Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Jan 27.
Published in final edited form as: J Clin Psychol. 2011 Dec 1;68(3):236–245. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20855

Between-Session Practice and Therapeutic Alliance as Predictors of Mindfulness After Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention

Sarah Bowen 1, Andrew S Kurz 1
PMCID: PMC3902179  NIHMSID: NIHMS353695  PMID: 22135221

Abstract

Objectives

Mindfulness-based treatments have demonstrated efficacy in reducing symptoms in clinical populations. Not surprisingly, research suggests increases in client mindfulness might be a mechanism of change in these treatments. However, little is known about specific factors that lead to increased mindfulness.

Design

The present study is a secondary analysis of 93 adults in outpatient treatment for substance abuse, assessing effects of between-session mindfulness practice and therapeutic alliance on levels of mindfulness after an 8-week Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention (MBRP) program.

Results

Between-session practice over the course of the 8 weeks was predictive of mindfulness at postcourse, although not at the 2-month or 4-month follow-up assessments. Client-rated therapeutic alliance was a significant predictor at the 2-month follow-up, although not at 4 months.

Conclusions

These findings suggest that between-session practice and therapeutic alliance might be important factors in the initial increases in mindfulness after mindfulness-based treatments, but factors supporting longer term mindfulness might shift over time.

Keywords: Mindfulness, homework, home practice, therapeutic alliance, substance abuse


The prescientific Buddhist roots of mindfulness practice date back thousands of years. However, the construct and practices of mindfulness are still relatively new to Western psychology and may require additional discussion and study before consensus is reached on definition, practice, and measurement (Brown, Ryan, & Creswell, 2007). Mindfulness is often defined in the psychological literature as, “awareness that emerges through paying attention, on purpose, in the present moment, and non-judgmentally to the unfolding of experience moment by moment” (Kabat-Zinn, 2003, p. 145). Mindfulness has also been described as a state of consciousness and enhanced present-centered awareness (Brown & Ryan, 2003), a two-component metacognitive skill and practice of awareness and nonjudgment (Bishop et al., 2004), and as a multicomponent cyclical process of intention, attention, and attitude (Shapiro, Carlson, Astin, & Freedman, 2006).

Mindfulness has received increasing attention in the empirical literature over the past decade, resulting in several new mindfulness-based treatments and efficacy studies for various medical and psychological disorders. Perhaps the most widely known and studied of these treatments are Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR; Kabat-Zinn, 1990), originally developed for individuals with chronic pain, and Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT; Segal, Williams, & Teasdale, 2002) for prevention of relapse to depression. Meta-analyses have shown evidence for MBSR’s efficacy for improving coping in both clinical and nonclinical populations (Grossman, Niemann, Schmidt, & Walach, 2004), and for treating symptoms of anxiety and depression in clinical populations (Hoffman, Sawyer, Witt, & Oh, 2010). Studies of MBCT have offered evidence for its efficacy in decreasing risk of relapse for individuals with three or more previous major depressive episodes (Ma & Teasdale, 2004; Teasdale et al., 2000), generalized anxiety disorder and panic disorder (Evans et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2009), mixed mood disorders (Ree & Craigie, 2007), and current treatment resistant depression (Kenny & Williams, 2007).

Based on the content and structure of MBSR (Kabat-Zinn, 1990) and MBCT (Segal et al., 2002), Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention (MBRP; Bowen, Chawla & Marlatt, 2010) was developed for treatment of addictive behaviors, integrating standard cognitive behavioral relapse prevention treatment (Marlatt & Gordon, 1985) with mindfulness meditation exercises and skills. In an initial efficacy trial, (Bowen et al., 2009), participants in MBRP, as compared to those in a treatment-as-usual control group, reported lower rates of substance use, greater decreases in craving, and greater increases in acceptance and mindful awareness. Other studies have similarly examined mindfulness-based treatments for addictive behaviors with encouraging results (e.g., Brewer et al., 2009; Vieten, Astin, Buscemi, & Galloway, 2010; Zgierska et al., 2009).

Historically, mindfulness research has centered on the effectiveness of mindfulness and mindfulness-based interventions (Shapiro et al., 2006). More recently, however, studies have assessed mechanisms by which mindfulness-based interventions improve outcomes (Keng, Moski, & Robins, 2011). It might be expected that such interventions would lead to an increase in levels of mindfulness, and this increase would in turn lead to improved treatment outcomes. Several studies have indeed reported changes in levels of self-report mindfulness after mindfulness-based treatment (e.g., Brä nströ m, Kvillemo, Brandberg, & Moskowitz, 2010; Brewer et al., 2009; Carmody & Baer, 2008; Carmody, Baer, Lykins, & Olendzki, 2009; Dobkin, 2008; Krasner et al., 2009; Raes, Dewulf, Van Heeringen, & Williams, 2009; Ree & Craigie, 2007; Shapiro, Oman, Thoresen, Plante, & Flinders, 2008), and studies have also demonstrated that increases in mindfulness mediate treatment outcomes. For example, studies suggest a meditating role of mindfulness in the relation between mindfulness-based treatment and rumination (Shapiro et al., 2008), cognitive reactivity (Raes et al.), depressive symptoms (Kuyken et al., 2010; Shaher Britton, Sbarra, Figueredo, & Bootzin, 2010), quality of life (Nyklíček & Kuipers, 2008), psychological symptoms (Carmody et al., 2009), and well-being (Carmody & Baer). Although the study of the role of mindfulness in improving clinical outcomes is young, it appears to hold promise for several commonly targeted psychological symptoms.

Based upon emerging evidence of its importance, newer studies are beginning to assess what factors might lead to increases in mindfulness. It has been suggested by the developers of mindfulness-based therapies (e.g., Kabat-Zinn, 2003; Segal et al., 2002) that it is essential for clients to engage in regular “formal” mindfulness practices (e.g., sitting meditation) between sessions. Mindfulness-based treatments introduce and provide instruction for these practices, in efforts to support the development of sustained mindfulness practice in daily life (e.g., Kabat-Zinn, 1990; Segal et al.). There has been surprisingly little research on between-session practice and increases in levels of mindfulness, however. A recent meta-analysis by Vettese, Toneatto, Stea, Nguyen, and Wang (2009) found that out of 15 studies of mindfulness-based interventions implementing validated measures of mindfulness, only three reported on the relation between hours practiced and mindfulness (Carmody & Baer, 2008; Carmody, Reed, Kristeller, & Merriam, 2008; Schenströ m, Rö nnberg, & Bodlund, 2006). Two of those three (Carmody & Baer; Schenström et al.) showed a significant positive correlation between mindfulness and hours of meditation practice.

Carmody and Baer (2008), followed 174 adults with a range of medical and psychiatric symptoms following a clinical 8-week MBSR program. Mindfulness was measured with the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ) and participants logged the number of minutes they engaged in formal and informal (mindful during everyday activities) mindfulness practice between sessions. On average, participants performed 31–35 minutes of formal mindfulness practice (e.g., body scan, mindful yoga, sitting meditation) per day and performed a formal practice on 34 of the 42 assigned days (80% compliance). Participant mindfulness significantly increased from pre-MBSR to post-MBSR, with moderate effect sizes. Time spent in formal practice was a significant predictor of increases in mindfulness, although informal practice was not. The study also found that increases in client mindfulness mediated the relation between meditation practice over the course of the treatment and improvement in main outcome variables, such as various psychological symptoms and stress.

Carmody, Reed, Kristellar, and Merriam (2008) followed 44 adults, referred by their primary care doctors for various (unmeasured) symptoms, who participated in an 8-week MBSR program. State mindfulness was measured using the Toronto Mindfulness Scale (Lau et al., 2006) and trait mindfulness with the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (Brown & Ryan, 2003). Participants looged time spent in home mindfulness practice was logged by participants using tracking worksheets. The mean number of minutes of formal practice was 31 minutes per day. Contrary to the Carmody and Baer (2008) study, no significant association was found between at-home practice and state or trait mindfulness.

In a study by Schenström and colleagues (2006), 52 caregivers in a primary care setting participated in a mindfulness-based cognitive attitude training, based on MBSR and designed to reduce the negative effects of stress on healthcare personnel. Mindfulness was measured with the Mindfulness Attention Awareness Scale (Brown & Ryan, 2003) and at-home practice was recorded in a simple numeric multiple-choice questionnaire. Results showed that participants who engaged in more at-home practice had significant increases in mindfulness, while participants who engaged in little or no at-home practice did not. Other than one study of effects of positive reappraisal on mindfulness (Garland, Gaylord, & Fredrickson, 2011), we were unable to locate any published research on factors other than formal practice that predicted increases in client mindfulness following mindfulness-based treatment.

Another hypothesized predictor of increases in mindfulness is the relationship between the therapist and client. Therapeutic alliance (also called working alliance) has been described as (a) agreement between client and therapist on goals, (b) agreement on tasks used to achieve those goals, and (c) the interpersonal bond between the client and therapist (Bordin, 1979; Tracey & Kokotovic, 1989). Meta-analytic studies reviewing literature on the role of therapeutic alliance in psychological treatments (Horvath & Symonds, 1991; Martin, Garske, & Davis, 2000) suggest it is predictive of treatment outcomes in various areas of psychotherapy (Horvath & Luborsky, 1993), behavior therapy (Sweet, 1984), cognitive therapy (Waddington, 2002), and substance misuse (Meier, Barrowclough, & Donmall, 2005). The potential role of therapeutic alliance in increasing mindfulness after MBRP may be due to the influence of the above factors. In MBRP, it is suggested by the therapist that mindfulness practice will help maintain treatment goals. Agreement upon treatment goals may be necessary for the client to adopt the treatment rationale (Bordin). Similarly, agreement on tasks necessary to achieve these goals might be of particular importance (Horvath & Luborsky, 1993). For example, MBRP clients are asked to bring purposeful attention to routine daily activities that are often done “mindlessly” as a way of increasing overall awareness of internal and external triggers and habitual reactions. This requires agreement on necessary tasks and commitment to engage in these “informal” mindfulness practices throughout the day. Finally, Siegel (2007) suggests that “secure interpersonal attunements [between therapist and client] likely create states of integration that promote internal attunement and mindfulness as a trait” (p. 317). Similarly, Bruce, Shapiro, Constantino, and Manber (2010) propose that the mindful psychotherapist’s “ability to form an attuned, empathic relationship with the patient can lead to improvement in the patient’s ability to self-attune” (p. 83). The therapeutic bond between client and therapist might therefore support a client’s willingness to attune to and mindfully explore his or her own experiences.

Although several studies report the therapeutic effects of the client-therapist relationship on substance abuse treatment retention and outcomes (e.g., Gilbert, 2011; Gibbons et al., 2010; Myers, Pasche, & Adam, 2010), few studies have assessed the relation between therapeutic alliance and mindfulness. In fact, only two locatable studies reported effects of therapists’ levels of mindfulness on the therapeutic relationship (Padilla, 2011;Wexler, 2006). No locatable studies were found, however, on effects of the strength of alliance on foeffects of stering mindfulness in clients.

Thus, the current study assessed changes in levels of mindfulness following an MBRP intervention, examining the effect of two factors on increases in mindfulness in a sample of individuals with substance use disorders. Hypothesized predictors were time spent between sessions engaged in mindfulness meditation practices and client ratings of therapeutic alliance. Specifically, it was expected that postcourse reports of duration and frequency of mindfulness practice would be positively associated with levels of mindfulness, as measured by the FFMQ, at 2-month and 4-month follow-up points. Similarly, postcourse ratings of therapeutic alliance, as measured by the Working Alliance Inventory, would be positively related to mindfulness at the follow-up points.

METHOD

Procedure

The current study is a secondary analysis of data from a feasibility and efficacy trial of MBRP. The parent study from which the current data are drawn evaluated effects of an 8-week MBRP course as compared with treatment as usual (TAU) in an outpatient sample of 168 adults with substance use disorders (Bowen et al., 2009). All participants were between 18 and 70 years of age, and had recently completed either inpatient or intensive outpatient treatment. The primary substance of choice was alcohol (45.2%), followed by cocaine/crack (36.2%), methamphetamines (13.7%), opiates/heroin (7.1%), marijuana (5.4%), and other substances (1.9%). Approximately 19.1% of the sample reported polysubstance use. Participants reported that for the 60 days before entering initial treatment they were using substances on an average of 27.29 (standard deviation [SD]=24.28) days. MBRP groups, ranging from 6–10 participants, met for 2 hours each week, with two therapists delivering each session. Participants in MBRP were assigned between-session practices each week, which included audio-recorded instructions for mindfulness exercises, worksheets identifying triggers and reactions, and other exercises designed to raise awareness of problematic reactive behaviors. Treatment provided in the TAU groups was based primarily on the 12-step philosophy, and included psychoeducational components and relapse prevention skills.

Therapist fidelity to the MBRP protocol was assessed using audio recordings of a randomly selected 50% of the 8-week groups, rated by independent coders. Coders used the MBRP Adherence Competence Scale (MBRP-AC; Chawla et al., 2010), designed to measure therapist adherence to individual components of MBRP, discussion of key concepts, and therapist style and performance. Findings showed high interrater reliability for all treatment adherence and competence subscales, and suggested relatively high levels of adherence (90%) for an initial feasibility and efficacy trial (Chawla et al.).

Assessments for all participants were administered at baseline, immediately after completion of the course, and 2 and 4 months after the end of the intervention period. Assessments included self-report measures of craving, affect, mindfulness, acceptance, therapeutic alliance, meditation practice, and substance use. Retention rates at postcourse, 2-month, and 4-month assessment points were 61%, 57%, and 73%, respectively. Results suggested that when compared with those in TAU, participants in MBRP reported fewer days of alcohol or other drug use over the 4-month follow-up period. Craving was also found to be significantly lower for MBRP participants than for those in TAU, and decreases in craving partially mediated the substance use outcomes. (See Bowen et al., 2009, for full report.)

Sample

The current study was designed to examine factors contributing to levels of client mindfulness after mindfulness-based therapy. Thus, analyses include only those participants randomized to MBRP (n=93). Specifically, we assessed changes in mindfulness after the intervention and the relation between time spent on between-session mindfulness practices and effects of client-rated therapeutic alliance on levels of mindfulness at postcourse, and 2-month, and 4-month follow-up time points.

Approximately 36% of participants in the MBRP condition were female. The majority of MBRP participants identified as Caucasian (63%), followed by African American (23%), American Indian or Alaska Native (10%), Hispanic or Latino/a (6%), Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander (3%), Asian (1%), or other (1%). The mean age was 40.84 years (SD=10.23). (See Table 1 for further demographic information.)

Table 1.

Demographic Information for MBRP participants

Variable MBRP (n = 93)
Age Mean (SD)
40.84 (10.23)
Ethnicity Percent (n)
  Caucasian 63 (59)
  African American 23 (21)
  American Indian or Alaskan Native 10 (9)
  Hispanic or Latino/a 6 (6)
  Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander 3 (3)
  Asian 1 (1)
  Other 1 (1)
Gender Percent (n)
  Male 65 (60)
  Female 36 (33)
Annual income Percent (n)
  0–$4999 60 (55)
  $5000–$9999 7 (6)
  $10,000–$14,999 7 (6)
  $15,000–$19,999 2 (2)
  $20,000–$34,999 8 (7)
  $35,000–$49,999 5 (5)
  $50,000 or higher 0 (0)
  Unknown 12 (11)
Highest education level Percent (n)
  Middle school 8 (7)
  G.E.D. 17 (16)
  High school 15 (14)
  Some college 26 (24)
  Community college 15 (14)
  Bachelor’s degree 9 (8)

Note: MBRP=Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention; SD=standard deviation.

Measures

All measures were self-report. Demographics were assessed at baseline, therapeutic alliance was assessed at postcourse, between-session practice was assessed mid-course (4 weeks into the intervention) and at postcourse, and mindfulness and substance use were measured at the baseline, postcourse, and 2-month and 4-month follow-up assessment points.

Demographic questions included age, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status.

Therapeutic alliance was measured using the 12-item client version of the Working Alliance Inventory, Short Form (WAI-S; Tracey & Kokotovic, 1989), a 12-item self-report measure based on the WAI (Horvath & Greenberg, 1989). The WAI-S has demonstrated high internal consistency (Busseri & Tyler, 2003). Cronbach’s alpha in the current study was .92.

Mindfulness was assessed with the FFMQ (Baer, Smith, Hopkins, Krietemeyer, & Toney, 2006), which is comprised of 39 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale. The FFMQ has demonstrated good internal consistency (Baer et al.), with a Cronbach’s alpha of .91 in the current study. The FFMQ sum score is often used in studies of mindfulness-based treatments (Brä nström et al., 2010; Garland, Gaylord, Boettiger, & Howard, 2010; Sanders & Lam, 2010). The sum score is similarly used in the current study to assess participants’ level of general mindfulness.

Meditation practice was assessed halfway through the course (4 weeks) and at the end of the course. Participants were asked the average length (minutes) and frequency (times per week) of meditation practice. A sum score of total minutes of practice per week was calculated.

Paired sample t test were used to assess baseline to postcourse changes in levels of mindfulness, and multiple regression analyses was used to assess effects of between-session practice and therapeutic alliance, measured at postcourse, on postcourse and follow-up levels of mindfulness.

RESULTS

Scores on the primary outcome variable, FFMQ, were assessed for univariate outliers and deviation from expected distributions. FFMQ scores were continuous and normally distributed, and no outliers were revealed.

Paired sample t test revealed a significant increase in levels of mindfulness between baseline and postcourse, t(33)=−2.43, p =.021, which remained significant through 4-month follow-up, t(33) = −2.57, p =.014 (see Table 2).

Table 2.

Means (SDs) of WAI and FFMQ Across Assessment Points Among MBRP Participants

Variable Pre Post 2-Mo 4-Mo
WAI ------ 5.0 (1.3) ------ ------
FFMQ 98.2 (14.9) 105.8 (15.9) 103.7 (16.4) 104.6 (14.8)

Note: SD=standard deviation; WAI=Working Alliance Inventory; FFMQ=Five Factor Mindfulness Questionnaire; MBRP=Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention.

Multiple regression analyses assessed effects of the hypothesized predictors of between-session practice and therapeutic alliance on postcourse and follow-up levels of mindfulness, covarying baseline FFMQ scores. Results revealed a significant relation between time spent in between-session practice over the course of the 8-week intervention and mindfulness at postcourse, β =.338, t(30) = 2.23, p = .033. However, there was not a significant relation between time spent in between-session practice and mindfulness at either 2-month follow-up, β = .327, t(26) = 1.94, p = .065, or 4-month follow-up, β = .233, t(28) = 1.43, p = .164.

WAI scores were significantly related to postcourse FFMQ scores, β = .479, t(31) = 3.51, p = .001, maintaining significance when between-session practice was included as a covariate, β = .414, t(29) = 2.78, p = .009, with higher scores on the WAI related to higher levels of mindfulness (see Table 2 for means). WAI remained a significant predictor of FFMQ scores at the 2-month follow-up, β = .439, t(23) = 2.65, p = .014. However, WAI scores did not predict FFMQ scores at the 4-month follow-up, β = .224, t(26) = 1.379 p = .180.

DISCUSSION

Results of the current study revealed significant increases in mindfulness for MBRP participants over the course of the study, which were maintained through the 2- and 4- month follow-up assessments. The amount of time spent practicing between sessions over the 8 weeks of the course was significantly related to levels of mindfulness at postcourse. Contrary to hypotheses, however, between-session practice was not predictive of levels of mindfulness at the 2-month or 4-month follow-up assessments. These results suggest that engagement in between-session practice is related to levels of mindfulness immediately after the course; however, they were not predictive of longer-term levels of mindfulness.

Results from similar studies are mixed. Schenström et al. (2006) similarly found that participants who regularly engaged in between-session mindfulness practice had significant increases in mindfulness following the course, maintained throughout the 3-month follow-up period. Carmody and Baer (2008) also found that formal practice significantly predicted increases in mindfulness at postcourse, but did not include follow-up assessments. In contrast, Carmody et al. (2008) found no relation between between-session practice and mindfulness at postcourse.

The current study assessed “formal” practice, such as sitting meditations, but did not include assessment of “informal” practices or use of mindfulness in daily life or high-risk or stressful situations. Although mindfulness-based interventions emphasize the importance of formal practice, such practices are theoretically carried forth in a client’s daily life (e.g., recognizing an urge to behave reactively and intentionally pausing before choosing a skillful response). Such integrated practices might lead to increases in mindfulness. The MBSR course in the Schenström et al. (2006) study included both formal and informal practices, but it is unclear whether measurement of between-session practice included informal practices. Carmody and Baer (2008) did not find informal practice to significantly predict mindfulness. However, such practices vary from treatment to treatment and thus should be measured and assessed. For example, MBRP contains “informal” practices such as “urge surfing” and “mini-mediations” for use in high-risk situations or in the event of cravings or urges to use substances. These practices might differ from the “mindfulness in everyday life” described in the Carmody and Baer MBSR study, and thus might have different effects on levels of mindfulness. Additionally, the population in the current study was different from the other studies (i.e., individuals with substance abuse disorders), and thus informal mindfulness practices might have different effects.

Hypothesized a positive relation between client-rated therapeutic alliance at postcourse and follow-up levels of mindfulness, which was partially supported by the current study. Postcourse client-rated measure of alliance was significantly related to postcourse levels of mindfulness, with stronger alliance related to higher scores on overall mindfulness, even after accounting for effects of between-session practice. Strength of alliance remained predictive of mindfulness at the 2-month follow-up assessment, but not at the 4-month follow-up. These results suggest an effect of quality of alliance in mindfulness-based therapy on development and short-term maintenance of client mindfulness, over and above effects of between-session formal mindfulness practices.

In the context of mindfulness-based therapy, agreement upon therapy tasks and goals, i.e., committing to practice “informal” mindful engagement in one’s day-to-day life and a therapeutic bond supporting mindful exploration of experience, might be necessary factors for increasing mindfulness.

Although significant effects of the hypothesized predictors of between-session practice and therapeutic alliance were found for postcourse and 2-month follow-up assessments, respectively, time spent on between-session practice was no longer predictive after postcourse, and alliance was no longer predictive after the 2-month assessment. There are several possible explanations for these findings. As mentioned above, the strength of these relations might decrease over time. Initially, the between-session practice and therapeutic alliance may lead to increases in clients’ levels of mindfulness in the earlier stages of practice, but over time, other maintaining factors might become more predictive, such as continued formal or informal mindfulness practice, levels of mindfulness of peers, family members or sponsors, or level of support in one’s social network for continuing mindful engagement in daily life. Future studies would benefit from investigating whether factors that sustain levels of mindfulness shift over time, with immediate effects of between-session practice and therapeutic alliance giving way to other maintaining influences.

The current study has several strengths. In support of literature showing benefits of increased mindfulness, results offer further insight into factors that might foster these increases in a clinical population. Moreover, the study highlights the need for exploration of additional predictors of mindfulness and assessment of how these predictive factors might shift over time. Additionally, the sample was drawn from an racially and socioeconomically diverse and understudied population. Alongside its strengths, the study has limitations to consider. Data were collected via participant self-report, and between-session practice was measured retrospectively at 4 weeks and at postcourse assessment, using a nonvalidated measure; future studies might benefit by assessing therapist ratings of alliance in addition to those reported by clients and assessing between-session practice weekly throughout the duration of the course. Attrition rates in the parent study present another limitation. Although commensurate with studies of similar populations (Farrington, Petrosino, & Welsh, 2001), the lack of complete data for follow-up assessments limits the conclusions that can be drawn.

CONCLUSIONS

Although both treatment developers and data from a handful of studies suggest formal mindfulness practice is the primary way in which clients increase mindfulness, the current study suggests there might be other important factors to consider as well, such as clients’ experiences of the therapeutic relationship. Future studies might offer further insight into how both mindfulness practice and therapeutic alliance are affecting changes in mindfulness and explore other factors influencing increases in and longer term maintenance of mindfulness in mindfulness-based therapies.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from the National Institute on Drug Abuse: R21 DA010562 (Marlatt, PI). The authors would like to acknowledge Dr. G. Alan Marlatt for his guidance and support, Katie Witkiewitz for her editorial consultation, and the Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention clinical and research teams for their dedication to this project.

REFERENCES

  1. Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, Toney L. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment. 2006;13:27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bishop S, Lau M, Shapiro S, Carlson L, Anderson N, Carmody J, et al. Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2004;11:230–241. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bordin ES. The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice. 1979;16:252–260. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bowen S, Chawla N, Collins S, Witkiewitz K, Hsu S, Grow J, et al. Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention for substance use disorders: A pilot efficacy trial. Substance Use. 2009;30:295–305. doi: 10.1080/08897070903250084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bowen S, Chawla N, Marlatt GA. Mindfulness-based relapse prevention for substance use disorders: A clinician’s guide. New York: Guilford Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bowen S, Witkiewitz K, Dillworth T, Marlatt G. The role of thought suppression in the relationship between mindfulness mediation and alcohol use. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:2324–2328. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.01.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bränström R, Kvillemo P, Brandberg Y, Moskowitz J. Self-report mindfulness as a mediator of psychological well-being in a stress reduction intervention for cancer patients—A randomized study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2010;39(2):151–161. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9168-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brewer J, Sinha R, Chen J, Michalsen R, Babuscio T, Nich C, et al. Mindfulness training and stress reactivity in substance abuse: Results from a randomized, controlled stage I pilot study. Substance Abuse. 2009;30:306–317. doi: 10.1080/08897070903250241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brown K, Ryan R. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;84:822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bruce NG, Shapiro SL, Constantino MJ, Manber R. Psychotherapist mindfulness and the psychotherapy process. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training. 2010;47:83–97. doi: 10.1037/a0018842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Busseri MA, Tyler JD. Interchangeability of the Working Alliance Inventory and Working Alliance Inventory, Short Form. Psychological Assessment. 2003;15:193–197. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.15.2.193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carmody J, Baer R. Relationships between mindfulness practice and levels of mindfulness, medical and psychological symptoms and well-being in a mindfulness-based stress reduction program. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2008;31:23–33. doi: 10.1007/s10865-007-9130-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Carmody J, Baer R, Lykins E, Olendzki N. An empirical study of the mechanisms of mindfulness in a mindfulness-based stress reduction program. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2009;65:613–626. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Carmody J, Reed G, Kristeller J, Merriam P. Mindfulness, spirituality, and health-related symptoms. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2008;64:393–403. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.06.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chawla N, Collins S, Bowen S, Hsu S, Grow J, Douglas A, Marlatt GA. The Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention Adherence and Competence Scale: Development, interrater reliability and validity. Psychotherapy Research. 2010;20:388–397. doi: 10.1080/10503300903544257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dobkin PL. Mindfulness-based stress reduction: What processes are at work? Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice. 2008;14:8–16. doi: 10.1016/j.ctcp.2007.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Evans S, Ferrando S, Findler M, Stowell C, Smart C, Haglin D. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2008;22:716–721. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Farrington DP, Petrosino A, Welsh BC. Systematic reviews and cost-benefit analyses of correctional interventions. The Prison Journal. 2001;81:339–359. [Google Scholar]
  19. Garland EL, Gaylord SA, Boettiger CA, Howard MO. Mindfulness training modifies cognitive, affective, and physiological mechanisms implicated in alcohol dependence: Results of a randomized controlled pilot trial. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs. 2010;42(2):177–192. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2010.10400690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Garland EL, Gaylord SA, Fredrickson BL. Positive reappraisal mediates the stress-reductive effects of mindfulness: Am upward spiral process. Mindfulness. 2011;2:59–67. [Google Scholar]
  21. Grossman P, Niemann L, Schmidt S, Walach H. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits: A meta-analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2004;57:35–43. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00573-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hofmann S, Sawyer A, Witt A, Oh D. The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2010;78:169–183. doi: 10.1037/a0018555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Horvath AO, Greenberg LS. Development and validation of the Working Alliance Inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 1989;36:223–233. [Google Scholar]
  24. Horvath AO, Luborsky L. The role of the therapeutic alliance in psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1993;61:561–573. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.61.4.561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Horvath AO, Symonds BD. Relation between working alliance and outcome in psychotherapy: A meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 1991;38:139–149. [Google Scholar]
  26. Kabat-Zinn J. Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your mind and body to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Delacorte; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  27. Kabat-Zinn J. Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice. 2003;10:144–156. [Google Scholar]
  28. Keng S-L, Smoski MJ, Robins CJ. Effects of mindfulness on psychological health: A review of empirical studies. Clinical Psychology Review. 2011;31(6):1041–1056. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kenny MA, Williams JMG. Treatment resistant depressed patients show a good response to mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2007;45:617–625. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kim Y, Lee S, Choi T, Suh S, Kim B, Kim C, et al. Effectiveness of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy as an adjuvant to pharmacotherapy in patients with panic disorder or generalized anxiety disorder. Depression and Anxiety. 2009;26:601–606. doi: 10.1002/da.20552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Krasner M, Epstein R, Beckman H, Suchman A, Chapman B, Mooney C, et al. Association of an educational program in mindful communication with burnout, empathy, and attitudes among primary care physicians. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association. 2009;302:1284–1293. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ma SH, Teasdale JD. Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for depression: Replication and exploration of differential relapse prevention effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:31–40. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Marlatt GA, Gordon JR. Relapse prevention: Maintenance strategies in the treatment of addictive behaviors. New York: Guilford Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  34. Martin D, Garske J, Davis M. Relation of the therapeutic alliance with outcome and other variables: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:438–450. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Meier P, Barrowclough C, Donmall M. The role of the therapeutic alliance in the treatment of substance misuse: A critical review of the literature. Addiction. 2005;100:304–316. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00935.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Raes F, Dewulf D, Van Heeringen C, Williams J. Mindfulness and reduced cognitive reactivity to sad mood: Evidence from a correlational study and a non-randomized waiting list controlled study. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2009;47:623–627. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Ree M, Craigie M. Outcomes following mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in a heterogeneous sample of adult outpatients. Behaviour Change. 2007;24:70–86. [Google Scholar]
  38. Schenström A, Rönnberg S, Bodlund O. Mindfulness-based cognitive attitude training for primary care staff: A pilot study. Complementary Health Practice Review. 2006;11:144–152. [Google Scholar]
  39. Segal ZV, Williams JMG, Teasdale JD. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: A new approach for preventing relapse. New York: Guilford Press; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  40. Shapiro S, Carlson L, Astin J, Freedman B. Mechanisms of Mindfulness. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2006;62:373–386. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Shapiro S, Oman D, Thoresen C, Plante T, Flinders T. Cultivating mindfulness: Effects on well-being. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2008;64:840–862. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Shaver PR, Lavy S, Saron CD, Mikulincer M. Social foundations of the capacity for mindfulness: An attachment perspective. Psychological Inquiry. 2007;18:264–271. [Google Scholar]
  43. Siegel DJ. The mindful brain: Reflection and attunement in the cultivation of well-being. New York: Norton; 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Sweet A. The therapeutic relationship in behavior therapy. Clinical Psychology Review. 1984;4:253–272. [Google Scholar]
  45. Teasdale JD, Segal ZV, Williams JMG, Ridgeway VA, Soulsby JM, Lau MA. Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2000;68:615–623. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.68.4.615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Tracey TJ, Kokotovic AM. Factor structure of the Working Alliance Inventory. Psychological Assessment. 1989;1:207–210. [Google Scholar]
  47. Vettese L, Toneatto T, Stea J, Nguyen L, Wang J. Do mindfulness meditation participants do their homework? And does it make a difference? A review of the empirical evidence. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy. 2009;23:198–225. [Google Scholar]
  48. Vieten C, Astin JA, Buscemi R, Galloway GP. Development of an acceptance-based coping intervention for alcohol dependence relapse prevention. Substance Abuse. 2010;31:108–116. doi: 10.1080/08897071003641594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Waddington L. The therapy relationship in cognitive therapy: A review. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy. 2002;30:179–192. [Google Scholar]
  50. Zgierska A, Rabago D, Chawla N, Kushner K, Koehler R, Marlatt A. Mindfulness meditation for substance use disorders: A systematic review. Substance Abuse. 2009;30:266–294. doi: 10.1080/08897070903250019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES