Abstract
The idea of retinal and ex-retinal sources of saccadic suppression has long been established in previous studies. However, how they are implemented in local circuit remains unknown. Researchers have suggested that saccadic suppression was probably achieved by contrast gain control, but this possibility has never been directly tested. In this study, we manipulated contrast gain control by contrast-adapting observers with sinusoidal gratings of different contrasts. Presaccadic and fixational contrast thresholds were measured and compared to give estimates of saccadic suppression at different adaptation states. Our results reconfirmed the selective saccadic suppression in achromatic condition, and further showed that, achromatic saccadic suppression diminished as contrast adaptation was accentuated, whereas no significant chromatic saccadic suppression was induced by greater contrast adaptation. Our data provided evidence for the involvement of contrast gain control in saccadic suppression in achromatic channel. We also discussed how the negative correlation between contrast adaptation and saccadic suppression could be interpreted with contrast gain control.
Introduction
Saccade is the fast switch between fixation points. Human and non-human primates direct their attention by making frequent saccadic eye movements. In fixation, high visual acuity becomes possible when image of the interested target falls onto the fovea of the retina. However, during and outlasting the saccade execution, substantial decrease of visual sensitivity, known as saccadic suppression, is observed. Saccadic suppression eliminates the drastic visual disturbance elicited by saccade itself, making it possible to acquire a stable percept. Saccade suppression generally starts from an average of 50 ms before saccade, peaks at saccade onset, diminishes all through the saccade duration and finally vanishes about 50 ms after saccade termination [1]. Retinal sources, such as motion blur and visual masking, have been confirmed playing major roles in the visual suppression [2], [3]. However, researchers noticed that retinal contributions were not sufficient to explain all the effects [4]. With the same visual inputs, observers showed different suppression patterns under simulated saccade and real saccade, affirming the necessity of an extra-retinal source of saccadic suppression [1].
Electrophysiological studies have found fluctuations of various significance in different stages of the visual system produced by saccade-visual interaction [5], [6], [7], [8], [9], [10]. However, how saccadic suppression is implemented in local neural circuit has rarely been reported. Results from several psychophysical researches suggest that contrast gain control could be the possible mechanism underlying saccadic suppression. One study utilizing masking paradigm shows that saccade attenuated visual sensitivity at all mask levels by a constant factor, which is a divisive way of gain modulation typically demonstrated in contrast gain control [11]. In another study, impulse response peaks faster during saccade than that in fixation for luminance modulation, consistent with the suggestion that saccadic suppression is mediated by contrast gain control [12]. Moreover, gain control mechanism has been incorporated into computation models to test and explain assumptions of saccadic suppression mechanism [1], [13], [14]. Thus contrast gain control has been raised as an explanation for the above studies, yet no psychophysical experiment is conducted to directly explore the relationship between saccadic suppression and contrast gain control.
Here we propose that saccadic suppression involves contrast gain control. To verify this, we manipulate the contrast adaptation level of participants and measure the contrast threshold under fixation and saccade. By comparing the two contrast thresholds, we derive direct evidence for the influence of contrast adaptation on saccadic suppression and further explain the interaction with contrast gain.
Methods
Ethics Statement
This study was conducted according to the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki and had approval from the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy of Sciences. All participants provided written informed consent for the participation.
Participants
One female and three males aged from 27 to 32 took part in the experiment. They all had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and normal color perception. Participants were not told about the purpose of the study until the experiment was finished, except for one author (GXJ). All of them had practiced enough trials before final data acquisition so that they fully understood and mastered the techniques required to finish the task.
Stimulus
Vertically modulated sinusoidal grating with a spatial frequency of 0.5 cycles/° was used as the carrier to construct the stimuli for both adaptation operation and contrast threshold measurement. A full screen adaptation grating, which was the carrier drifting up and down alternatively with a temporal frequency of 8 (achromatic grating) or 4/3 (chromatic grating) cycles/s, adapted the observers. Drifting adaptation grating was used to avoid local luminance/color afterimage. A static test grating, which was the same carrier enveloped by a 2-D circularly symmetric Gaussian window with a spatial constant of 3°, tested the contrast threshold of observers. The test grating patch was centered randomly above or below the center of the screen with a 4° displacement. Adaptation and test stimuli were illustrated in Figure 1B .
The contrast of sinusoidal modulation was defined by Michelson contrast. Achromatic grating (black–white) was composed by a single sinusoidal modulation across red, green and blue channels. Chromatic grating (red–green) was composed by two sinusoidal modulations in red and green channels with a 180° phase shift. The equiluminance of chromatic grating was determined by flicker photometry [15]. An amplitude ratio between red (CIE coordinates: x = 0.652, y = 0.336) and green (CIE coordinates: x = 0.279, y = 0.616) channel was provided by linear-fitting equiluminance data (green contrast versus red contrast) for each observer ( Figure 1B lower panel) to minimize perceived flicker when the chromatic grating was reversed in phase at 20 Hz. After the two amplitudes were associated, for convenience, the contrast of red grating was used as the color contrast measurement of chromatic modulation, instead of the RMS (Root Mean Square) cone contrast commonly defined in color vision. The contrasts of achromatic and chromatic grating were dynamically controlled by QUEST procedures respectively [16]. Three adaptation contrasts were used: 0, 0.2 and 0.85. For participant TH, lower adaptation contrasts, 0, 0.1 and 0.43 were used in chromatic experiment to avoid ceiling effect. The contrast combinations were composed with approximately equal-spaced values on logarithmic scale (we say “approximately” here because the lowest contrast was replaced by zero contrast since it was hardly discernable).
Stimulus was presented on a 22″ LCD monitor (SAMSUNG 2233RZ) [17] at a refresh rate of 120 Hz, with a gray scale of 8 bits/pixel and a spatial resolution of 1680×1050 pixels, subtending a visual area of 45°×30° at the distance of 57 cm. The monitor was calibrated with Spyder 3 Elite (Datacolor) so that display luminance was linear with gray scale value. LCD backlight was then adjusted to achieve an average luminance of 52 cd/m2. The display was turned on 30 min prior to every experiment session to stabilize the luminance level. Participants ran experiment in a dark room illuminated only by LCD display and they were adapted to the environment illumination for at least 2 min before the recording session started. The display was properly surrounded by black foam to eliminate light reflection and interference from background texture.
Stimulus generation and experiment procedure were controlled by MATLAB (MathWorks) scripts written using Psychophysics and Eyelink Toolbox extensions [18], [19], [20].
Procedure
Participants were seated and constrained by a chin rest to perform fixation and saccade task. A block was composed of 64 trials, of which 32 trials of each task were randomly interleaved. For both tasks, an adapt-test regimen was used and only one adaptation contrast was used in a single block. Block started with a 30 s block-adaptation, and followed by a 6 s trial-adaptation in each trial. Block and trial structure are illustrated in Figure 1A .
In achromatic blocks, achromatic grating were used as adaptation and test stimuli, whereas in chromatic blocks, chromatic grating were used. Adaptation grating and test grating had the same horizontal orientation. In saccade trials, participants were asked to fixate a central fixation cross (0.4°). After 400 ms, the cross disappeared and a target cross (0.4°) appeared 10° away on the right along the horizontal meridian, to which a saccade was made immediately. A one frame test grating patch (8.3 ms at 120 Hz refresh rate) was flashed about 190–230 ms (saccade latency for participants) after target onset, randomly centered 4° above or below the fixation cross. The saccade latency of each participant was determined in pilot experiment to ensure that test grating was presented in the range of 8.3 to 50 ms prior to the saccade onset ( Figure 1D ). In fixation trials, participants maintained fixation on the fixation cross, which was continuously displayed at the center of the monitor. Test grating was flashed after a fixation of 590–630 ms (sum of fixation duration and saccade latency in saccade trials; this duration is constant for the single participant but generally different from that of others), which was a comparable time after trial-adaptation with that of saccade trials.
After the test grating was presented, participants pressed keys on a gamepad (Microsoft SideWinder Game Controller) to indicate the perceived position of the test grating. The correctness of the response was recorded and fed into the QUEST core to produce a suggested value for the contrast of the next test grating. After the block was finished, estimates of fixation and saccade contrast threshold and their standard deviations were immediately given by QUEST algorithm, converging at a 75% correct performance. Blocks without a typical convergence curve ( Figure 1C ) of the test grating contrast would be abandoned and repeated. To maintain observers’ alertness on the task, a 5 min rest was allowed after each block. Three blocks (three adaptation contrasts for achromatic or chromatic condition) must be finished in one session. Chromatic and achromatic sessions were conducted on different days.
Eye Movements
Eye movements were monitored by an infrared eye tracker (940 nm) Eyelink 1000 (SR research), sampling at 2 kHz. Saccade and fixation were evaluated by online parser with a saccade velocity criterion of 22°/s. The time of saccade onset, termination and test grating presentation were available immediately after the completion of each trial, as well as the validity of the participants’ performance. Trials, in which occasional blink or unqualified saccade (saccade end position was out of a 3°×3° window centered on target cross) happened, or test grating was not presented in the expected time range, would be discarded and repeated.
Data Analysis
Contrast threshold and standard deviation were given by QUEST algorithm, online-recorded and sorted by a MATLAB working thread.
For simplicity in calculation, we used contrast threshold instead of contrast sensitivity, which is the inverse of contrast threshold. Since contrast threshold, as well as contrast sensitivity, is logarithmic measure in nature, a gain modulation (in decibel) which was commonly used as a measure of gain change or attenuation, was utilized to quantify the amplitude of saccadic suppression:
where T_sac and T_fix are contrast thresholds of saccade and fixation conditions at the same adaptation contrast. Obviously, the gain modulation defined here will give same value whether we use contrast threshold or contrast sensitivity as the original measure.
The significance of the main experiment operations was analyzed in SPSS with Repeated Measures ANOVA of a 2×2×3 (saccade/fixation, achromatic/chromatic, adaptation contrasts) within-subject design. Other tests would be stated where they were used.
Results
To assess the effect of adaptation operation, a proper combination of adaptation contrasts was used (see Stimulus) for all participants to avoid ceiling and floor effect (except for TH, a lower contrast combination was used in his chromatic adaptation blocks. For simplicity, this exception is not illustrated in Figure 2 ). Adaptation effect was examined by oral report and changes of the contrast threshold. In Figure 2 , contrast thresholds in fixation and saccade conditions were both progressively raised along with the elevation of adaptation contrast, indicating that the adaptation operation was effective and the adaptation level of visual system was successfully manipulated as expected. The main effect of contrast adaptation was significant, F(1,3) = 84.2, p<.01. Pairwise comparisons further showed significant contrast adaptation in all conditions of achromatic adaptation [all p<.05]. However, in fixation and saccade conditions of chromatic adaptation, difference between adaptation contrast 0.2 and 0.85 was not significant [fixation, F(1,3) = 5.0, p>.05; saccade, F(1,3) = 4.7, p>.05], possibly caused by the data of observer TH and WJK, whose performance in chromatic condition showed slightly ceiling effect at greater adaptation contrasts.
As the adaptation level was maintained at different steady states, contrast thresholds in saccade and fixation conditions were measured. We obtained the amplitude of saccadic suppression by comparing contrast thresholds of saccade and fixation. Significant and consistent saccadic suppression was observed in achromatic condition [F(1,3) = 136.6, p<.01], while in chromatic condition, no significant suppression was noticed [F(1,3) = .7, p>.05] ( Figure 2 ). The discrepancy between achromatic and chromatic channel in saccadic suppression has been widely reported. In our experiment, chromatic condition was included as a control to justify the effect of achromatic condition and verify that the absence of saccadic suppression in chromatic condition would not be systematically altered by contrast adaptation. Saccadic suppression was then quantified with Δgain (see Data analysis). The variation of Δgain, plotted in Figure 3 , illustrates how the amplitude of saccadic suppression changed when the gain adjustment mechanism was chromatically or achromatically adapted by greater contrasts. In achromatic condition, Δgain monotonically decreases as the adaptation level rises for all participants, showing that saccadic suppression is inversely correlated with the adaptation level [r(12) = −0.82, p<.001, Kendall’s Tau-b]. On the contrary, only a small amount of gain modulation is observed in chromatic condition. We have noticed a consistent decline from the data of participant ZZY, but no consistent dependence on the adaptation level emerges across participants. The salient disparity is explicitly demonstrated by group means in Figure 4 .
Discussion
In this paper, we psychophysically examined the relationship between contrast adaptation and saccadic suppression. Our result shows that in achromatic condition, saccadic suppression decreases as the contrast adaptation level of visual system increases, while in chromatic condition, saccadic suppression is not significant at all adaptation contrasts and no consistent tendency was induced by increasing adaptation contrast. The result is discussed hereafter.
What does Contrast Adaptation Change?
We used block- and trial-adaptation in the adapt-test paradigm to manipulate contrast adaptation state like earlier adaptation studies [21], [22], [23]. The prominent function of contrast adaptation is to maximize perceptual power across prevailing contrasts. Psychophysical studies have found that contrast threshold was uniformly elevated by stronger contrast adaptation, although consistent enhancement of contrast discrimination sensitivity around adaptation contrast was not detected [23], [24], [25]. However, it was suggested that test conditions may be responsible for the contradictory results [23], [24], meanwhile studies showing enhanced contrast discrimination sensitivity did use similar stimulus arrangements to perform contrast adaptation operation as we did [25], [26]. Reports from single cell recording experiments also unveiled the neural mechanism of contrast adaptation by describing a wide variety of contrast-induced gain changes [22], [27], [28], [29]. As shown in Figure 2 , fixation and saccade contrast thresholds were elevated as adaptation contrast increased, implying that contrast gain was changed by contrast adaptation operation so that fixation and saccade tasks were performed with the same baseline of contrast gain in a single block, i.e., contrast thresholds of saccade and fixation were tested based on the same population contrast response property. The difference between the two thresholds, i.e. saccadic suppression, could then be regarded as the influence posed on contrast gain by saccade. Thus the interaction between saccadic suppression and contrast adaptation can be interpreted like this: saccadic suppression is produced on the basis of different contrast gain baselines that have been modified by contrast adaptation.
How is Saccadic Suppression Affected?
Contrast adaptation has not been used before to evaluate the involvement of contrast gain control in saccadic suppression. We observed negative correlation between contrast adaptation and saccadic suppression in achromatic condition. This can be interpreted in terms of contrast gain as follows: After adaptation, contrast response function (CRF) was shifted by ambient stimulus contrasts, accompanied by the change of contrast threshold. In our experiment the contrast threshold of fixation was elevated by greater adaptation, however, it grew slower as gain modulation was getting stronger ( Figure 2 , fixation contrast threshold), implying that the limited capacity of gain modulation was further restricted by greater contrast adaptation. Since saccade shared the identical gain with fixation, less room was left for saccadic modulation thereafter (i.e. saccadic suppression became smaller). Moreover, perceptually, greater suppression power should be devoted into lower contrast range since lower contrast information is dominant in natural environment [30], implying again why it is a negative, not positive correlation between contrast adaptation and saccadic suppression. Our results therefore indicate that saccadic suppression has a mechanism that can be mediated by contrast gain control.
Although our experiment gave direct evidence for the correlation between saccadic suppression and contrast gain control, we deliberately avoid further expanding details of the relationship for two reasons. First, contrast gain control mechanism in itself is a big concept which can be categorized by its temporal profile, influence on CRF and underlying physiological mechanism [31]. It was originally composed in electrophysiological studies [21], [22], making it difficult to be evaluated similarly at behavioral level. Second, saccadic suppression also includes contributions from many factors [32], visual masking, motion blur and corollary discharge, active or passive, retinal or non-retinal, as we have known. Without exquisite experiment design and robust techniques to identify components of saccadic suppression, it is not convincing to make direct connections between different contrast gain control mechanisms and saccadic suppression of diverse origins, if ever existed. For example, presenting brief test stimulus slightly before saccade onset was regarded as an acceptable estimation of active (or non-retinal) saccadic suppression, combining careful control of retinal smear and visual masking by designing a horizontal saccade across a parallel grating on a uniform gray background [1], [33]. Our experiment was conceived and conducted with the same stimulus arrangement. The objective of control measures, first, was to ensure that test grating patch was presented at the identical retinal position under fixation and saccade and in the time interval closest to the strongest suppression, and then, to conveniently test mainly active saccadic suppression. In sum, due to the limitation of psychophysical method and experiment design, our experiment is not qualified to elaborate the relationship between saccadic suppression and contrast gain control.
Combining paradigms of contrast adaptation and saccadic suppression, our study extends the knowledge of contrast gain control mechanism in saccadic suppression. However, we cannot conclude that saccadic suppression is achieved by contrast gain control, which requires detailed examinations along the visual pathway using electrophysiological method. Experimental and theoretical evidences are still needed to answer whether and how contrast gain control could be physiologically related to different contributions of saccadic suppression.
Acknowledgments
We thank YZ Hu for his suggestions on the experiment design and H Tan for his technical assistant on coding, apparatus installment and meaningful discussion on data analysis. We also appreciate critical comments from anonymous reviewers.
Funding Statement
This research was supported by the Strategic Priority Research Program of the Chinese Academy of Science XDB02020000, 973 program 2012CB825500, 2012CBA01304 and 2011CB707800, the 863 program 2012AA020701, KSCX2-EW-J-23 and KSCX2-EW-R-11, the National Science Foundation of China NSFC 91332120, 31271167, 31070963, 31271168, 81271495 and 31070965. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Diamond MR, Ross J, Morrone MC (2000) Extraretinal control of saccadic suppression. J Neurosci 20: 3449–3455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Macknik SL (2006) Visual masking approaches to visual awareness. Visual Perception, Pt 2: Fundamentals of Awareness: Multi-Sensory Integration and High-Order Perception 155: 177–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Chen S, Bedell HE, Ogmen H (1995) A target in real motion appears blurred in the absence of other proximal moving targets. Vision Res 35: 2315–2328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Ross J, Morrone MC, Goldberg ME, Burr DC (2001) Changes in visual perception at the time of saccades. Trends in Neurosciences 24: 113–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Reppas JB, Usrey WM, Reid RC (2002) Saccadic eye movements modulate visual responses in the lateral geniculate nucleus. Neuron 35: 961–974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kagan I, Gur M, Snodderly DM (2008) Saccades and drifts differentially modulate neuronal activity in V1: effects of retinal image motion, position, and extraretinal influences. J Vis 8: 19 11–25. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 7. Richmond BJ, Wurtz RH (1980) Vision during saccadic eye movements. II. A corollary discharge to monkey superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol 43: 1156–1167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Bremmer F, Kubischik M, Hoffmann KP, Krekelberg B (2009) Neural dynamics of saccadic suppression. J Neurosci 29: 12374–12383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Ibbotson MR, Crowder NA, Cloherty SL, Price NS, Mustari MJ (2008) Saccadic modulation of neural responses: possible roles in saccadic suppression, enhancement, and time compression. J Neurosci 28: 10952–10960. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Nakamura K, Colby CL (2000) Visual, saccade-related, and cognitive activation of single neurons in monkey extrastriate area V3A. J Neurophysiol 84: 677–692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Burr DC, Morrone MC, Ross J (1994) Selective suppression of the magnocellular visual pathway during saccadic eye movements. Nature 371: 511–513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Burr DC, Morrone C (1996) Temporal Impulse Response Functions for Luminance and Colour During Saccades. Vision Research 36: 2069–2078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Guez J, Morris AP, Krekelberg B (2013) Intrasaccadic suppression is dominated by reduced detector gain. Journal of Vision 13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 14. Watson T, Krekelberg B (2011) An equivalent noise investigation of saccadic suppression. J Neurosci 31: 6535–6541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Bruno A, Brambati SM, Perani D, Morrone MC (2006) Development of saccadic suppression in children. J Neurophysiol 96: 1011–1017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Watson AB, Pelli DG (1983) QUEST: a Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Percept Psychophys 33: 113–120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Wang P, Nikolic D (2011) An LCD Monitor with Sufficiently Precise Timing for Research in Vision. Front Hum Neurosci 5: 85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Brainard DH (1997) The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spat Vis 10: 433–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Cornelissen FW, Peters EM, Palmer J (2002) The Eyelink Toolbox: eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 34: 613–617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10: 437–442. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Ohzawa I, Sclar G, Freeman RD (1985) Contrast gain control in the cat’s visual system. J Neurophysiol 54: 651–667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Ohzawa I, Sclar G, Freeman RD (1982) Contrast gain control in the cat visual cortex. Nature 298: 266–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Maattanen LM, Koenderink JJ (1991) Contrast adaptation and contrast gain control. Exp Brain Res 87: 205–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Abbonizio G, Langley K, Clifford CWG (2002) Contrast adaptation may enhance contrast discrimination. Spatial Vision 16: 45–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Greenlee MW, Heitger F (1988) The functional role of contrast adaptation. Vision Res 28: 791–797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Greenlee MW, Georgeson MA, Magnussen S, Harris JP (1991) The Time Course of Adaptation to Spatial Contrast. Vision Research 31: 223–236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Sclar G, Ohzawa I, Freeman RD (1985) Contrast gain control in the kitten’s visual system. J Neurophysiol 54: 668–675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Hu M, Wang Y (2011) Rapid dynamics of contrast responses in the cat primary visual cortex. Plos One 6: e25410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Albrecht DG, Geisler WS, Frazor RA, Crane AM (2002) Visual cortex neurons of monkeys and cats: temporal dynamics of the contrast response function. J Neurophysiol 88: 888–913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Balboa RM, Grzywacz NM (2003) Power spectra and distribution of contrasts of natural images from different habitats. Vision Research 43: 2527–2537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Kohn A (2007) Visual adaptation: physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits. J Neurophysiol 97: 3155–3164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Wurtz RH, Joiner WM, Berman RA (2011) Neuronal mechanisms for visual stability: progress and problems. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366: 492–503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Brooks BA, Fuchs AF (1975) Influence of stimulus parameters on visual sensitivity during saccadic eye movement. Vision Research 15: 1389–1398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]