Abstract
Visual performance and visual interactions in pelagic animals are notoriously hard to investigate because of our restricted access to the habitat. The pelagic visual world is also dramatically different from benthic or terrestrial habitats, and our intuition is less helpful in understanding vision in unfamiliar environments. Here, we develop a computational approach to investigate visual ecology in the pelagic realm. Using information on eye size, key retinal properties, optical properties of the water and radiance, we develop expressions for calculating the visual range for detection of important types of pelagic targets. We also briefly apply the computations to a number of central questions in pelagic visual ecology, such as the relationship between eye size and visual performance, the maximum depth at which daylight is useful for vision, visual range relations between prey and predators, counter-illumination and the importance of various aspects of retinal physiology. We also argue that our present addition to computational visual ecology can be developed further, and that a computational approach offers plenty of unused potential for investigations of visual ecology in both aquatic and terrestrial habitats.
Keywords: visual ecology, pelagic, visual range, computational
1. Introduction
For most terrestrial habitats, we have an intuitive feeling about the roles that vision potentially may have, and it is often possible to access the habitats for measurement and experiments. In the pelagic world, this is typically not the case. The visual world of pelagic animals is fundamentally different from our own world. In the open water and away from the surface, there are no stationary structures and no landmarks. The background of inanimate structures that so totally dominates most terrestrial visual scenes is absent in the pelagic realm. Instead, the vertical radiance distribution follows the observer, with the highest radiance seen straight upward and the lowest in the opposite direction [1]. Any objects that are visible against this background are possibly potential predators, mates or prey.
All but the upper part of the pelagic habitat is largely inaccessible to us, and pelagic animals are notoriously hard to keep in culture [2]. Behavioural and physiological experiments on these animals are either extremely challenging or impossible, but modelling of vision is comparatively tractable in the pelagic habitat because the visual background is simple and predictable. In this paper, we take the modelling approach to learn about visual ecology in the otherwise largely inaccessible pelagic realm. Our basic question is: what can animals see down there? The answer of course depends on a number of factors involving both the environment and the visual system. To be able to calculate what animals can see, it is first necessary to determine the nature of the visual task. A major choice is between pattern discrimination and object detection.
In the pelagic habitat, maximum detection range is a particularly relevant measure of visual performance [3]. Unlike vision in terrestrial habitats, where the detection range may be enormous or effectively infinite (we can see the stars), vision in aquatic habitats is severely limited by absorption and scattering of the water [3,4]. But water quality alone does not determine the range of underwater vision. It also depends on the target, the amount of daylight and the visual performance of the observer [5,6]. With good estimates of these properties, it is in principle possible to calculate the maximum range at which a target can be detected (the range also defines the equally important volume of visualization space around the animal). Obviously, the type of target must first be determined.
Bioluminescent point-sources are one obviously relevant target category, and extended opaque objects are another. But both these categories can be subdivided depending on the visual background. In the presence of any ambient light, detection of bioluminescent point sources is of different ranges depending on whether light is emitted by a transparent planktonic organism or by photophores on a non-transparent animal. Likewise, an extended black object may be covered by numerous unresolved point sources (animal with photophores) or just be a cloud of unresolved point sources on a transparent background (bioluminescent plankton in the wake behind a moving animal). To simplify calculations and still allow modelling of as many real objects as possible, we have chosen to calculate the detection range of both point sources and extended objects. For point sources, we distinguish between those seen on a black or transparent background. For detection of extended objects, we assume that they are either black or transparent and may contain point sources within their silhouette. To be visible, an extended transparent object must of course contain luminous points, for example a cloud of transparent plankton triggered to emit light in the wake of a swimming animal. Each of these four specified cases requires their own mathematical treatment.
Detection of an object against a background is a matter of comparing the signal from a visual pixel pointing at the object and a neighbouring pixel just viewing the background. In either case, light in the line of sight is constantly being lost owing to absorption and scattering, and new light is also scattered into the line of sight [3]. The original light emitted or reflected by a target is thus gradually lost and replaced. At a sufficiently long distance, the target can no longer be discriminated from the background. At exactly which distance this happens is a matter of how small contrasts the eye is able to discriminate. Ultimately, it is thus a discrimination task, which depends on the number of photons detected in each pixel and also the amount of intrinsic receptor noise (the number of false photons) contributing to the photoreceptor signals.
Even though a calculation of visual range is fundamentally straightforward, there are a significant number of factors involved in computing the photon counts [7–9]. Some of these factors can be accurately estimated but others are difficult to measure with good precision. The sheer number of parameters also implies that errors can accumulate and result in poor accuracy. For general theoretical modelling of pelagic visual ecology, this is however unlikely to be a major problem, but prediction of detection ranges in specific cases may be associated with considerable errors if many of the input parameters are inaccurate. As we show, modelling can also point to parameters that are particularly critical, and thus deserve more careful measurement.
In this paper, we present the detailed derivation of equations for calculating the range of vision for camera-type eyes spotting both point sources and extended objects. Parts of the theory were recently used to assess the significance of very large eyes in giant and colossal squid [10,11]. The treatment in this paper is more general and intended as a reference for future modelling. To demonstrate the power of this theoretical approach to pelagic visual ecology, we apply the equations to a number of different general questions relating to the significance of eye size, depth in the sea and water quality. We pay special attention to ecologically important questions such as the depth limit for the use of daylight, prey/predator relations and counter-illumination. Our aim in this paper is not to exhaust the potential of the theory, but rather to make a broad survey of questions where it may be an important tool.
2. Derivation of theory
(a). Discrimination criteria
Detection of a target against a background requires discrimination of signals from two visual channels (pixels), sampling light from the target and the background (figure 1). We can think of these pixels as being retinal areas at least as large as a single photoreceptor, but potentially much larger. We assume that the channels being compared have identical properties. The channels may represent single photoreceptors or larger circular pools of receptors. A target channel detects a mean of NT photons per integration time and the corresponding mean count for a background channel is NB. The photon counts are sums of real photons and intrinsic noise (false photons), and obey Poisson statistics [12,13], where the standard deviation is the square root of the mean. We follow Land [14] and assume a Gaussian distribution of photon samples (this approximation is good when at least one of the two signals to be compared exceeds 10 photon events). Discrimination between the signals in the two channels is possible when the difference is greater than or equal to a reliability constant R times the standard deviation of the difference (which is the square root of the sum of the two means [14]): . The discrimination threshold is then given by
2.1 |
Variables and constants are defined in table 1. By developing expressions for the photon counts, we can now use the criterion to calculate maximum distance at which a target can be detected. This will have to be done separately for each type of detection task.
Table 1.
variable | definition |
---|---|
NT | mean number of real and false photons detected per integration time in a visual channel aimed at the target (photons) |
NB | mean number of real and false photons detected per integration time in a visual channel viewing the background space-light (photons) |
Nbio | mean photon count originating from bioluminescent sources (photons) |
Nspace | mean photon count from background space-light (photons) |
Nblack | mean photon count originating from light scattered into the line of sight between target and observer (photons) |
Xch | number of false photons (dark noise) per integration time in a visual channel (photons) |
X | dark-noise rate per photoreceptor (photons s–1), TV = 2.8 × 10–5 s−1, see [15] |
A | pupil diameter (m) |
r | range: maximum visibility distance to target (m) |
E | number of photons emitted by bioluminescent point source in all directions per second (photons s–1) |
Ispace | radiance of space-light background in the direction of view at the position (depth) of the eye (photons m–2 s–1 sr–1) |
T | width of extended target (m) |
x | average distance between point sources across an extended object (m) |
α | beam attenuation coefficient of seawater (m–1), see [5] and the electronic supplementary material |
κ | attenuation coefficient of background radiance (m–1), see [5] and the electronic supplementary material |
λ | wavelength of light, taken as 480 nm for bioluminescence and transmitted daylight |
n | refractive index in object and image space, taken as the value of water, 1.33 |
d | photoreceptor diameter (m), TV = 3 µm, see [7] |
Δt | integration time (s), TV = 1.16 s [8,16,17] |
q | detection efficiency: ratio of detected and incident photons, depending on loss in the ocular optics, fraction absorbed in photopigment and transduction efficiency, TV = 0.36 [8] |
f | focal length (m) |
M | the ratio of focal length and pupil radius, 2f/A, here set to Matthiessen's ratio (2.55) |
R | reliability coefficient, here set to 1.96 for 95% confidence [14] |
(b). Detection of a point source on a transparent target
This case is applicable for detection of single transparent plankton organisms against the background space-light. We assume a pair of visual channels in a camera-type eye. A target channel is aimed at the bioluminescent point source, and its signal is compared to that of a channel aimed at the background next to the point source (figure 1). The target channel is assumed to receive all light that enters the eye from the bioluminescent point source. Because the source does not obscure the background, the target channel receives background space-light of the same intensity as that seen by the background channel. The target channel will receive an average of Nbio photons per integration time from the point source and Nspace photons from the background space-light, whereas the background channel only receives Nspace photons from the background. Each channel also generates an average of Xch false photons per integration time. The total average signal in the target channel will thus be and in the background channel, . Inserting this into equation (2.1) gives
2.2 |
which simplifies to
2.3 |
Before we develop Nbio and Nspace, we need to consider the angular size of the two channels. For maximum detection ability, the target channel should collect as much light as possible from the point source, and both channels should collect a minimum of background space-light. This requires that the angular size of the channels be matched to the resolution of the optical image in the eye. Assuming that aberrations are corrected well enough for diffraction-limited optics [18], the optimum visual angle of a channel is limited by diffraction. The main lobe of the diffraction pattern, the Airy disc, contains nearly all the light from a point source, and it spreads from the pupil over an angle of 2.44λ/nA radians [19], where λ is the wavelength of the stimulating light, n is the refractive index of water and A is the diameter of the pupil (table 1). We know that aquatic camera eyes typically have focal lengths of 2.55 lens radii (Matthiessen's ratio, M). Expressed in pupil diameters, the focal length, f, is 0.5 MA, and the angular diameter of the Airy disc becomes 2.44λM/2nf radians. If we multiply this by the focal length, we obtain the actual size of the Airy disc on the retina or 2.44λM/2n. This means that if M is constant, the diffraction blur spot has a constant size on the retina irrespective of eye size. For a wavelength of 480 nm and M = 2.55, the Airy disc is 1.1 µm wide, and for M as high as 3, the Airy disc is still only 1.3 µm wide. But photoreceptor diameters are typically somewhat larger (d = 2–6 µm for most rods, cones and rhabdoms), which implies that realistic angular dimensions of the spatial channels should be given by actual receptor diameters rather than any theoretical optimum. We assume a Gaussian profile of the sensitivity across single receptors, where the angular half-width in visual space is d/f and the solid angle is 1.13(d/f)2, in contrast to the solid angle of π/4(d/f)2 for a square angular profile [19].
Ultimately, we are interested in the relationship between the pupil diameter A and the range r, and search for expressions relating these to Nbio and Nspace. Following Warrant [20], optical geometry gives a photon flux density of E/4π r2 for a point source, attenuation by water is given by and the pupil area accepting the light is πA2/4. The product of these factors multiplied by the efficiency q of the retina and the integration time Δt provides the desired expression of Nbio:
2.4 |
The background space-light is an extended source, and the sensitivity [14] of a retinal channel is simply the product of the pupil area, π (A/2)2, the solid angle in visual space of the channel, 1.13(d/f)2, and the efficiency q by which the eye detects photons. We arrive at Nspace by multiplying the sensitivity by the radiance of the background space-light, Ispace, and the integration time Δt:
2.5 |
We know from above that , and substituting this into equation (2.5) we obtain
2.6 |
The dark noise per integration time is simply
2.7 |
We now insert the developed signal components into equation (2.3), and solve for A (for detailed derivation steps, including equations (2.8)–(2.13), see the electronic supplementary material):
2.14 |
which is the desired relation between A and r for detection of transparent point sources.
(c). Detection of a point source on a black target
This case is applicable for detection of single small photophores on the body of a black opaque animal or transparent bioluminescent plankton seen against the black body of an animal (for example with stimulated plankton bioluminescence). Here, we thus assume that the target holding the bioluminescent point source is a black rather than a transparent object (figure 1). For both channels, the target, in this case, interrupts the background space-light, and new light is scattered into the line of sight between the target and the observer. Even a black target will thus contribute light to a visual channel viewing it. The photon count contributed by light scattered into the line of sight, Nblack, then replaces Nspace in equation (2.3):
2.15 |
For an observer at constant depth in the sea, space-light enters the line of sight at the rate , where the attenuation coefficient κ depends on the viewing angle [3–5]. For a target at constant depth, the corresponding expression is , but here we want to determine the space-light at the depth of the observer (from where the eye is performing the discrimination), and thus use the form . The radiance seen in the direction of a black target then becomes . To get an expression for Nblack, we substitute for Ispace in equation (2.6):
2.16 |
We then repeat the derivation from the previous case and arrive at
2.17 |
(d). Detection of an extended black target triggering bioluminescence
This case models the visibility of an animal silhouette. The animal body is assumed to be black, but may contain any number of bioluminescent point sources, either being the animal's own photophores or transparent bioluminescent plankton triggered to emit light by the moving animal. We again assume an equal pair of visual channels, but now optimally sized to detect an extended object against the background space-light. To maximize the signal, the target channel fills the width of the object (figure 1), and both channels have square rather than Gaussian sensitivity profiles (all receptors in the circular pool have equal weight). We thus assume that the angular size of the visual channels (pixel) is dynamic and suited to the object at all times. This detection strategy is chosen because it offers the best detectability with circular pixels (the actual properties of visual channels in pelagic animals is yet unknown). The angle in visual space of such a channel is the target width T divided by its distance, T/r (radians), and with a square profile its solid angle is (π/4)(T/r)2 (steradians). Each individual photoreceptor within the circular pool occupies a solid angle of (π/4)(d/f)2 in visual space (see table 1 for definition of variables). The number of photoreceptors forming a channel is then (Tf/rd)2 and its diameter on the retina is Tf/r.
Even though the target itself is assumed to be black, it may contain bioluminescent point sources, but the background is just space-light and no bioluminescence (figure 1). Modelling this way, we are free to investigate both dark (E = 0) and luminous extended objects (E > 0). The signal of the target channel comes partly from target bioluminescence attenuated on its way to the eye, space-light having entered the line of sight between the target and the eye and dark noise from the contributing photoreceptors: , and the background channel sums background space-light and channel noise: .
The discrimination threshold (equation (2.1)) now becomes
2.18 |
which reduces to
2.19 |
(we here keep the absolute value of the difference because either NT or NB can have the largest value, depending on the amount of bioluminescence). Note that Nbio, Nblack, Nspace and Xch are in this case parameters for dynamic receptor pools, and are thus not identical to the same parameters in the point source cases.
We are now ready to work out expressions for Nbio, Nblack, Nspace and Xch, which happens to be easier in the reverse order. The channel noise Xch is derived as for the point source case, but here multiplied by the number of photoreceptors in the pool (see above):
2.20 |
With f = MA/2, this becomes
2.21 |
The photon count from background space-light is similar to the point-source case (equation (2.5)) but with T/r replacing d/f, and assuming a square rather than a Gaussian profile for the angular sensitivity, we thus replace 1.13 with π/4 as follows:
2.22 |
The contribution of light entering the line of sight between the target and the eye, Nblack, can be determined by replacing Ispace of equation (2.22) with as in equation (2.16):
2.23 |
The target may include bioluminescent point sources in the form of photophores on the target animal or planktonic organisms stimulated to emit light by the moving target. The total bioluminescent emission seen by the target pixel, Nbio, can be obtained by multiplying the expression for a single point source (equation (2.4)) with the total number, P, of point sources within the field of the target pixel:
2.24 |
The number of point sources, P, seen by the target pixel requires specific expressions depending on the geometric distribution of photophores, or in the case of stimulated planktonic bioluminescence, the expression depends on target motion direction. Different expressions for P are given in a separate section after the main derivations and in table 2.
Table 2.
type of point sources | point source density (per unit area or unit volume) | target area or volume | total number of point sources viewed by the target pixel |
---|---|---|---|
photophore array, square packing | |||
photophore array, hexagonal packing | |||
photophore array, random distribution | |||
SB, side view of moving target | |||
SB, side view of luminous wake | |||
SB, approaching target |
We now substitute equations (2.21)–(2.24) for Xch, Nspace, Nblack and Nbio in equation (2.19) and solve for A (for detailed derivation steps, including equations (2.25)–(2.30), see the electronic supplementary material):
2.31 |
which is the desired relation between A and r for detection of extended sources. For black objects without bioluminescence, the term disappears in both the numerator and denominator.
(e). Detection of an extended transparent target triggering bioluminescence
Finally, we consider the case where the extended target consists of triggered bioluminescence in the wake behind a moving object (figure 1d). This is similar to the previous case, except that the target does not block the background space-light. The signal of the target receptor then changes to , whereas that of the background receptor remains . We insert the developed terms as in the previous case and solve for A (for detailed derivation steps, including equation (2.32), see the electronic supplementary material):
2.33 |
This expression provides the desired relation between A and r.
(f). Expressions for the number of bioluminescent point sources in extended targets
A straightforward method to determine the number of photophores, P, on a target animal that is seen by the target pixel is to multiply the density of point sources (per unit area) with the area seen by the target pixel, (π/4)T2. For regular square arrays, the density is simply 1/x2, where x is the distance between neighbouring photophores. Hexagonal arrays have a slightly higher density of 1.16/x2 [21]. For random distributions, the density is 1/4x2 [21], and here x is the average distance to the nearest neighbour. The best expression for P thus depends on how the photophores are arranged on the side of the target facing the observer.
The situation is rather different if the bioluminescence is caused by plankton disturbed by the moving target animal. We here assume that the disturbed water volume can be estimated from that displaced by the moving target. The number of point sources, P, in this case is the part of the disturbed water volume seen by the target pixel multiplied by the density of bioluminescent plankton in the water (here per unit volume). With an average nearest-neighbour distance, x, in three dimensions, the density is 0.7/x3 [22]. For a target animal that moves perpendicular to the line of sight of the observer, we assume that the disturbed water volume is a cylinder with the same diameter as the moving animal. The target pixel, which also has the diameter of the target, thus defines an observed volume, which can be approximated by the intersection of two perpendicular cylinders, known as a Steinmetz solid (the visual field of the pixel is in fact a cone, but at sufficient target distances a cylinder is a good approximation). The target body obscures half this volume. With the target diameter, T, this volume is (1/3)T3 [23]. If the plankton continues to glow long enough, there will be a luminous wake behind the animal, and with the body out of the way, the full volume, (2/3)T3, will be visible to the target pixel. The disturbed water volume created by a target moving straight towards or away from the observer can be estimated as a cylinder of diameter T along the line of sight. Here, we assume that the unobscured length of this cylinder is 2T. The observed volume is then (π/2)T3. The different cases and expressions of P are summarized in table 2.
3. Results and discussion
The maximum distance at which objects can be visually detected is an important ecological factor because it shapes foraging, and prey/predator relations as well as interactions with conspecifics [24]. The theoretical framework developed here to investigate vision in pelagic habitats allows us to investigate general principles of visual ecology in this largely inaccessible habitat. In analogy with cameras and telescopes, visual performance is limited primarily by the physics of light. From this, it follows that eye size, or more specifically the pupil diameter, sets very sharp performance limits on the vision of all animals. Knowing these performance limits is a prerequisite for understanding both the roles of vision and the degrees of investment in vision made by different animal groups and species. To exploit all the possibilities opened up by the theoretical tools developed here, it would require comparisons with much of the existing knowledge on pelagic ecology in a wide range of animals. This would be a massive undertaking, far beyond the scope of this paper. Here, we restrict the discussion to short descriptions of some questions where we believe that the theory may provide valuable insight into pelagic visual ecology.
(a). Types of targets
Because downwelling daylight gradually becomes dimmer with depth, non-luminous objects are obviously best seen close to the surface, whereas bioluminescent objects are more visible in deep waters. A comparison of the visual range for detecting point-light sources, black silhouettes and extended luminous sources is illustrated in figure 2. Even though the functions depend on water quality, viewing angle, pupil diameter and the width of extended objects, the general features are similar under all conditions. In shallow water, large dark silhouettes offer by far the longest visual range, but below a certain depth they cannot be detected at all. Bioluminescent point sources can be seen surprisingly well even at shallow depths, and it does not matter much if they are seated on black or transparent objects. Below a rather well-defined depth, the competition with daylight ceases such that the visual range of point sources remains constant at increasing depth. Extended luminous objects, both black and transparent (luminous wake), compete with daylight much deeper in the ocean than point sources do. They are also much less visible in shallow water. A black silhouette covered with bioluminescent point sources behaves as a non-luminous object in shallow water, and as a luminous object in the deep. At an intermediate depth, the bioluminescence exactly corresponds to the space-light radiance, making the object perfectly camouflaged (here occurring at a depth of about 460 m). This type of diagram can provide valuable information on the types of objects that are most or least visible at different depths, and thus suggests the visual strategy of choice for particular species.
(b). Eye size and visual performance
An interesting outcome of modelling the relationship between pupil diameter (eye size) and visual range is that the performance returns of having a larger eye are lower the larger the eye is to start with (figure 3). This law of diminishing returns is inescapable and it results from absorption and scattering of light in the water [26]. Consequently, growing eyes large is much more rewarding in clear ocean water than in murky coastal water. Animals living in turbid water would thus be expected to have comparatively small eyes.
The law of diminishing returns depends quite significantly on the type of target, and also on depth and viewing angle (figure 3a). This can be seen as different declines of the slope of the curves, and more directly by plotting the performance return (figure 3b), which is the relative increase in visual range for a small increase in eye size. If the visual range increases by 0.5% as a result of a 1% increase in eye diameter, the performance return is 0.5. For very small eyes, the performance return is 1, but it drops significantly already at a pupil diameter of just a few mm.
For detection of objects that can appear in any direction, the total monitored water volume is probably more directly related to fitness than visual range is. Because the monitored water volume has a cubic relation to visual range, it pays much more to increase the eye size (figure 3c,d) with this measure of performance. The lesson is that species using vision primarily to scan their environment for detecting objects occurring in random directions should have relatively larger eyes compared with those that use vision mainly for pursuit or recognition tasks. This may explain some of the large variations in relative eye size reported in myctophid fish [27].
An interesting question is why giant and colossal squid have eyes that grow to about three times the diameter of the eyes of any other species, including large fish and whales (figure 4a). For most visual tasks under water, the law of diminishing returns would act against investment in such huge eyes. In two separate studies [10,11], we analysed this question and found that detection of stimulated bioluminescence caused by very large objects (about 2 m wide) provides a unique motivation for very large eyes (figure 4b), and it was suggested that early detection of foraging sperm whales at depths below 800 m might have generated particularly strong selection for huge eyes in these two species of squid.
(c). Sighting range of black targets
Especially in fresh-water bodies, visibility is regularly measured as the depth where a standardized Secchi disc is just visible [29,30], and divers often refer to a specific visibility distance associated with the water quality. In reality, the water quality does not determine a fixed distance beyond which vision does not reach. The amount of daylight, the size and contrast of the target, the pupil diameter and the viewing angle are all factors determining the range of vision (figure 5a,b). Very large ships seen from below in clear oceanic water would offer the longest visibility distance of non-luminous objects in the pelagic. But the optical water quality is an important factor that makes the conditions for vision in oceanic, coastal or estuarine habitats very different. In clear oceanic water, even small eyes can see black objects at considerable distance, but in less clear coastal water the visual ranges are much shorter even for very large eyes (figure 5a). The viewing angle also has a dramatic effect on visual range (figure 5b). Dark silhouettes are seen at very much longer distances looking upwards compared with looking horizontally or downwards [5,31]. This is the obvious reason why so many mesopelagic animals have tubular eyes for spotting prey above [15]. It is also the reason for counter-illumination, which offers the only way to conceal the silhouette [31]. In habitats with clear water and dense populations of animals, for example the epipelagic zone of tropical seas close to coral reefs, even a relatively small eye provides a visual range that may substantially exceed the animal's range of action or range of interest. In these habitats, investment in relatively large eyes should be rare.
(d). Depth limits for vision using the sun, the moon or starlight
The maximum depth at which daylight can be used for vision is typically suggested to be between 700 and 1000 m for clear oceanic water [20,31–33]. Our computational approach offers a way to find the theoretical depth limit for vision in downwelling daylight. A large black target, seen from below, allows the longest sighting range possible for non-luminous targets. Using a biologically realistic maximum target-width of 0.5 m, the visual range for an eye with 30 mm pupil diameter approaches zero at about 850 m in clear oceanic water (figure 6a, curve 3). But this is a significant overestimate of actual limits, because at very close range, the target occupies a very large visual angle (close to 180°), and thus totally occludes the background it is supposed to stand out against. This is assuming a purely spatial signal. If a large object suddenly gets very close, there will of course be a temporal signal (i.e. a darkening) that the animal can detect without any spatial comparison.
To give room for two adjacent visual channels (pixels) to compare a target with a background, we assume that the target must not come closer than, say, twice its own width (covering 28°). With this assumption, the maximum depth shrinks from 850 to 760 m (figure 6a). In coastal water, where the downwelling radiance drops more rapidly with depth, the daylight vision zone ends considerably further up in the water column. At sunset, dusk and night, the depths at which downwelling light can be used for vision is of course greatly reduced (figure 6b). Surprisingly, even starlight may be sufficient for some visual tasks at depths exceeding 200 m.
Because a large pupil diameter allows vision at lower intensities, it is of interest to calculate the depth at which giant and colossal squid can use their enormous eyes with 90 mm pupil diameter. It turns out that the limit is between 790 and 860 m depending on the integration time (160 ms and 1 s: figure 6a). It is important to note that these depths are calculated for the clearest ocean water at midday in the tropics. Everywhere else, the maximum depth for vision in downwelling daylight will be much less [34].
(e). Prey/predator relations
In terms of vision, a pelagic animal is surrounded by a volume of water beyond which vision is not possible. This visibility ‘bubble’ follows the animal as it moves, and visual interactions with other animals (prey, predators or conspecifics) can take place only within this bubble. Obviously, the bubble depends on pupil diameter and varies with the type of and size of target (figure 7). A large predator may have a large pupil area, but it will typically be searching for smaller prey. The prey, in contrast, has smaller pupils but will be looking out for a larger predator. Which of them has the largest visibility bubble will depend on eye sizes and body sizes, but an interesting observation is that the relative width and heights of predator/prey visibility bubbles do not change dramatically with depth (figure 7). That is because the radiance field assumes a relatively constant form for depths below about 200 m [1]. Another observation is that the diameters of the visibility bubbles in clear oceanic water are often one to two orders of magnitude larger than the body lengths of animals. In murky coastal or estuarine water, exhibiting much more absorption and scattering [25], the diameters of the visibility bubbles are instead in the same order of magnitude as body length. This, of course, implies that visual ecology is fundamentally different in different water types, and our theoretical framework provides a way to reveal some of these differences.
(f). Counter-illumination
Many pelagic animals (fish, squid and crustaceans) use ventral photophores to camouflage their otherwise prominent dark silhouette [15,35–37]. The principle is that bioluminescence replaces the light blocked by the body. This is an important strategy because, as we have seen earlier, a predator looking straight up can see a dark prey silhouette from a very long distance, whereas the prey has great difficulty in seeing what is approaching from below. But intensity matching is important if the camouflage is to be effective. It is well known that any angular or spectral mismatch in the emitted light will hamper the camouflage [35,36]. Our modelling (figure 8) reveals how extreme the precision must be, and how severe the consequences are of even a slight vertical displacement. Especially in clear ocean water, an animal becomes visible at very long distances if it is only a few metres shallower or deeper than the isoluminance depth (figure 8). In effect, this means that counter-illumination is not a simple and foolproof way to become invisible in the pelagic. It also suggests that mechanisms for precise intensity matching are essential, such as the bioluminescence control in hatchetfish [39] and the opsin expression in squid photophores [40]. The shorter detection ranges of less clear water makes the situation a little more forgiving.
(g). Resolving individual point sources or pooling many
For maximal visibility of point-light sources, the visual channels should cover as narrow an angle as possible. This way, the signal will be minimally contaminated with background space-light. But resolution of single point sources is not necessarily the best strategy if the target consists of multiple photophores or is a moving object stimulating bioluminescence from many planktonic organisms. With low resolution (extended-source viewing with large pixels), it is possible to sum up many point sources into the same visual channel, and thus obtain discrimination at longer distances than a single point source would allow (analogous to spatial pooling for improving vision in conventional extended scenes in dim light [8]). The large pixels will of course pick up more space-light, although this is a problem only when the background is bright. The question whether extended-source viewing or point source detection is the best strategy will depend how many point sources the larger pixels will see. In dark environments, extended source viewing will generally be superior if the pixels detect an average of more than one point source. Detection of many point sources in each target pixel can lead to much longer sighting distances compared with detection of individual point sources.
During dinoflagellate blooms in shallow water, there may be in excess of 100 cells per litre [41]. Under these conditions, a moving fish will trigger so many flashes that extended-source viewing makes for a longer visual range even for target widths below 10 cm (figure 9). But with common densities of gelatinous zooplankton in deep water (0.5 m nearest-neighbour distance [42]), detection of individual point sources is superior unless the moving target is very large. Similar considerations apply to ventral photophores, where the pattern of photophores may be resolved at close range by conspecifics, but matched to the background and thus invisible to predators at longer range [43].
(h). Sensitivity to physiological parameters
Our calculations are based on a large number of assumed values. In reality, these values may vary between species and assumed typical values may not be representative in all cases. By testing the sensitivity to variations in parameters, such as receptor dark-noise, quantum efficiency, integration time and receptor diameter, it is possible to assess the reliability of our modelling. But physiological parameters that are critical to our modelling are also critical to the animals. We can thus use the computational approach to learn how selection is likely to act on some key physiological parameters (figure 10).
Varying receptor noise over three orders of magnitude has surprisingly little effect on visual performance (figure 10a,b). It is only in the dim light below 400 m that receptor noise has any notable effect. The reason for this is that receptor noise in vertebrate rods and invertebrate rhabdoms is extremely low [44]. For extended sources, the impact of receptor noise grows with eye size, simply because there are more photoreceptors per unit angle in visual space. Retinal efficiency (the fraction of incident photons that are detected) and integration time have similar and very large effects on visual performance (figure 10c–f), especially under dim conditions. This means that calculations of visual performance in specific animal species should be interpreted with caution unless these parameters can be accurately estimated. It also points to a need for measurements of quantum efficiency and integration time in pelagic animals. The strong effect on performance suggests that quantum efficiency and integration time could be under strong selection, and this is probably true for efficiency but with integration time there is of course the standard trade-off with temporal resolution [8], which is not considered in our theory. The receptor diameter (figure 10g,h) has an impact mainly on the detection of point sources against a background of space-light. Although not the subject of this study, the receptor diameter may also be important for resolution of fine patterns. We also investigated the consequences of varying the F-number between 2.5 and 3.5, but found it to have only negligible effects (not illustrated).
(i). Spatial sampling
For point source detection, we have assumed that the visual system can use the full resolution of the retina and for extended sources we have assumed that receptor pooling can adjust dynamically to the target. Neither of these assumptions can be expected to hold entirely for all species, or for all parts of their retinas. To feed the brain with one axon for each photoreceptor is not possible unless the eye is tiny. But it is possible that single-channel detections can be summed onto a smaller number of neurons in early processing. This need not affect the visual range but it will reduce the number of pixels, and the possibility to resolve dense patterns of point sources. The dynamic spatial summation assumed for extended-source vision is a well-known phenomenon in visual systems [8]. But this does not mean we would expect summation dynamics to operate without limits. It is more likely that selection has shaped the visual system of each species such that spatial summation can be adjusted to cope with naturally occurring and biologically meaningful stimuli. For these reasons, individual species may come close to some of the theoretical performance limits, but not to others. But it would be surprising if evolution has failed to exploit the potential relevant to the life style of each species.
(j). Outlook
A framework for assessing visual performance is a versatile tool for investigating general principles of visual ecology. The examples given in this discussion only superficially touch on a few of the questions that can be analysed using the equations derived here. The theory has the potential to explain allometric relationships between eye size and body size, and to investigate relationships within and between species. An interesting and little understood area is the visual ecology of turbid river habitats (or algal blooms) where vision acts on completely different scales compared with vision in clear water. Vertical migrations, other than those that just follow isolumes, offer another area where the computational approach may help to reveal general principles. However, for accurate modelling of vision in the upper 100–200 m of oceanic water, where light is not effectively monochromatic [25,31], it is necessary to use more elaborate calculations of the daylight intensity available for absorption in a photoreceptor, Ispace (see the electronic supplementary material, S2: Modelling of radiance and absorption coefficients in the sea).
The theory developed here can also be expanded and elaborated. An obvious extension is to cover white or grey targets. This would allow analyses of the consequences of body coloration. It is also possible to develop expressions for pelagic vision at all angles (not just vertical and horizontal as in this paper), and to take the variation in sensitivity over the visual field into account. With such additions, it would be possible to assess in more detail the visual information available to different species. An urgent extension of the theory is of course to modify the expressions for other types of eyes, for example compound eyes. In fact, the theory is already valid for compound eyes of the superposition type, but not for apposition eyes.
An alternative to modelling visual range is to calculate spatial resolution, which would open the possibility of analysing most visual tasks that are not based on simple object discrimination against a homogeneous background. Such a development is particularly relevant for analysis of benthic and terrestrial habitats. Extensions to allow analysis of colour and polarization contrasts are also desirable. Taken together, computational visual ecology is a powerful tool that can inform us about how well different visual tasks can be performed under given circumstances, and thus how the adaptive landscape of vision actually appears.
References
- 1.Mobley CD. 1994. Light and water: radiative transfer in natural waters. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hamner WM, Madin LP, Alldredge AL, Gilmer RW, Hamner PP. 1975. Underwater observations of gelatinous zooplankton: sampling problems, feeding biology, and behavior. Limnol. Oceanogr. 20, 907–917. ( 10.4319/lo.1975.20.6.0907) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Duntley SQ. 1952. The visibility of submerged objects. Final Report to Office of Naval Research, Washington, DC, USA.
- 4.Mertens LE. 1970. In-water photography: theory and practice. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Johnsen S. 2002. Cryptic and conspicuous coloration in the pelagic environment. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269, 243–256. ( 10.1098/rspb.2001.1855) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Johnsen S, Sosik HM. 2003. Cryptic coloration and mirrored sides as camouflage strategies in near-surface pelagic habitats: implications for foraging and predator avoidance. Limnol. Oceanogr. 48, 1277–1288. ( 10.4319/lo.2003.48.3.1277) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Land MF, Nilsson D-E. 2012. Animal eyes, 2nd edn Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Warrant EJ. 1999. Seeing better at night: life style, eye design and the optimum strategy of spatial and temporal summation. Vision Res. 39, 1611–1630. ( 10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00262-4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Johnsen S. 2012. The optics of life: a biologist's guide to light in nature. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Nilsson D-E, Warrant EJ, Johnsen S, Hanlon R, Shashar N. 2012. A unique advantage for giant eyes in giant squid. Curr. Biol. 22, 683–688. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.031) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Nilsson D-E, Warrant EJ, Johnsen S, Hanlon R, Shashar N. 2013. The giant eyes of giant squid are indeed unexpectedly large, but not if used for spotting sperm whales. BMC Evol. Biol. 13, 187 ( 10.1186/1471-2148-13-187) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Barlow HB. 1956. Retinal noise and absolute threshold. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 46, 634–639. ( 10.1364/JOSA.46.000634) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ala-Laurila P, Pahlberg J, Koskelainen A, Donner K. 2004. On the relation between the photoactivation energy and the absorbance spectrum of visual pigments. Vis. Res. 44, 2153–2158. ( 10.1016/j.visres.2004.03.031) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Land MF. 1981. Optics and vision in invertebrates. In Handbook of sensory physiology, vol. VII/6B (ed. Autrum H.), pp. 471–592. New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Warrant EJ, Locket NA. 2004. Vision in the deep sea. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 79, 671–712. ( 10.1017/S1464793103006420) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Moeller JF, Case JF. 1994. Properties of visual interneurons in a deep-sea mysid, Gnathophausia ingens. Mar. Biol. 119, 211–219. ( 10.1007/BF00349559) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Moeller JF, Case JF. 1995. Temporal adaptations in visual systems of deep-sea crustaceans. Mar. Biol. 123, 47–54. ( 10.1007/BF00350322) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kröger RHH, Fritsches KA, Warrant EJ. 2009. Lens optical properties in the eyes of large marine predatory teleosts. J. Comp. Physiol. A 195, 175–182. ( 10.1007/s00359-008-0396-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Snyder AW. 1975. Photoreceptor optics: theoretical principles. In Photoreceptor optics (eds Snyder AW, Menzel R.), pp. 38–55. New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Warrant E. 2000. The eyes of deep-sea fishes and the changing nature of visual scenes with depth. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 355, 1155–1159. ( 10.1098/rstb.2000.0658) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Clark PJ, Evans FC. 1954. Distance to nearest neighbor as a measure of spatial relationships in populations. Ecology 35, 445–453. ( 10.2307/1931034) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hertz P. 1909. Über den gegenseitigen durchschnittlichen Abstand von Punkten, die mit bekannter mittlerer Dichte im Raume angeordnet sind. Mathematische Annalen 67, 387–398. ( 10.1007/BF01450410) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Angell IO, Moore M. 1987. Symmetrical intersections of cylinders. Acta Crystallogr. A 43, 244–250. ( 10.1107/S0108767387099501) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Aksnes DL, Utne ACW. 1997. A revised model of visual range in fish. Sarsia 82, 137–147. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Jerlov NG. 1968. Optical oceanography, Oceanography series 5 New York, NY: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Johnsen S. In press. Hide and seek in the open sea: pelagic camouflage and visual countermeasures. Annu Rev. Mar. Sci. ( 10.1146/annurev-marine-010213-135018) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.de Busserolles F, Fitzpatrick JL, Paxton JR, Marshall NJ, Collin SP. 2013. Eye-size variability in deep-sea lanternfishes (Myctophidae): an ecological and phylogenetic study. PLoS ONE 8, e58519 ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0058519) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Fritsches KA, Brill RW, Warrant EJ. 2005. Warm eyes provide superior vision in swordfishes. Curr. Biol. 15, 55–58. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2004.12.064) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lind OT. 1979. Handbook of common methods in limnology. St Louis, MO: C.V. Mosby Co. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Davies-Colley R. 1988. Measuring water clarity with a black disk. Limnol. Oceanogr. 33, 616–623. ( 10.4319/lo.1988.33.4.0616) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Lythgoe JN. 1979. The ecology of vision. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Nicol JAC. 1971. Physiological investigations of oceanic animals. In Deep oceans (eds Herring PJ, Clarke MR.), pp. 225–246. London, UK: Arthur Barker. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Denton EJ. 1990. Light and vision at depths greater than 200 metres. In Light and life in the sea (eds Herring PJ, Campbell AK, Whitceld M, Maddock L.), pp. 127–148. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Douglas RH, Partridge JC, Marshall NJ. 1998. The eyes of deep-sea fish I: lens pigmentation, tapeta and visual pigments. Prog. Retin. Eye Res. 17, 597–636. ( 10.1016/S1350-9462(98)00002-0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Denton EJ, Gilpin-Brown JB, Wright PG. 1972. The angular distribution of the light produced by some mesopelagic fish in relation to their camouflage. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 182, 145–158. ( 10.1098/rspb.1972.0071) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Denton EJ, Herring PJ, Widder EA, Latz MF, Case JF. 1985. The roles of filters in the photophores of oceanic animals and their relation to vision in the oceanic environment. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 225, 63–97. ( 10.1098/rspb.1985.0051) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Herring PJ. 2002. The biology of the deep ocean. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Claes JM, Dean MN, Nilsson D-E, Hart NS, Mallefet J. 2013. A deepwater fish with ‘lightsabers’: dorsal spine-associated luminescence in a counterilluminating lanternshark. Sci. Rep. 3, 1308 ( 10.1038/srep01308) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Krönström J, Holmgren S, Baguet F, Salpietro L, Mallefet J. 2005. Nitric oxide in control of luminescence from hatchetfish (Argyropelecus hemigymnus) photophores. J. Exp. Biol. 208, 2951–2961. ( 10.1242/jeb.01712) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Tong D, Rozas NS, Oakley TH, Mitchell J, Colley NJ, McFall-Ngai MJ. 2009. Evidence for light perception in a bioluminescent organ. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 9836–9841. ( 10.1073/pnas.0904571106) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Widder EA. 2001. Bioluminescence and the pelagic visual environment. Mar. Freshwat. Behav. Physiol. 35, 1–26. ( 10.1080/10236240290025581) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Widder EA, Johnsen S. 2000. 3D spatial point patterns of bioluminescent plankton: a map of the minefield. J. Plankton Res. 22, 409–420. ( 10.1093/plankt/22.3.409) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Johnsen S, Widder EA, Mobley CD. 2004. Propagation and perception of bioluminescence: factors affecting counterillumination as a cryptic strategy. Biol. Bull. 207, 1–16. ( 10.2307/1543624) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Fain GL, Hardie R, Laughlin SB. 2010. Phototransduction and the evolution of photoreceptors. Curr. Biol. 20, R114–R124. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.006) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]