Abstract
The peer-related social competence of children with Down syndrome was examined in an observational study. Dyadic interactions with peers of children with Down syndrome were compared to the dyadic interactions of matched groups of typically developing children and with playmates differing in both familiarity and social skills. Results suggested that both risk and protective factors influenced the peer interactions of children with Down syndrome. Recommendations were made for applying contemporary models of peer-related social competence to etiologic subgroups to better understand the mechanisms involved and to provide direction for the design of intervention programs.
Down syndrome is the most common genetic disorder associated with intellectual disability. Despite the availability of sophisticated prenatal screening and diagnosis (Malone, et al., 2005), the incidence of Down syndrome remains at approximately 1–800 live births (Patterson & Costa, 2005; Sherman, Allen, Bean, & Freeman, 2007). Knowledge of the genetic and neurobiological mechanisms underlying this disorder has expanded dramatically in recent years (Patterson, 2007), and research continues to refine what is now a well-established behavioral phenotype (Chapman & Hesketh, 2000). Extensive behavioral work in particular has identified important cognitive (Silverman, 2007) and language (Abbeduto, Warren, & Connors, 2007; Roberts, Price, & Malkin, 2007) profiles, and there is increasing interest with respect to the social development of children with Down syndrome.
Available evidence suggests that, in comparison to many aspects of cognition and language, the social development of children with Down syndrome appears to be a relative strength. In general, these children exhibit a strong orientation to social aspects of their environment and appear motivated to engage in social interactions (see Fidler & Nadel, 2007 and Kasari & Hodapp, 1996, for reviews). Certain developmental characteristics of children with Down syndrome such as well-developed representational skills are also compatible with involvement in social forms of play (Sigman & Ruskin, 1999). The perceptions of parents and teachers of children with Down syndrome are consistent with this sociable profile and with social development as a relative strength (Fidler, Hepburn, & Rogers, 2006; Gilmore, Campbell, & Cuskally, 2003; Hornby, 1995; Loveland & Kelley, 1991). Moreover, parents of children with Down syndrome actively support their child’s participation in settings containing typically developing peers during the early childhood and school years (Freeman, Alkin, & Kasari, 1999; Guralnick, 2002). Because Down syndrome is so well known and increasingly accepted (Lenhard, Breitenbach, Ebert, Schindelhauer-Deutscher, Zang, & Henn, 2007), lower levels of perceived stigma may further encourage parents to actively foster their child’s participation in community-based normative social activities (see Seltzer & Ryff, 1994). Taken together, these child characteristics along with parental and societal factors may well combine to support the development of social relationships and to foster all aspects of the social competence of children with Down syndrome. This includes children’s peer-related social competence, the focus of this study.
However, recent research relevant to the social competence of children with Down syndrome suggests vulnerabilities in precisely those processes closely associated with children’s peer-related social competence (see Crick & Dodge, 1994; Guralnick, 1999; Yeates, et al., 2007). Specifically, in studies employing various comparison groups, including typically developing children matched on chronological age, mental age, or language level, children with Down syndrome display substantial difficulties with respect to various aspects of social understanding and associated socio-cognitive skills (Cebula & Wishart, 2008; Wishart, 2007; Wishart, Cebula, Willis, & Pitcairn, 2007). Related research also has revealed difficulties with respect to emotion regulation abilities when faced with a frustrating situation (Jahromi, Gulsrud, & Kasari, 2008), and problems persisting when challenged with a difficult task (Wishart, 1996). In fact, aspects of their sociability such as high levels of approaching others may well be due to poor response inhibition (Porter, Coltheart, & Langdon, 2007). Other developmental characteristics common to children with Down syndrome, such as unusual difficulties in expressive language (Chapman, 2003), may further place these children at risk for significant peer competence problems.
The risk and protective factors outlined above likely interact over time to establish children’s levels of competence with peers. A crucial time period during which peer relationships are expected to flourish is when children participate in preschool or kindergarten programs (Howes, 1990; Rubin, Coplan, Chen, Buskirk, & Wojslawowicz, 2005; Yugar & Shapiro, 2001). Yet, we have virtually no information as to the levels of peer competence achieved by children with Down syndrome or their specific characteristics. The rare observational studies of the social interactions of children with Down syndrome with their peers during early childhood that have been carried out have only included comparison groups comprised of children with other disabilities (Sigman & Ruskin, 1999). However, as discussed below, important insights into the peer competence of children with Down syndrome or other delays can be achieved when observations are placed within a developmental framework that includes comparison groups of typically developing children.
In this study, participants were recruited from three groups: (1) children with Down syndrome, (2) typically developing children matched on mental age (MA-match) and other characteristics to the children with Down syndrome, and (3) typically developing children matched on chronological age (CA-match) and other characteristics to the children with Down syndrome. In the main portion of the study, focal children from each of the three groups were paired with typically developing playmates matched on chronological age and gender. These dyads were then observed in a series of laboratory observations designed to evaluate children’s entry into play and their ability to maintain that play -- key elements of socially competent functioning with peers (Guralnick, 1999; Putallaz & Wasserman, 1990). For each focal child, information was obtained for a range of peer interaction measures. Among the specific measures evaluated as highly relevant to children with Down syndrome were children’s interest level in their playmates (reflecting social orientation), cognitive level of play (reflecting representational play), peer conversation (representing expressive language), and the ability to sustain interactive play (representing social-cognitive information processing and task persistence). These comparisons among the three groups of focal children are represented in the first row of Table 1 (cells A, B, and C; two 50 minute observational sessions each).
Table 1.
Focal Group | |||
---|---|---|---|
| |||
Playmate Pairings | Down Syndrome | MA-Match | CA-Match |
CA-playmate (unfamiliar) | ●● (A) |
●● (B) |
●● (C) |
Friend | ● (D) |
● (E) |
● (F) |
MA (unfamiliar) | ●● (G) |
— | — |
Note. All CA- and MA- playmates are typically developing children. Each bullet in table represents one 50 minute observational session. Children in the MA-playmate comparison group were paired only with the focal children with Down syndrome (2 sessions).
Competing hypotheses can be generated regarding the patterns of peer competence likely to be exhibited by children with Down syndrome in comparison to the two matched groups of typically developing children. Consistent with cognitive level constraints that exist for many aspects of children’s peer interactions (Brownell, 1986; Howes, 1987), one possibility is that the peer interactions of children with Down syndrome will be highly similar to the MA-match group. It is also the case that protective factors noted earlier may combine with the approximately two years of additional experience with peers for children with Down syndrome in comparison to the younger typically developing group (MA-match) to bring some aspects of their interactions with peers beyond that level in the direction of their chronological age mates.
Alternatively, the many risk factors, especially involving social cognition and emotion regulation concerns, may exert their influence resulting in unusually low levels of peer competence. Indeed, children with Down syndrome may share many of the peer-related social competence problems exhibited by heterogeneous groups of children with developmental delays (Diamond, 2002; Guralnick & Groom, 1987b; Guralnick, Hammond, Connor, & Neville, 2006; Guralnick, Neville, Hammond, & Connor, 2007; Iarocci, Yager, Rombough, & McLaughlin, 2008). If this is the case, despite existing protective factors, the peer competence of children with Down syndrome might well be less well developed than even those of children in the MA-match group.
This possibile existence of unusual peer competence problems also was examined from a different perspective. In this case, comparisons of dyadic play were made between two groups in which both members of the dyad were identical in mental age but with one group containing a child with Down syndrome. To accomplish this, an additional group of typically developing playmates was recruited for the children with Down syndrome matched now in terms of mental age (rather than chronological age). This pairing with focal group children with Down syndrome can be found in cell G of Table 1 (two 50 minute observational sessions). The peer interactions of the children with Down syndrome in this pairing were then compared to those of the focal children in the MA-match group (cell B of Table 1). If risk factors associated with children with Down syndrome exert a strong influence on these two mental age equivalent groups, adverse effects on the peer interactions of children with Down syndrome, particularly sustained forms of interactive play, should be evident.
Of note, evidence based on indirect measures of children’s peer competence in longitudinal and group comparison studies addressing the peer social network and friendship patterns of children with Down syndrome in home and community settings does, in fact, suggest the possible influence of risk factors. In general, studies indicate that young children with Down syndrome have comparatively small peer social networks as well as less well developed linkages with peers across settings, exhibit a general absence of reciprocal friendships, and report high levels of loneliness (Byrne, Cunningham, & Sloper, 1988; Carr, 1995; Freeman & Kasari, 2002; Guralnick, 2002; Guralnick, Connor, & Johnson, 2009; Howell, Hauser-Cram, & Kersh, 2007; Sloper, Turner, Knussen, & Cunningham, 1990).
A hallmark of socially competent functioning with peers is the ability to adjust to and integrate with a playmate’s pattern of social interaction, thereby engaging in more sustained and productive forms of peer play over time (i.e., group play). This pattern is evident in circumstances in which playmates gradually become more familiar with one another (Doyle, Connolly, & Rivest, 1980; Shea, 1981). In this study, this important aspect of peer competence was examined in two ways. First, short-term changes in the social interaction patterns of the focal children in each of the three main groups (DS, MA-match, CA-match) were observed over time as they interacted with initially unfamiliar chronological age matched playmates. Comparisons were made between the first and second observational sessions (see first row in Table 1). Second, and at the other end of the familiarity continuum, each focal child was also observed interacting with mother-identified friends (see second row of Table 1, only one 50 minute observational session). Ideally, friendships are characterized by patterns of reciprocal and mutual social exchanges, with play among friends generating more sophisticated and extended social interactions than play among non-friends (Hartup, Laursen, Stewart, & Eastonson, 1988; Hartup & Stevens, 1997; Hinde, Titmus, Easton, & Tamplin, 1985; Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995, 1996; Rubin, et al., 2005; Vaughn, Colvin, Azira, Caya, & Krzysik, 2001). However, research with heterogeneous groups of children with developmental delays has indicated no advantage in play with friends for these children, clearly suggesting the influence of unusual peer competence problems (Guralnick, Gottman, & Hammond, 1996; Guralnick & Groom, 1988).
In contrast, comparisons involving children with Down syndrome have indicated that higher levels of play occur between friends than non-friends (Freeman & Kasari, 2002). Protective factors, including child characteristics with respect to their social orientation as well as positive parental and societal influences, may serve children with Down syndrome well when engaging friends in play. If this hypothesis is correct, this would be a substantial benefit to children with Down syndrome and minimize the impact of risk factors. Accordingly, as a further manifestation of children’s peer competence, both shorter-term adjustments in play with initially unfamiliar peers (across two observational sessions) and longer-term adjustments represented by interactions with identified friends in comparison to unfamiliar playmates were examined for all three focal groups of children.
Finally, it is not only the familiarity of playmates that can affect the quality of the dyadic play of the focal child but also their playmates’ skill level (Guralnick & Groom, 1987a). For heterogeneous groups of children with delays, a complex pattern exists with respect to the quality of peer interactions as influenced by combinations of the focal child’s and the playmate’s developmental levels (Skinner, Buysse, & Bailey, 2004). Nevertheless, children with Down syndrome may benefit substantially from the scaffolding of play provided by more skillful playmates. As noted, in the main portion of the study children with Down syndrome were paired with similar chronological age playmates who were typically developing children. One consequence of this might be better quality play than under conditions that did not have the advantage of having such a skillful playmate. To evaluate this possibility, we observed the peer interactions of children with Down syndrome when interacting with typically developing chronological age matched playmates and when interacting with younger typically developing playmates (comparison of cells A and G in Table 1). This younger group of typically developing playmates, although similar in mental age to the children with Down syndrome, may well find it more difficult to engage these children in dyadic play given their less sophisticated social play skills. As such, to the extent that children with Down syndrome depend upon the more skillful organization and orchestration of play by playmates of the same chronological age, dyadic play with younger typically developing playmates should yield lower levels of peer interactions. Alternatively, even with less skilled playmates, a receptive and interested partner, especially one with good representational play skills such as a child with Down syndrome, may generate levels of dyadic play similar to those in which the playmate is older and more skillful.
In summary, detailed observations of the peer interactions of young children with Down syndrome were carried out within a developmental framework. Comparisons with mental age and chronological age matched typically developing children were examined for a range of peer interaction measures to determine how risk and protective factors affecting the peer competence of children with Down syndrome manifest themselves in various interpersonal contexts. The role of playmate characteristics with respect to familiarity and level of social skills was also examined. These analyses are intended to identify the patterns of strength and vulnerability associated with the peer interactions of children with Down syndrome and to thereby contribute to our understanding of this important aspect of their social development.
Method
Participants
Young children with Down syndrome (DS) were recruited through contact with local Down syndrome parent groups, public schools, state agencies, and early intervention centers in a large metropolitan community. To be included in the sample a child with DS had to meet the following criteria: (1) be between the ages of 54 and 83 months, (2) have a karyotype which confirmed that the child’s diagnosis was due to trisomy 21, (3) obtain a Full Scale IQ (FSIQ) score which equated to a mental age of at least 2.5 years on the Stanford-Binet, Fifth Edition (SB5; Roid, 2003), and (4) had a primary female caregiver (minimum of a 6-month relationship, as mothers were our primary informants). Exclusion from the sample occurred if the child had a diagnosis of Pervasive Developmental Disorder, if English was not their primary language, or if they had unusual sensory or motor problems. Of the 30 children with DS who met all four criteria, 27 completed the entire study.
Two comparison groups of typically-developing children (N = 27 in each group) were recruited by sending similar study announcements to parents of children enrolled in child care centers and public schools. Parents who were interested in participating were asked to contact study staff directly and underwent qualifying testing to create two groups of typically developing children who were matched on a case-by-case basis to the children in the DS group. One group was matched on the basis of chronological age (CA-match) to the children with DS and the other group was matched on the basis of mental age (MA-match). To be included as a CA-match for each child with DS, the typically developing child had to meet the following criteria: (1) the child’s chronological age had to be similar (+/− 3 months at the time of testing) to that of the child with DS to be matched; (2) be the same gender as the child with DS; (3) obtain a FSIQ score between 90 and 130 on the SB5, and (4) have a primary female caregiver (minimum of a 6-month relationship). These inclusion requirements were similar for the MA-match group for each child with DS (e.g., same gender), except that the typically developing child’s mental age (based on the SB5) had to be within +/− 3 months of the child with DS at the time of testing. Although family demographics were not used as matching variables, these variables were monitored for equivalence and adjustments made if necessary in the participant selection process. Typically-developing children were excluded if they had any known developmental difficulties, a behavior problem defined as obtaining a total behavior problem score in the borderline clinical range or higher (T ≥65 on preschool version and T ≥60 on the school-age version) on the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000; 2001), a major uncorrected sensory or motor problem, or a primary language other than English. These three groups of children constituted the focal groups of Table 1.
For all 81 participants, mothers were asked about their child’s ethnicity, grade in school (preschool/child care, kindergarten, first grade), and siblings. In addition to child demographic information, standard demographic information about the family (marital status, ethnicity, educational and occupational status, and income) was gathered via self-reports from mothers. The Hollingshead Four Factor Index of Social Status (Hollingshead, 1975) was used to calculate a measure of family social status (range 8–66; see Table 2 for descriptive characteristics).
Table 2.
Measures | Focal Group
|
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Down Syndrome | MA-Match | CA-Match | ||||
| ||||||
M or % | SD | M or % | SD | M or % | SD | |
Child Demographics | ||||||
Age (years) | 5.62 | .60 | 3.21 | .35 | 5.61 | .60 |
Gender (% male) | 48.15 | 48.15 | 48.15 | |||
Ethnicity (% Caucasian)a | 81.48 | 81.48 | 85.19 | |||
Family Demographics | ||||||
Family Social Statusb | 49.00 | 11.00 | 56.58 | 7.74 | 55.89 | 6.74 |
Mother’s Age (years) | 39.12 | 5.88 | 36.98 | 3.88 | 39.17 | 5.32 |
Marital Status (% partnered) | 96.30 | 100.00 | 96.30 | |||
Grade in School | ||||||
Preschool/Child Care (%) | 51.85 | 100.00 | 44.44 | |||
Kindergarten (%) | 48.15 | .00 | 44.44 | |||
First Grade (%) | .00 | .00 | 11.11 | |||
Child Developmental Characteristics | ||||||
Full Scale IQc | 60.59x | 10.10 | 108.52y | 8.08 | 111.30y | 7.57 |
Mental Agec | 3.37x | .50 | 3.48x | 0.40 | 6.23y | .62 |
Total Languaged | 55.67x | 8.18 | 122.93y | 10.79 | 120.04y | 9.74 |
Adaptive Behavior Compositee | 64.19x | 10.82 | 116.00y | 11.78 | 109.78z | 9.74 |
Total Behavior Problemsf | 57.07x | 6.85 | 47.89y | 10.32 | 48.35y | 9.91 |
Hispanic, 2.5%; Asian 8.6%; and Native American 1.2%.
Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of Social Status;
Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale – Fifth Edition;
Preschool Language Scale – Fourth Edition, standard scores;
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, standard scores;
Child Behavior Checklist, T-scores.
Note. For each measure, groups with the same superscript did not differ significantly from one another whereas significant differences existed when superscripts differed.
A second group of typically developing children was recruited by study announcements sent home via day care centers and the public schools to serve as unfamiliar playmates in observations of the focal children’s play (DS, MA-match, CA-match). This group consisted of typically developing playmates selected to be of similar chronological ages (+/−3 months at time of observation; CA-playmates) and gender to the focal children from each of the three main groups (see row 1 in Table 1). In addition, a group of unfamiliar typically developing playmates matched in mental age to the children in the Down syndrome group was recruited (MA-playmates; cell G in Table 1). This was the additional comparison established for the existing group of children with Down syndrome. To be included, unfamiliar playmates for all pairings with focal children in the four groups of dyads (cells A, B, C, and G) had to obtain a standard score between 90 and 130 on the Abbreviated Battery IQ (ABIQ) of the SB5 and not have had any previous contact with the focal child with whom they would be paired. The ABIQ was used as an estimate of general intellectual ability due to its direct connection with the SB5 (see below), with corrected correlations of .81 with the SB5 FSIQ. Typically-developing playmates for the four dyadic groups were excluded if they had any known developmental difficulties, a behavior problem score in the borderline clinical range or above (T ≥ 65 on preschool version and T ≥ 60 on school-age version) on the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000; 2001), a major uncorrected sensory or motor problem, or a primary language other than English.
Finally, friends of the focal children were identified (see row 2 of Table 1). Mothers were asked to choose as a friend “a child with whom the focal child got along with the best or played with most frequently (minimum of once per month on average).” Friends could be either male or female but must have been at least 2.5-years old and not more than 10-years-old at the time of observation. Relatives not living in the home were allowed and the developmental status of the friend was not a concern. Information was collected from mothers with respect to each friend’s CA, gender, ethnicity, and whether or not the child had a disability or developmental delay. Information was also obtained to calculate a measure of family socioeconomic status (SES). All except four mothers of children with DS were able to identify one friend, and identified children participated in the laboratory session.
Child Developmental Measures
Children in all three focal groups were evaluated by a psychologist with extensive prior experience working with young children with developmental delays and typically developing children. The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales – Fifth Edition (SB5; Roid, 2003) was administered to assess children’s intellectual development. The Full Scale IQ (FSIQ) was of primary interest as was the mental age score to establish the match between younger typically developing focal children and the children with DS. To evaluate children’s language development, the Preschool Language Scale – Fourth Edition (PLS-4; Zimmerman, Steiner, & Pond, 2002) was administered. Scores on the Total Language scale were converted to standard scores for analysis. To obtain an estimate of children’s adaptive functioning, the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (VABS; Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984) Survey Form was administered to each mother (or primary female caregiver) by trained interviewers. The standard score for the adaptive behavior composite was used to compare the three groups in this study. Finally, each mother’s assessment of their child’s behavior problems was obtained using the preschool and school-age forms of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000, 2001). Ratings were based on mothers’ observations over the past two months on the preschool form (1 ½ –5 years), and the past six months on the school-age form (6–18 years). Mothers of children with DS with a mental age below 6 years were asked to complete the chronological age appropriate form of the CBCL. On each form mothers rated the frequency of different behavior problems using a 3-point scale: 0 = not true (as far as you know); 1 = somewhat or sometimes true; 2 = very true or often true. The total problem scores (T-scores) were used for analysis.
Experimental Design and Procedure
Each of the three groups of focal children paired with CA-playmates (DS, MA-match, CA-match) as well as the added group pairing the children with Down syndrome with MA-playmates underwent the same procedures with respect to testing and observations of dyadic play (see Table 1). Observational measures were obtained in a laboratory setting and were completed within a sixteen week period for each focal child after meeting all inclusion criteria. Testing and laboratory observations were administered to the child with DS first, followed by the two matched focal children (MA- and CA-matches) as they were identified. For observational measures, focal children in each of the three groups with CA-playmates were videotaped in a laboratory playroom for five play segments (50 minutes per session) on two consecutive sessions (time 1 and time 2; see below) while playing with unfamiliar, typically developing peers matched on chronological age and gender (CA-playmate). The MA-playmate pairings for children with Down syndrome (cell G in Table 1) were also videotaped on two consecutive laboratory sessions. Identified friends were similarly videotaped in dyadic play with the focal children for the five play segments but only on one occasion (see cell D, E, and F in Table 1). The order of observations for the CA-playmate pairings, the MA-playmate pairings for the DS children, and the friend pairings was randomly determined (except for the two required consecutive observational periods scheduled on separate days).
Laboratory Observations for Unfamiliar Peers and Friends
Laboratory observations were carried out within a 12′ × 21′ carpeted area of a laboratory playroom designed to be similar to a typical preschool classroom. To record play interactions, the room was equipped with video cameras operated by remote control, a radio telemetry microphone for the focal child (both had vests capable of housing the microphone), and an overhead microphone. A remote control panel with mixers balanced the auditory signals. Play was recorded on two Panasonic DMR-E50 DVD recorders and a time code was superimposed on the recording with a Horita RM 50 II time code generator. Dyadic play was constrained to a 7′ × 8′ space at one end of the room. This play space was created by using two walls in the room and bookshelves to create a third wall. A piece of carpet covered the play area and helped the children define the end of the play space where there was no wall.
All laboratory observations of focal children in each of the groups with unfamiliar peers and friends followed the same experimental protocol. Specifically, the focal child and each playmate were scheduled to arrive at the same time. They entered the playroom where they were supervised by an adult who directed them in a 10-minute warm-up session in which the children engaged in a structured play activity (e.g., puzzles, drawing, play dough, painting). Next, the children were videotaped engaging in a series of five consecutive play segments, each lasting 10 minutes. Two of the segments focused on the social task of entering into play (1 and 3) and two focused on the social task of maintaining play (2 and 4). The final segment was free play. During all five segments the children were supervised by an adult experienced with young children who was seated outside the play area.
The play materials for each segment were selected to promote interactive play, to be within the developmental abilities of a diverse group of children, and to be interesting. Five different sets of toys were used in the observations for each of the 5 segments respectively: (1) a castle set (including a plastic castle with human and animal figures), (2) a set of cars with a house, parking structure, and roadway rug, (3) a highly popular train set, (4) a kitchen set, and (5) free play toys (two babies, two stuffed dinosaurs, fire engine set, assorted cars, planes, space flight set, medical kit, and horses). During each segment, only the materials for that activity were available to the children and the children were restricted to the play space. As noted, each dyad was videotaped for ten minutes for each of the five segments.
After the initial warm-up, the focal child stayed in the outside room and was asked to wait while the castle was set up for segment 1 (entry). While this was occurring, the playmate (CA-playmate, MA-playmate, or friend) was allowed to begin play with the toy in the designated play space and was given the following instructions: “[Focal child] will join you in a moment.” The adult helped the playmate to become involved with the toy and then directed the focal child to join the playmate after he or she had been involved with the toy for approximately one minute. The focal child was given the following instructions to join the play of the playmate: “Now go play together with (playmate). Okay, now tell me what are you going to do.” Especially for the DS child, a simple verbalization (e.g., “play”) was all the response that was required. For all segments, at the five-minute mark the adult attempted to promote interactive play if none was taking place at that time (defined as the absence of verbal or nonverbal social exchanges by both children to one another in the context of play materials in the previous 30 seconds; at least one of the exchanges must be positive or neutral in form).
The adult then removed the first toy set after ten minutes and brought out the car set for the first maintaining play segment (segment 2). She helped the children get started playing together by showing them how the set could be used and she suggested an initial activity: If the children did not engage in play with the initial suggestion, the adult would attempt to help them engage in interactive play by making two additional suggestions. She would then back off as the timing for this segment began and re-engage only if no interactive play was occurring at the five minute mark. The same general procedures were repeated using the train set for entry (segment 3, same as segment 1 as the focal child waited in outer room area) and the kitchen set for maintenance (segment 4, same as segment 2). The fifth and final play activity was a free play segment. During this segment the dyad was instructed to play however they liked for 10 minutes with a collection of various types of toys (see above).
Child-Peer Play Coding Scheme
A time code superimposed on each videotape in conjunction with a remotely controlled tape-stop device allowed coders to view tapes at 10-second intervals. Only the play of the focal child was coded. Coders recorded the quality of social participation and levels of cognitive play during each 10-second interval using the revised version of the Play Observation Scale (POS; Rubin, 2000). This scale consists of 10 mutually exclusive and exhaustive categories. The first three were derived from Parten’s (1932) social participation categories consisting of the following play classifications: (a) solitary (playing alone), (b) parallel (playing next to another child), and (c) group (playing with another child; a combination of Parten’s associative and cooperative play categories). Nested within these three social participation categories are five measures of cognitive play based primarily on the work of Smilansky (1968): (a) functional (simple repetitive play), (b) exploratory (examining physical properties of objects), (c) constructive (learns to use materials, creates something), (d) dramatic (role taking and pretend play), and (e) games with rules (child behaves in accordance with prearranged rules). If any 10-second interval is coded as either solitary, parallel, or group play, then one of the five cognitive play categories is also scored (an “occupied” category was also available if the category of cognitive play could not be determined). The seven remaining main categories consisted of the following: (a) unoccupied behavior (child not playing), (b) onlooker behavior (child watches other children but does not enter into play), (c) peer conversation (talking, questioning, and suggesting to other children but not playing), (d) transitional (moving from one activity to another), (e) adult-directed (any activity with an adult), and (f) uncodeable. In addition, aggression (non-playful hitting, grabbing, etc.) and rough-and-tumble play (playful physical contact, mock fighting) were double-coded. This scale has been applied effectively to children with disabilities similar to those in this study for playgroups (e.g., Guralnick, Hammond, Connor, & Neville, 2006), and in situations involving dyads (Guralnick & Groom, 1987a; Guralnick, Neville, Hammond, & Connor, 2007). Evidence with respect to convergent and discriminant validity suggests that the scale of social participation, especially the group play measure, constitutes a useful index of a more general construct of peer competence (Provost & LaFreniere, 1991).
Before coding, coders were trained for a period of 10–12 weeks on the POS with two coders being trained at the University of Maryland. For pre-study reliability for the full variable matrix, including cognitive play categories nested within the social participation categories, all raters reached the minimum criterion obtaining an overall Cohen’s k of .75. After training was completed, interrater reliability on 20% of randomly selected dyadic sessions was calculated between pairs of coders and produced an overall k of .79. Intercoder differences were resolved through review and discussion.
Results
Child and Family Characteristics
Comparisons among the three focal groups were first carried out for all of the child and family characteristic measures found in Table 2. As indicated in the table, the three groups were matched successfully in accordance with the experimental design. Specifically, following significant ANOVAs for CA, F(2, 78) = 185.9, p < .001, partial η2= .83, and MA, F(2, 78) = 269.5, p < .001, partial η2= .87, we conducted pair-wise comparisons which indicated that children with Down syndrome were closely matched to the group of typically developing children (CA-match) on the basis of CA, t(52) = .10, p = .96, d = .01, and to the group of younger typically developing children (MA-match) on the basis of MA, t(52) = .80, p = .41, d = .23. These pair-wise measures of the absence of differences give us considerable confidence that our groups were well-matched (Frick, 1995; Mervis, 2004). Moreover, as expected, the MAs and CAs of the two typically developing groups did differ significantly from one another: CA, t(52) = 18.0, p < .001, d = 5.0, and MA, t(52) = 19.4, p <.001, d = 5.38. Of importance, the three groups did not differ significantly on any of the family demographic or other child demographic measures noted in Table 2 (p > .05). With respect to school placement, all the children in the MA-match group were enrolled in preschool programs, whereas children in the Down syndrome and CA-match groups were enrolled equally between preschool and kindergarten programs, overall χ2 (4, N = 81) = 26.1, p < .001.
Based on children’s developmental status and chronological age, expected differences and similarities among the three groups were obtained for the cognitive, language, adaptive behavior, and behavior problem measures (see Child Developmental Characteristics in Table 2). Matching superscripts in Table 2 indicate that no statistically significant difference (p > .05) between the groups was found whereas significance was detected (p < .05 at minimum) for groups with different superscripts (details of the ANOVAS and follow-up pair–wise comparisons can be obtained by contacting the first author).
Overall Peer Interaction Comparisons
For the initial analyses, POS measures were summed across all five segments (50 minutes/focal child) and transformed to percentages of intervals coded for each measure (see Table 3). A group (DS, MA-match, CA-match) x playmate (CA-playmate time 1, CA-playmate time 2, friend) mixed measures MANOVA was then carried out. The two time periods refer to the two consecutive 50 minute sessions. A significant effect was obtained only for the group variable, F(16, 134) = 7.8, p < .001, partial η2 = .48. A series of ANOVAs for the group variable was then conducted for all POS measures in the table, with significant effects obtained for all measures except parallel play and unoccupied. Findings were as follows: solitary play, F(2, 78) = 17.8, p < .001, partial η2 = .31; group play, F(2, 78) = 30.9, p < .001, partial η2 = .44; transition, F(2, 78) = 16.7, p < .001, partial η2 = .30; onlooker, F(2, 78) = 15.2, p < .001, partial η2 = .28; peer conversation, F(2, 78) = 29.8, p < .001, partial η2 = .43; and adult-directed, F(2, 78) = 13.2, p < .001, partial η2 = .25. Follow-up comparisons indicated that in all except two instances noted below, these effects could be accounted for by differences between the CA-match group in comparison to the other two groups. Specific comparisons are as follows: solitary play, DS/CA-match, t(52) = 5.9, p < .001, d= 1.64, MA-match/CA-match, t(52) = 4.2, p < .001, d= 1.18; group play, DS/CA-match, t(52) = 7.6, p < .001, d = 2.09, MA-match/CA-match, t(52) = 5.5, p < .001, d = 1.53); transition, DS/CA-match, t(52) = 5.9, p < .001, d= 1.65, MA-match/CA-match, t(52) = 4.3, p < .001, d = 1.18; and onlooker, DS/CA-match, t(52) = 5.0, p < .001, d =1.37, MA-match/CA-match, t(52) = 4.9, p < .001, d = 1.37. As expected, the CA-match group engaged playmates at a more advanced level than either of the other two groups; playing alone, being in transition, and observing their playmates less often but participating in group play to a much greater extent.
Table 3.
Measures | Focal Group
|
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Down Syndrome | MA-Match | CA-Match | ||||
| ||||||
M (%) | SD | M (%) | SD | M (%) | SD | |
Solitary Play | 32.36 | 14.86 | 25.35 | 13.06 | 11.77 | 10.31 |
Parallel Play | 29.88 | 8.40 | 33.91 | 9.56 | 34.42 | 7.46 |
Group Play | 10.12 | 9.05 | 14.03 | 13.37 | 35.15 | 14.65 |
Transition | 11.22 | 4.12 | 9.59 | 4.04 | 5.59 | 2.71 |
Unoccupied | .41 | .65 | .80 | 2.66 | .08 | .19 |
Onlooker | 9.80 | 6.22 | 7.36 | 3.29 | 3.37 | 2.59 |
Peer Conversation | 1.14 | 1.32 | 3.55 | 1.83 | 7.76 | 5.04 |
Adult Directed | 1.31 | 1.25 | 2.08 | 1.41 | .52 | .41 |
Constructiveness Composite | 3.49 | .26 | 3.53 | .27 | 3.79 | .10 |
Note. Data are percentages of intervals for each peer interaction measure. N = 27 per group except that four children in the Down syndrome group could not identify a friend to bring to the laboratory.
However, not only did the CA-match group converse more than both the DS group, t(52) = 6.6, p < .001, d = 1.83, and the MA-match group, t(52) = 4.1, p < .001, d =1.13, but the DS group also engaged in significantly less peer conversation than did the MA-match group t(52) = 5.5, p < .001, d = 1.54. Differences among all three groups were also found for the adult directed measure: CA-match/MA-match, t(52) = 5.5, p < .001, d = 1.53; CA-match/DS, t(52) = 3.1, p < .01, d = .87 and the DS/MA-match t(52) = 2.1, p < .05, d = .59. In this case, it was the chronologically youngest children (MA-match group) that required the most direction from adults. Uncodeable and double-coded measures occurred at very low rates.
Finally, a constructiveness of play composite score was created by assigning scale values to each of the five levels of play ranging from functional play (assigned level 1) to games with rules (assigned level 5). The group x playmate ANOVA also produced a significant effect only for group, F(2, 74) = 13.4, p < .001, partial η2 = .27. Pair-wise comparisons indicated that the DS and CA-match groups differed significantly from one another, t(48) = 5.4, p < .001, d = 1.57 as did the MA-match and the CA-match groups, t(52) = 4.6, p < .001, d = 1.27. Again, the constructive play of the CA-match group was at a higher level than either of the other two groups. No differences were obtained for the MA-match and DS group comparison (p > .05).
Effects of Friendships
To examine the possible influence of the friendships of children’s playmates more closely, we focused on comparisons between children in their first laboratory visit (time 1) with an unfamiliar peer (CA-playmate) and their first (and only) laboratory visit with a friend. Separate group (DS, MA-match, CA-match) x friendship (CA-playmate time 1, friend) ANOVAs were carried out for each of the four key measures described earlier most likely to be affected by the special characteristics of children with Down syndrome: group play, peer conversation, onlooker behavior, and constructiveness of play. All four measures produced significant effects for the group variable with pair-wise comparisons consistent with the familiarity analyses presented above that included all measures and playmates. Therefore, details of the findings for the group variable are not presented.
With respect to analyses involving the friendship variable, a significant effect was obtained for the group play measure, F(1, 74) = 6.66, p < .05, partial η2 = .08, but the group x friendship interaction was not statistically significant (p > .05). For this measure, children across groups engaged in group play more often when interacting with friends (M = 22.4, SD = 20.7) than with unfamiliar playmates (M = 17.2, SD = 17.7). Similarly, a significant friendship effect was obtained for the constructiveness of play composite, F(1,74) = 4.20, p < .05, partial η2 = .05. Again, children played more constructively with friends, (M = 3.64, SD = .28) than with unfamiliar playmates (M = 3.59, SD = .28). In contrast, no effects for friendship were found for the peer conversation or onlooker measures (p > .05). However, a significant group x playmate familiarity interaction was obtained for onlooker behavior, F(2,74) = 6.18, p < .01, partial η2 = .14. This interaction was primarily a result of the fact that children with DS engaged in more onlooker behavior with friends in comparison to unfamiliar playmates whereas the two typically developing groups had lower levels of onlooker behavior with their friends than with unfamiliar playmates.
Characteristics of Children’s Friends
It should be noted that the vast majority of identified friends for all three focal groups were typically developing children (percent with developmental delay or disability: DS, 17.39; MA-match, 3.7, CA-match, 0.0). As indicated by an ANOVA, approximately equal numbers of boys and girls were selected in all three groups (p > .05) and the vast majority (85.3%) were Caucasian. A significant ANOVA, however, was obtained for the family status measure F(2, 74) = 5.64, p < .001, partial η2 = .13. Pair-wise comparisons revealed that the only significant difference was between the DS (M = 46.5, SD = 14.3) and the MA-match (M = 56.7, SD = 8.5) groups, t(48) = 3.1, p <.01, d=.89, with family social status lower for friends of the children with Down syndrome. Finally, the chronological ages of identified friends differed significantly across groups, F (2,74) = 50.68, p < .001, partial η2 = .58, as expected. Chronological age of friends (years) was highest for the CA-match group (M = 5.54, SD = .93), followed by the DS group (M = 4.81, SD = 1.02), and then the MA-match group (M = 3.34, SD = .40). All three pair-wise comparisons were significant: DS/MA-match, t(48) = 6.9, p < .001, d = 1.99; DS/CA-match, t(48) = 2.65, p < .05, d = .77; and MA-match/CA-match, t(52) = 11.33, p < .001, d = 3.14.
Effects of Playmate’s Social Skills
In the previous analyses, children in all three focal groups were compared when engaged with same gender and similar chronological age typically developing children or with similar age friends, the vast majority also being typically developing children. The findings that more advanced peer interactions of children with DS, such as group play, were not significantly different from that of the MA-match group within the overall peer interaction analysis may, however, be specific to this type of dyad. That is, when children with DS are paired with similar chronological age but far more developmentally advanced playmates, it is the playmates who may have scaffolded the social interactions of the children with DS to achieve a level similar to that of focal children in the MA-match group. To examine this hypothesis, comparisons were made between DS children interacting with chronological age typically developing playmates and the additional pairing in which the same DS children interacted with younger, less socially skillful playmates matched on mental age. Separate analyses for each of the four key measures (group play, onlooker behavior, peer conversation, constructiveness of play) at the second time period were conducted for this comparison in order to allow time for any scaffolding to develop. Findings indicated that, despite differences in the chronological ages of the typically developing playmates and their corresponding play skills, none of the separate repeated measures analyses were significant for any of the four measures (p > .05). It should be noted that children with DS did engage in approximately 40% more group play with older playmates and that there was considerable interindividual variability. Nevertheless, the overall absence of differences found in this comparison suggests that other factors, such as the social orientation of children with DS and their ability to engage in representational play may have enabled younger, less skillful, playmates to scaffold social play adequately.
Comparisons with Equivalent Mental Age Dyads
Despite this apparent receptivity to social scaffolding even when interacting with younger typically developing children similar in mental age, children with Down syndrome may nevertheless exhibit levels of peer competence that are not commensurate with their developmental levels. An important reference group to evaluate this possibility consists of dyads of typically developing children in which both focal children and playmates have mental ages similar to those to the children with Down syndrome and their playmates. Accordingly, we compared dyads containing the DS children (focal child) interacting with typically developing (younger) playmates matched on mental age (DS/MA-playmate dyads; cell G in Table 1) with the group of typically developing children also interacting with typically developing playmates matched on chronological (and mental) age (i.e., the MA-match/CA-playmate dyads; cell B in Table 1). Therefore, all children in this comparison (focal children and playmates) were similar in mental age, the key difference being that one set of dyads contained a child with DS. However, the children with DS were, of course, older than their MA-playmates as well as the children in the typically developing comparison group.
Once again, these between group comparisons were carried out for the four key outcome measures during the second time period. Findings produced significant effects for group play (square root transformations were used to minimize variance differences) t(1,52) = 4.7, p < .05, partial η2 = .08 and peer conversation t(1.52) = 4.8, p < .05, partial η2 = .09, only. Specifically, younger typically developing children paired with same mental age (and chronological age) matched playmates engaged in higher levels of group play (M = 14.8; SD = 14.2) than did the children with DS also interacting with similar mental age typically developing (younger) children (M = 7.5; SD = 7.8). Similarly, focal children in dyads containing only typically developing children engaged in more conversation with their playmates (M = 3.3; SD = 2.5) than did the DS children paired with younger typically developing children of the same mental age (M = 1.8; SD = 2.5). Accordingly, even for dyads matched in mental age, typically developing children paired with other typically developing children exhibit more advanced forms of peer interactions than do children with Down syndrome also interacting with typically developing playmates. Although alternative interpretations exist, this finding is consistent with the hypothesis that in this interpersonal context children with Down syndrome exhibit peer competence problems that extend beyond those which might be expected based on their developmental level.
Discussion
In this study, we investigated one important aspect of the social development of children with Down syndrome -- their peer-related social competence. As noted, young children with Down syndrome present a complex picture with respect to their expected levels of peer competence. In particular, well established protective factors such as a pronounced social orientation and well-developed representational skills compete with risk factors such as limited expressive language and concerns regarding overarching social-cognitive abilities. As elaborated upon below, two important findings emerged with respect to the influence of these factors on the dyadic play of children with Down syndrome. The first finding appears to represent the influence of protective factors capable of facilitating dyadic interactions with typically developing as well as with familiar playmates. The second finding likely represents the influence of risk factors that contribute to peer competence problems that may be most evident when more challenging social circumstances are encountered.
Specifically, comparisons with mental age and chronological age matched typically developing focal groups provided a developmental framework within which to interpret the strengths and vulnerabilities of children with Down syndrome with respect to key measures of peer interactions. Initial analyses centered on comparisons among the three focal groups of children paired with typically developing playmates matched on chronological age and gender. Comparisons with playmates with these characteristics represent normative peer relationships for young children. This is the case even for children with Down syndrome as the vast majority of their playmates in home and neighborhood settings are typically developing children, although somewhat younger (Freeman & Kasari, 2002; Guralnick, et al., 2009).
Under these circumstances, as expected, comparisons among the three focal groups indicated that the peer interactions of older typically developing children were more advanced in virtually every respect in comparison to both younger typically developing children and children with Down syndrome. Of importance, and relevant to the possible influence of protective factors, the peer interactions of children with Down syndrome did not differ in any substantial manner from the mental age matched group of (younger) typically developing children, except for lower levels of peer conversation. This finding for children with Down syndrome contrasts with results for heterogeneous groups of children with developmental delays who exhibit unusual peer interaction problems in small playgroup settings in comparison to mental-age matched playmates (Guralnick & Groom, 1987b). In addition, children with Down syndrome (as well as the other two focal groups) had higher levels of play when paired with friends in comparison to unfamiliar playmates. This result also contrasts with results for heterogeneous groups of children with delays in which no advantage has been found (Guralnick et al., 1996). The finding of a positive effect of friendship for children with Down syndrome is, however, consistent with that of Freeman and Kasari (2002). Accordingly, this pattern of results supports the hypothesis that protective factors, presumably including high levels of representational play and a strong interest in interacting socially, may have been sufficient to compensate for risk factors such as limited language to enable children with Down syndrome to at least display levels of peer interaction commensurate with their developmental levels. These findings, based on social interactions occurring with typically developing playmates similar in chronological age, is also consistent with a behavioral phenotype in which the overall social development of children with Down syndrome is considered to be a relative strength (see Kasari & Hodapp, 1996).
One reason for this outcome may be related to the ability of typically developing playmates, including identified friends, to capitalize on the protective factors associated with children with Down syndrome to support and scaffold dyadic play. Even younger, less socially skillful, typically developing playmates matched on mental age to children with Down syndrome appeared to be able to provide adequate support, although there was a tendency for levels of group play to be lower for this pairing. Related evidence for this sample does support the hypothesis that social scaffolding by playmates commonly occurs. Specifically, during dyadic play in home settings, typically developing playmates similar in chronological age were found to exert control of play more often than children with Down syndrome (Guralnick, et al., 2009).
In contrast, the influence of risk factors emerged in a comparison between groups in which focal children and playmates had similar mental ages but one group consisted of focal children with Down syndrome. In this comparison, sustained interactive (group) play in particular, an important index of a more general construct of peer-related social competence (Provost & LaFreniere, 1991), occurred less often for children with Down syndrome despite the similar mental ages of all the participants. A lack of common interests may be partly responsible for this finding in view of the chronological age differences in the dyads that contained children with Down syndrome. At the same time, it would be reasonable to expect that the greater experience with peers (that likely was the case for children with Down syndrome given their chronological ages) would have conferred some advantage in this comparison.
Accordingly, the finding of lower levels of group play for children with Down syndrome in this comparison is consistent with the hypothesis that these children exhibit peer competence difficulties that extend beyond those expected on the basis of their developmental levels. As suggested above, these difficulties can be masked by a supportive partner – child or adult – capitalizing on protective factors. However, when a partner’s scaffolding is less skillful, such as when interacting with much younger typically developing children as in this comparison, sustained interactive play becomes more difficult to achieve in relation to dyads composed of similar mental age typically developing children. Consequently, in this interpersonal context, the relative strength of the peer interaction component of children’s social development is not apparent, as children with Down syndrome display unusual problems in peer competence similar to those identified for children with other developmental delays.
This interpretation suggests further that the influence of risk factors are more likely to be evident for children with Down syndrome when social situations become more complex, such as during unstructured or semi-structured play activities common to preschool programs. Part of the complexity lies in the fact that these settings involve numerous peers and mostly unknown expectations as to the pattern of play and social interaction. In this study involving only dyads, expectations for play and the structure provided by the context were clear, with few distractions for either child in the dyad. Of note, based on teacher reports, available evidence does in fact suggest that unusual peer interaction problems are apparent in comparison to mental age-matched typically developing children when children with Down syndrome participate in these types of less structured and more complex preschool contexts (Guralnick, Connor, & Johnson, in press). Peer competence difficulties also likely account for much of the peer social network concerns noted earlier that have been regularly reported for young children with Down syndrome (e.g., Carr, 1995; Guralnick et al., 2009; Howell et al., 2007).
Consequently, carefully programmed transitions to larger groups from the more highly structured and less complex dyadic context may provide the most ideal conditions to implement systematic intervention programs to promote the peer-related social competence for this group of children. Indeed, a less complex initial structure may well be needed for children with Down syndrome in particular in order to facilitate their problem solving strategies, including social strategies (see Landry, Miller-Loncar, & Swank, 1998). Moreover, the involvement of typically developing children capable of taking advantage of protective factors should also be an important consideration and is feasible in the context of inclusion programs in which children with and without disabilities are enrolled (Guralnick, 2005).
Finally, the pattern of results in this study clearly suggests that future research examining the social development of etiologic-specific subgroups such as children with Down syndrome, or other well-defined subgroups, can benefit from a framework that considers possible risk and protective factors relevant to this domain of development. This can be accomplished in studies of larger groups of children by directly measuring various risk and protective factors, including those relevant to social information processing. Moreover, by examining children’s social development in various interpersonal contexts a more complex behavioral profile relevant to these risk and protective factors may well emerge, as occurred in this investigation. Equally important is the potential for these factors, as they interact with one another in different contexts, to inform intervention efforts. Contemporary approaches to the study of peer-related social competence focusing on children with developmental delays and related disabilities now include many of these risk and protective factors in their models (Guralnick, 1999; Yeates et al., 2007). Taken together, the outcome of this process may well be a more precise understanding of the behavioral phenotype for the domain of social development for children with Down syndrome as well as for other defined subgroups, a better grasp of the social development of these children and the mechanisms involved, and a more effective set of interventions.
Acknowledgments
This research was facilitated by resources provided by grant P30 HD02274 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. The authors wish to thank the children and families who participated to help us better understand the social development of children with Down syndrome.
References
- Abbeduto L, Warren SF, Connors FA. Language development in Down syndrome: From the prelinguistic period to the acquisition of literacy. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2007;13:247–261. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for ASEBA Preschool Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families, University of Vermont; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA School-age Forms & Profiles. Burlington, VT: Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families, University of Vermont; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Brownell CA. Convergent developments: Cognitive-developmental correlates of growth in infant/toddler peer skills. Child Development. 1986;57:275–286. [Google Scholar]
- Byrne EA, Cunningham CC, Sloper P. The children: their activities, friends, sisters and brothers. In: Byrne EA, Cunningham CC, Sloper P, editors. Families and their children with Down’s syndrome: One feature in common. London: Routledge; 1988. pp. 25–47. [Google Scholar]
- Carr JH. Down’s syndrome: Children growing up. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Cebula KR, Wishart JG. Social cognition in children with Down syndrome. In: Glidden L, editor. International review of research in mental retardation. Vol. 35. New York: Academic Press; 2008. pp. 43–86. [Google Scholar]
- Chapman RS. Language and communication in individuals with Down syndrome. In: Abbeduto L, editor. International review of research in mental retardation. Vol. 27. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2003. pp. 1–34. [Google Scholar]
- Chapman RS, Hesketh LJ. Behavioral phenotype of individuals with Down syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2000;6:84–95. doi: 10.1002/1098-2779(2000)6:2<84::AID-MRDD2>3.0.CO;2-P. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crick NR, Dodge KA. A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;115:74–101. [Google Scholar]
- Diamond KE. The development of social competence in children with disabilities. In: Smith PK, Hart CH, editors. Blackwell handbook of childhood social development. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers; 2002. pp. 571–587. [Google Scholar]
- Doyle A, Connolly J, Rivest L. The effect of playmate familiarity on the social interactions of young children. Child Development. 1980;51:217–223. [Google Scholar]
- Fidler JJ, Nadel L. Education and children with Down syndrome: Neuroscience, development, and intervention. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2007;13:262–271. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fidler DJ, Hepburn S, Rogers S. Early learning and adaptive behaviour in toddlers with Down syndrome: Evidence for an emerging behavioural phenotype? Down Syndrome Research and Practice. 2006;9:37–44. doi: 10.3104/reports.297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman SF, Kasari C. Characteristics and qualities of the play dates of children with Down syndrome: Emerging or true friendships? American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2002;107:16–31. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2002)107<0016:CAQOTP>2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freeman SF, Alkin MC, Kasari CL. Satisfaction and desire for change in educational placement for children with Down syndrome: Perceptions of parents. Remedial and Special Education. 1999;20:143–151. [Google Scholar]
- Frick RW. Accepting the null hypothesis. Memory & Cognition. 1995;23:132–138. doi: 10.3758/bf03210562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilmore L, Campbell J, Cuskelly M. Developmental expectations, personality stereotypes, and attitudes towards inclusive education: Community and teacher views of Down syndrome. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education. 2003;50:65–76. [Google Scholar]
- Guralnick MJ. Family and child influences on the peer-related social competence of young children with developmental delays. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 1999;5:21–29. [Google Scholar]
- Guralnick MJ. Involvement with peers: Comparisons between young children with and without Down syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2002;46:379–393. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2002.00405.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guralnick MJ. Inclusion as a core principle in the early intervention system. In: Guralnick MJ, editor. The developmental systems approach to early intervention. Baltimore: Brookes; 2005. pp. 59–69. [Google Scholar]
- Guralnick MJ, Connor RT, Johnson LC. Home-based peer social networks of young children with Down syndrome: A developmental perspective. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. 2009;114:340–355. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-114.5.340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gurlanick MJ, Connor RT, Johnson LC. The peer social networks of young children with Down syndrome in classroom settings. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-3148.2010.00619.x. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guralnick MJ, Gottman JM, Hammond MA. Effects of social setting on the friendship formation of young children differing in developmental status. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 1996;17:625–651. [Google Scholar]
- Guralnick MJ, Groom JM. Dyadic peer interactions of mildly delayed and nonhandicapped preschool children. American Journal of Mental Deficiency. 1987a;92:178–193. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guralnick MJ, Groom JM. The peer relations of mildly delayed and nonhandicapped preschool children in mainstreamed playgroups. Child Development. 1987b;58:1556–1572. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1987.tb03866.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guralnick MJ, Groom JM. Friendships of preschool children in mainstreamed playgroups. Developmental Psychology. 1988;24:595–604. [Google Scholar]
- Guralnick MJ, Hammond MA, Connor RT, Neville B. Stability, change, and correlates of the peer relationships of young children with mild developmental delays. Child Development. 2006;77:312–324. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00872.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guralnick MJ, Neville B, Hammond MA, Connor RT. The friendships of young children with developmental delays: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2007;28:64–79. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartup WW, Stevens N. Friendships and adaptation in the life course. Psychological Bulletin. 1997;121:355–370. [Google Scholar]
- Hartup WW, Laursen B, Stewart MI, Eastonson A. Conflict and the friendship relations of young children. Child Development. 1988;59:1590–1600. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1988.tb03686.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinde RA, Titmus G, Easton D, Tamplin A. Incidence of “friendship” and behavior toward strong associates versus nonassociates in preschoolers. Child Development. 1985;56:234–245. [Google Scholar]
- Hollingshead AB. Four factor index of social status. Yale University, Department of Sociology; New Haven: 1975. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Hornby G. Fathers’ views of the effects on their families of children with Down syndrome. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 1995;4:103–117. [Google Scholar]
- Howell A, Hauser-Cram P, Kersh JE. Setting the stage: Early child and family characteristics as predictors of later loneliness in children with developmental disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2007;112:18–30. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2007)112[18:STSECA]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howes C. Social competence with peers in young children. Developmental Review. 1987;7:252–272. [Google Scholar]
- Howes C. Social status and friendship from kindergarten to third grade. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 1990;11:321–330. [Google Scholar]
- Iarocci G, Yager J, Rombough A, McLaughlin J. The development of social competence among persons with Down syndrome: From survival to social inclusion. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation. 2008;35:87–119. [Google Scholar]
- Jahromi LB, Gulsrud A, Kasari C. Emotional competence in children with Down syndrome: Negativity and regulation. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2008;113:32–43. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2008)113[32:ECICWD]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kasari C, Hodapp RM. Is Down syndrome different? Evidence from social and family studies. Down Syndrome Quarterly. 1996;1:1–8. [Google Scholar]
- Landry SH, Miller-Loncar CL, Swank PR. Goal-directed behavior in children with Down syndrome: The role of joint play situations. Early Education and Development. 1998;9:375–392. [Google Scholar]
- Lenhard W, Breitenbach E, Ebert H, Schindelhauer-Deutscher HJ, Zang KD, Henn W. Attitudes of mothers towards their child with Down syndrome before and after the introduction of prenatal diagnosis. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. 2007;45:98–102. doi: 10.1352/1934-9556(2007)45[98:AOMTTC]2.0.CO;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loveland KA, Kelley ML. Development of adaptive behavior in preschoolers with autism or Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1991;96:13–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malone FD, Canick JA, Ball RH, Nyberg DA, Comstock CH, Bukowski R, et al. First-trimester or second-trimester screening, or both, for Down’s syndrome. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2005;353:2001–2011. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa043693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mervis CB. Cross-etiolgy comparisons of cognitive and language development. In: Rice ML, Warren SF, editors. Developmental Language Disorders: From Phenotypes to Etiologies. Mahwah: Earlbaum; 2004. pp. 153–186. [Google Scholar]
- Newcomb AF, Bagwell CL. Children’s friendship relations: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;117:306–347. [Google Scholar]
- Newcomb AF, Bagwell CL. The developmental significance of children’s friendship relations. In: Bukowski WM, Newcomb AF, Hartup WW, editors. The company they keep. San Diego, CA: Cambridge; 1996. pp. 289–321. [Google Scholar]
- Parten MB. Social participation among preschool children. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology. 1932;27:243–269. [Google Scholar]
- Patterson D. Genetic mechanisms involved in the phenotype of Down syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2007;13:199–206. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patterson D, Costa AC. Down syndrome and genetics - a case of linked histories. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2005;6:137–147. doi: 10.1038/nrg1525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Provost MA, LaFreniere PJ. Social participation and peer competence in preschool children: Evidence for discriminate and convergent validity. Child Study Journal. 1991;21:57–72. [Google Scholar]
- Porter MA, Coltheart M, Langdon R. The neuropsychological basis of hypersociability in Williams and Down syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2839–2849. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Putallaz M, Wasserman A. Children’s entry behavior. In: Asher SR, Coie JD, editors. Peer rejection in childhood. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1990. pp. 60–89. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts JE, Price J, Malkin C. Language and communication development in Down syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2007;13:26–35. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roid GH. Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales. 5. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing; 2003. (SB:V) [Google Scholar]
- Rubin KH. The Play Observation Scale. University of Maryland; College Park: 2000. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
- Rubin KH, Coplan RJ, Chen X, Buskirk A, Wojslawowicz J. Peer relationships in childhood. In: Bornstein M, Lamb M, editors. Developmental science: An advanced textbook. 5. Hillsdale, N J: Erlbaum; 2005. pp. 469–512. [Google Scholar]
- Seltzer MM, Ryff CD. Parenting across the life span: The normative and nonnormative cases. Life Span Development and Behavior. 1994;12:1–40. [Google Scholar]
- Shea JDC. Changes in interpersonal distances and categories of play behavior in the early weeks of preschool. Developmental Psychology. 1981;17:417–425. [Google Scholar]
- Sherman SL, Allen EG, Bean LH, Freeman SB. Epidemiology of Down syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2007;13:221–227. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sigman M, Ruskin E. Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1999;64 doi: 10.1111/1540-5834.00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silverman W. Down syndrome: Cognitive phenotype. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. 2007;13:228–236. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skinner ML, Buysse V, Bailey DB. Effects of age and developmental status of partners on play of preschoolers with disabilities. Journal of Early Intervention. 2004;26:194–203. [Google Scholar]
- Sloper P, Turner S, Knussen C, Cunningham C. Social life of school children with Down’s syndrome. Child: Care, Health and Development. 1990;16:235–251. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.1990.tb00658.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smilansky S. The effects of sociodramatic play on disadvantaged preschool children. New York: John Wiley &Sons; 1968. [Google Scholar]
- Sparrow SS, Balla DA, Cicchetti DV. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Vaughn BE, Colvin TN, Azria MR, Caya L, Krzysik L. Dyadic analyses of friendship in a sample of preschool-age children attending Head Start: Correspondence between measures and implications for social competence. Child Development. 2001;72:862–878. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wishart JG. Avoidant learning styles and cognitive development in young children. In: Stratford B, Gunn P, editors. New approaches to Down syndrome. London: Cassell; 1996. pp. 173–205. [Google Scholar]
- Wishart JG. Socio-cognitive understanding: A strength or weakness in Down’s syndrome? Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2007;51:996–1005. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007.01007.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wishart JG, Cebula KR, Willis DS, Pitcairn TK. Understanding of facial expressions of emotion by children with intellectual disabilities of differing etiology. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research. 2007;51:551–563. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00947.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeates KO, Bigler ED, Dennis M, Gerhardt CA, Rubin KH, Stancin T, et al. Social outcomes in childhood brain disorder: A heuristic integration of social neuroscience and developmental psychology. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133:535–556. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.3.535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yugar JM, Shapiro ES. Elementary children’s school friendship: A comparison of peer assessment methodologies. School Psychology Review. 2001;30:568–585. [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman IL, Steiner VG, Pond RE. Preschool Language Scale 4. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2002. [Google Scholar]