Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Schizophr Res. 2013 Aug 7;150(1):223–228. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.040

Acculturative stress and psychotic-like experiences among Asian and Latino immigrants to the United States

Jordan E DeVylder a,b,*, Hans Y Oh a, Lawrence H Yang b,c, Leopoldo J Cabassa a,d, Fang-pei Chen a, Ellen P Lukens a,b
PMCID: PMC3896050  NIHMSID: NIHMS542822  PMID: 23932446

Abstract

Previous studies have shown variation in the prevalence and incidence of psychosis across immigrant groups, but the underlying mechanisms are not fully understood. Stress related to acculturation may increase risk for psychosis among immigrant groups. In this study we examine the association between acculturative stress and psychotic-like experiences in a sample of Latino- and Asian-American immigrants to the United States in the National Latino and Asian American Study (n = 2434). Acculturative stress was associated with visual and auditory hallucinations among Asians, but only with hearing voices among Latinos. Increased risk for psychotic-like experiences among Latinos was primarily associated with younger age of immigration. Acculturative stress appears to be a promising candidate mechanism explaining the relationship between immigration and psychosis, particularly among Asian Americans. Ethnic differences may reflect variability between groups that integrate more readily into the host culture and those that are subject to greater discrimination and environmental adversity.

Keywords: Psychosis, Migration, Epidemiology, Acculturation, Schizophrenia

1. Introduction

The association between immigration and psychosis has been firmly established through more than twenty international studies and two meta-analyses, with an estimated relative risk of 2.1 to 2.7 for first-generation immigrants relative to native-born populations (Cantor-Graae and Selten, 2005; Bourque et al., 2011). This appears to be driven by social factors rather than selective migration of people who have greater genetic risk (Selten et al., 2007). In this paper, we examine the association between acculturative stress and psychotic experiences in two first-generation immigrant groups.

1.1. Social determinants of psychosis among immigrants

Increased risk for psychosis among immigrants and ethnic minorities is associated with social adversity, perceived discrimination, and ethnic density within the host country (Morgan et al., 2010). Social adversity mediated differences in psychosis risk by ethnicity and immigration status in several case–control studies, whether defined as childhood separation from parents (Morgan et al., 2007) or cumulative social disadvantage across multiple domains, including education, employment, living environment, and social networks (Mallett et al., 2002; Hjern et al., 2004; Morgan et al., 2008a,b; Vanheusden et al., 2008). Ethnic minorities are also at increased risk for psychosis if exposed to racial attacks or employer racism (Karlsen and Nazroo, 2002) or societallevel discrimination (Veling et al., 2007), although not across all ethnic groups (Veling et al., 2008a). This is consistent with the ethnic density effect in immigrants, in which incidence of psychosis among immigrants declines when that particular immigrant group composes a larger segment of the host population (Boydell et al., 2001; Das-Munshi et al., 2012; Kirkbride et al., 2007; Schofield et al., 2011; Veling et al., 2008b). Given that the reviewed social determinants all constitute exposures that occur in the host country, younger age at immigration should be associated with greater duration of exposure and, consequently, increased risk for psychosis. This has been shown among non-western immigrants to The Netherlands, but not among western immigrants, who may be of higher SES and may assimilate more readily into the host culture (Veling et al., 2011).

1.2. Acculturative stress

Acculturation, an individual's cultural learning process that occurs while adapting to an alien culture (Rudmin, 2009), may contribute to risk for psychiatric problems as the individual struggles to integrate the values, behaviors, and social norms associated with the host culture while simultaneously maintaining the culture of origin (Berry, 1997). Acculturative stress refers to the tension or strain associated with the acquisition of a second-culture that may result in adverse physical or mental health effects (Rudmin, 2009). Acculturative stress has only minimally been studied in people with psychosis, with one case–control study finding that acculturation was correlated with global function, but not symptom severity, among immigrants with psychosis in Greece (Gonidakis et al., 2011).

1.3. Psychotic-like experiences

The psychosis phenotype has been conceptualized as a continuum, wherein symptoms occur across the population at both clinical and subclinical levels, varying in severity but sharing risk factors (van OS et al., 2009). Psychotic-like experiences (PLE) are associated with increased risk of later developing psychotic and, to a lesser extent, non-psychotic disorders (Werbeloff et al., 2012). PLE are considerably more prevalent than threshold psychotic disorders, and can be reliably assessed in general population studies (Kessler et al., 2005). As such, they provide a useful construct for assessing risk factors for psychosis using lay-administered population-level data sets. Immigration has been associated with increased risk for PLE (Linscott and van Os, 2010), specifically hallucinations (Vanheusden et al., 2008) and delusions (Janssen et al., 2003). To our knowledge, acculturative stress has not been studied with respect to PLE in a population-level sample.

1.4. Aims and hypotheses

This study examines the relationship between acculturative stress and PLE in a sample of first-generation Latino- and Asian-American immigrants in the United States (U.S.). Prior evidence suggests that social factors mediate the association between immigration and psychosis-risk, with acculturative stress as a promising yet under-studied candidate mechanism. We predicted that acculturative stress would be associated with greater risk for lifetime PLE and for specific symptoms (hallucinations and delusions) among Latino and Asian immigrants to the U.S., controlling for demographics and age of immigration.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

Data were drawn from the National Latino and Asian American Study (NLAAS; collected May 2002–November 2003), a national area probability sample of non-institutionalized adults (n = 4649) of Latino and Asian origin (Heeringa et al., 2004; Alegria et al., 2006). The NLAAS used a multi-stage probability design to draw samples of housing units and household members from primary sampling units (metropolitan statistical areas and county units) and secondary units (contiguous groupings of census blocks) with probability proportionate to size, with oversampling of census blocks with greater than 5% density of target ancestry groups. Secondary respondents were recruited from households that had one eligible member who already completed the interview.

Respondents were excluded if they were 65 years of age or older (n = 207) to rule out aging-related medical or neurological causes of PLE, consistent with prior studies (van Os et al., 2009). Participants were also excluded if they were not first-generation immigrants (n = 1996), or had missing data (n = 12). The final sample included 2434 participants, who significantly differed from those excluded in several ways: they were younger, 37.3 (SE = 0.4) versus 40.5 (SE = 0.8) years, t(4647) = 4.38, p < 0.001, more likely to be Asian, 30.3% (SE = 2.3) versus 22.8% (SE = 2.3), χ2 (df = 1) = 10.1, p = 0.001, with lower prevalence of PLE, 10.7% (SE = 1.0) versus 7.0% (SE = 0.8), χ2 (df = 1) = 6.7, p = 0.01.

2.2. Measures

Lay interviewers were bilingual and trained to administer the survey through computer-assisted interviewing software. Interviews were conducted in-person unless a phone interview was requested or face-to-face interview was not possible. Instruments were available in English, Spanish, Mandarin, Cantonese, Tagalog, and Vietnamese. NLAAS data collection methods were previously described in greater detail (Alegria et al., 2006).

Demographic variables (age, sex, ethnicity, education, income, age at immigration, years in U.S.) were self-reported by respondents. Ethnicity was coded as eight nationalities from Latin American and Asian countries, which were consolidated into two yes/no ethnicity variables (Latino and Asian; Table 1) due to the low frequency of PLE among each nationality.

Table 1.

Nation of origin for Latino and Asian respondents.

Unweighted n (%) Weighted % (SE)
Asian (total) 1226 (50.4) 30.3 (2.3)
  Vietnamese 347 (14.3) 4.6 (0.8)
  Chinese 337 (13.8) 8.6 (1.1)
  Filipino 288 (11.8) 6.4 (0.9)
  Other 315 (10.4) 10.7 (1.3)
Latino (total) 1208 (49.6) 69.7 (2.3)
  Mexican 394 (16.2) 39.6 (3.9)
  Cuban 315 (12.9) 3.6 (0.4)
  Puerto Rican 140 (5.8) 4.3 (0.5)
  Other 359 (14.7) 22.2 (2.1)

Note: All analyses utilized weighted estimates of the total Latino and Asian groups.

Lifetime prevalence of PLE was assessed using the WHO-CIDI 3.0 psychosis screen (Kessler and Üstün, 2004), previously used with Latino (Lewis-Fernández et al., 2009) and, to a lesser extent, Asian samples (Kessler et al., 2005) in the U.S., and internationally in more than 50 countries (Nuevo et al., 2012). Symptoms included visual hallucinations, hearing voices, and four variables that were collectively recoded as delusions (any present versus all absent) given their low frequency in this sample: 1) mind control experience, 2) mind taken over by strange forces, 3) communication attempts from strange forces, and 4) unjust plot to harm you that no one else believed. No results were significantly changed in a sensitivity analyses in which each delusion symptom was examined separately. Responses were not considered PLE if they occurred in the context of falling asleep, dreaming, or substance use.

Participants responded to nine dichotomous acculturative stress items, which were summed into a total score. These items were developed to assess the experiences of Latinos (Guarnaccia et al., 2007) but have also been used with Asians (Lueck and Wilson, 2010, 2011). The use of items specific to acculturation-related stress exposures alleviates some problems of prior acculturative stress measures, which overlapped conceptually with general psychological distress and depression (Rudmin, 2009). The reliability of the scale is adequate (Hair et al., 2009), Cronbach's alpha = 0.66, although greater in Latinos (alpha = 0.69) than in Asians (alpha = 0.59).

2.3. Analyses

All analyses were conducted using the complex sample features of SPSS version 20 (IBM Corp., 2012). Statistical estimates were weighted to account for individual-level sampling factors including non-response and unequal probabilities of selection, as well as post-stratification, to ensure that the sample was representative based on the 2001 census estimates. Separate sample weights specific to Asian and Latino subsamples were used. Design-based analyses were used to estimate standard errors that accounted for the complex multistage clustered design of the NLAAS sample. Demographic variables, acculturative stress items, and PLE were compared between ethnic groups using the Wald chi-square tests.

We used logistic regression models to examine the associations between acculturative stress and PLE, adjusted for demographic variables and stratified by ethnicity. Acculturative stress was recorded as a categorical variable to facilitate calculation of adjusted odds ratios and to eliminate problems related to the non-normal (positively skewed) distribution of acculturative stress scores. Adjusted odds ratios were calculated for all predictors. Acculturative stress was tested for dose– response relationships using the χ2 analysis of linear trends. Associations were considered significant at two-tailed salpha = 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Demographics and clinical variables

Compared to Latino respondents, Asian respondents were older, predominantly female, of higher income and education, and older at time of immigration (Table 2). Approximately 7% of the sample reported PLE over the course of their lifetime, which primarily consisted of visual or auditory hallucinations (Table 3).

Table 2.

Demographics of the sample.

Asian
n = 1226
Latino
n = 1208
Wald χ2, p
Age 24.8, <0.001
  18–29 23.2 (1.8) 31.0 (1.4) 12.7, <0.001
  30–39 29.0 (1.8) 33.3 (1.7) 3.0, 0.081
  40–49 25.4 (1.9) 20.0 (1.5) 4.3, 0.039
  50–64 22.4 (1.8) 15.7 (1.1) 13.0, <0.001
Sex 7.3, 0.007
  Male 46.8 (1.7) 54.3 (2.1)
  Female 53.2 (1.7) 45.7 (2.1)
Income 113.7, <0.001
  <26,000 16.7 (1.6) 44.0 (2.7) 88.8, <0.001
  26,000–64,999 31.5 (2.2) 37.4 (1.9) 4.1, 0.043
  >65,000 48.1 (2.4) 16.4 (1.8) 86.8, <0.001
Education 184.1, <0.001
  <12 years 14.7 (1.7) 53.3 (2.0) 181.5, <0.001
  12 years 15.9 (1.4) 21.0 (1.4) 6.5, 0.011
  13–15 years 24.5 (2.0) 16.6 (1.6) 11.7, 0.001
  >16 years 44.9 (2.5) 9.1 (1.3) 117.3, <0.001
Age at immigration 48.4, <0.001
  <12 years 15.9 (2.1) 20.3 (1.5) 2.2, 0.138
  13–17 years 7.4 (0.9) 20.3 (1.5) 48.0, <0.001
  18–64 years 76.7 (2.4) 59.4 (2.1) 19.9, <0.001
Years in United States 0.6, 0.724
  <10 years 35.7 (2.9) 34.4 (2.7) 0.1, 0.763
  11–20 years 34.7 (1.9) 33.8 (1.9) 0.1, 0.745
  >20 years 29.6 (2.2) 31.7 (1.7) 0.5, 0.488

Note: Values indicate % (SE).

Table 3.

Frequency of endorsement of acculturative stress items and psychotic-like experiences.

Asian Latino Wald χ2, p
Acculturative stress items
Limited contact with family and friends 37.2 (2.5) 50.0 (2.4) 11.3, 0.001
Interaction hard due to difficulty with English language 30.7 (2.4) 43.9 (1.9) 21.8, <0.001
Difficult to find work due to Latino/Asian descent 24.7 (1.8) 32.6 (1.8) 10.9, 0.001
Do not have the same respect in US as in country of origin 23.6 (1.7) 31.3 (1.2) 12.6, <0.001
Questioned about legal status 19.2 (1.8) 32.5 (2.0) 25.3, <0.001
Treated badly due to poor/accented English 26.7 (2.0) 23.5 (1.7) 1.7, 0.199
Felt guilty about leaving family/friends in country of origin 18.8 (1.4) 17.3 (1.1) 0.8, 0.373
Think might be deported if go to social/government agency 1.9 (0.7) 19.0 (2.1) 36.7, <0.001
Avoid health service due to INS 0.6 (0.3) 12.1 (1.4) 26.8, <0.001
Psychotic-like experiences
Any psychotic-like experience 5.3 (0.8) 7.7 (1.1) 3.4, 0.064
Visual hallucinations 3.7 (0.8) 5.4 (0.8) 1.8, 0.175
Hearing voices 2.5 (0.6) 4.0 (0.6) 3.4, 0.065
Delusions 0.9 (0.3) 1.7 (0.6) 1.7, 0.194

Note: Values represent % (SE) endorsing each item. The number (%) of respondents endorsing 0, 1, 2, and 3+ acculturative stress items was 322 (26.3), 326 (26.6), 198 (16.2), and 380 (31.0) among Asians, respectively, and 233 (19.3), 264 (21.9), 217 (18.0), and 494 (40.9) among Latinos.

3.2. Acculturative stress

The most commonly endorsed items for both Asians and Latinos pertained to limitations in contact with family and friends and difficulty with interaction due to language barriers, whereas few experienced stress related to fear of deportation or the involvement of government immigration agencies, particularly among Asian-Americans (Table 3). Acculturative stress total score was greater among Latinos than among Asians (2.6 (SD = 0.1) versus 1.8 (SD = 1.1), f(1,69) = 40.1, p < .001).

3.3. Logistic regression analyses

When examining the Asian group, acculturative stress was significantly associated with the presence of any PLE, as well as visual hallucinations and hearing voices specifically, in a significant dose–response fashion, such that risk for PLE increased linearly with an increasing number of endorsed acculturative stress items (Table 4). Associations with either class of hallucinations were not meaningfully changed when excluding individuals endorsing both subtypes. Acculturative stress was not associated with delusions. Among the Latino group, acculturative stress was associated with hearing voices, again in a significant linear dose–response fashion, but not with any other subtype of PLE (Table 5). However, there appeared to be a close relationship between age of immigration and PLE among Latinos, with childhood immigration (prior to age 12) significantly associated with the presence of any PLE and visual and auditory hallucinations, specifically.

Table 4.

Multiple logistic regression models of psychotic-like experiences among Asian respondents.

Total PLE Visual hallucinations Hearing voices Delusions




Wald χ2, p OR (95% CI) Wald χ2, p OR (95% CI) Wald χ2, p OR (95% CI) Wald χ2, p OR (95% CI)
Acculturative stress
  0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
  1 3.0, 0.084 2.8 (0.8–9.4) 1.6, 0.203 3.3 (0.5–21.5) 5.4, 0.021 13.6 (1.4–132.3) 0.1, 0.795 1.3 (0.2–8.8)
  2 8.7, 0.003 4.9 (1.7–14.6) 4.6, 0.032 4.3 (1.1–16.9) 11.9, 0.001 66.9 (5.7–785.8) 0.0, 0.969 1.0 (0.1–10.6)
  3+ 8.9, 0.003 7.2 (1.9–27.2) 7.7, 0.006 9.9 (1.9–53.2) 9.3, 0.002 38.4 (3.4–428.4) 1.3, 0.258 2.5 (0.5–12.7)
  Linear trend 11.3, 0.001 10.0, 0.002 11.3, 0.001 0.9, 0.349
Age
  18–29 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
  30–39 1.5, 0.218 0.6 (0.3–1.3) 1.6, 0.211 0.5 (0.2–1.5) 4.6, 0.033 0.4 (0.2–0.9) 0.6, 0.431 2.7 (0.2–33.9)
  40–49 2.7, 0.102 0.4 (0.1–1.2) 1.4, 0.236 0.4 (0.1–1.9) 11.7, 0.001 0.0 (0.0–0.3) 0.1, 0.819 1.4 (0.1–31.9)
  50–64 1.5, 0.214 0.5 (0.1–1.6) 0.7, 0.329 0.5 (0.1–2.8) 12.0, 0.001 0.0 (0.0–0.3) 0.3, 0.604 1.9 (0.2–23.2)
Female 5.7, 0.017 0.5 (0.3–0.9) 9.4, 0.002 0.3 (0.2–0.7) 1.6, 0.202 0.6 (0.2–1.4) 1.7, 0.195 2.2 (0.6–7.7)
Income
  <26,000 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
  26,000 – 64,999 0.1, 0.748 1.2 (0.4–3.3) 0.0, 0.914 0.9 (0.2–3.6) 1.5, 0.214 2.0 (0.6–6.2) 0.1, 0.768 1.3 (0.2–10.5)
  >65,000 0.9, 0.343 0.6 (0.2–1.9) 2.7, 0.104 0.3 (0.1–1.3) 0.0, 0.982 1.0 (0.3–3.8) 0.0, 0.999 1.0 (0.1–7.5)
Education
  <12 years 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
  12 years 2.0, 0.163 0.4 (0.1–1.5) 2.1, 0.143 0.2 (0.0–1.8) 0.3, 0.568 0.6 (0.1–3.3) 0.0, 0.856 0.8 (0.0–16.5)
  13–15 years 0.6, 0.434 1.5 (0.5–4.0) 0.0, 0.983 1.0 (0.3–2.9) 0.1, 0.702 1.3 (0.3–4.7) 0.6, 0.444 2.7 (0.2–38.6)
  >16 years 0.0, 0.943 1.0 (0.3–3.0) 0.1, 0.731 0.8 (0.2–3.0) 2.2, 0.135 0.4 (0.1–1.4) 0.5, 0.494 2.4 (0.2–32.2)
Age at immigration
  <12 years 0.1, 0.772 0.8 (0.2–3.0) 0.1, 0.806 0.8 (0.1–5.4) 1.0, 0.312 0.6 (0.2–1.8)
  13–17 years 4.3, 0.037 0.3 (0.1–1.0) 4.0, 0.047 0.3 (0.1–1.0) 2.1, 0.143 0.4 (0.1–1.4)
  18–64 years 1.0 1.0 1.0
Years in USA
  <10 years 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
  11–20 years 2.5, 0.117 2.5 (0.8–8.3) 2.7, 0.098 3.3 (0.8–13.8) 3.9, 0.047 6.0 (1.0–37.5) 0.8, 0.373 2.4 (0.3–17.6)
  >20 years 2.9, 0.089 2.9 (0.8–10.4) 4.4, 0.036 5.2 (1.1–25.1) 1.7, 0.193 4.3 (0.4–41.7) 1.1, 0.300 2.9 (0.4–24.3)

Note: Bold font indicates significance, p < 0.05.

Table 5.

Multiple logistic regression models of psychotic-like experiences among Latino respondents.

Total PLE Visual hallucinations Hearing voices Delusions




Wald χ2, p OR (95% CI) Wald χ2, p OR (95% CI) Wald χ2, p OR (95% CI) Wald χ2, p OR (95% CI)
Acculturative stress
  0 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
  1 0.0, 0.890 0.9 (0.4–2.1) 0.5, 0.476 0.7 (0.3–1.8) 3.5, 0.060 2.7 (0.9–9.5) 2.6, 0.106 0.2 (0.0–1.4)
  2 1.5, 0.215 1.4 (0.8–2.4) 0.3, 0.580 1.2 (0.7–2.1) 5.5, 0.019 3.7 (1.2–11.6) 1.0, 0.309 0.5 (0.2–1.8)
  3+ 0.0, 0.864 1.1 (0.5–2.5) 0.5, 0.468 0.7 (0.2–2.0) 17.6, <0.001 7.3 (2.8–19.0) 0.5, 0.466 0.7 (0.3–1.7)
  Linear trend 0.3, 0.588 0.2, 0.641 18.0, <0.001 0.0, 0.999
Age
  18–29 1.0 1.0 1.0
  30–39 0.0, 0.990 1.0 (0.4–2.5) 4.3, 0.038 2.3 (1.0–5.0) 0.3, 0.602 0.6 (0.1–3.9) 10.9, 0.001 0.0 (0.0–0.2)
  40–49 3.1, 0.077 1.0 2.6 (0.9–7.5) 9.8, 0.002 4.9 (1.8–13.5) 0.7, 0.387 2.0 (0.4–9.9) 0.4, 0.538 1.5 (0.4–5.5)
  50–64 3.3, 0.071 2.5 (0.9–7.1) 12.2, <0.001 6.2 (2.2–17.6) 0.2, 0.667 1.5 (0.2–11.3) 0.1, 0.741 0.7 (0.1–5.5)
Female 1.2, 0.266 1.3 (0.8–2.3) 1.2, 0.264 1.4 (0.8–2.4) 0.8, 0.363 1.6 (0.6–4.1) 0.5, 0.459 0.5 (0.1–2.8)
Income
  <26,000 1.1, 0.292 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
  26,000 – 64,999 0.8, 0.370 0.8 (0.5–1.3) 0.6, 0.450 0.8 (0.4–1.4) 0.0, 0.880 0.9 (0.4–2.4) 2.0, 0.156 0.3 (0.1–1.6)
  >65,000 0.7 (0.3–1.5) 0.0, 0.948 1.0 (0.4–2.4) 0.1, 0.721 1.3 (0.3–4.7) 0.1, 0.781 0.8 (0.1–4.2)
Education
  <12 years 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
  12 years 1.7, 0.195 1.5 (0.8–2.7) 0.0, 0.940 1.0 (0.4–2.5) 0.4, 0.515 1.4 (0.5–3.9) 3.2, 0.075 4.4 (0.8–23.0)
  13–15 years 7.1, 0.008 1.9 (1.2–3.2) 0.4, 0.537 1.2 (0.6–2.6) 2.8, 0.092 3.0 (0.8–11.2) 6.3, 0.012 3.5 (1.3–9.6)
  >16 years 2.4, 0.124 1.8 (0.8–4.1) 0.4, 0.511 0.7 (0.3–1.9) 1.2, 0.280 2.6 (0.4–15.4) 0.6, 0.445 2.5 (0.2–26.3)
Age at immigration
  <12 years 9.6, 0.002 4.0 (1.6–9.7) 32.4, <0.001 7.7 (3.8–16.0) 4.4, 0.035 3.1 (1.1–9.4) 0.0, 0.859 0.9 (0.3–3.1)
  13–17 years 1.7, 0.190 2.0 (0.7–6.1) 3.7, 0.055 2.4 (1.0–6.1) 0.5, 0.472 1.5 (0.5–4.8) 0.5, 0.496 0.5 (0.1–3.8)
  18–64 years 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
Years in USA
  <10 years 1.0 1.0 1.0 1.0
  11–20 years 0.0, 0.865 1.0 (0.6–1.8) 0.0, 0.995 1.0 (0.5–2.1) 0.3, 0.599 1.4 (0.4–4.3) 3.7, 0.053 3.7 (0.9–14.1)
  >20 years 4.8, 0.029 0.4 (0.2–0.9) 12.1, 0.001 0.2 (0.1–0.5) 2.2, 0.137 0.5 (0.2–1.3) 0.0, 0.996 1.0 (0.2–4.5)

Note: Bold font indicates significance, p < 0.05.

Logistic regression models were also tested with the exclusion of age, age at immigration, and years in U.S. variables, to ensure that results were not influenced by potential collinearity among these predictors. No results were meaningfully changed.

4. Discussion

4.1. Main findings

To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine risk for psychosis among Asian and Latino immigrants to the U.S., and the first study to report an association between acculturative stress and PLE among a national community sample of first-generation immigrants. Asian respondents who endorsed increasing numbers of acculturative stress items were at progressively greater risk for PLE (particularly visual and auditory hallucinations, but not delusions) in a dose–response fashion. In contrast, acculturative stress was only related to risk for hearing voices among Latinos, even though Latinos experienced significantly greater acculturative stress and prevalence of PLE. Varying findings for visual hallucinations may reflect cross-cultural differences in the relationship between acculturative stress and PLE, or may indicate phenomenological differences in experiences being endorsed in response to the visual hallucinations item across the two groups. The only prior study to examine acculturation and psychosis among immigrants found an effect on global function but not symptom severity (Gonidakis et al., 2011); however, their findings were based on a sample of people with psychotic disorders (primarily schizophrenia) and focused on symptom severity rather than symptom occurrence. Further, they may have captured the effects of acculturation as a broad construct, thereby potentially confounding stress with other aspects of acculturation (Rudmin, 2009).

4.2. Acculturative stress and specific psychotic-like symptoms

The association between acculturative stress and hallucinations but not delusions was unexpected based on the prior literature, which has primarily focused on risk for psychotic disorders rather than specific symptoms (Cantor-Graae and Selten, 2005; Morgan et al., 2010; Bourque et al., 2011). Vanheusden et al. (2008) previously showed a positive relationship between immigration and self-reported hallucinations among non-western immigrants to The Netherlands, although they did not distinguish between visual and auditory hallucinations. Berg et al. (2011) found increased positive symptom severity among immigrants (primarily African or Asian) in Norway, but did not distinguish between subtypes of positive symptoms. Other studies have found greater severity of delusions and hallucinations at initial intake among Moroccan immigrants in the Netherlands (Veling et al., 2007) or depressive symptoms among Turkish immigrants in Germany (Haasen et al., 2001) but not hallucinations exclusively. Findings across studies may vary for reasons related to measurement, sample ascertainment, and differences between particular immigrant groups and host nations. There also may have been insufficient power to detect an association with delusions in our study, given their low prevalence relative to hallucinations in this sample.

4.3. Acculturative stress as a mechanism of risk

Several studies have prospectively demonstrated associations between daily stress and increased psychotic-like symptom severity (DeVylder et al., 2013; Tessner et al., 2011) among help-seeking youth at clinical risk for psychosis. Acculturative stress may likewise consist primarily of the experience of persistent minor hassles. Increased sensitivity to ongoing stressors associated with adjustment and integration to a new culture, and the inability to effectively cope with these ongoing stressors, may contribute to the association between psychosis and immigration. However, it is likely that acculturative stress is just one of multiple mechanisms mediating the relationship between immigration and psychosis, which may also include perceived discrimination (Karlsen and Nazroo, 2002; Veling et al., 2007) and ethnic density (Boydell et al., 2001; Das-Munshi et al., 2012; Kirkbride et al., 2007; Schofield et al., 2011; Veling et al., 2008b), which were not examined in our models. Notably, acculturative stress is a mechanism that is potentially modifiable through individual- and family-level intervention, unlike ethnic density or societal discrimination. For example, interventions have been proposed to combat the stress of acculturation through individual and family support groups and bicultural skill trainings (Bacallao and Smokowski, 2005; Smokowski and Bacallao, 2009). Interventions for immigrants experiencing acculturative stress may have promise in reducing the risk for psychosis, particularly among Asian-Americans.

4.4. Age of immigration

Among Latino respondents, younger age of immigration was associated with increased risk for any PLE and specifically for visual and auditory hallucinations. Younger age at the time of immigration has previously been associated with increased risk of psychosis among non-western immigrants to The Netherlands (Veling et al., 2011). Interestingly, the same study found no effect of age of immigration among Western immigrants, consistent with a prior study that found no association between immigration from predominantly western nations to Denmark and psychosis (Cantor-Graae et al., 2003). One possible explanation is that western immigrants to The Netherlands and Asian immigrants to the U.S. more closely resemble the dominant ethnic group in terms of SES, allowing for easier assimilation into the host culture, leaving those that experience a more stressful acculturation process at an increased risk for psychosis. Conversely, risk for PLE in Latinos in the US (and likewise in non-western immigrants to The Netherlands) may be related to environmental factors, particularly those that occur or begin during childhood (e.g. perceived discrimination, childhood separation from parents), with risk increasing as duration of exposure increases.

4.5. Limitations and future directions

This study has several potential limitations. First, we could not control directly for overall acculturation, although acculturation is likely to overlap with years in the host country, which was included in our models. Second, we were not able to include a non-immigrant control group since the acculturative stress scale was only assessed among first-generation immigrants. Also, individual acculturative stress items that probed guilt and feelings of persecution may have some conceptual overlap with psychotic-like experiences, but these were the least commonly endorsed items in our sample and thus less likely to have substantially influenced our main findings. The association between acculturative stress and hearing voices in Latinos may be confounded by legal status, which was not assessed in this survey. We also may have obscured important differences between individual nationalities given that ethnicities were collapsed into two broad categories to increase statistical power. Statistical power may have also limited our ability to detect associations with delusions, which were of much lower prevalence compared to hallucinations. The modest internal consistency of our acculturative stress measure also may have introduced random error and therefore biased our findings towards the null, possibly preventing detection of potential associations between acculturative stress and delusions.

There were insufficient data on age of immigration or age of onset for particular PLE to determine their temporal order in our cross-sectional data, allowing for the possibility of reverse causality. Acculturation may be more stressful for those with psychiatric symptoms, as previously suggested regarding depression and anxiety (Rudmin, 2009). This interpretation is less consistent with the substantial body of literature including several prospective studies linking immigration to psychosis (Bourque et al., 2011), although none of these studies included measures of acculturative stress, necessitating replication of our results in a prospective design to clarify the causal direction of this relationship.

The NLAAS over-sampled areas census tracts with high ethnic density of Latinos or Asians, which has been associated with reduced risk for psychosis (Veling et al., 2008b). It would be of interest in future studies to examine whether acculturative stress mediates the association between ethnic density and psychosis by sampling from a broader range of communities.

This is the first study to our knowledge to examine acculturative stress as a risk factor for psychosis, and to examine PLE among Asian and Latino immigrants in the US. Our findings suggest that acculturative stress is associated specifically with hallucinatory symptoms among immigrants to the United States, particularly those from Asian countries. Phenomenological analyses of PLE across cultures may help us to better understand how acculturative stress may relate to PLE, and the nature of PLE when occurring in this context. Future studies should continue to explore the heightened prevalence of psychosis commonly found among immigrant groups, not only in terms of varying mechanisms of risk between different nationalities, but also variations in how these risk factors influence specific psychotic symptoms.

Acknowledgment

The authors would like to thank those involved with the Collaborative Psychiatric Epidemiology Surveys for making their data publicly available to the research community.

Role of funding source

This project was not supported through external funding.

Footnotes

Contributors

Jordan DeVylder developed the hypotheses and statistical analyses to evaluate the association between acculturative stress and psychotic-like experiences, conducted some of the literature review, and wrote a series of drafts, which were reviewed and edited by the other authors. Hans Oh conducted some of the literature review and did primary editing of the initial drafts. Lawrence Yang contributed to the development of the hypotheses and application of appropriate statistical methods for epidemiological analysis, and to the revision of drafts. Leopoldo Cabassa contributed to developing the analysis, and to interpretation of data and revision of drafts in terms of expertise on acculturative stress. Fang-pei Chen and Ellen Lukens contributed to the interpretation of data and revision of drafts in terms of expertise on psychosis.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no affiliations with industrial or commercial entities and have not received financial support from commercial entities for their work.

References

  1. Alegria M, Takeuchi D, Canino G, Duan N, Shrout P, Meng XL, Vega W, Zane N, Vila D, Woo M. Considering context, place and culture: the National Latino and Asian American Study. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 2006;13(4):208–220. doi: 10.1002/mpr.178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bacallao ML, Smokowski PR. “Entre dos mundos” (between two worlds): bicultural skills training with Latino immigrant families. J. Prim. Prev. 2005;26(6):485–509. doi: 10.1007/s10935-005-0008-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Berg AO, Melle I, Rossberg J, Romm K, Larsson S, Lagerberg TV, Andreassen OA, Hauff E. Perceived discrimination is associated with severity of positive and depression/anxiety symptoms in immigrants with psychosis: a cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry. 2011;11(1):77. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-11-77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Berry JW. Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Int. Rev. Appl. Psychol. 1997;46(1):5–68. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bourque F, Van der Ven E, Malla A. A meta-analysis of the risk for psychotic disorders among first- and second-generation immigrants. Psychol. Med. 2011;41(5):897–910. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Boydell J, Van Os J, McKenzie K, Allardyce J, Goel R, McCreadie RG, Murray RM. Incidence of schizophrenia in ethnic minorities in London: ecological study into interactions with environment. BMJ. 2001;323(7325):1336–1338. doi: 10.1136/bmj.323.7325.1336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cantor-Graae E, Selten J-P. Schizophrenia and migration: a meta-analysis and review. Am.J. Psychiatry. 2005;162(1):12–24. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.1.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cantor-Graae E, Pedersen CB, McNEIL TF, Mortensen PB. Migration as a risk factor for schizophrenia: a Danish population-based cohort study. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2003;182:117–122. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.2.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Das-Munshi J, Bécares L, Boydell JE, Dewey ME, Morgan C, Stansfeld SA, Prince MJ. Ethnic density as a buffer for psychotic experiences: findings from a national survey (EMPIRIC) Br. J. Psychiatry. 2012;201(4):282–290. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.102376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. DeVylder JE, Ben-David S, Schobel SA, Kimhy D, Malaspina D, Corcoran CM. Temporal association of stress sensitivity and symptoms in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. Psychol. Med. 2013;43(2):259–268. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gonidakis F, Lembesi E, Kontaxakis VP, Havaki-Kontaxaki BJ, Ploumpidis D, Madianos M, Papadimitriou GN. A study of acculturation in psychotic and non-psychotic immigrants living in Athens. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry. 2011;59(2):157–164. doi: 10.1177/0020764011430031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Guarnaccia PJ, Pincay IM, Alegría M, Shrout PE, Lewis-Fernández R, Canino GJ. Assessing diversity among Latinos results from the NLAAS. Hisp. J. Behav. Sci. 2007;29(4):510–534. doi: 10.1177/0739986307308110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Haasen C, Yagdiran O, Mass R, Krausz M. Schizophrenic disorders among Turkish migrants in Germany. Psychopathology. 2001;34(4):203–208. doi: 10.1159/000049308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Hair JF, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE. Multivariate Data Analysis. 7th ed. Engelwood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  15. Heeringa SG, Wagner J, Torres M, Duan N, Adams T, Berglund P. Sample designs and sampling methods for the Collaborative Psychiatric Epidemiology Studies (CPES) Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 2004;13(4):221–240. doi: 10.1002/mpr.179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hjern A, Wicks S, Dalman C. Social adversity contributes to high morbidity in psychoses in immigrants—a national cohort study in two generations of Swedish residents. Psychol. Med. 2004;34(6):1025–1033. doi: 10.1017/s003329170300148x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. IBM Corp. SPSS Statistics for Macintosh, Version 20.0. Armonk NY: IBM Corp; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  18. Janssen I, Hanssen M, Bak M, Bijl RV, De Graaf R, Vollebergh W, McKenzie K, Van Os J. Discrimination and delusional ideation. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2003;182:71–76. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.1.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Karlsen S, Nazroo JY. Relation between racial discrimination, social class, and health among ethnic minority groups. Am. J. Public Health. 2002;92(4):624–631. doi: 10.2105/ajph.92.4.624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kessler RC, Üstün TB. The World Mental Health (WMH) survey initiative version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 2004;13(4):93–121. doi: 10.1002/mpr.168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kessler RC, Birnbaum H, Demler O, Falloon IRH, Gagnon E, Guyer M, Howes MJ, Kendler KS, Shi L, Walker EF, Wu EQ. The prevalence and correlates of non-affective psychosis in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) Biol. Psychiatry. 2005;58(8):668–676. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kirkbride JB, Morgan C, Fearon P, Dazzan P, Murray RM, Jones PB. Neighbourhood-level effects on psychoses: re-examining the role of context. Psychol. Med. 2007;37(10):1413–1425. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707000499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Lewis-Fernández R, Horvitz-Lennon M, Blanco C, Guarnaccia PJ, Cao Z, Alegría M. Significance of endorsement of psychotic symptoms by US Latinos. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2009;197(5):337. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181a2087e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Linscott RJ, Van Os J. Systematic reviews of categorical versus continuum models in psychosis: evidence for discontinuous subpopulations underlying a psychometric continuum. Implications for DSM-V, DSM-VI, and DSM-VII. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2010;6:391–419. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.032408.153506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Lueck K, Wilson M. Acculturative stress in Asian immigrants: the impact of social and linguistic factors. Int. J. Intercult.Relat. 2010;34(1):47–57. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lueck K, Wilson M. Acculturative stress in Latino immigrants: the impact of social, socio-psychological and migration-related factors. Int. J Intercult.Relat. 2011;35(2):186–195. [Google Scholar]
  27. Mallett R, Leff J, Bhugra D, Pang D, Zhao JH. Social environment, ethnicity and schizophrenia—a case–control study. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2002;37(7):329–335. doi: 10.1007/s00127-002-0557-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Morgan C, Kirkbride J, Leff J, Craig T, Hutchinson G, McKENZIE K, Morgan K, Dazzan P, Doody GA, Jones P. Parental separation, loss and psychosis in different ethnic groups: a case–control study. Psychol. Med. 2007;37(4):495–504. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706009330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Morgan C, Fisher H, Hutchinson G, Kirkbride J, Craig TK, Morgan K, Dazzan P, Boydell J, Doody GA, Jones PB. Ethnicity, social disadvantage and psychotic-like experiences in a healthy population based sample. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2008a;119(3):226–235. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01301.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Morgan C, Kirkbride J, Hutchinson G, Craig T, Morgan K, Dazzan P, Boydell J, Doody GA, Jones PB, Murray RM. Cumulative social disadvantage, ethnicity and first-episode psychosis: a case–control study. Psychol. Med. 2008b;38(2):1701–1715. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Morgan C, Charalambides M, Hutchinson G, Murray RM. Migration, ethnicity, and psychosis: toward a sociodevelopmental model. Schizophr. Bull. 2010;36(4):655–664. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Nuevo R, Chatterji S, Verdes E, Naidoo N, Arango C, Ayuso-Mateos JL. The continuum of psychotic symptoms in the general population: a cross-national study. Schizophr. Bull. 2012;38(3):475–485. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Rudmin F. Constructs, measurements and models of acculturation and acculturative stress. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 2009;33(2):106–123. [Google Scholar]
  34. Schofield P, Ashworth M, Jones R. Ethnic isolation and psychosis: re-examining the ethnic density effect. Psychol.Med. 2011;41(6):1263–1269. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Selten J-P, Cantor-Graae E, Kahn RS. Migration and schizophrenia. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry. 2007;20(2):111–115. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328017f68e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Smokowski PR, Bacallao M. Entre dos mundos/between two worlds: youth violence prevention for acculturating Latino families. Res. Soc. Work. Pract. 2009;19(2):165–178. [Google Scholar]
  37. Tessner KD, Mittal V, Walker EF. Longitudinal study of stressful life events and daily stressors among adolescents at high risk for psychotic disorders. Schizophr. Bull. 2011;37(2):432–441. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Gemerys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psycohsis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol. Med. 2009;39(2):179–195. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Vanheusden K, Mulder CL, Van Der Ende J, Selten JP, Van Lenthe FJ, Verhulst FC, Mackenbach JP. Associations between ethnicity and self-reported hallucinations in a population sample of young adults in The Netherlands. Psychol. Med. 2008;38(8):1095–1102. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707002401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Veling W, Selten JP, Mackenbach JP, Hoek HW. Symptoms at first contact for psychotic disorder: comparison between native Dutch and ethnic minorities. Schizophr. Res. 2007;95(1–3):30–38. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.06.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Veling W, Hoek HW, Mackenbach JP. Perceived discrimination and the risk of schizophrenia in ethnic minorities. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2008a;43(12):953–959. doi: 10.1007/s00127-008-0381-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Veling W, Susser E, Van Os J, Mackenbach J, Selten J-P, Hoek H. Ethnic density of neighborhoods and incidence of psychotic disorders among immigrants. Am.J Psychiatry. 2008b;165(1):66–73. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Veling W, Hoek HW, Selten J-P, Susser E. Age at migration and future risk of psychotic disorders among immigrants in the Netherlands: a 7-year incidence study. Am J. Psychiatry. 2011;168(12):1278–1285. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11010110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Werbeloff N, Drukker M, Dohrenwend BP, Levav I, Yoffe R, Van Os J, Davidson M, Weiser M. Self-reported attenuated psychotic symptoms as forerunners of severe mental disorders later in life. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2012;69(5):467–475. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES