Abstract
The purpose of this study was to investigate the emergence of displayed alcohol references on Facebook for first-year students from two universities. Graduated high school seniors who were planning to attend one of the two targeted study universities were recruited. Participants’ Facebook profiles were evaluated for displayed alcohol references at baseline and every four weeks throughout the first year of college. Profiles were categorized as Non-Displayers, Alcohol Displayers or Intoxication/Problem Drinking Displayers. Analyses included logistic regression, univariate and multivariate Cox proportional hazard analysis and multi-state Markov modeling. A total of 338 participants were recruited, 56.1% were female, 74.8% were Caucasian, and 58.8% were from University A. At baseline, 68 Facebook profiles (20.1%) included displayed alcohol references. During the first year of college, 135 (39.9%) profiles newly displayed alcohol. In multivariate Cox proportional hazard analysis, university (University B versus A, HR = 0.47, 95% CI: 0.28–0.77, p = 0.003), number of Facebook friends (HR = 1.19, 95% CI: 1.09–1.28, p < 0.001 for every 100 more friends), and average monthly status updates (HR = 1.03, 95% CI: 1.002–1.05, p = 0.033) were identified as independent predictors for new alcohol display. Findings contribute to understanding the patterns and predictors for displayed alcohol references on Facebook.
Keywords: Facebook, College student, Alcohol
1. Introduction
Adolescents’ transition from high school to college is often accompanied by escalation of alcohol behaviors. For some first-year students, experimentation with alcohol use begins with arrival at college and exposure to both new social settings and increased independence. Among students who did not drink heavily in high school, approximately 20% initiate this behavior in college (Wechsler et al., 2002). For other students, arrival at college may prompt a transition from experimentation to frequent alcohol use (Johnson, O’Malley, Bachman, & Schulenberg, 2007). Though alcohol use is common, high-risk drinking remains a major cause of morbidity and mortality in the college population (Association, 2009).
While alcohol use is readily visible in the corporal collegiate setting, it is also displayed in virtual collegiate contexts. Alcohol use and abuse is often displayed on college students’ Facebook profiles; up to 83% of college students’ profiles include displayed references to alcohol (Egan & Moreno, 2011; Moreno, Parks, Zimmerman, Brito, & Christakis, 2009) These displayed alcohol references on Facebook are likely to have broad reach, as the vast majority of college students maintain a Facebook profile, most report daily use and college students have large social networks within the website (Buffardi & Campbell, 2008; Lewis, Kaufman, & Christakis, 2008; Sachdev et al., 2012).
1.1. Influence on alcohol behavior
Previous work, rooted in social learning theory, has established strong links between what adolescents see in daily life and how they act (Bandura, 1986). While there are many influences involved in a first-year college student’s decision to initiate alcohol use, two salient ones that may be encountered on a daily basis are peers and media. Observation of peers is a major source of influence on adolescent health attitudes, intentions and behaviors (Keefe, 1994; Wood, Read, Mitchell, & Brand, 2004). Previous work has illustrated that both close peers and the larger social network influence alcohol behaviors (Mundt, 2011). Equally strong are links between adolescent health behaviors and exposure to media content (Dalton et al., 2009, 2003; Gidwani, Sobol, DeJong, Perrin, & Gortmaker, 2002; Titus-Ernstoff, Dalton, Adachi-Mejia, Longacre, & Beach, 2008). Studies have shown that exposure to substance use in traditional media such as television or movies is associated with initiation of these behaviors, leading some to describe television as a “superpeer” (Dalton et al., 2009; Gidwani et al., 2002; Klein et al., 1993; Robinson, Chen, & Killen, 1998; Strasburger, Wilson, & Jordan, 2008).
1.2. Social media combines peer and media influences
Social media sites such as Facebook allow adolescents to display information about their identities, communicate with peers and build an online social network. As social media combines peer and media effects, it thereby represents a powerful potential motivator of behavior. Adolescents are uniquely positioned to be vulnerable to the influence of what they see on social media: they are at once early adopters of technologies, nearly ubiquitous users and highly susceptible to peer influences (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007; Lenhart & Madden, 2007; Lenhart, Madden, & Hitlin, 2005; Lenhart, Purcell, Smith, & Zickuhr, 2010). A previous study found that younger teens viewed displayed references to alcohol on social networking site profiles by their peers as both influential and believable as representations of offline behaviors (Moreno, Briner, Williams, Walker, & Christakis, 2009). Further, adolescents who view alcohol content on Facebook profiles are more likely to perceive that alcohol use is a normative behavior, and are more likely to report interest in initiating alcohol use (Litt & Stock, 2011). Thus, social media likely has influence over those who view and interact with it, particularly regarding substance use.
1.3. First year college students: seeking a new peer group
Incoming college students may be particularly influenced by peers’ Facebook content as they learn to navigate social norms and expectations in their new college setting. An early task when entering college is establishing new friends. In this process it is likely that adolescents use Facebook to evaluate a new acquaintance’s profile to learn more about a potential new friend (Chou & Edge, 2012). If displayed alcohol references are present on a new peer’s Facebook profile, this may contribute to perceptions of social norms of students regarding alcohol behaviors. Social learning theory suggests that exposure to alcohol use in media may influence behavior through promoting positive attitudes and intentions towards the displayed behavior. (Bandura, 1977; Bandura, 2001; Glanz, Rimer, & Lewis, 2002) If an adolescent is exposed to displayed alcohol content prior to or upon arriving at college there are potential implications for that adolescents’ attitudes, intentions and behaviors regarding alcohol.
1.4. Purpose
While it is well understood that the first year of college often leads to changes in alcohol behaviors in the offline world, little is understood regarding how these exposures or experiences change online displays on Facebook. A current gap in the literature involves understanding the emergence of alcohol references on Facebook among early college students. Previous work has evaluated displayed alcohol content on Facebook profiles; however, these evaluations have been limited by being observational only and by collecting cross sectional data at one time point (Egan & Moreno, 2011; Moreno, Parks et al., 2009). Thus, the purpose of this study was to investigate the emergence of displayed alcohol references on Facebook for two universities over the first year of college. Specifically, we investigated baseline prevalence and predictors of displayed alcohol references on Facebook, and the emergence of displayed alcohol content over time. We sought to understand predictors in the emergence of displayed alcohol content, including the role of university context and time of year. This information may contribute to a deeper understanding regarding which students choose to display alcohol content on Facebook and what these displays may mean both to those who display and those who view such content.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Setting
This study was conducted between May 2011 and July 2012 and received approval from the two relevant university Institutional Review Boards.
2.2. Subjects
Graduated high school seniors who were planning to attend one of the two targeted study universities were recruited the summer prior to beginning college. Participants were eligible if they were between the ages of 17 and 19 years and enrolled as first-year full-time students for fall 2011 at one of these two universities. A subset of approximately 600 potential participants were randomly selected from the full registrar’s lists of incoming first-year students from both universities for recruitment towards our goal of recruiting 320 participants.
2.3. Recruitment
Students were recruited through several steps, beginning with a pre-announcement postcard. Over a 4 week recruitment period potentially eligible students were recruited through up to 4 rounds of emails, phone calls and Facebook messages. Students were excluded if they were outside the age range for this study. Students were also excluded if they had already arrived on campus for summer early-enrollment programs, as baseline measures were intended to measure pre-college experiences.
2.4. Consent process and Facebook friending
During the consent process potential participants were informed that this was a longitudinal study involving a baseline phone interview as well as evaluation of Facebook profiles, and that friending our research team profile was a requirement of the study. Participants were informed that content would be viewed, but that no one on the research team would post any information to the participant’s profile. Participants were asked to maintain open security settings with our research team during the study. Students who provided consent to enroll in the study were sent a Facebook friend request from one of our research assistant Facebook profiles designated for use in this study.
2.5. Codebook and variables
An existing codebook was used to evaluate displayed alcohol references. This codebook has been described in previous publications and studies (Egan & Moreno, 2011; Moreno, Egan, & Brockman, 2011). This research codebook was initially designed to evaluate displayed alcohol references on Facebook that represented alcohol behaviors. For the purposes of this study, the definition of displayed alcohol content was expanded applying the theory of planned behavior as a conceptual framework (Ajzen, 1985; Ajzen, 1991). This theory supports the importance of attitudes and intentions predicting behaviors. Thus, displayed alcohol content referred to attitudes, intentions or behaviors regarding alcohol were considered displayed alcohol references.
Profiles were categorized into one of three groups. Profiles without any alcohol references were considered “Non-Displayers.” Profiles with one or more references to alcohol attitudes, intentions or behaviors but no references to intoxication or problem drinking were considered “Alcohol Displayers.” Example references included personal photographs in which the profile owner was drinking from a beer bottle, or text references describing drinking vodka at a party. Only text references that explicitly mentioned the profile owner’s attitudes, intentions or behaviors towards alcohol or photographs that included the profile owner with a clearly labeled alcoholic beverage were coded as an alcohol reference. The codebook was designed to be conservative in approach; thus, while a red solo cup may be indicative of alcohol beverages to some college students it would not be counted in our coding approach.
Profiles in which there were one or more references to attitudes, intentions or behaviors regarding intoxication or problem drinking behaviors were considered “Intoxication/Problem Drinking (I/PD) Displayers.” Examples of intoxication references included text describing the profile owner as “being wasted” or “getting drunk.” Similar to previous work, problem drinking was defined using the CRAFFT problem drinking criteria which has been validated in adolescent populations (Knight et al., 1999; Wilson, Sherritt, Gates, & Knight, 2004). Criteria include driving or riding in a car while intoxicated (C = car), drinking to relax (R = relax), drinking alone (A = alone), forgetting what one did while drinking or blacking out (F = forget), having friends or family ask you to cut down on alcohol (F = friends/family) or getting into trouble because of alcohol use such as being arrested (T = trouble) (Knight et al., 1999; McGee & Begg, 2008; Wilson et al., 2004). Because of the concern regarding subjectivity of evaluating photographs as depictions of alcohol use versus intoxication, photographs were not considered in this category.
Other recorded data from Facebook profiles included the type of displayed alcohol reference (e.g. status update, photograph or group). We also recorded the total number of Facebook friends to assess the participants’ Facebook social network, as well as the number of posted status updates by the profile owner in that four week period as a measure of interactive Facebook use.
2.6. Coding procedure
The Facebook profile of each participant was initially evaluated at the time of enrollment in the study by a trained coder. This baseline evaluation included a 3-month period of the participant’s senior year of high school (March, April and May). Once the participant arrived at college, the profile was evaluated every four weeks for displayed content during that 4-week period. Each coding period began at the day after the last evaluation and included 28 days. The ninth and final coding period was at the conclusion of the academic year. At each Facebook profile evaluation, we recorded displayed alcohol reference data, including the coder’s typewritten description of any image references or verbatim text from profiles, as well as Facebook use data. If present, identifiable information was removed from text references. Sections of the Facebook profile that were evaluated included the wall, photographs, groups and likes/interests.
A total of 7 coders evaluated profiles in this study, and all had undergone a minimum 3 month training period. A 20% random subsample of profiles were evaluated by all coders to test interrater reliability. Fleiss’ Kappa statistic was used to evaluate the extent to which there was overall agreement in the coding of the presence or absence of alcohol references on a profile, as well as agreement among coders for the number of references on a profile. Fleiss’ kappa was 0.82 for the presence or absence of alcohol references present on profiles indicating near-perfect agreement, and 0.74 for the agreement among all coders for the number of alcohol references indicating substantial agreement.
2.7. Interview variables
Interviews assessed demographic data including age, gender, ethnicity/race and university attending. At baseline, participants were asked whether they had ever consumed alcohol.
2.8. Interview procedure
Phone interviews were conducted with all participants at the time of enrollment as a baseline evaluation. Phone interviews were used because many participants were more than an hour away from the primary research site, and because phone interviews have been used successfully in the past as a way to investigate stigmatizing topics such as risky health behaviors (Fortney et al., 2004; Meyer, Rossano, Ellis, & Bradford, 2002). Interviews were scheduled at a time of convenience for the participants. Interviews lasted approximately 30–40 min and included other questions relevant to the larger context of the study not reported here. Participants received $30 as an incentive for completing the interview.
2.9. Analysis
Demographic variables and displayed alcohol references on Facebook were evaluated with descriptive statistics. All P values were 2-sided, and p < .05 was used to indicate statistical significance. Statistical analyses were performed using SAS software version 9.2 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC) and R software version 2.15.1 (www.cran.r-project.org).
2.9.1. Prevalence and types of displayed references to alcohol on Facebook
To describe the prevalence and types of displayed alcohol content, demographic variables and displayed alcohol references on Facebook were summarized in frequency tables for categorical variables and in terms of means and standard deviations for continuous variables.
2.9.2. Predictors of emergence of alcohol displays over the first year of college
To evaluate predictors of displayed alcohol content on Facebook at baseline and over the first year of college we conducted logistic regression and Cox proportional hazard analysis. First, to determine predictors of baseline display of alcohol use on Facebook, we used univariate and multivariate logistic regression analyses. Baseline characteristics included as predictors in this model included gender, race, university, number of Facebook friends and whether the participant had ever used alcohol at baseline.
Second, we used univariate and multivariate Cox proportional hazard analysis to evaluate predictors for time to emergence of displayed alcohol references on Facebook. In these analyses, a new alcohol display on Facebook was defined as an event. The follow-up durations of subjects who did not have an alcohol display were censored at the end of the academic year. Predictive variables were selected via forward stepwise selection with a p-value cutoff of <0.05 to identify a final parsimonious Cox proportional hazard model; a previously deleted variable was allowed to re-enter the final model if its p-value was <0.05. Standard model diagnostic tools were utilized to verify the proportional hazard assumption. Hazard ratios (HR) and the corresponding 95% confidence intervals were reported for each predictor.
2.9.3. Patterns in displayed references over time
Finally, to understand the emergence of alcohol displays over time we used a multi-state Markov modeling approach to describe the dynamics of moving through the three categories of Facebook alcohol display: Non-Displayer, Alcohol Displayer and I/PD Displayer (Welton & Ades, 2005). These three categories were defined as the states of the Markov model. In the context of this model, the transitions were assumed to be unidirectional and included transitions from a Non-Displayer to an Alcohol Displayer, or from a Non-Displayer to a I/PD Displayer, or from Alcohol Displayer to I/PD displayer. As Facebook profiles were evaluated every four weeks for displayed content, each coding period was considered one time period. The instantaneous risk or hazard of moving from one state to the next state for each time period was modeled using a log-linear model. Both univariate and multivariate analysis which incorporated gender, race, number of Facebook friends at baseline and university was conducted. Hazard ratios and the corresponding 95% confidence intervals for the probabilities of transitioning from one state to the next state were reported for the comparisons between groups.
3. Results
3.1. Participants
A total of 338 participants were recruited, of these 56.1% were female, 74.8% were Caucasian, and 58.8% were from University A, average age was 18.4 years (SD = 0.6). Our initial response rate was 54.6%, and by the end of the yearlong study our retention rate was 93.1%. Table 1 illustrates demographic characteristics of the participants in this study.
Table 1.
N = 338 | Number (%) |
---|---|
Gender | |
Female | 189 (56.08%) |
Male | 148 (43.92%) |
University | |
University A | 198 (58.75%) |
University B | 139 (41.25%) |
Ethnicity | |
Caucasian/White | 252 (74.8%) |
Asian | 39 (11.6%) |
More than One | 21 (6.2%) |
Hispanic | 13 (3.9%) |
African American/Black | 5 (1.5%) |
East Indian | 3 (0.9%) |
Native American/Alaskan | 2 (0.6%) |
Other | 2 (0.6%) |
Number of Facebook friends at baseline | Median (range) |
470 (46–2045) |
3.2. Prevalence and types of displayed references to alcohol on Facebook
At baseline, prior to starting college, 68 Facebook profiles (20.1%) included references to alcohol. Among these initial displayers, 46 (13.6%) were Alcohol Displayers and 22 (6.5%) were Intoxication/Problem Drinking displayers. There were no significant differences in baseline distribution of alcohol references by gender, race or university.
During the first year of college, 135 profiles newly displayed alcohol references on Facebook; these included 105 new Alcohol Displayers and 30 new Intoxication/Problem Drinking displayers. Among those who already were Alcohol Displayers at baseline, 35 of the 46 Alcohol Displayer profiles (76%) newly displayed Intoxication/Problem Drinking references. Thus, a total of 257 participants’ remained Non-Displayers before and throughout the first year of college. Table 2 illustrates these data.
Table 2.
Facebook alcohol display category | Time 1: Prior to entering college N (%, 95% CI) |
Time 2: By the end of the first year of college N (%, 95% CI) |
---|---|---|
Any alcohol display on Facebook | 68 (20.1%, 16–24.8%) | 203 (60.1%, 54.6–65.3%) |
Alcohol display on Facebook | 46 (13.6%, 10.1–17.7%) | 116 (34.3%, 29.3–39.7%) |
I/PD display on Facebooka | 22 (6.5%, 4.1–9.7%) | 87 (25.7%, 21.2–30.8%) |
Non-displayers | 268 (79.3%, 74.6–83.5%) | 135 (39.9%, 34.7–45.4%) |
I/PD = Intoxication/Problem Drinking.
Displayed alcohol references were present in a variety of formats on the Facebook profile including status updates, photographs, likes and joining groups. Displayed references included attitudes towards alcohol; examples included “liking” groups such as alcohol brands (i.e. Bud Light, Corona). References to drinking intentions were also present, often through planning future alcohol-related events. Examples of these types of references included comments such as “Can’t wait for Friday beer pong night!” or “I’ve got a six pack of MGD and just waiting for the game to start.” References to alcohol use also included displayed behaviors through photographs, and text describing drinking experiences, including problem drinking behaviors. Table 3 illustrates example references from both Alcohol Displayers and I/PD Displayers.
Table 3.
Status update | Photograph | Group | |
---|---|---|---|
Alcohol references | “Throwin’ a few Buds back with my buds tonight” | “Beer Pong!” | |
“save water, drink margaritas” | “Prague Pub Crawl” | ||
Intoxication/Problem Drinking references | “Blacked out last night… the question is…go to class or go to sleep…” “sooooooooo druuuuuunnnkkkkk” |
“Drunk people taking care of drunker people” |
3.3. Predictors of emergence of alcohol displays over the first year of college
Multivariate logistic regression using baseline data showed that the odds of alcohol display prior to college among current drinkers was 2.68 times (95% CI: 1.44–4.99) that of nondrinkers (p = 0.002). Further, the odds of displaying alcohol at baseline increased by 10.5% (95% CI: 0–22.1%) for every 100 more Facebook friends.
The likelihood of displaying alcohol references on Facebook during the first year of college did not differ by gender or race. In the multivariate Cox proportional hazard analysis, university (University B versus A, HR = 0.47, 95% CI: 0.28–0.77, p = 0.003), number of Facebook friends (HR = 1.19, 95% CI: 1.09–1.28, p < 0.001) for every 100 more friends, and total average monthly status updates (HR = 1.03, 95% CI: 1.002–1.05, p = 0.033) were identified as independent predictors for new alcohol display.
3.4. Facebook alcohol displays over time
Facebook alcohol displays varied in quantity over time and across university site. The univariate Cox proportional hazard analysis illustrates these temporal variations for each of the two university sites in Fig. 1. One noted difference is the increased display rates concomitant with the start of November at University A, which was associated with increased displays related to alcohol-themed Halloween parties. A second noted increase in displays at University A was seen in early May, which corresponds to increased displays related to a large alcohol-themed block party.
Multi-state Markov modeling revealed that while many profiles remained in the same Facebook alcohol display category in which they started for many coding periods, that there was often a progression from Non-Displayer to Alcohol Displayer to I/PD Displayer. Fig. 2 illustrates the path of these transitions. In any given month, the predicted transition probability for progressing from Non-Displayer to Alcohol Displayer was 5.4% (95% CI: 4.5–6.4%), for progressing from an Alcohol Displayer to an I/PD Displayer it was 5.0% (95% CI: 3.5–6.6%), and for progressing from an Non-Displayer to an I/PD Displayer it was 1.5% (95% CI: 1.1–2.1%) (Table 2). In the multivariate analysis, males demonstrated smaller risk for transitioning from Alcohol Displayers to I/PD Displayers compared to females (HR = 0.45, 95% CI: 0.23–0.86). Race was not significantly associated with transition between alcohol displayer categories. Participants with a large number of Facebook friends at baseline (>500) were at higher risk of transitioning from Non-Displayer to Alcohol Displayer (HR = 2.80, 95% CI: 1.89–4.18) when compared to participants with a smaller number of Facebook friends at baseline (≤500). Participants from University A were at greater risk (HR = 2.89, 95% CI: 1.75–4.75) of transitioning from Non-Displayer to Alcohol Displayer compared to University B (Table 2).
4. Discussion
This longitudinal study evaluated displayed content on Facebook over the first year of college from students at two universities, and assessed the emergence of displayed alcohol references during this critical transition time. We found that displays were uncommon prior to college, and similar prevalence of displays among students intending to attend both universities. Over the first year of college, alcohol displays on Facebook dramatically increased in a variety of multimedia formats. We assessed predictors of displayed alcohol use and found that university, number of Facebook friends and Facebook activity were predictors. Finally, we assessed patterns in display over time and found that displayed references to alcohol on Facebook often increased concomitant with campus drinking events.
4.1. Displays provide rich and contextual data
Findings illustrate that displayed alcohol content is featured across a variety of multimedia formats. Placing these findings in the context of previous work regarding peer and media influences on attitudes, intentions and behaviors illustrates several key points. First, previous work has illustrated that media can be a powerful source of influence on behavior. Social learning theory predicts that adolescents who observe media characters referencing or engaging in behaviors such as alcohol use without experiencing negative consequences will be more likely to adopt the behaviors portrayed (Bandura, 1977, 1986, 2001; Glanz et al., 2002). Previous work has shown that adolescents who are exposed to alcohol use in media such as television are more likely to drink alcohol and view this as a normative behavior (Robinson et al., 1998; Sargent, Wills, Stoolmiller, Gibson, & Gibbons, 2006). Second, displayed Facebook content is rooted in the daily and familiar lives of college students. Previous work has illustrated that when something experienced or seen through the media is similar to one’s experiences in the social environment, the influential effects of the media can be much more powerful (Greene et al., 2012). Third, social media presents a form of media created by peers, who are also understood to have great influence over adolescent behavior. Personal sources of information are typically viewed with more credibility and persuasiveness than traditional media (Brook, Lee, Brown, Finch, & Brook, 2012; Du, Huang, Zhao, & Hser, 2013). As social media likely combines the influence of both peers and media, Facebook likely represents a new and influential source of influence for adolescents’ attitudes, intentions and behaviors regarding alcohol use.
4.2. Differences between the two universities
Findings also suggest significant differences in the prevalence of displayed alcohol references on Facebook between the two universities in this study. At baseline, there were no significant differences in prevalence of displayed alcohol references on Facebook between the students from the two university settings. By the end of the first year of college, students attending University B were half as likely to display references to alcohol on Facebook compared to students at University A; these differences are also clearly visualized through Fig. 1. These differences only emerge after students have arrived at college and become immersed in the social norms of that school. These norms may impact both alcohol behaviors as well as what material is socially acceptable to display on Facebook at that school. It is well understood that different universities have different drinking rates, different drinking policies and consequences, as well as varying social norms regarding the role of alcohol use at a given university. The culture at University A may have placed increased importance on alcohol use behaviors, leading to an increased presence of alcohol content on students’ Facebook profiles. It is also possible that findings reflect differences in the states in which these universities reside, such as variations in laws regarding underage alcohol use. Conversely, these two universities may have cultural differences regarding what is acceptable to display on Facebook. Previous work evaluated differences in two countries’ cultures in what level of personal information is acceptable or desirable to display on Facebook and found marked differences (Huang & Park, 2012). In contrast, findings in this study derive from similar populations of college students at large state universities who are displaced by location in the country. Thus, findings suggest the importance of local culture and context within a university impacting Facebook displays.
4.3. Links between behavior and display
Findings suggest that participants who displayed alcohol references on Facebook prior to college were also more likely to report lifetime alcohol use prior to college. This finding may be unsurprising given that previous work has suggested that display of alcohol on Facebook is associated with alcohol use (Moreno, Christakis, Egan, Brockman, & Becker, 2012). Students who displayed references to alcohol prior to college were highly likely to escalate Facebook displays to include references to intoxication or problem drinking during the first year of college. These potential links between behavior and display are further supported by our findings regarding the timing of displays. Displays were more common for University A participants during times in which large alcohol-themed events were taking place on campus. This suggests links between attending these events, drinking alcohol and displaying on Facebook. Further, the progression of transition between Facebook alcohol display categories from Non-Displayer to Alcohol Displayer to I/PD Displayer suggests a logical progression in display that mirrors a common progression in behavior.
4.4. Other factors that may contribute to Facebook alcohol displays
There are other reasons to consider for why Facebook displays regarding alcohol use may have increased during the first year of college. It may be that once they are in college, students perceive fewer risks in displaying these behaviors publicly on social media sites. As high school students may face suspension or other negative consequences from such displays, many college students who have engaged in drinking since prior to college may decide upon arrival to college that it is now safe to display these ongoing behaviors on Facebook.
4.5. Limitations
There are several limitations to our study. Content analysis coding can be considered subjective; in order to provide the most detailed and comprehensive coding possible we provided extensive training to every coder. Our interrater agreement suggests high reliability among our coders. Second, though we included two large universities in this study with varied locations and student profiles, there was limited racial diversity present at both schools. Thus, generalization to other colleges or non-college attending older adolescents should be approached with caution. Third, our study did not present results related to participants who lived on campus versus with parents which could have bearing on alcohol behaviors. Finally, our study provides rich description over time and assessments of associations, but no causality linking alcohol displays and behaviors can be inferred from these findings.
4.6. Conclusions
Despite these limitations, our findings have important implications. This study provides a detailed longitudinal assessment of similarities and differences across two universities first-year students’ displayed alcohol content. Future research should consider why differences in disclosure may occur between universities, and further expand our understanding of how computer-mediated-communication may be influenced by both corporal and virtual contexts.
Practical implications for universities include the possibility of using these findings to enhance ongoing prevention efforts. The population of incoming students who already display references to alcohol prior to college may represent a unique population to target with prevention messages. Universities could provide prevention messages to these incoming students within Facebook by creating pop-up advertisements linked to alcohol-related keywords and targeted to first-year students.
Further, universities may consider whether time periods in which Facebook alcohol displays escalate are associated with high risk times for student alcohol use or abuse. At University A, the increases in Facebook alcohol displays correspond with known high-risk times for alcohol abuse on campus, including a yearly Halloween party. However, the patterns in timing of emergent displayed alcohol references illustrate some patterns that may merit investigation, such as the rise in newly displayed alcohol references in early March for University B. While our findings highlight differences in timing of display between the two universities in this study, it is likely that patterns of high and low alcohol displays on Facebook occur across many universities. Universities could consider using Facebook alcohol displays as a barometer to predict time periods of heightened awareness for university health services or law enforcement. It is important to consider that these efforts could be limited by college student privacy settings on social media. However, colleges could also consider creating targeted Facebook alcohol advertisements featuring alcohol safety messages or health care services that pop-up in response to common alcohol-related terms displayed on Facebook. By applying an understanding of the emergence and patterns of displayed alcohol content on Facebook, colleges could enhance provision of relevant alcohol safety messages to first-year students linked to the messages that students themselves present on Facebook.
Finally, it is worth considering whether universities should play a role in discouraging displayed alcohol content on Facebook by their students. Students may underestimate the potential implications for employment or future educational opportunities that could be impacted by displayed alcohol content on Facebook. Further, the potential for legal repercussions from posting potential evidence of underage drinking remains an additional serious possible negative consequence.
Acknowledgements
This study was funded by Grant R01DA031580-03 which is supported by the Common Fund, managed by the OD/Office of Strategic Coordination (OSC). The authors would also like to thank Megan Pumper, Libby Brockman, Mara Stewart, Leah Wachowski, Hope Villiard and Natalie Goniu for their assistance with this study.
Footnotes
Implications and contribution
Alcohol is frequently referenced on college students’ Facebook profiles. Using content analysis, we found that university context and Facebook use were predictors in emergence of displayed alcohol references over the first year of college.
References
- Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 1991;50:179–211. [Google Scholar]
- Ajzen I. From intentions to actionas: A theory of planned behavior. In: Kuhl J, Beckman J, editors. Action-control: From cognition to behavior. Heidelberg: Springer; 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Association ACH. American College Health Association: National College Health Assessment II: Reference Group Data Report Fall 2008. Baltimore: American College Health Association; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Social learning theory. New York: General Learning Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Review of Psychology. 2001;52:1–26. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brook JS, Lee JY, Brown EN, Finch SJ, Brook DW. Individuality and contextual influences on drug dependence: a 15-year Prospective longitudinal study of adolescents from Harlem. Journal of Genetic Psychology. 2012;173(4):355–373. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2011.620998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buffardi LE, Campbell WK. Narcissism and social networking Web sites. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2008;34(10):1303–1314. doi: 10.1177/0146167208320061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chou HT, Edge N. “They are happier and having better lives than I am”: the impact of using Facebook on perceptions of others’ lives. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2012;15(2):117–121. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2011.0324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalton MA, Beach ML, Adachi-Mejia AM, Longacre MR, Matzkin AL, Sargent JD, et al. Early exposure to movie smoking predicts established smoking by older teens and young adults. Pediatrics. 2009;123(4):e551–e558. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalton MA, Sargent JD, Beach ML, Titus-Ernstoff L, Gibson JJ, Ahrens MB, et al. Effect of viewing smoking in movies on adolescent smoking initiation: A cohort study. Lancet. 2003;362(9380):281–285. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)13970-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Du J, Huang D, Zhao M, Hser YI. Drug-abusing offenders with co-morbid mental disorders: Gender differences in problem severity, treatment participation, and recidivism. Biomedical and Environmental Sciences. 2013;26(1):32–39. doi: 10.3967/0895-3988.2013.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egan KG, Moreno MA. Alcohol references on undergraduate males’ facebook profiles. American Journal of Mens Health. 2011 doi: 10.1177/1557988310394341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellison NB, Steinfield C, Lampe C. The benefits of Facebook “Friends:” Social Capitol and College Students’ Use of Online Social Network Sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 2007;12:1143–1168. [Google Scholar]
- Fortney J, Mukherjee S, Curran G, Fortney S, Han XT, Booth BM. Factors associated with perceived stigma for alcohol use and treatment among at-risk drinkers. Journal of Behavioral Health Services and Research. 2004;31(4):418–429. doi: 10.1007/BF02287693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gidwani PP, Sobol A, DeJong W, Perrin JM, Gortmaker SL. Television viewing and initiation of smoking among youth. Pediatrics. 2002;110(3):505–508. doi: 10.1542/peds.110.3.505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glanz K, Rimer B, Lewis F. Health behavior and health education: Theory, research and practice. San Francisco, CA: Wiley and Sons; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Greene GW, White AA, Hoerr SL, Lohse B, Schembre SM, Riebe D, et al. Impact of an online healthful eating and physical activity program for college students. American Journal of Health Promotion. 2012;27(2):e47–e58. doi: 10.4278/ajhp.110606-QUAN-239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang CM, Park D. Cultural influences on Facebook photographs. International Journal of Psychology. 2012 doi: 10.1080/00207594.2011.649285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE. Monitoring the future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2006: Volume II, College students and adults aged 19–45. In: N.I.o.D. Abuse, editor. Bethesda, MD: NIH; 2007. Publication No 07-6206. [Google Scholar]
- Keefe K. Perceptions of normative social pressure and attitudes toward alcohol use: changes during adolescence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1994;55(1):46–54. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1994.55.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein JD, Brown JD, Childers KW, Oliveri J, Porter C, Dykers C. Adolescents’ risky behavior and mass media use. Pediatrics. 1993;92(1):24–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knight JR, Shrier LA, Bravender TD, Farrell M, Vander Bilt J, Shaffer HJ. A new brief screen for adolescent substance abuse. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 1999;153(6):591–596. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.153.6.591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenhart A, Purcell K, Smith A, Zickuhr K. In: Social media and young adults. P.I.a.A.L. Project, editor. Washington, DC: Pew Internet and American Life Project; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Lenhart A, Madden M. Social networking sites and teens: An overview. Pew Internet and American Life Project. 2007 [Google Scholar]
- Lenhart A, Madden M, Hitlin P. Teens and technology: Youth are leading the transition to a fully wired and mobile nation. Pew Internet and American Life Project. 2005 [Google Scholar]
- Lewis K, Kaufman J, Christakis N. The taste for privacy: An analysis of college student privacy settings in an online social network. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication. 2008;14(1):79. [Google Scholar]
- Litt DM, Stock ML. Adolescent alcohol-related risk cognitions: The roles of social norms and social networking sites. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2011 doi: 10.1037/a0024226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGee JB, Begg M. What medical educators need to know about “Web 2.0”. Medical Teacher. 2008;30(2):164–169. doi: 10.1080/01421590701881673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer IH, Rossano L, Ellis JM, Bradford J. A brief telephone interview to identify lesbian and bisexual women in random digit dialing sampling. Journal of Sex Research. 2002;39(2):139–144. doi: 10.1080/00224490209552133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moreno MA, Briner LR, Williams A, Walker L, Christakis DA. Real use or “real cool”: adolescents speak out about displayed alcohol references on social networking websites. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2009;45(4):420–422. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moreno MA, Christakis DA, Egan KG, Brockman LN, Becker T. Associations between displayed alcohol references on Facebook and problem drinking among college students. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2012;166(2):157–163. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moreno MA, Egan KG, Brockman L. Development of a researcher codebook for use in evaluating social networking site profiles. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2011;49(1):29–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.04.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moreno MA, Parks MR, Zimmerman FJ, Brito TE, Christakis DA. Display of health risk behaviors on MySpace by adolescents: Prevalence and Associations. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine. 2009;163(1):35–41. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2008.528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mundt MP. The impact of peer social networks on adolescent alcohol use initiation. Academic Pediatric. 2011;11(5):414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2011.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TN, Chen HL, Killen JD. Television and music video exposure and risk of adolescent alcohol use. Pediatrics. 1998;102(5):E54. doi: 10.1542/peds.102.5.e54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachdev PS, Lipnicki DM, Crawford J, Reppermund S, Kochan NA, Trollor JN, et al. Risk profiles for mild cognitive impairment vary by age and sex: The Sydney memory and ageing study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2012 doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31825461b0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sargent JD, Wills TA, Stoolmiller M, Gibson J, Gibbons FX. Alcohol use in motion pictures and its relation with early-onset teen drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2006;67(1):54–65. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strasburger VC, Wilson BJ, Jordan A. Children, adolescents and the media. Beverly Hills: Sage; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Titus-Ernstoff L, Dalton MA, Adachi-Mejia AM, Longacre MR, Beach ML. Longitudinal study of viewing smoking in movies and initiation of smoking by children. Pediatrics. 2008;121(1):15–21. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-0051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler H, Lee JE, Kuo M, Seibring M, Nelson TF, Lee H. Trends in college binge drinking during a period of increased prevention efforts. Findings from 4 Harvard School of Public Health College Alcohol Study surveys: 1993–2001. Journal of American College Health. 2002;50(5):203–217. doi: 10.1080/07448480209595713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welton N, Ades A. Estimation of Markov chain transition probabilities and rates from fully and partially observed data: Uncertainty propagation, evidence synthesis, and model calibration. Medical Decision Making. 2005;25:633–645. doi: 10.1177/0272989X05282637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson CR, Sherritt L, Gates E, Knight JR. Are clinical impressions of adolescent substance use accurate? Pediatrics. 2004;114(5):e536–e540. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood MD, Read JP, Mitchell RE, Brand NH. Do parents still matter? Parent and peer influences on alcohol involvement among recent high school graduates. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2004;18(1):19–30. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.18.1.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]