that dogs (especially during defecation) align in a certain direction, which is actually a different one from the direction of interest and they turn their head then in that other direction. Also we have to take into account that dogs are smaller than humans, they look at a different angle over the horizon and even in situations when we are dazzled, they might be not. Quite important: note also that the preference is axial - there are many cases when the dog actually looks southwards. There is no evidence for shift of the alignment axis during the day.
It is still enigmatic why the dogs do align at all, whether they do it “consciously” (i.e., whether the magnetic field is sensorial perceived (the dogs “see”, “hear” or “smell” the compass direction or perceive it as a haptic stimulus) or whether its reception is controlled on the vegetative level (they “feel better/more comfortable or worse/less comfortable” in a certain direction). Our analysis of the raw data (not shown here) indicates that dogs not only prefer N-S direction, but at the same time they also avoid E-W direction. The fact that larger and faster changes in magnetic conditions result in random distribution of body directions, i.e., a lowering of the preferences and ceasing of the avoidances, can be explained either through disturbing or conscious “shutdown” of the magnetoreception mechanism. From the two putative mechanisms that are discussed in birds and other vertebrates (radical-pairs and single-domain or superparamagnetic particles [30,31]) both might account for the observed alignment of the dogs and their sensitivity to declination changes.
An answer may lie in the biological meaning of the behavior: if dogs would use a visual (radical-pair based) magnetic map to aid general orientation in space as has been proposed for rodents [32], they might have the need to center/calibrate the map now and then with regard to landmarks or a magnetic reference. Aligning the map and the view towards North (or South) facilitates reading the map. Furthermore, calibration only makes sense when the reference is stable and reliable. We might think of this the same way as a human is stopping during a hike to read a map. When the map is blurred or the reference (perceived magnetic direction) is dispersed or moving due to magnetic disturbances, however, calibration is impossible. In the case of the dogs it thus would totally make sense to not pay attention to magnetic body alignment any more under conditions of a shifting magnetic field.
Conclusions
We demonstrate, for the first time (a) magnetic sensitivity in dogs, (b) a measurable, predictable behavioral reaction upon natural magnetic field (MF) fluctuation in a mammal, and (c) high sensitivity to small changes in polarity, rather than in intensity, of the MF. Our findings open new horizons in magnetoreception research. The newly introduced animal model (dog), paradigm (alignment during excretion) and parameter (relative declination change) open new horizons for biomagnetic research. Particularly the findings that already small fluctuations in Earth’s magnetic field elicit a behavioral response and the fact that “normal” magnetic conditions under which dogs express their orientation behavior occur only in about 30% of all cases call for caution. When extrapolated upon other animals and other experiments and observations on animal magnetoreception, this might explain the non-replicability of many findings and high scatter in others. Behavioral scientists need to revise their former experiments and observations and consider the phenomenon in their current and future experiments. The phenomenon challenges biophysicists to formulate testable hypotheses for mechanisms responsible for magnetoreception of inconsistencies of the direction of the MF direction. Finally, it forces biologists and physicians to seriously reconsider effects magnetic storms might pose on organisms.
Methods
Alignment of the body (along the thoracic spine) in direction towards the head (heading) was measured in freely moving dogs (i.e., not on the leash) in “open field” (on meadows, fields, in the wood etc., i.e., unconstrained, and uninfluenced by linear structures, such as walls and fences) away from the road traffic, high voltage power lines, and conspicuous steel constructions during defecation and urination by a hand-held compass (Figure 4). Dog breed, sex, age, body mass, condition, dog-ID were protocolled as well as date, time, locality, and further circumstances of recordings (e.g. within the home range, in unfamiliar surroundings etc.). Thanks to the commitment of altogether 37 dog owners/reporters and the involvement of 70 dogs (28 males, 42 females) belonging to 37 breeds (Tables 10 and 11) we collected data on heading during defecation (n = 1,893 observations; 55 dogs) and urination (n = 5,582; 59 dogs) from December 2011 till July 2013. The samples were collected in the Czech Republic and in Germany.
Table 10.
ID dog | Breed | Reporter (Abbrev.) | Age (years) | Weight (kg) | n defecation | n urination | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. |
F22 |
Airedale Terrier |
Hanz |
4 |
25 |
39 |
14 |
2. |
F06 |
Beagle |
Nova |
3 |
10 |
0 |
7 |
3. |
F29 |
Beagle |
Krej |
7 |
10 |
92 |
52 |
4. |
F01 |
Bearded Collie |
Niets |
11 |
25 |
102 |
30 |
5. |
F23 |
Border Terrier |
Hanz |
13 |
7 |
15 |
2 |
6. |
F69 |
Bouvier des Flandres |
Elli |
1 |
34 |
4 |
6 |
7. |
F35 |
Dachshund |
Hart |
2.5 |
7 |
22 |
110 |
8. |
F37 |
Dachshund |
Faif |
4 |
9 |
33 |
36 |
9. |
F48 |
Dachshund |
Bene |
11 |
7 |
31 |
21 |
10. |
F54 |
Dachshund |
Bene |
6 |
4 |
16 |
8 |
11. |
F55 |
Dachshund |
Bene |
3.5 |
10 |
0 |
21 |
12. |
F57 |
Dachshund |
Bene |
1 |
4 |
18 |
9 |
13. |
F58 |
Dachshund |
Bene |
10 |
4 |
17 |
91 |
14. |
F60 |
Dachshund |
Bene |
1 |
4 |
11 |
7 |
15. |
F82 |
Dachshund |
Bene |
5.5 |
6.5 |
0 |
0 |
16. |
F83 |
Dachshund |
Bene |
13 |
6 |
0 |
0 |
17. |
F90 |
Dachshund |
Dohm |
2 |
4.5 |
5 |
42 |
18. |
F40 |
Dalmatian |
Kriv |
14 |
20 |
64 |
153 |
19. |
F21 |
English Springer Spaniel |
Hanz |
1 |
20 |
58 |
18 |
20. |
F81 |
English Springer Spaniel |
Zdar |
7 |
21 |
0 |
0 |
21. |
F77 |
Entlebucher Mountain Dog |
Hron |
4 |
18 |
0 |
34 |
22. |
F41 |
Fox Terrier Smooth |
Adam |
12 |
8 |
37 |
75 |
23. |
F42 |
Fox Terrier Smooth |
Adam |
2 |
6.5 |
33 |
39 |
24. |
F43 |
Fox Terrier Smooth |
Adam |
6 |
8.5 |
20 |
65 |
25. |
F44 |
Fox Terrier Smooth |
Adam |
2 |
10 |
16 |
16 |
26. |
F36 |
German Spaniel |
Faif |
3 |
13 |
46 |
33 |
27. |
F13 |
German Wirehaired Pointer |
Cuko |
4 |
30 |
5 |
0 |
28. |
F14 |
German Wirehaired Pointer |
Cuko |
7 |
30 |
3 |
0 |
29. |
F20 |
Golden Retriever |
Hanz |
10 |
30 |
29 |
16 |
30. |
F39 |
Irish Terrier |
Tres |
1.5 |
15 |
15 |
16 |
31. |
F24 |
Jack Russell Terrier |
Jura |
3 |
7 |
0 |
32 |
32. |
F75 |
Mix |
Hron |
12 |
20 |
4 |
79 |
33. |
F66 |
Mix: Lhasa Apso/Jack Russell Terrier |
Rick |
4 |
x |
22 |
0 |
34. |
F09 |
Rhodesian Ridgeback |
Nova |
5 |
30 |
0 |
59 |
35. |
F71 |
Small Münsterländer |
Pali |
10 |
24 |
24 |
50 |
36. |
F32 |
Standard Schnauzer |
Posp |
12 |
6 |
34 |
125 |
37. |
F45 |
Tibetan Spaniel |
Hegl |
6 |
5 |
14 |
101 |
38. |
F70 |
Transylvanian Hound |
Zema |
0.7 |
30 |
0 |
10 |
39. |
F08 |
Weimaraner |
Nova |
6 |
30 |
2 |
71 |
40. |
F10 |
West Highland White Terrier |
Nova |
3 |
7 |
0 |
42 |
41. |
F34 |
West Highland White Terrier |
Hart |
8 |
7 |
56 |
212 |
42. |
F11 |
Yorkshire Terrier |
Garc |
6.5 |
2 |
30 |
0 |
records/dogs | 917 | 1702 |
Table 11.
ID dog | Breed | Reporter (Abbrev.) | Age (years) | weight (kg) | n defecation | n urination | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. |
M27 |
Beagle |
Krej |
4 |
10 |
95 |
53 |
2. |
M28 |
Beagle |
Krej |
2 |
10 |
92 |
53 |
3. |
M33 |
Beagle |
Posp |
3 |
10 |
14 |
39 |
4. |
M04 |
Bernese Mountain Dog |
Leu |
5 |
40 |
29 |
0 |
5. |
M76 |
Border Terrier |
Hron |
7 |
8 |
0 |
37 |
6. |
M07 |
Borzoi |
Nova |
4 |
40 |
96 |
2478 |
7. |
M31 |
Coton de Tulear |
Acke |
4 |
4 |
11 |
106 |
8. |
M05 |
Dachsbracke |
Cerv |
7 |
15 |
54 |
127 |
9. |
M26 |
Dachshund |
Komi |
5 |
7 |
46 |
92 |
10. |
M52 |
Dachshund |
Bene |
7 |
5 |
0 |
23 |
11. |
M53 |
Dachshund |
Bene |
8 |
4 |
10 |
43 |
12. |
M59 |
Dachshund |
Bene |
1 |
4 |
20 |
19 |
13. |
M61 |
Dachshund |
Bene |
12 |
6 |
2 |
50 |
14. |
M62 |
Dachshund |
Bene |
1 |
6 |
15 |
16 |
15. |
M65 |
Dachshund |
Faif |
3 |
7 |
10 |
7 |
16. |
M74 |
German Spitz |
Hron |
3.5 |
5 |
0 |
36 |
17. |
M72 |
Hanoverian Hound |
Krau |
5.5 |
45 |
15 |
0 |
18. |
M03 |
Irish Red Setter |
Gros |
3 |
30 |
47 |
0 |
19. |
M80 |
Mix: German Shepherd x Schnauzer |
Spor |
10 |
35 |
71 |
85 |
20. |
M63 |
Mix: Husky-Australian Shepherd |
Rick |
5 |
25 |
46 |
0 |
21. |
M16 |
Norfolk Terrier |
Kust |
3 |
9 |
48 |
245 |
22. |
M73 |
Norwich Terrier |
Hron |
3 |
8 |
0 |
36 |
23. |
M46 |
Old English Sheepdog |
Baum |
4 |
45 |
38 |
122 |
24. |
M19 |
Pug |
Plac |
3 |
9 |
66 |
60 |
25. |
M25 |
Rhodesian Ridgeback |
Jura |
3 |
30 |
0 |
34 |
26. |
M02 |
Schapendoes |
Kour |
1.5 |
25 |
86 |
84 |
27. |
M30 |
Styrian Coarse-haired Hound |
Kubi |
7 |
15 |
45 |
19 |
28. |
M38 |
Transylvanian Hound |
Klem |
0.5 |
30 |
20 |
16 |
records/dogs | 976 | 3880 |
After sampling and the first analysis (which yielded negative or at least ambiguous results) had been completed, we decided to sort the data according to the geomagnetic conditions predominating during the respective sampling times. Correlative values on Earth’s magnetic field strength and direction for all the particular times of recordings were obtained from the Geomagnetic Observatory Fürstenfeldbruck (Munich, Germany): http://www.geophysik.uni-muenchen.de/observatory/geomagnetism
Data on K and C values expressing the magnitude of disturbances in horizontal intensity of the Earth’s magnetic field were obtained from: ftp://ftp.ngdc.noaa.gov/stp/geomagnetic_data/indices/kp_ap/.
Relative declination and intensity changes during the respective dog walks were assorted into the categories according to the relative changes (in percent) calculated from graphs by dividing the difference between the initial and end (minimum/maximum) values by the duration (in minutes) of the respective period of changes (Figure 5).
Circular statistics were carried out with Oriana 4.02 (Kovach Computing). Both pooled individual data and means of particular dogs or walks were considered and analyzed. We performed angular and axial analysis on the measurements of each dog. Second order analysis was performed on the data which yielded the higher significance in the first order analysis (angular or axial). Only dogs with at least five measurements were analyzed. Statistically significant deviations from random distributions were investigated using the Rayleigh test of circular statistics. Differences between distributions were tested for significance with the Mardia-Watson-Wheeler test. Level of significance was set at 5%. Since about 44% data on urination under control conditions originated from one dog (M07, male borzoi) we also performed analyses for this particular dog separately.
Ethics statement
The study did not involve any disturbance of the animals under observation.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors’ contributions
Bur, Har, Nov conceived the study with significant input from Beg and Mal; All authors sampled data and/or coordinated sampling by assistants; Beg, Bur, Har, Mal, Nov carried out statistical analyses; Beg, Bur, Mal wrote the paper with input from Har and Nov; Bur discovered dependency of alignment upon Earth’s magnetic conditions; All authors discussed the findings; Beg, Bur, Har, Mal, Nov interpreted the observations. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Contributor Information
Vlastimil Hart, Email: hart@fld.czu.cz.
Petra Nováková, Email: novakovap@fld.czu.cz.
Erich Pascal Malkemper, Email: pascal.malkemper@uni-due.de.
Sabine Begall, Email: sabine.begall@uni-due.de.
Vladimír Hanzal, Email: hanzal@fld.czu.cz.
Miloš Ježek, Email: jezekm@fld.czu.cz.
Tomáš Kušta, Email: kusta@fld.czu.cz.
Veronika Němcová, Email: vnemcova@fld.czu.cz.
Jana Adámková, Email: adamkjana@seznam.cz.
Kateřina Benediktová, Email: info@valentinka.cz.
Jaroslav Červený, Email: cerveny@fld.czu.cz.
Hynek Burda, Email: hynek.burda@uni-due.de.
Acknowledgements
The study was supported by the Grant Agency of the Czech Republic (project. nr. 506/11/2121). We thank P. Němec for valuable comments on the manuscript. Usable (i.e., fully protocolled) data were provided by the co-authors and our students, colleagues and friends: Ackermannová, Baum, Cukor, Dohmen, Faifr, Garcia-Montero, Grosse, Heglas, Hronková, Juráčková, Klementová, Komín, Kouřimský, Kraus, Krejzková, Kubíček, Leu, Nietsche, Palivcová, Pospíchalová, Ricken, Sporka, Tresky, Zeman, Ždárská.
References
- Begall S, Červený J, Neef J, Vojtěch O, Burda H. Magnetic alignment in grazing and resting cattle and deer. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:13451–13455. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803650105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burda H, Begall S, Červený J, Neef J, Němec P. Extremely low-frequency electromagnetic fields disrupt magnetic alignment of ruminants. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:5708–5713. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811194106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Begall S, Burda H, Červený J, Gerter O, Neef-Weisse J, Němec P. Further support for the alignment of cattle along magnetic field lines. Reply to Hert et al. J Comp Physiol A. 2011;197:1127–1133. doi: 10.1007/s00359-011-0674-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slabý P, Tomanová K, Vácha M. Cattle on pastures do align along the North–South axis, but the alignment depends on herd density. J Comp Physiol A. 2013. DOI 10.1007/s00359-013-0827-5. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Červený J, Begall S, Koubek P, Nováková P, Burda H. Directional preference may enhance hunting accuracy in foraging foxes. Biol Lett. 2011;7:355–357. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2010.1145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Begall S, Malkemper EP, Červený J, Němec P, Burda H. Magnetic alignment in mammals and other animals. Mammal Biol. 2013;78:10–20. [Google Scholar]
- Hart V, Kušta T, Němec P, Bláhová V, Ježek M, Nováková P, Begall S, Červený J, Hanzal V, Malkemper EP, Štípek K, Vole C, Burda H. Magnetic alignment in carps: evidence from the Czech Christmas fish market. PLoS One. 2012;7:e51100. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart V, Malkemper EP, Kušta T, Begall S, Nováková P, Hanzal V, Pleskač L, Ježek M, Policht R, Husinec V, Červený J, Burda H. Directional compass preference for landing in water birds. Frontiers Zool. 2013;10:38. doi: 10.1186/1742-9994-10-38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanderstraeten J, Burda H. Does magnetoreception mediate biological effects of power-frequency magnetic fields? Sci. Total Environm (STOTEN) 2012;417–418:299–304. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2011.08.071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harry W. The homing of a dog. Science. 1922;56:339. doi: 10.1126/science.56.1447.339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips R, Mech L. Homing behavior of a red fox. J Mammal. 1970;51:621. doi: 10.2307/1378406. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Henshaw RE, Stephenson RO. Homing in the gray wolf (Canis lupus) J Mammal. 1974;55:234–237. doi: 10.2307/1379281. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Danner DA, Fisher AR. Evidence of homing by a coyote (Canis latrans) J Mammal. 1977;58:244–245. [Google Scholar]
- Kusak J, Skrbinšek AM, Huber D. Home ranges, movements, and activity of wolves (Canis lupus) in the Dalmatian part of Dinarids, Croatia. Europ J Wildlife Res. 2005;51:254–262. doi: 10.1007/s10344-005-0111-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bensky MK, Gosling SD, Sinn DL. The world from a dog’s point of view: a review and synthesis of dog cognition research. Adv Study Anim Behav. 2013;45:209–406. [Google Scholar]
- Bähr A, Wolf E. Domestic animal models for biomedical research. Repr Domest Anim. 2012;47:59–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0531.2012.02056.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burda H, Marhold S, Westenberger T, Wiltschko W, Wiltschko R. Magnetic compass orientation in the subterranean rodent Cryptomys hottentotus (Bathyergidae, Rodentia) Experientia. 1990;46:528–530. doi: 10.1007/BF01954256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agnetta B, Hare B, Tomasello M. Cues to food location that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different ages do and do not use. Anim Cogn. 2000;3:107–112. doi: 10.1007/s100710000070. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Miklósi Á, Polgárdi R, Topál J, Csányi V. Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs. Anim Cogn. 1998;1:113–121. doi: 10.1007/s100710050016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Virányi Z, Topál J, Gácsi M, Miklósi Á, Csányi V. Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans’ attentional focus. Behav Proc. 2004;66:161–172. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2004.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Courtillot V, Le Mouel JL. Time variations of the Earth’s magnetic field: from daily to secular. Ann Rev Earth Planet Sci. 1988;16:389–476. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ea.16.050188.002133. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Liboff AR. Why are Living Things Sensitive to Weak Magnetic Fields? Electromagn Biol Med. 2013. pp. 1–5. doi:10.3109/15368378.2013.809579. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Southern WE. Influence of disturbances in the earth’s magnetic field on Ring-billed Gull orientation. Condor. 1972;74:102–105. [Google Scholar]
- Keeton WT, Larkin TS, Windsor DM. Normal fluctuations in the earth’s magnetic field influence pigeon orientation. J Comp Physiol. 1974;95:95–103. doi: 10.1007/BF00610108. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moore FR. Geomagnetic disturbance and the orientation of nocturnally migrating birds. Science. 1977;196:682–684. doi: 10.1126/science.854743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin H, Lindauer M. Der Einfluss des Erdmagnetfeldes auf die Schwereorientierung der Honigbiene (Apis mellifera) J Comp Physiol. 1977;122:145–187. doi: 10.1007/BF00611888. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vanselow KH, Ricklefs K. Are solar activity and sperm whale Physeter macrocephalus strandings around the North Sea related? J Sea Res. 2005;53:319–327. doi: 10.1016/j.seares.2004.07.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dimitrova S, Stoilova I, Cholakov I. Influence of local geomagnetic storms on arterial blood pressure. Bioelectromagnetics. 2004;25:408–414. doi: 10.1002/bem.20009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berk M, Dodd S, Henry M. Do ambient electromagnetic fields affect behaviour? A demonstration of the relationship between geomagnetic storm activity and suicide. Bioelectromagnetics. 2006;27:151–155. doi: 10.1002/bem.20190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ritz T, Thalau P, Phillips JB, Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W. Resonance effects indicate a radical-pair mechanism for avian magnetic compass. Nature. 2004;429:177–180. doi: 10.1038/nature02534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winklhofer M, Kirschvink JL. A quantitative assessment of torque-transducer models for magnetoreception. J Roy Soc Interface. 2010;7:273–289. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2009.0435.focus. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips JB, Muheim R, Jorge PE. A behavioral perspective on the biophysics of the light-dependent magnetic compass: a link between directional and spatial perception? J exp Biol. 2010;213:3247–3255. doi: 10.1242/jeb.020792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]