Skip to main content
Age logoLink to Age
. 2013 Jun 5;36(1):435–444. doi: 10.1007/s11357-013-9550-y

The use of Ginkgo biloba in healthy elderly

Andreas G Franke 1,, Isabel Heinrich 1, Klaus Lieb 1, Andreas Fellgiebel 1
PMCID: PMC3889903  PMID: 23736956

Abstract

To promote health-conscious behavior in the aging society and gain insight into the sources of knowledge on which preventive strategies are based, analyzing the behavior of elderly people who are recognized as highly health conscious may be useful. We focused on the use of Ginkgo biloba, which is commonly considered to be effective in preventing cognitive decline and dementia, among elderly adults. A total of 1,672 questionnaires were distributed among geriatric participants (60–94 years) who attended university lectures at 22 universities throughout Germany. Response rate was 36.1 %. We collected data on demographic characteristics, preventive strategies (use of Ginkgo and other supplements), health-conscious behavior, sources of knowledge concerning health behavior, and factors associated with the participants’ concept of aging. The prevalence of Ginkgo use was 15.3 %. Ginkgo was assumed to be effective for cognitive enhancement and the treatment of cognitive decline by two thirds of the surveyed participants and one third believed Ginkgo to be effective for preventing dementia. Ginkgo use was significantly higher among participants using natural remedies and herbal and food supplements. The use of Ginkgo was recommended by physicians (57.3 %), chemists (16 %), and healthcare magazines (10.7 %). Food supplements were taken by 65.8 % of the sample: this percentage was significantly higher among subjects who exhibited health-conscious behavior. “Knowledge” about strategies to enhance cognition or prevent cognitive decline among the elderly do not appear to be evidence based. Thus, there is a need to establish reliable and independent sources of scientific information for healthcare professionals and the general public.

Keywords: Ginkgo biloba, Cognitive enhancement, Elderly, Cognitive decline, Dementia

Introduction

The advancing age of the population worldwide has necessitated the increase in research on disorders primarily affecting older people, such as dementia and cognitive decline. Using observational data, some authors have suggested that up to 50 % of dementia might be preventable (Brayne 2007), and it has been shown that people with higher levels of education are at lower risk of dementia (Stern et al. 1994). The reasons for this difference remain unclear. Additionally, the concept of “successful aging” has become increasingly important in an aging society (Baltes 1997; Jeste et al. 2010; Rowe and Kahn 1987). Cognition plays a crucial role in the definition of this concept, as the maintenance of cognition allows elderly people to remain actively involved in society.

Understanding self-contained strategies to prevent cognitive impairment and dementia and to engage in successful aging has become increasingly important in societies of advancing age of the population. A high level of education is not only associated with a lower risk of dementia but also with higher rates of health conscious behavior (Stern et al. 1994; Pearson et al. 2009; Hui and Rubenstein 2006; van der Bij et al. 2002; Rhodes et al. 1999). Health-conscious behavior is determined by behavioral factors and factors related to knowledge and awareness. Indeed, to engage in high levels of health-conscious behavior, people must know and be well aware of factors that could either promote or impair health. Furthermore, they must be convinced that they are responsible for their own health and can influence their health using certain strategies. Beyond that, they must be willing to influence their own health positively in the areas of nutrition, lifestyle, physical exercise (sports), etc. (Dlugosch and Krieger 1995).

However, the basis from which such knowledge is obtained by elderly people and how they choose which behaviors will, in their opinion, actually lead to successful aging remains unclear. Integral parts of the concept of successful aging are the use of food supplements, natural remedies, and other over the counter (OTC) drugs. Ginkgo biloba supplements have a leading position in this market and are commonly considered to be effective in preventing cognitive decline; thus, they represent a strategy that is not evidence-based to prevent cognitive impairment and dementia (Blumenthal 1997). G. biloba is one of the oldest living tree species in the world and is an integral part of the Asian culture and traditional medicine. G. biloba extracts contain high concentrations of flavonoids and terpenoids which have, among other (possible) advantages, antioxidative effects (Oyama et al. 1996). Further assumed mechanisms of action are only partially evidence-based thus far and include the following: neuroprotection via anti-apoptotic effects, inhibition of β-amyloid-aggregation, and changes in gene expression (Ahlemeyer and Krieglstein 2003; Bastianetto et al. 2000; DeFeudis and Drieu 2000; Fowler et al. 2000; Maclennan et al. 2002; Rimbach et al. 2003).

G. biloba supplements are currently one of the most popular herbal drugs in the world (Blumenthal 1997). In Germany, pharmaceuticals containing G. biloba essence are approved for the therapy of peripheral arterial disease, tinnitus, vertigo, and “dementia related syndrome”. More than 20 different pharmaceutical companies provide G. biloba, the most common OTC drug, in Germany. The effectiveness of these supplements, however, remains unclear.

Numerous randomized placebo-controlled trials and meta-analyses have investigated the (pro-) cognitive effects of G. biloba. The large majority of studies suggest that G. biloba does not have procognitive effects in younger or older subjects or in those with or without cognitive impairment or dementia. While there is no evidence that reliably shows that G. biloba has a positive impact on cognition, this supplement also has minimal relevant side effects and can be used with little concern. Several studies have shown the lack of positive effects on particular cognitive domains, such as vigilance, attentiveness, reaction time, memory or mood, regardless of whether G. biloba extracts are used once or over a period of several months (Birks et al. 2002; Birks and Grimley Evans 2007, 2009; Franke and Lieb 2010; Franke et al. 2009; Solomon et al. 2002). The recent GuidAge trial highlighted that even the long-term use of G. biloba is not efficacious (Schneider 2012; Vellas et al. 2012).

Despite the missing clinical evidence, G. biloba supplements are one of the best-selling herbal products on the market (Morris and Avorn 2003). Epidemiological studies show prevalence rates for G. biloba usage ranging from 2 to 13 % in the USA, and no epidemiological studies exist outside the USA (Abebe et al. 2011; Barnes et al. 2008; Kelly et al. 2005; Adusumilli et al. 2004; Watts et al. 2001; Kaufman et al. 2002).

The study presented here examines the prevalence of G. biloba use and sources of information about further potentially effective preventative strategies in highly educated elderly people. Therefore, we focused on strategies that are commonly considered to be effective in preventing cognitive decline and dementia, thus enabling successful aging and factors associated with these strategies.

Methods

Participants

This study presents data from a survey performed between July and December 2011 of 1,672 university students over the age of 60 in Germany at 22 public universities offering specialized university lectures for elderly people.

Procedure

Staff from all German universities offering specialized studies/lectures for elderly people were asked if they were interested in participating; subsequently, they were sent questionnaires. The students were informed of the study by the heads of the department and were asked to participate. The students gave written informed consent prior to participation. Questionnaires were distributed by staff and participants were asked to return them by post in prepaid envelopes. The study was approved by the local Ethics Committee.

Assessments

An extensive self-report paper-and-pencil questionnaire was developed to capture data about health behavior and the use of G. biloba (frequency, period, dose, etc.). Demographic data, health insurance status, educational background, etc. were assessed to characterize G. biloba users. Answers were given by free recall (free space for writing in the questionnaire), dichotomous (yes/no) questions, or via Likert scales. Standardized questionnaires were used for further characterization: Anxiety about Aging Scale (AAS, 20 questions about anxiety and attitudes about aging in general using six-step Likert scale, 6 = very correct, 1 = very false; high values = high level of anxiety about aging) for evaluation of anxiety of aging processes (Lasher and Faulkender 1993; Watkins et al. 1998); the 12-item version of the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12, 12 questions about various psychiatric symptoms using four-step Likert scale, 1 = not at all, 4 = considerably worse than normal; high values = subjectively felt high level of impairment caused by psychiatric symptoms) for screening for acute psychiatric disorders (Goldberg and Blackwell 1970); the Symptom Check List (SCL-90-R, 90 items about physical and mental symptoms using five-step Likert scale, 1 = not at all, 5 = very strong/pronounced; sum score = GSI; high values = numerous physical and mental symptoms) as a self-assessment instrument for physical and mental symptoms (Franke 2002); the Subjective Memory Complaints Questionnaire (SMCQ, 14 dichotomous (yes/no) questions about subjective memory complaints; high values = subjectively pronounced memory complaints) for assessment of subjective memory deficits (Youn et al. 2009); the Health locus of control scale (Fragebogen zur Erhebung von Kontrollüberzeugungen zu Krankheit und Gesundheit, KKG, 21 questions about beliefs of health control using six-step Likert scale, 6 = very correct, 1 = very false; high values = high level of conviction to control one’s own health; Lohaus and Schmitt 1998; Schuler 2006); a questionnaire to assess health-conscious behavior (Fragebogen zur Erfassung des Gesundheitsverhaltens, FEG, 13 questions about frequency of health-conscious behavior using five-step Likert scale, 5 = very often, 1 = never; high values = pronounced health-conscious behavior; Dlugosch and Krieger 1995).

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were carried out using SPSS for Windows, Version 17.0. For descriptive statistics, minima/maxima, means, and standard deviation (MI ± SD) were used. User data were weighted on independent variables (e.g., age, gender, etc.) using χ2 test and the Clopper–Pearson confidence intervals (CI) for categorical variables or the Wilcoxon–Mann–Whitney test in cases of continuous variables. Significance was defined as p ≤ 0.05. Odds ratios (OR) were calculated based on logistic regression analysis. Statistical analysis was supported by the Institute of Medical Biostatistics, Epidemiology and Informatics.

Results

Sample characteristics

All public universities offering specialized university lectures for elderly people throughout Germany (n = 64) were contacted and informed about the investigation, and 22 universities (34.4 %) cooperated.

Of the 1,672 questionnaires distributed, 603 were returned (response rate, 36.1 %). All participants under the age of 60 were excluded (n = 73). The answers of 530 participants were considered for analysis. Of the participants, 37.5 % (n = 199) were male, 61.7 % (n = 327) were female, and 0.7 % (n = 4) were left unspecified. The age of the participants (male and female) ranged from 60 to 94 years (69.74 ± 5.68 years), with men being slightly older (male, 60–90 years; 70.26 ± 5.48) than women (female, 60–94 years; 69.44 ± 5.81). The vast majority of participants (88.3 %; n = 468) were retired. The majority of participants (63.8 %, n = 338) had a university degree, 19.4 % (n = 103) had completed professional training, 9.1 % (n = 48) had a general qualification for university entrance, 5.1 % (n = 27) had a certificate of finished Secondary Education, and 2.5 % (n = 13) had a Master’s degree. More than half of all subjects were married (59.1 %; n = 313), 17.5 % (n = 93) widowed, and 14.0 % (n = 74) divorced. Most subjects participated in statutory health (73.4 %; n = 389), while only 24.9 % (n = 132) had private healthcare insurance.

Knowledge and assumptions about G. biloba

The vast majority of participants (90.4 %, n = 473) reported that they knew about G. biloba. A significantly larger proportion of female participants than male participants had heard about G. biloba (female: n = 305, 94.7 %; male: n = 165, 83.8 %; p < 0.001). Most participants (66.7 %) thought that G. biloba could be used for cognitive enhancement. In particular, 61.5 % assumed that G. biloba would be useful for memory enhancement. Approximately two thirds (64.1 %) of the sample assumed that G. biloba is effective for treating cognitive decline, while 31.8 % “knew” that G. biloba is effective in prevention of dementia.

Prevalence of G. biloba use

A total of 15.3 % (95 % CI, 11.8–18.0; n = 78) of participants reported the use of G. biloba supplements, with slightly more women (15.4 %; n = 50) than men (14.7 %; n = 27; p = 0.819) doing so. There were no significant differences between subjects who did and did not use G. biloba with respect to education, current professional situation, family status, or health insurance (see Table 1). However, there was a significant difference between users and non-users with respect to age, with higher prevalence rates of G. biloba use noted among older participants (user of G. biloba, 71.51 ± 6.65 years old; non-user, 68.00 ± 5.41 years old; p = 0.006). The average intake reported was 139.4 ± 82.7 mg per day; male participants used a slightly higher dosage (148.8 ± 113.3 mg) than female participants (134.4 ± 61.6 mg; p = 0.563). Substances were mainly used to improve cognitive performance (80.5 %; n = 60) or to prevent dementia (47.6 %; n = 30). Sex differences could not be detected for “improve cognitive performance” (male: 92.6 %%, n = 25; female: 73.5 %, n = 36; p = 0.097) and for “prevent dementia” (male: 61.9 %, n = 13; female: 40.5 %, n = 17; p = 0.194). In addition, the pharmaceuticals were also used to treat tinnitus (36.4 %), to enhance the immune system (32.7 %), and to improve peripheral arterial disease (30.9 %). No significant differences concerning gender could be found regarding the use of G. biloba for somatic diseases. Most people had heard about the substances from physicians and pharmacists (73.3 %). For over half of all users (53.8 %), the drug had been recommended by a physician (see Fig. 1). Pharmaceutical companies associated with G. biloba products could be spontaneously named by 76.6 % of all participants by free recall. The following pharmaceutical companies were named by the users and had produced the used G. biloba drug: Dr. W. Schwabe GmbH & Co. KG (Tebonin®; 32.8 %), Hexal AG (Gingium®; 29.7 %), ratiopharm GmbH (Ginkobil®; 14.1 %). Most of the substances had been purchased in pharmacies (58.0 %). When asked whether the effects of G. biloba are scientifically proven, the majority of participants were convinced that the effect of G. biloba supplements had been scientifically proven (six-step Likert scale: 1 = “very correct”, 6 = “very false”; all participants, 2.35); furthermore, a higher number of users of G. biloba were convinced that the effect of G. biloba has been scientifically proven than non-users (six-step Likert scale: 1 = “very correct”, 6 = “very false”; users, 3.27; non-users, 2.58; p < 0.001).

Table 1.

Socio-economic characteristics of the sample according to G. biloba use

Characteristics Users (n = 78) (%) Non-users (n = 452) (%) p values
Mean age in years (±standard deviation) 71.51 (±6.65) 68.00 (±5.41) 0.006
Gender
  Female 63.6 64.9 0.819
  Male 36.4 35.1
Education
  University degree 55.1 65.7 0.389
  Professional training completed 23.1 18.5
  Secondary school certificate (Realschule) 2.6 3.2
  Qualification for university entrance 14.1 8.1
  Secondary school certificate (Hauptschule) 1.3 2.3
  Master certificate 3.8 2.1
Current professional situation
  Retired 89.6 88.9 0.165
  Partial retirement 1.3 4.6
  Housewife/man 2.6 3.5
  Employed 2.6 2.1
  Part time employed 3.9 0.7
  N/C 0.0 0.2
Family status
  Married 56.4 58.9 0.380
  Widowed 21.8 16.6
  Divorced 10.3 15.2
  Single 10.3 9.0
  N/C 1.3 0.2
Health insurance
  Private healthcare 28.2 24.7 0.380
  Others 71.8 75.3

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Sources of information on G. biloba

Characteristics of G. biloba supplement users

Bivariate analysis showed that participants taking G. biloba had higher values for health-conscious behavior (analysis according FEG manual: five-step Likert scale, 5 = very often, 1 = never; G. biloba users, 3.52; non-users, 3.29; p < 0.001), used natural remedies more often (use of natural remedies among G. biloba users: n = 65 = 83.3 %, use of natural remedies among non-users of G. biloba: n = 274 = 63.3 %, p = 0.001) and were more likely to use vitamins (use of vitamins among user of G. biloba: n = 42 = 53.8 %; use of vitamins among non-users of G. biloba: n = 155 = 34.2 %, p = 0.001). Moreover, G. biloba users were more concerned about aging when compared to non-users (analysis according to AAS manual: six-step Likert scale, 6 = very correct, 1 = very false; G. biloba users, 57.86; non-users, 55.52; p = 0.036) and self-reported a larger number of SMCQ (analysis according SMCQ manual: dichotomous questions (yes/no); G. biloba users, 3.71 ± 2.86; non-users, 2.86 ± 2.30, p = 0.022) compared to non-users of G. biloba. Multivariate analysis by logistic regression showed that age (OR, 1.06; CI, 1.01–1.11), the use of vitamins and herbal supplements (OR, 2.16; CI, 1.06–4.40), and the use of natural remedies (OR, 1.42; CI, 1.08–1.85) were positively related to G. biloba use. In the case of slight disorders (e.g., symptoms of a cold, etc.), G. biloba users more frequently used naturopathic treatments than non-users (p < 0.001; OR, 1.42; CI, 1.08–1.85).

G. biloba users generally evaluated their own memory to be significantly worse than non-users (according to SMCQ: p = 0.022). Furthermore, compared to non-users, a significantly higher number of G. biloba users evaluated their capacity of memory subjectively to be so poor that—according to the questionnaire’s specifications—physicians would have to consider the diagnosis of dementia (G. biloba users, n = 21 = 28.8 %; non-users, n = 57 = 13.4 %; p = 0.001).

We detected no differences between G. biloba users and non-users regarding self-efficacy beliefs for illness and health (analysis according KKG manual: six-step Likert scale, 6 = very correct, 1 = very false). Indeed, none of the three subscales showed any significant differences (internal health locus of control: user of G. biloba, 43.34; non-users, 45.48 (p = 0.507); external-p (powerful others) user of G. biloba: 67.32; non-users, 65.00 (p = 0.666); external-c (chance) user of G. biloba: 80.41; non-users, 77.17 (p = 0.196)). Furthermore, the SCL-90-R detected no significant differences between users of G. biloba and non-users (analysis according to SCL-90 manual: T-value GSI: users of G. biloba: 44.65 ± 9.62; non-users, 44.60 ± 10.25 (p = 0.282)). However, significant differences were detected using the GHQ-12 (analysis according GHQ-12 manual: G. biloba users, 1.28 ± 2.46; non-users, 0.31 ± 2.74 (p = 0.012)), implying that G. biloba users evaluated their general health to be worse than non-users. The significance of influencing factors for the use of G. biloba is shown in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Factors determining the use of G. biloba

Prevalence of vitamins/dietary supplement use

The prevalence of vitamin and dietary supplement use was 65.8 %. Bivariate analysis showed that supplement usage was significantly higher among subjects who were female (male: n = 117, 58.8 %; female: n = 229, 70.0 %; p = 0.01), exhibited health-conscious behavior (p < 0.001), self-reported memory complaints most often (p = 0.003), and used natural remedies (p = 0.003). A multivariate analysis by logistic regression revealed that subjects with higher values for health-conscious behavior (FEG) (OR, 3.73; CI, 2.32–6.02) and subjects with higher values regarding subjective memory complaints (SMCQ; OR, 1.12; CI, 1.02–1.23) used vitamins/dietary supplements more often.

Sources of information

The use of G. biloba was most often recommended by health professionals such as physicians (57.3 %, n = 43) or chemists (16.0 %, n = 12; see Fig. 1). Moreover, 28.0 % of all users reported that friends and family recommended the supplement. Only 15 % of all users had not discussed the use of G. biloba with their (family) physician and none of the users reported that their doctor had advised against taking the supplement. No significant distinctions were observed between the responses of male and female subjects.

Discussion

In this study, we found that the prevalence of G. biloba use in a population of 1,672 highly educated people was 15.3 %. Although there was a significant difference between users and non-users with respect to age, there were no differences between the groups with respect to education, current professional situation, family status, or health insurance.

Compared to our study, three US household studies revealed a prevalence of G. biloba use of 2 % (Barnes et al. 2008), 1.9–2.2 % among males and 1.2–4 % among female participants (Kelly et al. 2005) and <1–4 % among male and 1–5 % among female participants (Kaufman et al. 2002). A study performed by our research group of regular German students (mean, 24.4 years) revealed a lifetime prevalence for the use of G. biloba of 4.5 %, which is consistent with previous results. In addition, people aged over 65 years more often used G. biloba than younger ones which fits with our results (Kelly et al. 2005). Similar findings were noted for female participants aged ≥65 years in the study by Kaufmann et al. (2002). These data, taken together, suggest higher prevalence rates for the use of G. biloba among people as they get older, highlighting our results showing that G. biloba users were significantly older than non-users.

Regarding the types of substances used to increase cognition, the most recent study about cognitive enhancement among 2,600 students using a specialized technique shows a 1-year prevalence of 20 % for the nonmedical use of prescription and illicit drugs for cognitive enhancement (Dietz et al. 2013). Furthermore, an online poll conducted by the journal “Nature” demonstrated a lifetime prevalence of 20 % for the use of stimulants, modafinil, or beta blockers for cognitive enhancement purposes (Maher 2008). Compared to G. biloba, these so-called “smart pills” seem to play a more important role among regular students and employed persons than G. biloba (Franke and Lieb 2010; Smith and Farah 2011; de Jongh et al. 2008). However, substance use to increase cognition among regular students is captured by the “Cognitive Enhancement” paradigm, which suggests that to increase academic performance and enhance their position, students are often prepared to take high risks (Franke et al. 2012).

Regarding educational status, the majority of participants in our study had a university degree (63.8 %, n = 338), which highlights the high educational status of the surveyed population. Although we did not aim to include participants with considerably different education statuses, we can demonstrate a relatively high prevalence of G. biloba use among this group of highly educated participants. In this respect, the study of Adusumil and colleagues compared participants’ educational status and revealed higher prevalence of G. biloba use among participants with higher educational status (Adusumilli et al. 2004) which underlines our findings.

With respect to recommendations regarding the use of G. biloba, more than 50 % of participants in our study were advised to use G. biloba by a physician, which is one of the most important findings of this study and certainly explains, at least in part, the high prevalence of G. biloba use. This observation confirms results of a previous US study which revealed that 38 % of surveyed US geriatricians recommend the use of G. biloba to their patients (Watts et al. 2001). One can only speculate as to why physicians recommend G. biloba—especially given that the use of G. biloba does not conform to treatment guidelines, which have shown the lack of efficacy of G. biloba and advise against the recommendation of G. biloba to patients (Maier 2010; Vollmar et al. 2008).

One of the most important reasons for primary care physicians to recommend such drugs to patients (and for subjects asking for preventive strategies against cognitive decline) may be the patients’ expectation of a solution and their desire for a prescription (Bradley 1992; Lewis and Tully 2011). This may increase pressure among physicians to prescribe “lifestyle” or “herbal” medicine such as G. biloba (Walley 2002). In addition, physicians may feel the need to prescribe a drug with (nearly) no side effects, which may at least offer a “placebo” effect to healthy subjects.

A further explanation for the high prevalence of G. biloba use is the users’ assumed “knowledge” about G. biloba and its scientifically proven procognitive effectiveness. In this respect, a physicians’ recommendation is quite important. Further sources of knowledge available to users include the internet, healthcare magazines, and patient brochures containing information about G. biloba and other herbal products from unknown sources. The pharmaceutical industry may have a significant influence over the distribution of this information to patients and healthy subjects searching for preventive strategies against cognitive decline (e.g., via the internet, etc.; Morris and Avorn 2003). Therefore, it seems particularly difficult to inform oneself objectively about G. biloba and further herbal products which are marketed for cognitive well-being and the prevention of cognitive decline (Morris and Avorn 2003).

However, in regard to scientific literature, older clinical and experimental laboratory studies, which often used poor methodological approaches, seem to support the procognitive clinical effectiveness of G. biloba (Weinmann et al. 2010; Kanowski et al. 1996; Le Bars et al. 1997). Such studies are often sponsored by pharmaceutical companies that produce G. biloba products (e.g., Dr. Willmar Schwabe AG). Independent studies using elaborated methods (i.e., randomized controlled double-blind clinical trials) and meta-analyses (e.g., the Cochrane collaboration) clearly demonstrate the lack of procognitive effects of G. biloba (Birks et al. 2002; Birks and Grimley Evans 2007, 2009; Solomon et al. 2002).

Based on a previous study, we were able to demonstrate that G. biloba users also use other natural remedies, food supplements, and beverages that seem to be known to be salutary among the general public (Morris and Avorn 2003), suggesting that the act of using substances without scientific evidence is not limited to G. biloba. This behavior fits well with the high values for health-conscious behavior, as we were able to demonstrate. Furthermore, G. biloba users are very critical of their own cognitive and memory skills, thus highlighting their awareness concerning the loss of their cognitive skills. It may be that their perception of their cognitive capabilities makes them more likely to use non-evidence-based methods to conserve their remaining cognitive capacity. One important reason for this awareness and the assumed consequence of using G. biloba and other herbal products may be the high degree of popularity of the supplement, as 90.4 % of our sample knew about G. biloba.

Additionally, cognition, which is the basis for further psychosocial functioning, is an integral part of the multidimensional definition of “successful aging” (Jeste et al. 2010; Baltes 1997; Rowe and Kahn 1987). However, cognitive decline is part of the normal aging process which the studied population attempts to avoid. Indeed, they are prepared to use non-evidence-based, herbal “lifestyle” drugs to avoid cognitive decline regardless of the effectiveness of the substances used. G. biloba and other used herbal substances have nearly no side effects that could decrease the user’s quality of life. Therefore, these substances can only ameliorate the aging process from a well-being perspective and do not impair their users.

This study has several limitations. (1) First, the data are not representative of the healthy elderly population, as the entire focus of the study was understanding a particular subset of elderly adults, i.e., those who are healthy, elderly, and highly educated. Furthermore, our participants were actively using cognition within the scope of their studies and had a higher degree of introspection as a result of being afraid of losing cognitive function. These factors might have led them to be more sensitive to cognitive decline. (2) In addition, only 34 % of the German universities offering specialized study programs for elderly people participated; thus, the response rate was only 36 %. Therefore, the national representativeness of this study was jeopardized. However, the study included participants from all corners of Germany, which increased the power of the data. To our knowledge, these are the only available data concerning preventive strategies with respect to cognitive decline among healthy, elderly, and highly educated people in Germany. (3) An important limitation lies in the fact that G. biloba had been investigated as a representative of other herbal supplements. Although it is plausible that the findings presented here apply to other supplements as well, this remains speculative and should be explored in future studies.

In this respect, our study should be regarded as preliminary and should be followed by other more representative surveys. Further studies should investigate the knowledge, assumed knowledge, and the prevalence of evidence-based and non-evidence-based strategies for maintaining health among healthy elderly people.

Finally, we can conclude that this study primarily reveals the extent to which healthcare professionals recommend the use of G. biloba and the extent to which users assume G. biloba to be an effective and scientifically proven strategy for enhancing cognition and treating cognitive decline. This conclusion is consistent with the high prevalence of G. biloba use, although there is poor scientific evidence supporting its effectiveness. Access to objective and reliable information proved to be limited in the surveyed population. With regard to the growing use of herbal and dietary supplements such as G. biloba, reliable and not misleading information should be provided to the physicians who recommend and prescribe these products and to consumers themselves.

Acknowledgments

The project was funded by the BMBF (German Ministry of Education and Research; no. 01GP0807); statistical analyses were supported by the Institute for Medical Biometry, Epidemiology and Informatics (IMBEI) of the University Medical Centre of the Johannes Gutenberg-University Mainz.

References

  1. Abebe W, Herman W, Konzelman J. Herbal supplement use among adult dental patients in a USA dental school clinic: prevalence, patient demographics, and clinical implications. Oral Surg Oral Med Oral Pathol Oral Radiol Endod. 2011;111(3):320–325. doi: 10.1016/j.tripleo.2010.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adusumilli PS, Ben-Porat L, Pereira M, Roesler D, Leitman IM. The prevalence and predictors of herbal medicine use in surgical patients. J Am Coll Surg. 2004;198(4):583–590. doi: 10.1016/j.jamcollsurg.2003.11.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ahlemeyer B, Krieglstein J. Neuroprotective effects of Ginkgo biloba extract. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2003;60(9):1779–1792. doi: 10.1007/s00018-003-3080-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baltes PB. On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny. Selection, optimization, and compensation as foundation of developmental theory. Am Psychol. 1997;52(4):366–380. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.4.366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barnes PM, Bloom B, Nahin RL. Complementary and alternative medicine use among adults and children: United States, 2007. Natl Health Stat Report. 2008;12:1–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bastianetto S, Zheng WH, Quirion R. The Ginkgo biloba extract (EGb 761) protects and rescues hippocampal cells against nitric oxide-induced toxicity: involvement of its flavonoid constituents and protein kinase C. J Neurochem. 2000;74(6):2268–2277. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2000.0742268.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Birks J, Grimley Evans J (2007) Ginkgo biloba for cognitive impairment and dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2):CD003120 [DOI] [PubMed]
  8. Birks J, Grimley Evans J (2009) Ginkgo biloba for cognitive impairment and dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (1):CD003120 [DOI] [PubMed]
  9. Birks J, Grimley EV, Van Dongen M (2002) Ginkgo biloba for cognitive impairment and dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (4):CD003120 [DOI] [PubMed]
  10. Blumenthal M (1997) Herbal market levels after five years of boom. HerbalGram 47:64–5
  11. Bradley CP. Uncomfortable prescribing decisions: a critical incident study. BMJ. 1992;304(6822):294–296. doi: 10.1136/bmj.304.6822.294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brayne C. The elephant in the room - healthy brains in later life, epidemiology and public health. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(3):233–239. doi: 10.1038/nrn2091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. de Jongh R, Bolt I, Schermer M, Olivier B. Botox for the brain: enhancement of cognition, mood and pro-social behavior and blunting of unwanted memories. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32(4):760–776. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. DeFeudis FV, Drieu K. Ginkgo biloba extract (EGb 761) and CNS functions: basic studies and clinical applications. Curr Drug Targets. 2000;1(1):25–58. doi: 10.2174/1389450003349380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dietz P, Striegel H, Franke AG, Lieb K, Simon P, Ulrich R (2013): Randomized Response Estimates for the One-Year Prevalence of Cognitive Enhancement in University Students. Pharmacotherapy, (in press) [DOI] [PubMed]
  16. Dlugosch G, Krieger W. Fragebogen zur Erfassung des Gesundheitsverhaltens: FEG. Frankfurt: Swets Test Services; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  17. Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Logan J, Franceschi D, Franceschi M, MacGregor R, Shea C, Garza V, Liu N, Ding YS. Evidence that gingko biloba extract does not inhibit MAO A and B in living human brain. Life Sci. 2000;66(9):PL141–PL146. doi: 10.1016/S0024-3205(99)00660-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Franke AG, Lieb K. Pharmacological neuroenhancement and brain doping : Chances and risks. Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundheitsforschung Gesundheitsschutz. 2010;53(8):853–859. doi: 10.1007/s00103-010-1105-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Franke AG, Lieb K, Fellgiebel A. From symptomatic to disease modifying therapy? Recent developments in the pharmacotherapy of Alzheimer’s disease. Fortschr Neurol Psychiatr. 2009;77(6):326–333. doi: 10.1055/s-0028-1109378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Franke AG, Bonertz C, Christmann M, Lieb K. Attitudes Toward Cognitive Enhancement in Users and Nonusers of Stimulants for Cognitive Enhancement: A Pilot Study. AJOB Prim Res. 2012;3(1):48–57. doi: 10.1080/21507716.2011.608411. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Franke GH (2002) SCL-90-R: Die symptom checkliste von L. R. Derogatis. Göttingen: Beltz Test GmbH
  22. Goldberg DP, Blackwell B. Psychiatric illness in general practice. A detailed study using a new method of case identification. Br Med J. 1970;1(5707):439–443. doi: 10.1136/bmj.2.5707.439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hui EK, Rubenstein LZ. Promoting physical activity and exercise in older adults. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2006;7(5):310–314. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2006.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Jeste DV, Depp CA, Vahia IV. Successful cognitive and emotional aging. World Psychiatry. 2010;9(2):78–84. doi: 10.1002/j.2051-5545.2010.tb00277.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kanowski S, Herrmann WM, Stephan K, Wierich W, Horr R. Proof of efficacy of the Ginkgo biloba special extract EGb 761 in outpatients suffering from mild to moderate primary degenerative dementia of the Alzheimer type or multi-infarct dementia. Pharmacopsychiatry. 1996;29(2):47–56. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-979544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kaufman DW, Kelly JP, Rosenberg L, Anderson TE, Mitchell AA. Recent patterns of medication use in the ambulatory adult population of the United States: the Slone survey. Jama. 2002;287(3):337–344. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.3.337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kelly JP, Kaufman DW, Kelley K, Rosenberg L, Anderson TE, Mitchell AA. Recent trends in use of herbal and other natural products. Arch Intern Med. 2005;165(3):281–286. doi: 10.1001/archinte.165.3.281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lasher KP, Faulkender PJ. Measurement of aging anxiety: development of the Anxiety about Aging Scale. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 1993;37(4):247–259. doi: 10.2190/1U69-9AU2-V6LH-9Y1L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Le Bars PL, Katz MM, Berman N, Itil TM, Freedman AM, Schatzberg AF. A placebo-controlled, double-blind, randomized trial of an extract of Ginkgo biloba for dementia. North American EGb Study Group. Jama. 1997;278(16):1327–1332. doi: 10.1001/jama.278.16.1327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lewis PJ, Tully MP. The discomfort caused by patient pressure on the prescribing decisions of hospital prescribers. Res Social Adm Pharm. 2011;7(1):4–15. doi: 10.1016/j.sapharm.2010.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lohaus A, Schmitt GM. Fragebogen zur Erhebung von Kontrollüberzeugungen zu Krankheit und Gesundheit (KKG) Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  32. Maclennan KM, Darlington CL, Smith PF. The CNS effects of Ginkgo biloba extracts and ginkgolide B. Prog Neurobiol. 2002;67(3):235–257. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(02)00015-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Maher B. Poll results: look who’s doping. Nature. 2008;452(7188):674–675. doi: 10.1038/452674a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Maier W. Diagnose- und Behandlungsleitlinie Demenz: Interdisziplinäre S3-Praxisleitlinien. Berlin: Springer; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  35. Morris CA, Avorn J. Internet marketing of herbal products. Jama. 2003;290(11):1505–1509. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.11.1505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Oyama Y, Chikahisa L, Ueha T, Kanemaru K, Noda K. Ginkgo biloba extract protects brain neurons against oxidative stress induced by hydrogen peroxide. Brain Res. 1996;712(2):349–352. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01440-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Pearson N, Biddle SJ, Gorely T. Family correlates of fruit and vegetable consumption in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr. 2009;12(2):267–283. doi: 10.1017/S1368980008002589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Rhodes RE, Martin AD, Taunton JE, Rhodes EC, Donnelly M, Elliot J. Factors associated with exercise adherence among older adults. An individual perspective. Sports Med. 1999;28:397–411. doi: 10.2165/00007256-199928060-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Rimbach G, Wolffram S, Watanabe C, Packer L, Gohil K. Effect of Ginkgo biloba (EGb 761) on differential gene expression. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2003;36(Suppl 1):S95–S99. doi: 10.1055/s-2003-40457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Rowe JW, Kahn RL. Human aging: usual and successful. Science. 1987;237(4811):143–149. doi: 10.1126/science.3299702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Schneider LS. Ginkgo and AD: key negatives and lessons from GuidAge. Lancet Neurol. 2012;11(10):836–837. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70212-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Schuler H (2006) Lehrbuch der Personalpsychologie. Göttingen: Hogrefe
  43. Smith ME, Farah MJ. Are prescription stimulants "smart pills"? The epidemiology and cognitive neuroscience of prescription stimulant use by normal healthy individuals. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(5):717–741. doi: 10.1037/a0023825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Solomon PR, Adams F, Silver A, Zimmer J, DeVeaux R. Ginkgo for memory enhancement: a randomized controlled trial. Jama. 2002;288(7):835–840. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.7.835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Stern Y, Gurland B, Tatemichi TK, Tang MX, Wilder D, Mayeux R. Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. Jama. 1994;271(13):1004–1010. doi: 10.1001/jama.1994.03510370056032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. van der Bij AK, Laurant MG, Wensing M. Effectiveness of physical activity interventions for older adults: a review. Am J Prev Med. 2002;22(2):120–133. doi: 10.1016/S0749-3797(01)00413-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Vellas B, Coley N, Ousset PJ, Berrut G, Dartigues JF, Dubois B, Grandjean H, Pasquier F, Piette F, Robert P, Touchon J, Garnier P, Mathiex-Fortunet H, Andrieu S. Long-term use of standardised Ginkgo biloba extract for the prevention of Alzheimer’s disease (GuidAge): a randomised placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Neurol. 2012;11(10):851–859. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70206-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Vollmar HC, Mand P, Butzlaff ME. DEGAM-Leitlinie Nr. 12: Demenz. Stand: 2008. Düsseldorf: Omikron Publ; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  49. Walley T. Lifestyle medicines and the elderly. Drugs Aging. 2002;19(3):163–168. doi: 10.2165/00002512-200219030-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Watkins RE, Coates R, Ferroni P. Measurement of aging anxiety in an elderly Australian population. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 1998;46(4):319–332. doi: 10.2190/0LU1-0UWE-2TLW-QVAP. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Watts D, Damasco-Ty E, Ryan F, Goodman B. Geriatricians health survey 2000. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001;49(11):1535–1538. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.4911250.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Weinmann S, Roll S, Schwarzbach C, Vauth C, Willich SN. Effects of Ginkgo biloba in dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Geriatr. 2010;10:14. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-10-14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Youn JC, Kim KW, Lee DY, Jhoo JH, Lee SB, Park JH, Choi EA, Choe JY, Jeong JW, Choo IH, Woo JI. Development of the Subjective Memory Complaints Questionnaire. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2009;27(4):310–317. doi: 10.1159/000205512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Age are provided here courtesy of American Aging Association

RESOURCES