Abstract
Motion sickness is a complex condition that includes both overt signs (e.g., vomiting) and more covert symptoms (e.g., anxiety and foreboding). The neural pathways that mediate these signs and symptoms are yet to identified. This study mapped the distribution of c-fos protein (Fos)-like immunoreactivity elicited during a galvanic vestibular stimulation paradigm that is known to induce motion sickness in felines. A principal components analysis was used to identify networks of neurons activated during this stimulus paradigm from functional correlations between Fos labeling in different nuclei. This analysis identified five principal components (neural networks) that accounted for greater than 95% of the variance in Fos labeling. Two of the components were correlated with the severity of motion sickness symptoms, and likely participated in generating the overt signs of the condition. One of these networks included neurons in locus coeruleus, medial, inferior and lateral vestibular nuclei, lateral nucleus tractus solitarius, medial parabrachial nucleus and periaqueductal gray. The second included neurons in the superior vestibular nucleus, precerebellar nuclei, periaqueductal gray, and parabrachial nuclei, with weaker associations of raphe nuclei. Three additional components (networks) were also identified that were not correlated with the severity of motion sickness symptoms. These networks likely mediated the covert aspects of motion sickness, such as affective components. The identification of five statistically independent component networks associated with the development of motion sickness provides an opportunity to consider, in network activation dimensions, the complex progression of signs and symptoms that are precipitated in provocative environments. Similar methodology can be used to parse the neural networks that mediate other complex responses to environmental stimuli.
Introduction
Vomiting is usually considered to be a protective reflex to rid the body of ingested toxins. However, this response is also elicited following surgery or exposure to radiation, during cancer chemotherapy or pregnancy, and even as a consequence of some psychological stimuli [1], [2]. Vestibular stimulation can also result in emesis, particularly during conditions where sensory inputs provide contradictory information regarding body position in space [3], [4]. It is generally assumed that emesis, despite its triggering mechanism, is mediated through a “final common pathway” [4]–[8]. The same output pathways that produce vomiting in response to toxins are thus also presumably involved in generating motion sickness-related emesis. One strong piece of evidence to support the final common pathway hypothesis is the existence of broad-spectrum antiemetics, such as neurokinin-1 (NK1) receptor antagonists, that prevent vomiting despite the provocation [9]–[15]. NK1 receptor antagonists are effective in a variety of species, including humans, musk shrews, ferrets, dogs, and cats, suggesting that the neural pathways that produce vomiting are similar across emetic animals. However, many animals, including the most commonly used species in biomedical research (rodents and rabbits), lack the capacity to vomit [16]. This is due both to reduced muscularity of the diaphragm and a stomach geometry that is not well structured for moving contents towards the esophagus in non-emetic animals. In addition, the brainstem circuitry that regulates the respiratory muscle contractions that result in vomiting differs between emetic and non-emetic animals [16], [17].
The sensation of nausea usually precedes vomiting, and is complex [4], as it includes epigastric awareness and discomfort along with anxiety and foreboding regarding the emesis that could ensue [18]–[20]. Some of the symptoms that occur during motion sickness, such as pallor and cold sweating, have been associated with the stress accompanying the condition [5]. Studies conducted in a variety of animal species have attempted to determine the brain regions that mediate nausea and vomiting by mapping the distribution of c-fos protein (Fos)-like immunoreactivity elicited during this behavior [21]–[28]. c-fos is an immediate-early gene that is rapidly expressed in response to neuronal activation. After being synthesized in the cytoplasm, Fos is quickly translocated to the nucleus where, with the Jun protein, it forms a heterodimer that regulates the expression of other genes [29], [30]. As such, Fos expression indicates that a neuron is activated during a particular response. However, most previous experiments that included emetic stimuli only considered Fos distribution in a restricted region of the brainstem such as nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS) [26], [28], [31] or circumscribed areas of the caudal medulla [21], [23], [24], [27]. Furthermore, only two studies have mapped Fos expression during motion sickness [23], [24]. Both studies were conducted on shrews, which were placed on a tabletop shaker to stimulate the vestibular system, and limited the mapping of Fos to NTS and the adjacent reticular formation.
The goal of the present experiment was to perform a comprehensive analysis of the distribution of Fos immunoreactivity during vestibular-elicited nausea and emesis. Felines were used as the model animal during these studies, since most neurophysiological experiments probing the neural mechanisms that produce vomiting have been conducted in cats [2], [27], [32]–[53]. The cat is a species for which there is extensive background information regarding the vestibular and respiratory control systems [54]–[58]. There are limited data about either the vestibular system or respiratory regulation in other emetic animals, including dogs, shrews and ferrets. Although nonhuman primates have been extensively employed in studies of the vestibular system, little work in these animals has addressed the neural mechanisms that regulate respiratory muscle contractions. Consequently, cats were the most appropriate emetic animals to be employed in these experiments.
To evoke motion sickness, 90° out-of-phase galvanic stimulation of the two labyrinths was delivered [59], [60]. Prior studies have shown that this regimen effectively produces emesis and related prodromal symptoms in a subset of animals by generating a novel pattern of vestibular inputs. However, unlike the complex motion stimuli required to evoke motion sickness [61]–[78], this method does not stimulate nonlabyrinthine receptors [59], [60]. Thus, use of galvanic electrical stimulation in these experiments discounted the possibility that Fos labeling was related to stimulation of receptors whose inputs were unrelated to the generation of motion sickness, such as those activated by fluid shifts in the body. Due to the possibility that different animals might have distinct responses during stimulation, with some focused on the stress component of motion sickness and others focused on epigastric awareness and discomfort, we also considered whether the pattern of Fos immunoreactivity was matched to the particular signs and symptoms that each animal expressed. Furthermore, our initial studies indicated that brainstem regions containing serotoninergic neurons exhibited high levels of Fos immunoreactivity during galvanic vestibular stimulation, so we additionally performed dual-labeling immunohistochemistry to detect Fos and tryptophan hydroxylase-2 (TPH2), the brain-specific isoform of the enzyme responsible for the initial and rate-limiting step in serotonin synthesis [79].
Materials and Methods
All experimental procedures conformed to the National Research Council Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the University of Pittsburgh’s Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. Data were collected from 10 purpose-bred adult cats (Liberty Research, Waverly, NY) of either sex, weighing 2.4 to 4.6 kg at the conclusion of the experiment. Information about the animals is provided in Table 1.
Table 1. Characteristics about the animals used in these experiments, as well as the maximal voltage delivered to the labyrinths to induce motion sickness and the period of acclimation to experimental conditions prior to the stimulation session.
Animal Number | Maximum Stimulation Voltage | Acclimation Period (days) | Weight at Perfusion (kg) | Gender |
C39 | 3 V | 31 | 4.6 | Male |
C52 | 4 V | 34 | 4.1 | Female |
C15 | 3 V | 56 | 4.0 | Male |
C62 | 3 V | 29 | 2.4 | Female |
C64 | 5 V | 44 | 3.3 | Female |
C02 | 5 V | 64 | 2.5 | Female |
C20 | 5 V | 67 | 4.0 | Male |
C41 | 5 V | 30 | 4.2 | Male |
C83 | 0 V | 58 | 4.0 | Male |
C84 | 0 V | 66 | 3.8 | Male |
Animals were brought to the testing room daily for 29–66 days (median of 50 days), for acclimation to the environment and the investigators. During this period, animals were also gradually conditioned for 90 min of restraint in a vinyl bag that encompassed the limbs and torso. We found that animals adapted more readily to restraint when held by an investigator during the experimental session. The same individual handled a particular animal during the course of the experiment, from initial acclimation through the final session when labyrinthine stimulation and then euthanasia were performed. During each session, the laboratory was dark and quiet to avoid startling the animal. Following daily restraint periods, animals were allowed to play in the testing room and were offered a food reward, to further reinforce that the environment was not threatening.
Surgical Procedures
Midway through the acclimation period, an aseptic surgical procedure was performed in a dedicated operating suite to implant stimulating electrodes adjacent to the labyrinth on each side. During the surgery, animals were initially anesthetized using an intramuscular injection of ketamine (20 mg/kg) and acepromazine (0.2 mg/kg), an endotracheal tube was inserted, and anesthesia was maintained using 1–2% isoflurane vaporized in O2. The tympanic bulla was opened using a ventrolateral approach. A silver ball electrode with a tip diameter of ∼0.6 mm was secured using dental cement to the round window, and a second electrode was attached <5 mm away, adjacent to the promontory of the tympanic cavity. The electrodes were insulated except at the tip and attached to Cooner wire, which was led underneath the skin to the top of the head and soldered to a connector. The connector was subsequently attached to the skull using dental cement. After this surgery, animals received antibiotics (amoxicillin, two 50-mg oral doses per day) for 10 days, and analgesia (fentanyl transdermal system, 25 µg/h; Janssen Pharmaceutical Products, Titusville, NJ) for 72 h. The final stimulation session occurred 7–41 days (median of 19 days) following the surgical implantation of electrodes.
Labyrinthine Stimulation and Euthanasia
During acclimation periods after the implantation of electrodes, the stimulator was attached to the head-mounted connector via a cable, although no voltage was delivered, to mimic conditions during the final stimulation session. In the final session, 0.5 Hz sinusoidal galvanic stimulation was provided through the electrodes to the two labyrinths; the sine waves delivered to the left and right sides were 90° out-of-phase. Voltage sinusoids were generated by a Micro1401 mk 2 data acquisition system controlled by Spike2 version 6 software (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). We began the session by delivering 1 V stimuli, and the intensity was gradually increased over the next few minutes to a level that generated nystagmus and sinusoidal head movements. The maximal voltage employed was 3 V for two animals, 4 V for one animal, and 5 V for four animals (see Table 1). However, 5 V stimuli elicited little perceptible response in two of the animals (C20 and C41). The other two animals (C83 and C84) served as controls, and were connected to the stimulator but no current was delivered through the electrodes. The finding that symptom severity was not correlated with the stimulus intensities delivered is not surprising. It is established that individuals have varying susceptibility for motion sickness, and that motion sickness symptoms vary between individuals [3]–[5].
Throughout the stimulation period, the animals were carefully monitored, and the presence of the following behaviors was scored: sinusoidal head roll, nystagmus, sinusoidal limb movement (periodically observed by unzipping the restraint bag), licking, retching, salivation, sinusoidal pinna movements, panting, defecation, urination, sedation, vocalization, or thrashing in the restraint bag (interpreted as an attempt to escape the stimulus). If the latter two behaviors persisted for more than a few seconds, the stimulus intensity was decreased until they abated. When scoring behaviors, we noted whether they were overt or just weakly perceptible. A semiquantitative, cumulative behavioral score was also generated for each animal by assigning animals with overt symptoms in a particular category a score of 2, and those with weak symptoms a score of 1. These scores were then added for the 13 behaviors monitored, as indicated in Table 2.
Table 2. Behaviors exhibited by animals during galvanic vestibular stimulation: A, sinusoidal head roll at the frequency of the stimulus; B, nystagmus; C, frequent licking; D, retching; E, excessive salivation; F, sinusoidal pinna movement at the frequency of the stimulus; G, vocalization; H, panting; I, thrashing in the restraint bag, presumably as an attempt to escape the stimulus; J, defecation during the stimulation session; K, urination during the stimulation session; L, sinusoidal limb movements at the frequency of the stimulus; M, sedation (sleeping during the majority of the stimulation session).
Animal | Classification(Score sum) | A | B | C | D | E | F | G | H | I | J | K | L | M |
C39 | 1 (20) | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | |||
C52 | 1 (20) | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | + | + | ||
C15 | 1 (12) | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | ++ | |||||||
C62 | 2 (7) | ++ | ++ | ++ | + | |||||||||
C64 | 2 (6) | ++ | + | + | ++ | |||||||||
C02 | 2 (5) | ++ | ++ | + | ||||||||||
C20 | 3 (4) | + | + | ++ | ||||||||||
C41 | 3 (3) | + | ++ | |||||||||||
C83 | 3 (0) | |||||||||||||
C84 | 3 (0) |
The behaviors were graded as either being overt (++) or only weakly perceptible (+). Blank cells indicate that the behavior was not present. Based on these behaviors, we classified the stimulus as being highly effective in generating responses (1), moderately effective in generating responses (2), or ineffective (3). The later category includes two animals (C83 and C84) that served as unstimulated controls. A score sum was generated by assigning a score of 2 to overt symptoms (++), and a score of 1 to weak symptoms (+).
After 90 minutes, stimulation was discontinued, and the animals were released from restraint and allowed to ambulate in the laboratory for 60 minutes, as during the acclimation period, but were not provided food or water. This post-stimulation period allowed for the expression of Fos by neurons that were activated during stimulation. The animals with the most profound responses to stimulation typically remained sedentary during the recovery period. Subsequently, animals were anesthetized using ketamine (15 mg/kg) and acepromazine (1 mg/kg) injected intramuscularly, followed by pentobarbital sodium (40 mg/kg) injected intraperitoneally. After verifying the absence of nociceptive reflexes, the animals were perfused transcardially with 1 liter of heparinized saline followed by 2 liters of 4% paraformaldehyde-lysine-periodate fixative [80].
Tissue Processing
The brainstem was removed, postfixed 1–2 days in 4°C paraformaldehyde-lysine-periodate, and cryoprotected in 30% sucrose in 0.1 M phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) for 2 days. Sections were cut at a thickness of 40 µm using a freezing microtome and collected in six bins of cryoprotectant [81]. Procedures for Fos immunohistochemistry were adapted from a protocol supplied by Dr. Charles Horn [22], [82]. Two wells of sections were rinsed in PBS to remove cryoprotectant, and then a sequence of incubation steps was done in 1% sodium borohydride in PBS, 0.3% hydrogen peroxide in PBS, and 5% normal goat serum (NGS) in PBS containing 0.2% triton X-100 (PBS-TX), with rinses between each step. Subsequently, sections were incubated for 24 h at room temperature with gentle agitation in 1∶5000 rabbit polyclonal anti-Fos antibody (sc-52; Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Santa Cruz, CA) or 1∶1000 mouse monoclonal anti-Fos antibody (sc-8047; Santa Cruz Biotechnology) containing 1% NGS in PBS. Following rinses in PBS, sections were placed in 1∶1000 biotinylated secondary antibody (Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories, West Grove, PA) for 3 h at room temperature with gentle agitation. Sections were then rinsed twice in PBS-TX and once in PBS, and then incubated in ABC reagent (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) for three hours. After rinsing in 0.1 M acetate buffer, sections were placed in 3,3′-diaminobenzidine (5 mg/ml in 175 mM acetate–10 mM imidazole buffer, pH 7.4) with nickel sulfate (25 mg/ml) for 0.5–1 min for the chromogen reaction. Sections were then mounted on gelatin-coated slides, cleared in ascending concentrations of ethanol followed by three changes of xylene, and coverslipped with Cytoseal 60 (VWR Scientific, West Chester, PA) or DPX (Sigma-Aldrich, St Louis, MO). One bin of sections was counterstained with neutral red for identification of cytoarchitecture.
A third bin of tissue from all the cases except the unstimulated controls (animals C83 and C84) was processed for dual localization of Fos and TPH2. As a first step, tissue was processed as described above using mouse monoclonal anti-Fos antibody. After completing the chromagen reaction to visualize Fos as a blue-black reaction product in the cell nuclei, sections were rinsed in PBS, and avidin-biotin immunoperoxidase techniques [83] were used to detect TPH2-containing neurons. A 1∶2000 concentration of rabbit anti-TPH2 antibody (supplied by the laboratory of Dr. Stanley Watson at the University of Michigan) was employed in the analysis; we have previously described the specificity of this antibody in detecting serotoninergic neurons in cats [84]. Subsequently, sections were mounted onto slides and coverslipped as described above.
Tissue Analysis
Following an initial qualitative analysis, 10 or more sections from each animal were selected for quantitative analysis of the distribution of Fos immunoreactivity. The distance of each section anterior (A) or posterior (P) to stereotaxic zero was determined by reference to Berman’s atlas [85]. The following brainstem levels were included in the quantitative analysis: P16, through the commissural nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS); P13.5, through the obex; P12.5, through the rostral NTS; P9.5, through the inferior vestibular nucleus and caudal aspect of the medial vestibular nucleus; P7.5, through the lateral vestibular nucleus; P5.5, through the superior vestibular nucleus; P4, through the parabrachial nucleus; P2, through the caudal aspect of the inferior colliculus; P1, through the caudal periaqueductal gray; A2, through the rostral periaqueductal gray. Sections were photographed using a 4X objective of a Nikon Eclipse E600N photomicroscope equipped with a Spot RT monochrome digital camera (Diagnostic Instruments, Sterling Heights, MI) and MetaMorph imaging software (Universal, Downingtown, PA). Montages of images were assembled using PTGui-Pro photostitching software (New House Internet Services B–V, The Netherlands). These montages, in conjunction with observations of sections at high magnification, were used to generate counts of labeled cells in target areas, as well as plots of the locations of labeled cells. These plots provided the data utilized in subsequent statistical analyses.
When analyzing sections processed for co-localization of Fos and TPH2, we counted the number of single- and double-labeled cells in regions of every section containing TPH2-postive neurons (37–65 sections/animal, median of 48 sections/animal). For this analysis, the following divisions of the raphe nuclei were considered, as defined in a previous manuscript [84]: raphe obscurus, raphe pallidus, raphe magnus, and the medial and lateral regions of the dorsal raphe nucleus.
Statistical Analysis
Principal component analysis, conducted using Systat 11 (Systat Software, Chicago, IL), was used to identify a set of statistically independent (orthogonal) principal components that are sufficient to explain the numbers of Fos labeled cells in 23 sampled nuclear or subnuclear groups. The analysis was performed on the correlation matrix. An equamax rotation was used as a compromise of varimax and quartimax criteria, which minimizes both the number of neural structures (variables) that load highly on a component (network) and the number of components (networks) needed to explain the behavior of a neural structure (variable). The standardized component scores were calculated for each subject.
Results
Table 2 provides details about behaviors exhibited by the animals during the stimulation session. Throughout the period of galvanic vestibular stimulation, three of the animals (C39, C52, C15) displayed motor responses characteristic of activation of labyrinthine receptors (sinusoidal head movement at the frequency of the stimulus and nystagmus), as well as prodromal signs of vomiting (periods of retching, copious salivation, frequent licking, panting). At the end of the session, two of the three animals were found to have defecated in the restraint bag; the watery diarrhea was suggestive of an acute stress effect on gastrointestinal motility [86]. The cumulative behavioral score for these three animals ranged from 12–20 (see Table 2), and they were classified as having strong autonomic responses to labyrinthine stimulation (response type 1). Three other animals (C62, C64, C02) exhibited overt behaviors indicating that the vestibular system was activated by the stimulus, particularly sinusoidal head roll at the frequency of the stimulus, although only one or two potential indicators of motion sickness were evident for each animal (see Table 2). These three animals had behavioral scores of 5–7, and were classified as having response type 2. Two other animals (C20 and C41) had little motor response to galvanic vestibular stimulation, although both animals slept throughout most of the stimulation session; their somnolence could have been a consequence of the stimulus. These two animals were grouped with two control animals that were not stimulated as having response type 3. The cumulative behavioral scores for type 3 animals ranged from 0–4.
Expression of Fos by Brainstem Neurons Following Galvanic Vestibular Stimulation
Maps of the locations of Fos-labeled neurons in animals with a strong autonomic response are provided in Fig. 1, whereas Fig. 2 shows the locations of Fos labeling in response type 3 animals. Photomicrographs of Fos-labeled neurons from a response type 1 animal are provided in Fig. 3. Fig. S1 also allows for a comparison of Fos labeling in several brainstem regions containing serotoninergic neurons in animal C62. Following a qualitative review of the sections, counts of the number of Fos-immunopositive neurons were obtained from the following areas having a high density of labeling in a subset of animals: inferior vestibular nucleus, medial vestibular nucleus (caudal and rostral aspects), lateral vestibular nucleus (dorsal and ventral divisions), superior vestibular nucleus (lateral and medial aspects), NTS (lateral, medial, and commissural nuclei), periaqueductal gray (dorsal, lateral, ventral, and ventrolateral divisions), parabrachial nucleus complex (medial and lateral parabrachial nuclei and the adjacent Kölliker-Fuse nucleus), locus coeruleus, the subtrigeminal nucleus (region ventral to the spinal trigeminal nucleus), the external cuneate nucleus, and the subdivisions of the raphe nuclei. Because there was no significant difference between the number of labeled cells in any structure on the left versus right side, the total number of labeled cells was used for principal component analysis. Since the raphe nuclei contained many Fos-immunopositive neurons in some animals, we performed dual-labeling immunohistochemistry on an additional well of tissue from all animals except the unstimulated controls to co-localize Fos and TPH2; examples of dual-labeled cells are illustrated in Fig. 4, as well as Fig. S1.
Correlations between Fos Labeling and Symptoms
The numbers of Fos labeled neurons in a subset of the sampled brain regions showed a strong positive correlation with both the total symptom severity score and a subscore for autonomic manifestations of motion sickness (subscore for frequent licking, retching, excessive salivation, vocalization, panting, defecation during the stimulation session, urination during the stimulation session, and sedation [sleeping during the majority of the stimulation session]) (Table 3). The positive correlation was strong in the medial, lateral and inferior vestibular nuclei, but a weak negative correlation was present in the superior vestibular nucleus. The correlation was also strong in medial and lateral subnuclei of the solitary nucleus, but not significant in the commissural solitary nucleus. Strong positive correlations were also observed with Fos labeling in locus coeruleus, Kölliker-Fuse nucleus, non-serotonergic cells (i.e., cells that were not immunopositive for TPH2) in the nucleus raphe magnus and the medial and lateral parabrachial nuclei, but not in the serotonergic (TPH2-immunopositive) cells in the raphe nuclear groups, subtrigeminal nucleus, or any division of the periaqueductal gray. There was a weak negative correlation between the number of labeled cells and symptom severity in the dorsal raphe nucleus and the raphe pallidus et obscurus. Stepwise multiple regression (removal criterion p = 0.15) indicated that the motion sickness symptom score could be represented (adjusted multiple r-squared = 0.653) as a weighted sum of the number of labeled cells in lateral subnucleus of the nucleus of the solitary tract (coefficient: 0.123), subtrigeminal nucleus (coefficient: 0.143) and external cuneate nucleus (coefficient: 0.033) plus a constant term (−1.567). The subscores for autonomic manifestations of motion sickness showed a more robust regression result (adjusted multiple r-squared = 0.951) as a weighted sum of the number of labeled cells in the Kölliker-Fuse nucleus (coefficient: 0.119) and subtrigeminal nucleus (coefficient: −0.088) plus a constant term (−0.398).
Table 3. Correlation of labeling with symptom score and component loadings from principal component analysis with equamax rotation.
Structure | Correlation re: Signs,Total (Autonomic only) | Component 1 | Component 2 | Component 3 | Component 4 |
Lateral Vestibular N. | 0.879 (0.886) | 0.901 | 0.305 | 0.074 | 0.297 |
Medial Vestibular N. | 0.715 (0.680) | 0.926 | 0.287 | 0.201 | 0.136 |
Locus Coeruleus | 0.874 (0.844) | 0.861 | 0.248 | 0.084 | 0.434 |
Periaqueductal N. | 0.145 (0.056) | 0.856 | 0.091 | −0.504 | −0.001 |
Nucleus Tractus Solitarius | 0.647 (0.600) | 0.687 | 0.698 | 0.134 | 0.001 |
Inferior Vestibular N. | 0.784 (0.789) | 0.672 | 0.545 | 0.076 | 0.493 |
Medial Parabrachial N. | 0.710 (0.702) | 0.636 | 0.440 | −0.255 | 0.580 |
Kölliker-Fuse N. | 0.901 (0.947) | 0.538 | 0.630 | 0.309 | 0.447 |
Lateral Parabrachial N. | 0.785 (0.786) | 0.006 | 0.967 | 0.213 | 0.132 |
Dorsal Raphe N. (TPH2) | −0.395 (−0.430) | −0.359 | −0.612 | 0.422 | −0.517 |
Superior Vestibular N. | 0.487 (0.476) | −0.068 | −0.329 | −0.815 | −0.468 |
External Cuneate | 0.392 (0.371) | −0.060 | −0.260 | −0.951 | 0.075 |
Subtrigeminal N. | 0.450 (0.500) | −0.020 | 0.334 | −0.732 | 0.468 |
Dorsal Raphe N. (non-TPH2) | −0.23 (−0.316) | −0.015 | −0.029 | 0.007 | −0.990 |
% Variance explained | 34.99% | 22.98% | 20.15% | 19.87% | |
r2 of Score re: Signs | 0.41 (0.29) | 0.26 (0.29) | 0.26 (0.31) | 0.05 (0.11) |
Bold type is used to highlight regions with strong negative or positive correlations between the number of Fos-immunopositive neurons and symptom severity scores. Bold type also designates large positive component loadings; bold and italicized type designates negative component loadings. TPH2: Tryptophan hydroxylase 2, a marker of brain serotoninergic neurons.
Principal Component Analysis: Identifying Connected Network-like Behavior from Fos Data
The range of behavioral responses provides an opportunity for identifying interconnected neural response networks from functional correlations between Fos labeling in different nuclei in the animals. The relationships between labeling across nuclei were identified with an approach from principal component analysis, a multivariate technique that was developed initially by Hotelling [87] to identify a smaller number of independent variables that can determine the values of observations from a larger number of correlated independent variables. From a practical perspective, it provides criteria to identify statistically independent, linear combinations (principal components) of the measured variables that explain the majority of the variance in the data [88]. Network contributions of different nuclei can then be identified by shared, high loadings on a particular component.
Results of principal component analysis of data from twelve nuclear groups, plus serotonergic and non-serotonergic dorsal raphe nucleus cells, are summarized in Table 3. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin statistic of 0.74 and significant Bartlett’s test for sphericity (p<0.05) indicated that the sample was adequate for the principal component analysis [89]. Four orthogonal principal components (eigenvalues: 8.062, 2.924, 1.724, and 1.009) were identified. These statistically independent components are expressed as component loadings for each of the brain regions; the loadings represent the relationship (on a range between −1 and 1) between the component and the normalized (z-transformed) labeling in that region. The polarities are arbitrary; opposite polarities indicate a ‘push-pull’ relationship. For a given factor, large magnitude loadings for different nuclei represent the strength of the linear relationship of Fos labeling to that component. If a group of structures is part of a known connected pathway, then large magnitude loadings across those structures are consistent with engagement of that network.
Component 1 (explains 34.99% of the variance) had large positive loadings for the inferior vestibular nucleus, medial vestibular nucleus, lateral vestibular nucleus, periaqueductal gray, solitary nucleus, locus coeruleus and medial parabrachial nucleus, and a weaker positive loading from the Kölliker-Fuse nucleus. Component 2 included large positive contributions from the solitary nucleus, inferior vestibular nucleus, Kölliker-Fuse nucleus, and lateral parabrachial nucleus, with an opposite polarity contribution from serotonergic dorsal raphe cells. Component 3 had large contributions from the superior vestibular nucleus, periaqueductal gray, external cuneate nucleus and the subtrigeminal nucleus. Finally, Component 4 had a strong contribution from non-serotonergic dorsal raphe neurons, with a lesser contribution from serotonergic dorsal raphe cells and a contribution of opposite polarity from the medial PBN. A principal components analysis that included nuclear subdivision and other raphe nuclei (Table 4) indicated that five principal components (eigenvalues of 12.263, 6.011, 3.81, 2.61 and 1.307) could account for greater than 95% of the variance in Fos labeling. Four of the components correspond to the results of the analysis in Table 3 and have been named identically.
Table 4. Correlation of labeling with symptom score and component loadings from principal component analysis with equamax rotation, with nuclear subdivisions included.
Structure | Correlation re:Symptoms, Total(Autonomic only) | Component 1 | Component 2 | Component 2a | Component 3 | Component 4 |
Dorsal Lateral Vestibular N. | 0.742 (0.757) | 0.934 | 0.184 | 0.12 | 0.261 | −0.108 |
Rostral Medial Vestibular N. | 0.726 (0.673) | 0.929 | 0.176 | 0.269 | 0.154 | −0.099 |
Caudal Medial Vestibular N. | 0.687 (0.662) | 0.872 | 0.346 | 0.321 | −0.081 | 0.096 |
Locus Coeruleus | 0.874 (0.844) | 0.85 | 0.292 | 0.25 | 0.135 | 0.334 |
Lateral Periaqueductal Gray | 0.202 (0.120) | 0.837 | 0.019 | −0.095 | 0.537 | 0.031 |
Ventral Lateral Vestibular N. | 0.780 (0.773) | 0.826 | 0.361 | 0.321 | 0.087 | 0.278 |
Lateral Nucleus Tractus Solitarius | 0.652 (0.601) | 0.764 | 0.55 | 0.16 | −0.203 | 0.219 |
Dorsal Periaqueductal Gray | 0.082 (−0.009) | 0.734 | 0.048 | −0.022 | 0.102 | −0.67 |
Ventrolateral Periaqueductal Gray | 0.115 (0.074) | 0.738 | 0.101 | 0.127 | 0.65 | 0.083 |
Inferior Vestibular N. | 0.784 (0.789) | 0.635 | 0.527 | 0.398 | 0.241 | 0.321 |
Ventral Periaqueductal Gray | 0.079 (−0.129) | 0.614 | 0.007 | −0.409 | 0.674 | −0.034 |
Medial Parabrachial N. | 0.710 (0.702) | 0.606 | 0.428 | 0.185 | 0.509 | 0.395 |
Kölliker−Fuse N. | 0.901 (0.947) | 0.491 | 0.584 | 0.58 | 0.067 | 0.276 |
Lateral Parabrachial N. | 0.785 (0.786) | −0.056 | 0.929 | 0.363 | 0.039 | 0.005 |
Raphe Magnus (non-TPH2) | 0.492 (0.467) | 0.171 | 0.894 | 0.3 | −0.253 | 0.137 |
Medial Nucleus Tractus Solitarius | 0.652 (0.635) | 0.27 | 0.873 | 0.128 | 0.273 | 0.271 |
Raphe Magnus (TPH2) | 0.096 (0.066) | −0.167 | 0.751 | 0.28 | −0.079 | −0.569 |
Dorsal Raphe Nucleus (TPH2) | −0.395 (−0.43) | −0.331 | −0.641 | 0.049 | −0.585 | −0.367 |
Raphe Pallidus/Obscurus (TPH2) | −0.359 (−0.487) | 0.085 | −0.262 | −0.951 | −0.102 | 0.097 |
Raphe Pallidus/Obscurus (non−TPH2) | −0.450 (−0.538) | −0.432 | 0.081 | −0.753 | 0.489 | 0.019 |
Medial Superior Vestibular N. | 0.502 (0.495) | −0.029 | −0.428 | −0.689 | 0.108 | −0.575 |
External Cuneate | 0.392 (0.371) | −0.044 | −0.266 | −0.611 | 0.744 | 0.029 |
Lateral Superior Vestibular N. | 0.454 (0.442) | −0.079 | −0.253 | −0.545 | 0.733 | −0.308 |
Subtrigeminal N. | 0.450 (0.500) | −0.077 | 0.118 | 0.157 | 0.964 | 0.161 |
Commissural Nucleus Tractus Solitarius | −0.051 (−0.100) | 0.062 | −0.037 | 0.07 | 0.075 | −0.992 |
Dorsal Raphe N. (non-TPH2) | −0.230 (−0.316) | −0.042 | −0.104 | −0.15 | −0.25 | −0.95 |
Bold type is used to highlight regions with strong negative or positive correlations between the number of Fos-immunopositive neurons and symptom severity scores. Bold type also designates large positive component loadings; bold and italicized type designates negative component loadings. TPH2: Tryptophan hydroxylase 2, a marker of brain serotoninergic neurons.
The first component (Component 1 in Table 4) corresponds to an anatomical refinement of Component 1 from the initial analysis (Table 3). Large positive loadings were observed for most vestibular nucleus divisions (except the superior vestibular nucleus), the lateral subnucleus of the solitary nucleus, medial parabrachial and Kölliker-Fuse nuclei and (in decreasing order) the dorsal, lateral, ventrolateral and ventral divisions of the periaqueductal gray. It also has very high loading for the locus coeruleus, but very low loadings for the dorsal raphe and nucleus raphe magnus. It accounts for 31.2% of the between-animal variance in the data. The normalized component scores showed a positive correlation (r2 = 0.387) with the cumulative behavioral scores and with the cumulative autonomic subscores (r2 = 0.53).
Component 2 in Table 4 corresponds to a refinement of Component 2 from the initial analysis presented in Table 3. It reflects push-pull interactions between two subnetworks. One subnetwork reflects strong factor loadings with Fos expression in the lateral parabrachial nucleus, medial subnucleus of the solitary tract, and nucleus raphe magnus (both serotonergic and non-serotonergic cells) as well as more moderate loadings from the Kölliker-Fuse nucleus, inferior vestibular nucleus, and lateral subnucleus of the solitary tract. A contribution of opposite polarity comes from serotonergic cells in the dorsal raphe nucleus. Component 2 accounts for 20.5% of the between-animal variance in the data. The normalized component scores were uncorrelated (r2 = 0.141) with the cumulative behavioral scores and with the autonomic subscores (r2 = 0.018).
Component 2a reflects a moderate positive loading from the Kölliker-Fuse nucleus, with contributions of opposite polarity from a network that includes strong influences of the serotonergic and non-serotonergic cells in the nuclei raphe pallidus et obscurus and the medial aspect of the superior vestibular nucleus, with more moderate contributions from the lateral aspect of the superior vestibular nucleus and external cuneate nucleus. It accounts for 15.7% of the variance. The normalized component scores were uncorrelated (r2 = 0.04) with the cumulative behavioral scores and with the autonomic subscores (r2 = 0.002). It appears to have been embedded in components 2–4 of the initial analysis, primarily by the pooling of the medial and lateral superior vestibular nucleus.
Component 3 in Table 4 corresponds to an anatomical refinement of component 3 from the initial analysis. It has large positive loadings for the subtrigeminal nucleus, lateral aspect of the superior vestibular nucleus, external cuneate nucleus, and the ventral and ventrolateral divisions of the periaqueductal gray, and moderate loadings from the medial parabrachial nucleus, lateral subnucleus of the periaqueductal gray and non-serotonergic neurons in nuclei raphe pallidus et obscurus. The serotonergic dorsal raphe neurons have a relatively strong loading of opposite polarity. Factor 4 accounts for 17.3% of the between-animal variance. Normalized component scores showed a positive correlation (r2 = 0.35) with the cumulative behavioral scores and with the autonomic subscores (r2 = 0.325).
Finally, Component 4 in Table 4, an anatomical refinement of Component 4 of the initial analysis, has high loadings (negative polarity) for non-serotonergic cells in the dorsal raphe nucleus, commissural subnucleus of the solitary nucleus, and the dorsal region of the periaqueductal gray, with more moderate loadings for the medial aspect of the superior vestibular nucleus (the terminus of periventricular plexus (small caliber) non-serotonergic dorsal raphe axons) and serotonergic cells in nucleus raphe magnus. Component 5 accounts for 15.2% of the between-animal variance. The normalized component scores among animals were uncorrelated (r2 = 0.080) with the cumulative behavioral scores and with the autonomic subscores (r2 = 0.125).
Discussion
The nuclear expression of the early immediate gene transcription factor Fos has been used for several decades to identify neurons affected by a variety of peripheral and central stimuli [29], [90]. For example, Fos expression patterns can be used as a marker of anatomically and physiologically identified pain and autonomic pathways in different contexts [91]–[95]. The brain stem structures that were selected for Fos quantification have been implicated in either (1) vestibular contributions to autonomic, affective and somatic pathways [96], [97], (2) Fos activation by vestibular stimulation [98] or (3) generation of nausea, emesis and responses to noxious stimulation [22], [27], [91], [92], and include both the raphe nuclei and locus coeruleus. These structures have also shown Fos activation after both natural vestibular stimulation [99], [100] and acute unilateral vestibular damage [101].
Principal component analysis is an exploratory multivariate statistical approach to identify a reduced set of orthogonal principal components that account for the variability in a larger data set of many measured variables [88], [102]. This approach was pioneered in the early 1930s by Hotelling [87]. From a mathematical perspective, principal components are the characteristic vectors of the covariance or the correlation matrix of a data set. Principal components are linear combinations of measured variables that have large variances and, therefore, can account for the variation in the data in terms of positive and negative interactions among the measurements. Hence, the approach is suited well for identifying presumptive interactions between groups of neurons (measured variables such as cFos labeling) contributing to functional pathways (principal components).
The experiment was designed explicitly to facilitate the use of principal component analysis for identifying functional pathways on the basis of synchronous Fos expression during the generation of behaviors culminating in motion sickness. Firstly, existing knowledge regarding vestibular and autonomic pathways permits ‘sense-making’ by associating the identified principal components with neuronal networks. By sampling cFos labeling from nuclei that contribute to known networks for processing vestibular and autonomic information, the contributions of neuronal populations to individual principal components can be interpreted in terms of network activity. Secondly, because the data set consisted of individual experimental animals displaying different degrees (manifestations) of motion sickness, the components across the population provide an estimate of activity in the associated networks across the behavioral response spectrum. Hence, the approach exploits the individual variation in the severity of behavioral motion sickness to identify co-modulated neuronal pathways as principal components that explain underlying variance in the Fos labeling. It is analogous to strategies for identifying synchronous, connected networks from electrophysiological [103] or functional imaging data [104]. Therefore, the five components identified by this approach represent coordinated engagement of pathways along the progression from mild discomfort to emesis.
Components 1 and 4 showed a positive correlation with the cumulative behavioral scores, especially with the autonomic subscore. Hence, they are likely related to the overt signs of motion sickness reflected in the behaviors monitored in this study. Component 1 reflects correlates of changes in vestibular nucleus Fos activation during galvanic vestibular stimulation. The factor is anchored by the strong covariance in Fos activation within locus coeruleus, vestibular nuclei, lateral NTS, medial parabrachial nucleus and periaqueductal gray, and a more moderate relationship with the Kölliker-Fuse nucleus. Because anatomical studies have documented direct, strong interconnections among these structures, the Fos co-regulation represents coordinated engagement of this network across animals with different levels of behavioral responses to galvanic (or sham) stimulation. Locus coeruleus receives direct projections from the vestibular nuclei [105], [106] and its neurons respond to both vestibular and neck stimulation [107]. Locus coeruleus also contributes a regionally specialized projection to the vestibular nuclei [108]. The medial parabrachial nucleus is connected reciprocally with the vestibular nuclei [105], [109]; the lateral NTS receives direct input from the vestibular nuclei [51], [110] and projects to the external medial parabrachial and Kölliker-Fuse nuclei [111]. The periaqueductal gray receives light projections from the vestibular nuclei [112], is connected reciprocally with the locus coeruleus [113], [114], and projects to the medial and lateral parabrachial nuclei [115]. Recent studies in rats indicate that Fos activation in locus coeruleus and the periaqueductal gray is observed in animals displaying escape responses in an elevated T-maze task [116], which may include recruitment of anxiolytic effects of norepinephrine via the dorsal periaqueductal gray [117].
Component 3, like Component 1, identifies correlated Fos labeling in a network involving interconnections between the periaqueductal gray and medial parabrachial nucleus [115]. However, unlike Component 1, it has negligible association with Fos labeling in the lateral, medial and inferior vestibular nuclei. Rather, the labeling is associated with Fos activation in the lateral aspect of the superior vestibular nucleus and two precerebellar regions that are activated by prolonged, natural linear acceleration (otolith organ) stimuli, the subtrigeminal and external cuneate nuclei [98]. The positive association of Fos labeling in non-serotonergic raphe pallidus et obscurus neurons is also of opposite polarity to the contribution to Component 1, and the serotonergic dorsal raphe labeling has a loading similar to Component 2. Thus, this Fos activation may reflect the fact that the lateral aspect of the superior vestibular nucleus receives large caliber serotonergic dorsal raphe nucleus projections, but negligible non-serotonergic inputs from the dorsal raphe nucleus via periventricular plexus [118].
A Fos coactivation of cells in the lateral periaqueductal gray and dorsal raphe nucleus was noted recently in association with avoidance responses to an elevated T-maze task [116] and during defensive behaviors in rodents [119]. Finally, the correlated periaqueductal gray and subtrigeminal nucleus contributions are of interest in light of similar activation in imaging studies during offset analgesia [120]. We suggest that this network is reflects responses to aversive aspects of visuospatial discomfort and anxiety.
Components 2, 2a, and 4 were uncorrelated with the cumulative behavioral scores, and thus were not related to the overt signs of motion sickness. Instead, these factors were likely related to the covert symptoms of motion sickness that are not readily evident (e.g., affective components of the condition). Component 2 reflects interactions of opposite polarity from two interconnected networks related to nucleus raphe magnus and the serotonergic cells in the dorsal raphe nucleus, respectively. Strong positive factor loadings for Fos labeling were identified in a network of structures that are interconnected with serotonergic and non-serotonergic cells of nucleus raphe magnus [121]–[125], including strong loadings for the lateral parabrachial nucleus and medial subnucleus of the solitary tract and more moderate, positive loadings for labeling in the medial parabrachial nucleus, Kölliker-Fuse nucleus, inferior vestibular nucleus, and lateral subnucleus of the solitary tract. This Fos activity likely reflects interconnections between the inferior vestibular nucleus, Kolliker-Fuse nucleus and nuclei of the solitary tract [51], [105], [109], [111], [126]. These interconnections are likely involved in generation of discharges of serotonergic nucleus raphe magnus neurons in association with autonomic and respiratory activity [127]. Contributions of opposite polarity are provided by Fos labeling of serotonergic cells in the dorsal raphe nucleus, and, to a more moderate extent, from the medial aspect of the superior vestibular nucleus, which receives serotonergic input from the dorsal raphe nucleus [118]. Because several structures had significant positive loadings on this component, and nucleus raphe magnus and the medial and lateral subnuclei of the solitary nucleus provide inputs to the dorsal raphe nucleus [128], this component suggests a prominent push-pull interplay between raphe magnus and dorsal raphe circuits in the development of autonomic and respiratory components of nausea and emesis. More significantly, they likely reflect the role of nucleus raphe magnus neurons in descending modulation of sensitivity to aversive visceral and somatic sensory information [129], analogous to its role in maintenance of hyperalgesia and allodynia after nerve injury and inflammation [130].
Component 2a reflects an opposite relationship between Fos activation in the Kölliker-Fuse nucleus and concurrent Fos inhibition in a network that includes serotonergic and non-serotonergic cells in the nuclei raphe pallidus et obscurus, neurons in the superior vestibular nucleus and external cuneate nucleus. Although Fos labeling in each of the latter structures is correlated negatively with symptom score (Table 4), the factor scores were uncorrelated with the magnitude of the symptom score. The relationships between Fos labeling in the caudal raphe nuclei, superior vestibular nucleus and Kölliker-Fuse nucleus in Component 2a are statistically independent of the relationships between the latter structures and more rostral raphe nuclei in Component 2, which is suggestive of different functions. Both the Kölliker-Fuse and superior vestibular nuclei project to raphe pallidus et obscurus [122] and these caudal raphe nuclei contribute serotonergic and non-serotonergic projections to the vestibular nuclei [131]. A recent optogenetic study [132] reported that activation of serotonergic neurons in raphe obscurus increased respiratory amplitude and frequency as well as the sensitivity of central respiratory chemoreflexes. The caudal raphe nuclei have also been implicated as a modulator of other somatosympathetic responses, including cardiovascular sympathoexcitatory reflexes during acupuncture [133]. Hence, the network contributing to this factor may be modulating respiratory and autonomic motor activity associated with the magnitude of development of motion sickness signs.
Component 5 is anchored by high magnitude contributions from Fos labeling in the commissural nucleus tractus solitarii and non-serotonergic dorsal raphe neurons and moderate loadings from the dorsal periaqueductal gray, serotonergic neurons in the nucleus raphe magnus and the medial aspect of the superior vestibular nucleus. This activity may reflect a network anchored by commissural nucleus tractus solitarii projections to the dorsal raphe nucleus [128] and non-serotonergic dorsal raphe projections to the medial aspect of the superior vestibular nucleus [118]. The serotonergic raphe magnus contribution may also reflect its afferent relations with the periaqueductal gray [134] and strong projections to nucleus tractus solitarii [123]. Because activation the dorsal periaqueductal gray contributes to aversion and anxiety-like responses [135], we suggest this network is involved in generation of integrated vestibular-visceral aversive signals related to motion sickness.
The principal component analysis approach also highlighted several striking global relationships for Fos activation patterns in the locus coeruleus (noradrenergic), and the serotonergic and non-serotonergic cell groups in the dorsal raphe nucleus, nucleus raphe magnus and nuclei raphe pallidus et obscurus. Specifically, different combinations of these groups were associated with different statistically independent components. Firstly, locus coeruleus Fos labeling patterns are independent statistically from Fos labeling of serotonergic neurons in the dorsal raphe, raphe magnus and raphe pallidus et obscurus. Secondly, the Fos labeling of serotonergic and non-serotonergic dorsal raphe nucleus cells tend to behave independently, which suggests that their differential projections to the vestibular nuclei and ascending pathways [118] are associated with different roles in responses to galvanic stimulation leading to autonomic arousal. Thirdly, reciprocal (push-pull) relationships between these different cell groups were prominent in three of the factors: locus coeruleus versus nuclei raphe pallidus et obscurus for Component 1, serotonergic and non-serotonergic nucleus raphe magnus neurons versus serotonergic dorsal raphe nucleus neurons for Component 2, and serotonergic dorsal raphe neurons versus non-serotonergic raphe pallidus et obscurus neurons on Component 4. Finally, Component 5 featured parallel Fos labeling of serotonergic nucleus raphe magnus and non-serotonergic dorsal raphe nucleus neurons.
Summary and Conclusions
The identification of five statistically independent component networks associated with the development of motion sickness provides an opportunity to consider, in network activation dimensions, the complex progression of signs and symptoms that are precipitated in provocative environments (both real and virtual). The inability of numerous studies to identify autonomic pathognomonic patterns during the development of motion sickness [136] leave us to rely on subjective symptoms and symptom clusters. Hence, it will be important to determine the relationships between temporal patterns of activity in these networks and the behavioral and perceptual dimensions used to assess the severity of motion sickness. In essence, this approach may allow us to formulate motion sickness in terms of General Recognition Theory [137], by framing the component networks as orthogonal dimensions (processes) that produce progressive, perceptually independent clusters of signs and symptoms.
Schemata for the dimensions underlying subjective signs and symptoms of motion sickness have been derived from multivariate analysis of responses to questionnaires [138], [139]. For example, the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ) scoring metric was constructed from a principal component analysis of the original Pensacola Motion Sickness Questionnaire [138]. The authors identified orthogonal dimensions that they termed nausea (and/or gastrointestinal awareness), vision and visuomotor (eyestrain) function, and disorientation (dizziness, blurred vision and difficulty focusing) which have been used to document psychophysical differences in responses to different visual environments, simulator types, and stimulus patterns in vection drums [140]. A more recent Motion Sickness Assessment Questionnaire (MSAQ) is based upon four factors identified from a broader, two phase exploratory analysis [141]. The MSAQ gastrointestinal factor spans sensations from queasiness to nausea and vomiting. The MSAQ central factor includes dizziness, lightheadedness, disorientation and blurred vision. The MSAQ peripheral factor includes reports associated with autonomic responses such as sweatiness, clamminess, and hot/warm sensations. Finally, the MSAQ sopite-like factor captures diffuse fatigue, affective and emotional components associated with the ‘sopite syndrome’ [142], [143]. Because these questionnaire-based factors are only clusters of correlated symptoms, their relationship to metrics of activation of the networks identified in this study (e.g., from functional imaging studies) has the potential to bring etiologic precision to the diagnosis of prodromal trajectories of motion sickness.
The networks identified in this study include component pathways that have been described in the pain literature as subserving the affective dimension(s) of pain [144], particularly interoception and generation of feelings and emotions associated with activity in nociceptive afferent pathways [145]–[147]. In a broader sense, this overlap of networks for a pain and motion sickness is consistent with their role in core circuitry for determining the aversiveness of sensory patterns [148]. These networks also are likely contributors to the affective and emotional process of ‘being in pain’, which, as noted in 1968 by Melzack and Casey [149] and elaborated later by Grahek [150], can be dissociated from pain sensation and perception. Psychophysical studies have attempted to separate pain sensation and perception from pain aversiveness by asking subjects to rate both intensity and unpleasantness. It is of interest that for visceral and somatic pain stimuli of equal estimated intensity, the visceral pain is judged to be significantly more unpleasant [151]. Hence, activity in the five networks identified in our study may contribute to the aversive aspects of the development of motion sickness.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Lucy Cotter for technical assistance in completing this study. Dr. Stephen Cass assisted with implanting electrodes in the labyrinths. We also thank Dr. Charles Horn (University of Pittsburgh) for providing the protocol for Fos immunohistochemistry, and Dr. Stanley Watson (University of Michigan) for supplying TPH-2 antibody.
Funding Statement
NIH grant R01-DC003732. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Grelot L, Miller A (1994) Vomiting - Its Ins and Outs. News Physiol Sci 9: 142–147. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Miller AD, Grélot L (1996) The neural basis of nausea and vomiting. In: Yates BJ, Miller AD, editors. Vestibular Autonomic Regulation. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. 85–94.
- 3.Money KE, Lackner JR, Cheung RSK (1996) The autonomic nervous system and motion sickness. In: Yates BJ, Miller AD, editors. Vestibular Autonomic Regulation. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. 147–173.
- 4. Yates BJ, Miller AD, Lucot JB (1998) Physiological basis and pharmacology of motion sickness: an update. Brain Res Bull 47: 395–406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Money KE (1970) Motion sickness. Physiol Rev 50: 1–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Miller AD, Leslie RA (1994) The area postrema and vomiting. Front Neuroendocrinol 15: 301–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Money KE, Cheung BS (1983) Another function of the inner ear: facilitation of the emetic response to poisons. Aviation Space Environ Med 54: 208–211. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Treisman M (1977) Motion sickness: an evolutionary hypothesis. Science 197: 493–495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Fukuda H, Koga T, Furukawa N, Nakamura E, Shiroshita Y (1999) The tachykinin NK1 receptor antagonist GR205171 abolishes the retching activity of neurons comprising the central pattern generator for vomiting in dogs. Neurosci Res 33: 25–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Gardner C, Perren M (1998) Inhibition of anaesthetic-induced emesis by a NK1 or 5-HT3 receptor antagonist in the house musk shrew, suncus murinus. Neuropharmacol 37: 1643–1644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Fukuda H, Koga T, Furukawa N, Nakamura E, Shiroshita Y (1998) The tachykinin NK1 receptor antagonist GR205171 prevents vagal stimulation-induced retching but not neuronal transmission from emetic vagal afferents to solitary nucleus neurons in dogs. Brain Res 802: 221–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Gonsalves S, Watson J, Ashton C (1996) Broad spectrum antiemetic effects of CP-122,721, a tachykinin NK1 receptor antagonist, in ferrets. Eur J Pharmacol 305: 181–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Gardner CJ, Armour DR, Beattie DT, Gale JD, Hawcock AB, et al. (1996) GR205171: a novel antagonist with high affinity for the tachykinin NK1 receptor, and potent broad-spectrum anti-emetic activity. Regul Pept 65: 45–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Watson JW, Gonsalves SF, Fossa AA, McLean S, Seeger T, et al. (1995) The anti-emetic effects of CP-99,994 in the ferret and the dog: role of the NK1 receptor. British J Pharmacol 115: 84–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Bountra C, Bunce K, Dale T, Gardner C, Jordan C, et al. (1993) Anti-emetic profile of a non-peptide neurokinin NK1 receptor antagonist, CP-99,994, in ferrets. Eur J Pharmacol 249: R3–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Horn CC, Kimball BA, Wang H, Kaus J, Dienel S, et al. (2013) Why can’t rodents vomit? A comparative behavioral, anatomical, and physiological study. PLoS ONE 8: e60537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Lois JH, Rice CD, Yates BJ (2009) Neural circuits controlling diaphragm function in the cat revealed by transneuronal tracing. J Appl Physiol 106: 138–152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Kim Y, Morrow GR (2007) The effects of family support, anxiety, and post-treatment nausea on the development of anticipatory nausea: a latent growth model. J Pain Symptom Manag 34: 265–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Zachariae R, Paulsen K, Mehlsen M, Jensen AB, Johansson A, et al. (2007) Anticipatory nausea: the role of individual differences related to sensory perception and autonomic reactivity. Ann Behav Med 33: 69–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Boakes RA, Tarrier N, Barnes BW, Tattersall MH (1993) Prevalence of anticipatory nausea and other side-effects in cancer patients receiving chemotherapy. European J Cancer 29A: 866–870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Onishi T, Mori T, Yanagihara M, Furukawa N, Fukuda H (2007) Similarities of the neuronal circuit for the induction of fictive vomiting between ferrets and dogs. Auton Neurosci 136: 20–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Horn CC, Ciucci M, Chaudhury A (2007) Brain Fos expression during 48 h after cisplatin treatment: neural pathways for acute and delayed visceral sickness. Auton Neurosci 132: 44–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Ito H, Nishibayashi M, Maeda S, Seki M, Ebukuro S (2005) Emetic responses and neural activity in young musk shrews during the breast-feeding/weaning period: comparison between the high and low emetic response strains using a shaking stimulus. Exp Anim 54: 301–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Ito H, Nishibayashi M, Kawabata K, Maeda S, Seki M, et al. (2003) Induction of Fos protein in neurons in the medulla oblongata after motion- and X-irradiation-induced emesis in musk shrews (Suncus murinus). Auton Neurosci 107: 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Billig I, Yates BJ, Rinaman L (2001) Plasma hormone levels and central c-Fos expression in ferrets after systemic administration of cholecystokinin. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 281: R1243–1255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Boissonade FM, Davison JS (1996) Effect of vagal and splanchnic nerve section on fos expression in ferret brain stem after emetic stimuli. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 40: R228–R236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Miller AD, Ruggiero DA (1994) Emetic reflex are revealed by expression of the Immediate-Early gene C-Fos in the cat. J Neurosci 14: 871–888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Boissonade FM, Sharkey KA, Davison JS (1994) Fos expression in ferret dorsal vagal complex after peripheral emetic stimuli. Am J Physiol 266: R1118–R1126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Herrera DG, Robertson HA (1996) Activation of c-fos in the brain. Prog Neurobiol 50: 83–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Morgan JI, Curran T (1991) Stimulus-transcription coupling in the nervous system: involvement of the inducible proto-oncogenes fos and jun. Annu Rev Neurosci 14: 421–451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Ariumi H, Saito R, Nago S, Hyakusoku M, Takano Y, et al. (2000) The role of tachykinin NK-1 receptors in the area postrema of ferrets in emesis. Neurosci Lett 286: 123–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Grelot L, Milano S, Portillo F, Miller AD (1993) Respiratory interneurons of the lower cervical (C4–C5) cord: membrane potential changes during fictive coughing, vomiting, and swallowing in the decerebrate cat. Pflugers Arch 425: 313–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Grelot L, Milano S, Portillo F, Miller AD, Bianchi AL (1992) Membrane potential changes of phrenic motoneurons during fictive vomiting, coughing, and swallowing in the decerebrate cat. J Neurophysiol 68: 2110–2119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Miller AD, Ezure K (1992) Behavior of inhibitory and excitatory propriobulbar respiratory neurons during fictive vomiting. Brain Res 578: 168–176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Miller AD, Ezure K, Suzuki I (1985) Control of abdominal muscles by brain stem respiratory neurons in the cat. J Neurophysiol 54: 155–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Miller AD, Jakus J, Nonaka S (1994) Plasticity of emesis to a 5-HT3 agonist: effect of order of visceral nerve cuts. NeuroReport 5: 986–988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Miller AD, Nonaka S (1990) Mechanisms of abdominal muscle activation during vomiting. J Appl Plysiol 69: 21–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Miller AD, Nonaka S (1990) Botzinger expiratory neurons may inhibit phrenic motoneurons and medullary inspiratory neurons during vomiting. Brain Res 521: 352–354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Miller AD, Nonaka S, Jakus J, Yates BJ (1996) Modulation of vomiting by the medullary midline. Brain Res 737: 51–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Miller AD, Nonaka S, Lakos SF, Tan LK (1990) Diaphragmatic and external intercostal muscle control during vomiting: behavior of inspiratory bulbospinal neurons. J Neurophysiol 63: 31–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Miller AD, Nonaka S, Siniaia MS, Jakus J (1995) Multifunctional ventral respiratory group: bulbospinal expiratory neurons play a role in pudendal discharge during vomiting. J Auton Nerv Syst 54: 253–260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Miller AD, Tan LK (1987) Possible role of brain stem respiratory neurons in mediating vomiting during space motion sickness. Aviat Space Environ Med 58: A126–128. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Miller AD, Yamaguchi T, Siniaia MS, Yates BJ (1995) Ventral respiratory group bulbospinal inspiratory neurons participate in vestibular-respiratory reflexes. J Neurophysiol 73: 1303–1307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Nakazawa K, Umezaki T, Zheng Y, Miller AD (1999) Behaviors of bulbar respiratory interneurons during fictive swallowing and vomiting. Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 120: 412–418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Nonaka S, Miller AD (1991) Behavior of upper cervical inspiratory propriospinal neurons during fictive vomiting. J Neurophysiol 65: 1492–1500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Shiba K, Siniaia MS, Miller AD (1996) Role of ventral respiratory group bulbospinal expiratory neurons in vestibular-respiratory reflexes. J Neurophysiol 76: 2271–2279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Umezaki T, Nakazawa K, Miller AD (1998) Behaviors of hypoglossal hyoid motoneurons in laryngeal and vestibular reflexes and in deglutition and emesis. Am J Physiol 274: R950–955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Umezaki T, Shiba K, Zheng Y, Miller AD (1998) Upper airway motor outputs during vomiting versus swallowing in the decerebrate cat. Brain Res 781: 25–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Umezaki T, Zheng Y, Shiba K, Miller AD (1997) Role of nucleus retroambigualis in respiratory reflexes evoked by superior laryngeal and vestibular nerve afferents and in emesis. Brain Res 769: 347–356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Yates BJ, Balaban CD, Miller AD, Endo K, Yamaguchi Y (1995) Vestibular inputs to the lateral tegmental field of the cat: potential role in autonomic control. Brain Res 689: 197–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Yates BJ, Grelot L, Kerman IA, Balaban CD, Jakus J, et al. (1994) Organization of vestibular inputs to nucleus tractus solitarius and adjacent structures in cat brain stem. Am J Physiol 267: R974–983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Zheng Y, Umezaki T, Nakazawa K, Miller AD (1997) Role of pre-inspiratory neurons in vestibular and laryngeal reflexes and in swallowing and vomiting. Neurosci Lett 225: 161–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Bianchi AL, Grelot L (1989) Converse motor output of inspiratory bulbospinal premotoneurones during vomiting. Neurosci Lett 104: 298–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Ezure K (1990) Synaptic connections between medullary respiratory neurons and considerations on the genesis of respiratory rhythm. Prog Neurobiol 35: 429–450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Feldman JL (1986) Neurophysiology of breathing in mammals. In: Bloom FE, editor. Handbook of Physiology The Nervous System, IV Intrinsic Regulatory Systems of the Brain. Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society. 463–524.
- 56. Bianchi AL, Denavitsaubie M, Champagnat J (1995) Central control of breathing in mammals: neuronal circuitry, membrane properties, and neurotransmitters. Physiol Rev 75: 1–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Ezure K (1996) Respiratory control. In: Yates BJ, Miller AD, editors. Vestibular Autonomic Regulation. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. 53–84.
- 58. Cohen MI (1981) Central determinants of respiratory rhythm. Ann Rev Physiol 43: 91–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Miller AD, Wilson VJ (1983) Vestibular-induced vomiting after vestibulocerebellar lesions. Brain Behav Evol 23: 26–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Bard P, Woolsey CN, Snider RS, Mountcastle VB, Bromiley RB (1947) Delimitation of central nervous mechanisms involved in motion sickness. Fed Proc 6: 72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Borison HL (1983) A 1983 neuropharmacologic perspective of space sickness. Brain Behav Evol 23: 7–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Borison HL, Borison R (1986) Motion sickness reflex arc bypasses the area postrema in cats. Exp Neurol 92: 723–737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Cheung BS, Money KE (1992) The influence of age on susceptibility to motion sickness in monkeys. J Vestib Res 2: 247–255. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Fox RA, Keil LC, Daunton NG, Crampton GH, Lucot J (1987) Vasopressin and motion sickness in cats. Aviation Space Environ Med 58: A143–147. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Igarashi M, Himi T, Kulecz WB, Kobayashi K (1987) Role of otolith endorgans in the genesis of vestibular-visual conflict sickness (pitch) in the squirrel monkey (first report). Aviation Space Environ Med 58: A207–211. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Igarashi M, Kobayashi K, Kulecz WB, Isago H (1986) Vestibular-visual conflict in pitch and yaw planes in the squirrel monkey. Aviation Space Environ Med 57: 1071–1074. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Javid FA, Naylor RJ (1999) Variables of movement amplitude and frequency in the development of motion sickness in suncus murinus. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 64: 115–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Kakimito S, Saito H, Matsuki N (1997) Antiemetic effects of morphine on motion- and drug-induced emesis in Suncus murinus. Pharmaceutical Society Japan 20: 739–742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Lang IM, Sarna SK, Shaker R (1999) Gastrointestinal motor and myoelectric correlates of motion sickness. Am J Physiol 277: G642–652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Lucot JB (1998) Effects of n-methyl-d-aspartate antagonists on different measures of motion sickness in cats. Brain Res Bull 47: 407–411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Lucot JB, Crampton GH (1987) Buspirone blocks motion sickness and xylazine-induced emesis in the cat. Aviation Space Environ Med 58: 989–991. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Lucot JB, Crampton GH (1989) 8-OH-DPAT suppresses vomiting in the cat elicited by motion, cisplatin or xylazine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 33: 627–631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Lucot JB, Takeda N (1992) alpha-Fluoromethylhistidine but not diphenhydramine prevents motion-induced emesis in the cat. Am J Otolaryngol 13: 176–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Okada F, Saito H, Matsuki N (1995) BLockade of motion- and cisplatin-induced emesis by a 5-HT2 receptor agonist in Suncus murinus. British J Pharmacol 114: 931–934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Okada F, Saito H, Matsuki N (1996) Prophylactic effect of serotonin uptake inhibitors against motion sickness in Suncus murinus. European J Pharmacol 309: 33–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Ueno S, Matsuki N, Saito H (1988) Suncus murinus as a new experimental model for motion sickness. Life Sci 43: 413–420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Wilpizeski CR, Lowry LD, el Raheb M, Eyyunni U, Contrucci RB, et al. (1985) Experimental motion sickness induced in squirrel monkeys by continuous off-axis horizontal rotation. Am J Otolaryngol 6: 1–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Wilpizeski CR, Lowry LD, Goldman WS (1986) Motion-induced sickness following bilateral ablation of area postrema in squirrel monkeys. Laryngoscope 96: 1221–1225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Kerman IA, Shabrang C, Taylor L, Akil H, Watson SJ (2006) Relationship of presympathetic-premotor neurons to the serotonergic transmitter system in the rat brainstem. J Comp Neurol 499: 882–896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. McLean IW, Nakane PK (1974) Periodate-lysine-paraformaldehyde for immunoelectron microscopy. J Histochem Cytochem 22: 1077–1083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Watson RE Jr, Wiegand SJ, Clough RW, Hoffman GE (1986) Use of cryoprotectant to maintain long-term peptide immunoreactivity and tissue morphology. Peptides 7: 155–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. De Jonghe BC, Horn CC (2009) Chemotherapy agent cisplatin induces 48 h Fos expression in the brain of a vomiting species, the house musk shrew (Suncus murinus). Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 296: R902–R911. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Hsu SM, Raine L, Fanger H (1981) Use of avidin-biotin-peroxidase complex (ABC) in immunoperoxidase techniques: a comparison between ABC and unlabeled antibody (PAP) procedures. J Histochem Cytochem 29: 577–580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Rice CD, Lois JH, Kerman IA, Yates BJ (2009) Localization of serotoninergic neurons that participate in regulating diaphragm activity in the cat. Brain Res 1279: 71–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Berman AI (1968) The Brain Stem of the Cat. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.
- 86.Beaver BV (2003) Feline Behavior St. Louis: Saunders.
- 87.Hotelling H (1933) Analysis of a complex of statistical variables into components. J Educational Psychol 24: 417–441 and 498–520.
- 88.Anderson TW (1958) An Introduction to Multivariate Statistical Analysis. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
- 89. Dziuban CD, Shirkey EC (1974) When is a correlation matrix appropriate for factor analysis? Some decision rules. Psychol Bull 81: 358–361. [Google Scholar]
- 90. Dragunow M, Faull R (1989) The use of c-fos as a metabolic marker in neuronal pathway tracing. J Neurosci Methods 29: 261–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Keay KA, Li QF, Bandler R (2000) Muscle pain activates a direct projection from ventrolateral periaqueductal gray to rostral ventrolateral medulla in rats. Neurosci Lett 290: 157–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Clement CI, Keay KA, Podzebenko K, Gordon BD, Bandler R (2000) Spinal sources of noxious visceral and noxious deep somatic afferent drive onto the ventrolateral periaqueductal gray of the rat. J Comp Neurol 425: 323–344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. Coggeshall RE (2005) Fos, nociception and the dorsal horn. Prog Neurobiol 77: 299–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94. Dampney RA, Horiuchi J (2003) Functional organisation of central cardiovascular pathways: studies using c-fos gene expression. Prog Neurobiol 71: 359–384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. Polson JW, Potts PD, Li YW, Dampney RA (1995) Fos expression in neurons projecting to the pressor region in the rostral ventrolateral medulla after sustained hypertension in conscious rabbits. Neurosci 67: 107–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Balaban CD, Jacob RG, Furman JM (2011) Neurologic bases for comorbidity of balance disorders, anxiety disorders and migraine: neurotherapeutic implications. Expert Rev Neurotherapeutics 11: 379–394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Balaban CD, Yates BJ (2004) Vestibulo-autonomic interactions: a teleologic perspective. In: Highstein SM, Fay RR, Popper AN, editors. Anatomy and Physiology of the Central and Peripheral Vestibular System. Heidelberg: Springer. 286–342.
- 98. Baizer JS, Corwin WL, Baker JF (2010) Otolith stimulation induces c-Fos expression in vestibular and precerebellar nuclei in cats and squirrel monkeys. Brain Res 1351: 64–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99. Kaufman GD, Anderson JH, Beitz AJ (1992) Fos-defined activity in rat brainstem following centripetal acceleration. J Neurosci 12: 4489–4500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100. Lai CH, Tse YC, Shum DK, Yung KK, Chan YS (2004) Fos expression in otolith-related brainstem neurons of postnatal rats following off-vertical axis rotation. J Comp Neurol 470: 282–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101. Kaufman GD, Anderson JH, Beitz AJ (1993) Otolith-brain stem connectivity: evidence for differential neural activation by vestibular hair cells based on quantification of FOS expression in unilateral labyrinthectomized rats. J Neurophysiol 70: 117–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Kshirsagar AM (1973) Multivariate Analysis. New York: Marcel Dekker.
- 103. Yu S, Huang D, Singer W, Nikolic D (2008) A small world of neuronal synchrony. Cereb Cortex 18: 2891–2901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104. Bullmore E, Sporns O (2009) Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 186–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105. Balaban CD (1996) Vestibular nucleus projections to the parabrachial nucleus in rabbits: implications for vestibular influences on the autonomic nervous system. Exp Brain Res 108: 367–381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106. Fung SJ, Reddy VK, Bowker RM, Barnes CD (1987) Differential labeling of the vestibular complex following unilateral injections of horseradish peroxidase into the cat and rat locus coeruleus. Brain Res 401: 347–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Manzoni D, Pompeiano O, Barnes CD, Stampacchia G, d’Ascanio P (1989) Responses of locus coeruleus neurons to convergent neck and vestibular inputs. Acta Oto-Laryngologica - Supplement 468: 129–135. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 108. Schuerger RJ, Balaban CD (1999) Organization of the coeruleo-vestibular pathway in rats, rabbits, and monkeys. Brain Res Rev 30: 189–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109. Balaban CD (2004) Projections from the parabrachial nucleus to the vestibular nuclei: potential substrates for autonomic and limbic influences on vestibular responses. Brain Res 996: 126–137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110. Balaban CD, Beryozkin G (1994) Vestibular nucleus projections to nucleus tractus solitarius and the dorsal motor nucleus of the vagus nerve: potential substrates for vestibulo-autonomic interactions. Exp Brain Res 98: 200–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111. Herbert H, Moga MM, Saper CB (1990) Connections of the parabrachial nucleus with the nucleus of the solitary tract and the medullary reticular formation in the rat. J Comp Neurol 293: 540–580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Balaban C.D. (2003) Vestibular nucleus projections to the ventrolateral and lateral columns of the periaqueductal gray (vIPAG) and (lPAG): Potential links between balance, nociception and coping responses. Assoc Res Otolaryngol Abstr: 1511.
- 113. Jones BE, Moore RY (1977) Ascending projections of the locus coeruleus in the rat. II. Autoradiographic study. Brain Res 127: 25–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114. Luppi PH, Aston-Jones G, Akaoka H, Chouvet G, Jouvet M (1995) Afferent projections to the rat locus coeruleus demonstrated by retrograde and anterograde tracing with cholera-toxin B subunit and Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. Neurosci 65: 119–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115. Krout KE, Jansen AS, Loewy AD (1998) Periaqueductal gray matter projection to the parabrachial nucleus in rat. J Comp Neurol 401: 437–454. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116. de Andrade JS, Abrao RO, Cespedes IC, Garcia MC, Nascimento JO, et al. (2012) Acute restraint differently alters defensive responses and fos immunoreactivity in the rat brain. Behav Brain Res 232: 20–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117. Pelosi GG, Resstel LL, Soares VP, Zangrossi H, Guimaraes FS, et al. (2009) Anxiolytic-like effect of noradrenaline microinjection into the dorsal periaqueductal gray of rats. Behav Pharmacol 20: 252–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118. Halberstadt AL, Balaban CD (2007) Selective anterograde tracing of the individual serotonergic and nonserotonergic components of the dorsal raphe nucleus projection to the vestibular nuclei. Neurosci 147: 207–223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119. Pobbe RL, Zangrossi H Jr, Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ (2011) Involvement of dorsal raphe nucleus and dorsal periaqueductal gray 5-HT receptors in the modulation of mouse defensive behaviors. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 21: 306–315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120. Derbyshire SW, Osborn J (2009) Offset analgesia is mediated by activation in the region of the periaqueductal grey and rostral ventromedial medulla. Neuroimage 47: 1002–1006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121. Holstege G (1988) Anatomical evidence for a strong ventral parabrachial projection to nucleus raphe magnus and adjacent tegmental field. Brain Res 447: 154–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122. Hermann DM, Luppi PH, Peyron C, Hinckel P, Jouvet M (1997) Afferent projections to the rat nuclei raphe magnus, raphe pallidus and reticularis gigantocellularis pars alpha demonstrated by iontophoretic application of choleratoxin (subunit b). J Chem Neuroanat 13: 1–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123. Sim LJ, Joseph SA (1992) Efferent projections of the nucleus raphe magnus. Brain Res Bull 28: 679–682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124. Gang S, Mizuguchi A, Kobayashi N, Aoki M (1990) Descending axonal projections from the medial parabrachial and Kolliker-Fuse nuclear complex to the nucleus raphe magnus in cats. Neurosci Lett 118: 273–275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125. Gang S, Nakazono Y, Aoki N (1993) Differential projections to the raphe nuclei from the medial Parabrachial-Kolliker-Fuse (NPBM-KF) nuclear complex and the retrofacial nucleus in cats - retrograde WGA-HRP tracing. J Auton Nerv Syst 45: 241–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126. Porter JD, Balaban CD (1997) Connections between the vestibular nuclei and brain stem regions that mediate autonomic function in the rat. J Vestib Res 7: 63–76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127. Mason P, Gao K, Genzen JR (2007) Serotonergic raphe magnus cell discharge reflects ongoing autonomic and respiratory activities. J Neurophysiol 98: 1919–1927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128. Peyron C, Luppi PH, Fort P, Rampon C, Jouvet M (1996) Lower brainstem catecholamine afferents to the rat dorsal raphe nucleus. J Comp Neurol 364: 402–413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129. Brink TS, Hellman KM, Lambert AM, Mason P (2006) Raphe magnus neurons help protect reactions to visceral pain from interruption by cutaneous pain. J Neurophysiol 96: 3423–3432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130. Wei F, Dubner R, Zou S, Ren K, Bai G, et al. (2010) Molecular depletion of descending serotonin unmasks its novel facilitatory role in the development of persistent pain. J Neurosci 30: 8624–8636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131. Halberstadt AL, Balaban CD (2003) Organization of projections from the raphe nuclei to the vestibular nuclei in rats. Neurosci 120: 573–594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132. Depuy SD, Kanbar R, Coates MB, Stornetta RL, Guyenet PG (2011) Control of breathing by raphe obscurus serotonergic neurons in mice. J Neurosci 31: 1981–1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133. Moazzami A, Tjen ALSC, Guo ZL, Longhurst JC (2010) Serotonergic projection from nucleus raphe pallidus to rostral ventrolateral medulla modulates cardiovascular reflex responses during acupuncture. J Appl Physiol 108: 1336–1346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134. Mantyh PW (1983) Connections of midbrain periaqueductal gray in the monkey. I. Ascending efferent projections. J Neurophysiol 49: 567–581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135. Jenck F, Moreau JL, Martin JR (1995) Dorsal periaqueductal gray-induced aversion as a simulation of panic anxiety: elements of face and predictive validity. Psychiatry Res 57: 181–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136. Muth ER (2006) Motion and space sickness: intestinal and autonomic correlates. Auton Neurosci 129: 58–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137. Ashby FG, Townsend JT (1986) Varieties of perceptual independence. Psychol Rev 83: 154–179. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138. Kennedy R, Lane N, Berbaum K, Lilienthal M (1993) Simulator sickness questionnaire: an enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. International J Aviation Psychol 3: 203–220. [Google Scholar]
- 139. Gianaros P, Muth E, Mordkoff J, Levine M, RM S (2001) A questionnaire for the assessment of the multiple dimensions of motion sickness. Aviat Space Environ Med 72: 115–119. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140. Kennedy R, Drexler J, Kennedy R (2010) Research in visually induced motion sickness. Applied Ergonomics 41: 494–503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141. Gianaros PJ, Stern RM, Morrow GR, Hickok JT (2001) Relationship of gastric myoelectrical and cardiac parasympathetic activity to chemotherapy-induced nausea. J Psychosom Res 50: 263–266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142. Graybiel A, Knepton J (1976) Sopite syndrome: a sometimes sole manifestation of motion sickness. Aviat Space Environ Med 47: 873–882. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143. Lawson BD, Mead AM (1998) The sopite syndrome revisited: drowsiness and mood changes during real or apparent motion. Acta Astronaut 43: 181–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144. Price D (2000) Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain. Science 288: 1769–1772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145. Craig A (2002) How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci 3: 655–666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146. Craig A (2003) A new view of pain as a homeostatic emotion. Trends Neurosci 26: 303–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147. Craig A (2009) How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci 10: 59–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148. Hayes D, Northoff G (2012) Common brain activations for painful and non-painful aversive stimuli. BMC Neurosci 13: 60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Melzack R, Casey K (1968) Sensory, motivational, and central control determinants of pain: a new conceptual model. In: Kenshalo D, editor. The Skin Senses. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. 423–443.
- 150.Grahek N (2007) Feeling in Pain and Being in Pain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Bradford Books. [Google Scholar]
- 151. Strigo I, Bushnell M, Boivin M, Duncan G (2002) Psychophysical analysis of visceral and cutaneous pain in human subjects. Pain 97: 235–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.