Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2014 Jan 27;9(1):e87653. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087653

The Impact of Stressful Life Events on Excessive Alcohol Consumption in the French Population: Findings from the GAZEL Cohort Study

Sara L Tamers 1,2,*, Cassandra Okechukwu 1, Alex A Bohl 3, Alice Guéguen 4,5, Marcel Goldberg 4,5, Marie Zins 4,5
Editor: Antonio Verdejo García6
PMCID: PMC3903768  PMID: 24475318

Abstract

Background

Major life changes may play a causative role in health through lifestyle factors, such as alcohol. The objective was to examine the impact of stressful life events on heavy alcohol consumption among French adults.

Methods

Trajectories of excessive alcohol consumption in 20,625 employees of the French national gas and electricity company for up to 5 years before and 5 years after an event, with annual measurements from 1992. We used repeated measures analysis of time series data indexed to events, employing generalized estimating equations.

Results

For women, excessive alcohol use increased before important purchase (p = 0.021), children leaving home (p<0.001), and death of loved ones (p = 0.03), and decreased before widowhood (p = 0.015); in the year straddling the event, increased consumption was observed for important purchase (p = 0.018) and retirement (p = 0.002); at the time of the event, consumption decreased for marriage (p = 0.002), divorce, widowhood, and death of loved one (all p<0.001), and increased for retirement (p = 0.035). For men, heavy alcohol consumption increased in the years up to and surrounding the death of loved ones, retirement, and important purchase (all p<0.001), and decreased after (all p<0.001, except death of loved one: p = 0.006); at the time of the event, consumption decreased for all events except for children leaving home and retirement, where we observed an increase (all p<0.001). For women and men, heavy alcohol consumption decreased prior to marriage and divorce and increased after (all p<0.001, except for women and marriage: p = 0.01).

Conclusion

Stressful life events promote healthy and unhealthy alcohol consumption. Certain events impact alcohol intake temporarily while others have longer-term implications. Research should disentangle women's and men's distinct perceptions of events over time.

Introduction

Stressful life events (SLEs) leading to or requiring a life adjustment can have a significant impact on health [1], [2], [3], [4], [5], [6], [7], [8]. Defined as events that significantly disturb daily routines [9]—as opposed to minor stressors—SLEs can include both desirable events (e.g., marriage), undesirable events (e.g., death of loved ones), as well as mixed ones that comprise both desirable and undesirable elements (e.g., retirement) [10]. As a result of stress, certain SLEs can lead to the uptake of unhealthy behaviors, such as excessive alcohol intake [11]—a chief cause of morbidity [12] and early mortality [13]; conversely, others may contribute to healthy behaviors [14]. An increasing number of studies have examined the complex relationship between SLEs and alcohol [15], [16], [17], [18], [19], [20]; however, methodological differences restrict the extent of their application to public health interventionists and policy makers.

Relatively few studies have explored the relationships between SLEs and alcohol, longitudinally [14], [15], [21], [22], [23], which curbs the ability to understand temporality and trajectories in alcohol use. Of those studies that have taken a longitudinal approach, many have grouped SLEs rather than assess the individual impact of a number of these key changes [22]. Given that events may be perceived as desirable, undesirable, or mixed, this method is limited as it does not capture the fact that each SLE may influence health behaviors differently. Additionally, much of the research on this theme has focused on younger generations, despite the fact that middle-aged and older adult populations have become an increasingly prominent group. As such, they are overwhelmingly affected by important SLEs, such as children leaving home and widowhood; however, comparatively little is known about the natural history of alcohol use or misuse among these groups [15], [24], [25] and of its association with SLEs experienced across a broad timeframe.

Finally, a vast majority of alcohol research [26] and studies on its relationship with SLEs stem from the U.S. [15], [17], [21], [22]. Exploring these associations in non-U.S. contexts is important to further the field, in view of the vast social and cultural differences in both the aging process and alcohol use in European contexts. France is a unique country to study alcohol trajectories and their relationship with SLEs, as the French have one of the world's longest life expectancies while also having one of the highest alcohol consumption rates among industrialized nations [27]. Indeed, alcohol is often consumed daily by a majority of the French population [28], with only 35 percent of women and 17 percent of men abstaining from alcohol [28]. More recently, excessive drinking has become a public health issue in France, resulting in premature deaths [28], [29].

Building upon earlier work [27], [30], [31], the goal of this study was to examine the impact of SLEs representing two major life domains (i.e., interpersonal and financial) on excessive alcohol intake among a group of middle-aged and older French adults.

Methods

Study population

We used data from the GAZEL cohort consisting of 20,625 employees, aged 35–50 years at inception, of the French national gas and electricity company who were recruited to complete an annual self-administered questionnaire since 1989. Participants were asked a variety of questions related to their socio-demographics, health behaviors, life experiences, and occupational characteristics. The GAZEL cohort has been described in great detail in previous studies [32], [33].

Ethics statement

Approval for the GAZEL study was received by La Commission Nationale de l'Informatique et des Libertés and from L'Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale's Instutional Review Board.

Measures

Stressful life events

Guided by previous literature, we included eight SLEs that capture common experiences in interpersonal and financial-related domains [15], [27], [30], [34]; interpersonal: 1) marriage, 2) children leaving home, 3) divorce, 4) widowhood, and 5) death of loved ones; financial: 6) employment promotion, 7) important purchases, and 8) retirement. All SLEs were ranked in Holmes' and Rahe's Social Readjustment Scale as significant life events that pertain to major areas impacting adult life [10]. We determined the first instance after 1989 that an individual experienced a given SLE in the past 12 months. SLEs were coded as dichotomous (yes/no) variables.

Excessive alcohol consumption

Given that heavy drinking has become an important public health concern among many industrialized nations, including France [28], [29], we focused on excessive alcohol intake as our outcome. GAZEL participants' alcohol consumption was ascertained from a validated instrument, culturally-adapted to the drinking habits of populations who consume alcohol on a usual basis, such as the French and Italians [31], [35]. Questions included: “Have you consumed any wine, beer or cider, spirits over the past week? If yes, what was the maximum quantity per day (number of glasses)? On how many days during the past week did you drink wine, beer or cider, and spirits?” To facilitate participant response, drawings representing standard glasses for each alcohol type and amount were included below each question. Weekly maximum alcohol consumption was computed for wine, beer or cider, and spirits by multiplying the maximum number of glasses consumed by the number of days of drinking. As per the World Health Organization, the U.S. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, and France's Institut de Recherche et Documentation en Economie de la Santé guidelines, this was defined as at least 14 drinks per week for women and at least 28 drinks per week for men [28], [36], [37].

Covariates

Research has shown that certain socio-demographic variables, including age (35–39, 40–44, 45–49, 50–54, 55–59, 60–64, 65+) [18], [20], health status (excellent, good, fair, poor) [38], education (< high school, high school, > high school) [18], [20], and employment grade (manual worker/clerk, administrative associate/technician, manager) [31] may account for some of the observed effect of SLEs on alcohol. These covariates were included a priori and adjusted for in our multivariable models as they were found to be statistically significant at the bivariate stage.

Statistical analyses

The GAZEL study began collecting participant data in 1989; however, alcohol data were only collected from 1992 onwards. At the time this study was performed, the authors had access to survey data through the end of 2010. We included solely those individuals who provided information on alcohol consumption for the year of the SLE, as well as for at least two surveys before and two surveys after each SLE. Thus, the final analyses used up to 11 years of data for each individual who experienced an event. Individuals could experience multiple events within and between categories. Evidence has shown that first occurrences are more impactful than subsequent ones as the latter often become progressively less critical [39]; thus, our analysis focused on the first time an event was experienced in a given category for each individual. Women and men typically have different drinking habits [28] and may respond differently to SLEs [18], [40]. Further, evidence has shown that the maginitude of the effect between SLEs and alcohol behavior may be different by sex [18], [20]; since effect modification was found, we present stratified analyses.

The statistical analyses had two purposes. First, we sought to test for differences in heavy alcohol consumption in four different time periods (before, during, after, and at the time of each event), adjusting for socio-demographics and temporal trends in alcohol consumption. To test for heavy alcohol consumption before the experienced event—considered as time 0—we tested for a difference one year pre-event compared to five years pre-event (year −1 versus −5). To test for heavy alcohol consumption during the time of the event, we tested for a difference one year post-event compared to one year pre-event (year +1 versus −1). To test for heavy alcohol consumption after the event, we tested for a difference five years post-event compared to one year post-event (year +5 versus +1). Finally, we examined heavy alcohol consumption at the time of the event (year 0).

Second, we plotted model-predicted excessive alcohol intake for five years before and five years after each SLE, adjusted for covariates. Least-squares means estimates were used to produce graphs showing trajectories in heavy alcohol consumption rates over time.

The data were correlated by individuals; thus, we used repeated measures analysis of time series data indexed to life events. To do so, we used logistic regression with generalized estimating equations (GEE) and a logit link and binomial family, in STATA version 10 [41]. To allow for time varying covariates, we used an independent working correlation with robust standard errors. The unit of analysis was the person-year, and each person contributed at least five but up to 11 observations. Such a model has been used previously for studies using GAZEL data [42], [43].

Results

Over 17 years of observation (1992 through 2008), 98% (20,224 of 20,625) of the GAZEL cohort participants experienced at least one SLE. Missing survey response rates varied by SLE and survey period relative to the event, but the majority of participants (80–95%) had observations in pre- and post-event survey periods. Depending on the event and sex, between 577 (women who got married) to 14,442 (men who retired) individuals experienced an event; thus, the final sample size varied for each event and outcome.

Tables 1 and 2 describe the percentage of participants who experienced SLEs, separately for women and men, within socio-demographic categories. For both sexes, the most frequently experienced event was retirement; the majority of SLEs occurred before the age of 60; and widowhood occurred at older ages. Across all events, roughly 60% of women and 70% of men had greater than a high school education, and the majority reported being in excellent or good health, at the time of the event. Most women and men were employed in an intermediate position (defined as administrative associate/technician) as of 1990.

Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics for GAZEL women, at time of each life event.

Marriage Employment promotion Important purchase Children leaving home Divorce Retirement Widowhood Death of a loved one
N 577 829 3237 2417 987 4465 589 4147
Age at event
35–39 24% 10% 3% 0% 24% 0% 10% 3%
40–44 37% 37% 17% 8% 33% 0% 15% 20%
45–49 22% 33% 25% 35% 17% 0% 16% 27%
50–54 9% 17% 25% 36% 11% 33% 14% 27%
55–59 5% 3% 19% 16% 9% 53% 16% 16%
60–64 2% 0% 9% 4% 5% 14% 20% 7%
65+ 1% 0% 2% 1% 1% 0% 9% 2%
Education
<High school 27% 26% 26% 24% 26% 29% 29% 26%
High school 11% 15% 12% 12% 13% 9% 9% 11%
> High school 60% 56% 60% 62% 58% 60% 59% 60%
Self-rated health
Excellent 28% 21% 24% 21% 29% 17% 24% 23%
Good 44% 40% 53% 53% 41% 38% 45% 53%
Fair 12% 11% 19% 22% 18% 12% 22% 21%
Poor 1% 1% 2% 2% 3% 1% 3% 2%
Employment
Manual Worker/Clerk 29% 63% 21% 22% 26% 22% 23% 22%
Admin Associate/Technician 64% 37% 69% 68% 66% 70% 70% 69%
Manager 8% 0% 10% 10% 8% 8% 7% 9%

Note: Cells may not always add up to 100% or total N due to rounding and/or missing data

Table 2. Socio-demographic characteristics for GAZEL men, at time of each life event.

Marriage Employment promotion Important purchase Children leaving home Divorce Retirement Widowhood Death of a loved one
N 738 1518 10012 6449 1845 14442 1126 11459
Age at event
35–39 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0%
40–44 35% 15% 4% 0% 30% 0% 7% 5%
45–49 38% 49% 26% 18% 27% 0% 12% 26%
50–54 17% 32% 33% 43% 15% 34% 14% 33%
55–59 7% 3% 24% 29% 16% 55% 25% 22%
60–64 2% 0% 11% 8% 10% 10% 30% 11%
65+ 1% 0% 3% 1% 2% 0% 12% 3%
Education
<High school 20% 19% 18% 16% 19% 19% 17% 19%
High school 8% 8% 6% 6% 7% 6% 7% 6%
> High school 70% 72% 74% 76% 72% 73% 73% 74%
Self-rated health
Excellent 35% 26% 31% 29% 32% 23% 26% 30%
Good 43% 43% 53% 52% 43% 38% 52% 52%
Fair 10% 11% 14% 17% 16% 11% 17% 15%
Poor 2% 1% 1% 1% 2% 1% 1% 1%
Employment
Manual Worker/Clerk 14% 29% 10% 10% 13% 13% 11% 11%
Admin Associate/Technician 57% 71% 53% 53% 56% 56% 53% 55%
Manager 30% 0% 36% 38% 31% 32% 35% 34%

Note: Cells may not always add up to 100% or total N due to rounding and/or missing data

Tables 3 and 4 show odds of heavy alcohol consumption in four different time periods (years −1 versus −5; +1 versus −1; +5 versus +1; year 0) for each SLE and for women and men, separately. Most of the significant results observed for both sexes were increases in heavy alcohol use, especially during the time period surrounding the event (men). For women, excessive alcohol use increased in the years before important purchase, children leaving home, and death of loved one, and decreased for widowhood. In the year straddling the event, increased consumption was observed for important purchase and retirement, only. At the time of the event, excessive alcohol use decreased for marriage, divorce, widowhood, and death of loved one, and increased for retirement. For men, heavy alcohol consumption increased in the years up to and surrounding the death of a loved one, retirement, and important purchase, and then decreased after the experienced event. Increased consumption was observed in the years before children leaving home, and in the year surrounding and after receiving an employment promotion. At the time of the event, excessive alcohol use decreased for all events except for children leaving home and retirement, where we observed an increase.

Table 3. Heavy alcohol use for women during and around stressful life events*.

Year −1 vs. −5 Year +1 vs. −1 Year +5 vs. +1 Year 0
OR P 95% CI OR P 95% CI OR P 95% CI OR P 95% CI
Marriage 0.192 0.028 0.04–0.84 1.7 0.568 0.27–10.71 5.35 0.01 1.49–19.24 0.24 0.002 0.1–0.59
Employment promotion 0.991 0.99 0.25–3.86 2.06 0.073 0.94–4.54 1.41 0.152 0.88–2.29 0.6 0.09 0.33–1.07
Important purchase 1.4 0.021 1.05–1.86 1.26 0.018 1.04–1.55 0.854 0.135 0.7–1.05 0.87 0.075 0.74–1.01
Children leaving home 1.9 <0.001 1.48–2.64 1.09 0.37 0.9–1.34 0.958 0.717 0.76–1.21 0.91 0.31 0.75–1.1
Divorce 0.15 <0.001 0.07–0.33 1.47 0.278 0.73–2.99 3.16 <0.001 1.77–5.68 0.22 <0.001 0.14–0.36
Retirement 1.23 0.063 0.99–1.54 1.34 0.002 1.11–1.62 0.877 0.187 0.72–1.07 1.19 0.035 1.01–1.4
Widowhood 0.516 0.015 0.3–0.88 0.857 0.453 0.58–1.28 1.28 0.356 0.75–2.21 0.4 <0.001 0.24–0.64
Death of loved one 1.33 0.03 1.03–1.74 1.11 0.245 0.93–1.35 1.04 0.607 0.88–1.25 0.76 <0.001 0.66–0.88

Year 0 is the year of the experienced life event

*

Adjusted for age, self-rated health, and education and employment grade at time of event

Table 4. Heavy alcohol use for men during and around stressful life events*.

Year −1 vs. −5 Year +1 vs. −1 Year +5 vs. +1 Year 0
OR P 95% CI OR P 95% CI OR P 95% CI OR P 95% CI
Marriage 0.101 <0.001 0.04–0.24 2.45 0.015 1.19–5.05 4.41 <0.001 2.79–6.99 0.14 <0.001 0.09–0.23
Employment promotion 1.08 0.7 0.72–1.64 1.74 <0.001 1.37–2.21 1.26 0.014 1.05–1.53 0.69 <0.001 0.56–0.84
Important purchase 1.36 <0.001 1.25–1.49 1.32 <0.001 1.25–1.41 0.885 <0.001 0.83–0.94 0.9 <0.001 0.85–0.95
Children leaving home 2.27 <0.001 2.06–2.52 0.989 0.769 0.93–1.06 0.938 0.086 0.87–1.01 1.3 <0.001 1.22–1.39
Divorce 0.379 <0.001 0.3–0.48 1.28 0.009 1.06–1.55 2 <0.001 1.66–2.42 0.39 <0.001 0.33–0.46
Retirement 1.15 <0.001 1.08–1.23 1.38 <0.001 1.32–1.47 0.869 <0.001 0.83–0.92 1.21 <0.001 1.15–1.27
Widowhood 0.845 0.141 0.68–1.06 1.1 0.251 0.93–1.31 1 0.976 0.8–1.26 0.71 <0.001 0.6–0.83
Death of loved one 1.27 <0.001 1.17–1.38 1.36 <0.001 1.29–1.45 0.925 0.006 0.88–0.98 0.86 <0.001 0.81–0.9

Year 0 is the year of the experienced life event.

*

Adjusted for age, self-rated health, and education and employment grade at time of event.

For both sexes, marriage and divorce followed similar patterns in that heavy alcohol intake initially decreased and then increased in the years after the event. Of note, men increased their intake around the time of the event and in the period after, while women only increased theirs in the years after the event. Further supplemental analyses of risk of excessive alcohol use for additional time periods (Tables S1 and S2 in File S1) are included as a supplement to the paper.

Figure 1 plots model-predicted heavy alcohol consumption in the five years before and five years after each experienced SLE, adjusted for covariates. These graphs provide perspective to the results provided in tables 3 and 4, as they illustrate trajectories of excessive alcohol consumption relative to a life event. In all cases, heavy consumption was more frequent for men. As seen in Figure 1, at the time of marriage and divorce, we noted the most significant decrease in the percentage of heavy alcohol consumers. For all other events, we observed a gradual increase over time.

Figure 1. Percentage of heavy drinkers around time of stressful life events, separately for women and men.

Figure 1

Discussion

Summary of findings

In this study, we sought to examine the relationship between a number of commonplace SLEs capturing important life domains and excessive alcohol consumption before, during, and after the experienced event, among a large sample of French women and men. We also further explored trends of heavy alcohol rates over time for each SLE. With some exceptions, overall, within each sample of women and men, our findings suggest a differential impact by time period, domain, and SLE type; thus, some generalizations can be made.

In the years prior to experiencing an event, for both women and men, we perceived decreases in heavy alcohol consumption before marriage, divorce, and widowhood (women only), and increases before important purchase, children leaving home, retirement (men only), and death of loved ones. These fluxes in heavy alcohol use in the years prior to experiencing an event may be due to the anticipation of said events; evidence has shown that anticipated or lead effects may be reflected months or even years before the event occurs [44]. In the year before compared to the year after an experienced event, we noted only increases in excessive alcohol intake, with women experiencing these for important purchase and retirement, and men experiencing these for all SLEs except for children leaving home and widowhood. In the years subsequent to experiencing an event, we saw an increase in heavy alcohol consumption for women after marriage and divorce. For men, we also found an increase in the years after marriage and divorce, as well as after employment promotion, and decreases after important purchase, retirement, and death of loved one. These findings suggest that certain SLEs may impact heavy alcohol use even years after the event [45]. At the time of the event, we observed mostly decreased heavy alcohol use for over half of the examined events for women and all events for men.

Comparison with previous studies

Though some disparities were noted, by and large, our results are in accordance with previous research on similar themes revealing that many SLEs have an impact on alcohol behavior not only at the time of the event, as supported by our findings, but also in the years before and after [16], [21], [22], [23], [31], [46]. Like Perreira and Sloan (2001), we found that retirement was associated with increases in intake and that marriage and divorce were associated with both increases and decreases in heavy alcohol use at different time periods [22]. As reflected in prior evidence, we found increases in alcohol consumption in the years precipitating children leaving home [47], [48]. Previous studies have found that experiencing the death of a loved one and widowhood can have varying impacts on drinking practices [22], [49]. Both involve loss; still, similar alcohol behavior patterns are not assured for both, across time, or by sex, which could help explain differences in the directionality of our findings. For instance, an anticipated or unexpected death might influence health outcomes differently [50], and the degree of care-giving in the case of an anticipated bereavement [51] might also play a role. The nature and saliency of the relationship between the bereaved and deceased could have a variable influence on alcohol intake [49], [52], as could a change in social network norms [53]. We found no significant results with respect to heavy alcohol use for women receiving an employment promotion in our main analyses but significant ones for men, which is in line with other like studies [54]; however, the directionality for men was opposite. Making a large purchase, which could be viewed as a proxy measure for reduced finances or more financial strain, has been associated with increased alcohol use among older adults [55].

While we cannot compare differences between the two separate samples of women and men, we can comment on perceived trends across both. Men's excessive alcohol consumption tended to be more frequently impacted by life events, which has been observed previously [18], [23]. This finding may be explained in part by the fact that there are sex differences in both alcohol consumption [56] and in the impact of life events on intake; posited explanations include distinct support seeking and coping styles [57], as well as stigmatization and societal norms [18]. We also observed that women typically experienced increases in their heavy alcohol intake across most of the time periods, whereas men appeared to experience both increases and decreases in intake, more equally. Financial-related SLEs seemed to influence men's heavy alcohol use more often compared to women, who were more frequently impacted by interpersonal events. Still, we observed similar patterns for interpersonal events for both sexes, which may suggest that a change in social relationships has a greater impact on heavy alcohol use.

When examining overall differences across time, certain SLEs influenced excessive alcohol intake years after the event, while others appeared to have only short-term effects or none at all. The latter may be a result of adaptation [45], meaning that individuals tend to adjust relatively quickly to life changes [58] because they find explanations for undesirable events [59] and get used to desirable ones [45]. This may partially explain some perceived trends in the years subsequent to experiencing certain SLEs, intimating that levels of heavy alcohol use eventually return to those similar pre-event. It may also provide clarity for some seemingly counter-intuitive results—marital shift resulting in a desirable outcome (i.e., marriage) and undesirable one (i.e., divorce)—showing similar alcohol use patterns. Indeed, we found that a number of SLEs—be they desirable, undesirable, or mixed—had both a healthy and unhealthy impact on heavy alcohol intake in different time periods. Research has found that SLEs perceived to be positive (i.e., marriage, employment promotion), negative (i.e., divorce, widowhood, death of loved one), or mixed (i.e., important purchase, children leaving home, retirement) may have distinct associations with well-being measures [45] and that this impact may even prove to be differential by SLE, across time. Further, the impact of negatively-perceived SLEs may be stronger and/or more persistent than positively- or mixed-perceived ones [60], [61], [62].

Limitations and strengths

One potential limitation of this study is recall bias given that respondents were asked about events experienced in the previous 12 months. It has also been suggested that certain SLEs may be subject to reporting bias as those perceived to be negative may be more likely to be recollected [16], [63]. Overestimation of the proportion of excessive drinkers could be present if levels of drinking vary greatly day-to-day; however, this would be balanced by the fact that alcohol intake is typically underreported [31]. Even so, the way weekly intake was computed should not contribute much to overestimation; this is further strengthened by the fact that the rate of heavy drinkers in the GAZEL cohort is comparable to that in the French population measured via other surveying tools [64]. There likely exist cultural differences that may limit the generalizability of our findings to solely French or Mediterranean populations with similar alcohol drinking practices. Still, this study has important strengths. We incorporated a number of SLEs that are universal experiences covering central life domains. We also studied heavy alcohol use, which has become an increasingly common public health issue among industrialized nations. An additional strength of our study is that the GAZEL cohort enabled us to look at trajectories within a large and stable population over a number of years. Moreover, few large-scale epidemiological studies have explored these relationships longitudinally and among both a middle-aged and/or older-adult population in a non-US context [14], [21], [22], [23].

Conclusion

In summary, our findings suggest that a number of SLEs across significant domains may promote both healthy and unhealthy alcohol consumption at different periods in time. While some events might impact alcohol intake only temporarily, others may have longer-term implications. What’s more, even if a short-term effect is not detected, experiencing a SLE might set an individual on a trajectory of increased heavy alcohol consumption over time. From a clinical practice stand-point, providers should regularly ascertain middle-aged and older adults' drinking habits—paying particular focus to experienced or imminent SLEs—and offer appropriate resources to address unhealthy drinking practices, should they exist. From a research perspective, given observed differences by domain, SLE type, and timeframe, future studies should attempt to disentangle distinct perceptions of SLEs (i.e., desirable, undesirable, or mixed) over time. Such evidence could help elucidate the mechanisms by which SLEs can both promote deleterious drinking practices and encourage healthier ones.

Supporting Information

File S1

Tables S1 and S2. Table S1. Logistic regression predicting heavy alcohol use around stressful life events for women in the GAZEL cohort. Table S2. Logistic regression predicting heavy alcohol use around stressful life events for men in the GAZEL cohort.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to express their gratitude to the EDF-GDF and acknowledge the invaluable assistance of the GAZEL cohort study team.

Funding Statement

This research was supported by the EDF-GDF, l'INSERM, l'Association de la Recherche sur le Cancer, la Fondation de France, le Ministère de la Santé, the NIH/NCI Harvard Education Program in Cancer Prevention (R25 CA057713; PI: Glorian Sorensen, PhD), and the NIH/NCI Reducing Social Disparities in Cancer Risk (K05 CA108663-05 to GS). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Rahe RH, Meyer M, Smith M, Kjaer G, Holmes TH (1964) Social Stress and Illness Onset. J Psychosom Res 54: 35–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Graham D, Stevenson I (1963) Disease as a response to life stress, The Psychological Basis of Medical Practice. New York: Harper & Row.
  • 3. Greene WA Jr (1954) Psychological factors and reticuloendothelial disease. I. Preliminary observations on a group of males with lymphomas and leukemias. Psychosom Med 16: 220–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Fischer HK, Dlin BM, Winters WL Jr, Hagner SB, Russell GW, et al. (1964) Emotional Factors in Coronary Occlusion. Ii. Time Patterns and Factors Related to Onset. Psychosomatics 5: 280–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lief A (1948) The Commonsense Psychiatry of Dr. Adolf Meyer. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  • 6. Rahe RH, Christ AE (1966) An unusual cardiac (ventricular) arrhythmia in a child: psychiatric and psychophysiologic aspects. Psychosom Med 28: 181–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Wolff H, Wolf S, Hare C (1950) Life Stress and Bodily Disease. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
  • 8. Rahe RH, McKean JD Jr, Arthur RJ (1967) A longitudinal study of life-change and illness patterns. J Psychosom Res 10: 355–366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Turner RJ, Wheaton B (1997) Checklist measurement of stressful life events.; Cohen S, Kessler RC, Gordon LU, editors. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • 10. Holmes TH, Rahe RH (1967) The Social Readjustment Rating Scale. J Psychosom Res 11: 213–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Pohorecky LA (1991) Stress and alcohol interaction: an update of human research. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 15: 438–459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Mokdad AH, Marks, J S, Stroup, D F, Gerberding, J L (2004) Actual causes of death in the United States, 2000. JAMA 291: 1238–1245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Klatsky AL, Armstrong MA, Friedman GD (1992) Alcohol and mortality. Ann Intern Med 117: 646–654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Brennan PL, Schutte KK, Moos RH (1999) Reciprocal relations between stressors and drinking behavior: a three-wave panel study of late middle-aged and older women and men. Addiction 94: 737–749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Platt A, Sloan FA, Costanzo P (2010) Alcohol-consumption trajectories and associated characteristics among adults older than age 50. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 71: 169–179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Keyes KM, Hatzenbuehler ML, Hasin DS (2011) Stressful life experiences, alcohol consumption, and alcohol use disorders: the epidemiologic evidence for four main types of stressors. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 218: 1–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Veenstra MY, Lemmens PH, Friesema IH, Garretsen HF, Knottnerus JA, et al. (2006) A literature overview of the relationship between life-events and alcohol use in the general population. Alcohol Alcohol 41: 455–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Dawson DA, Grant BF, Ruan WJ (2005) The association between stress and drinking: modifying effects of gender and vulnerability. Alcohol Alcohol 40: 453–460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Cole G, Tucker L, Friedman GM (1990) Relationships among measures of alcohol drinking behavior, life-events and perceived stress. Psychol Rep 67: 587–591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. King AC, Bernardy NC, Hauner K (2003) Stressful events, personality, and mood disturbance: gender differences in alcoholics and problem drinkers. Addict Behav 28: 171–187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Romelsjo A, Lazarus NB, Kaplan GA, Cohen RD (1991) The relationship between stressful life situations and changes in alcohol consumption in a general population sample. Br J Addict 86: 157–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Perreira KM, Sloan FA (2001) Life events and alcohol consumption among mature adults: a longitudinal analysis. J Stud Alcohol 62: 501–508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Glass TA, Prigerson H, Kasl SV, Mendes de Leon CF (1995) The effects of negative life events on alcohol consumption among older men and women. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 50: S205–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Bobo JK, Greek AA, Klepinger DH, Herting JR (2010) Alcohol use trajectories in two cohorts of U.S. women aged 50 to 65 at baseline. J Am Geriatr Soc 58: 2375–2380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Merrick EL, Horgan CM, Hodgkin D, Garnick DW, Houghton SF, et al. (2008) Unhealthy drinking patterns in older adults: prevalence and associated characteristics. J Am Geriatr Soc 56: 214–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Van Ruyven M, Veenstra J (1993) An inventory of the alcohol research literature. Alcohol Alcohol 28: 599–605. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Zins M, Carle F, Bugel I, Leclerc A, Di Orio F, et al. (1999) Predictors of change in alcohol consumption among Frenchmen of the GAZEL study cohort. Addiction 94: 385–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Com-Ruelle L, Dourgnon P, Jusot F, Lengagne P Questions d'économie de la Santé: Alcohol consumption in France: one more glass of French paradox, 2008.
  • 29.Ministère de l'Emploi et de la Solidarité Le Haut Comité de la santé publique: La Santé en France 2002.
  • 30. Zins M, Gueguen A, Leclerc A, Goldberg M (2003) Alcohol consumption and marital status of French women in the GAZEL cohort: a longitudinal analysis between 1992 and 1996. J Stud Alcohol 64: 784–789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Zins M, Gueguen A, Kivimaki M, Singh-Manoux A, Leclerc A, et al. (2011) Effect of retirement on alcohol consumption: longitudinal evidence from the French gazel cohort study. PLoS One 6: e26531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Goldberg M, Leclerc A, Bonenfant S, Chastang JF, Schmaus A, et al. (2007) Cohort profile: the GAZEL Cohort Study. Int J Epidemiol 36: 32–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Zins M, Leclerc A, Goldberg M (2009) The French GAZEL Cohort Study: 20 years of epidemiologic research. Advances in Life Course Research 14: 135–146. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Rahe RH, Ryman DH, Ward HW (1980) Simplified scaling for life change events. J Human Stress 6: 22–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Corrao G, Lepore A, Rapone C, Miccoli C, Di Orio F (1991) Reproducibility of an alcohol questionnaire for a case-control study on chronic liver diseases. Epidemiol 13: 45–49. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.World Health Organization (2000) International Guide for Monitoring Alcohol Consumption and Related Harm. Geneva: WHO.
  • 37.National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism website (2013). Available: http://www.niaaa.nih.gov/Publications/AlcoholResearch/. Accessed June 2.
  • 38. Bobo JK, Greek AA (2011) Increasing and decreasing alcohol use trajectories among older women in the U.S. across a 10-year interval. Int J Environ Res Public Health 8: 3263–3276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Post RM (1992) Transduction of psychosocial stress into the neurobiology of recurrent affective disorder. Am J Psychiatry 149: 999–1010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Kendler KS, Thornton LM, Prescott CA (2001) Gender differences in the rates of exposure to stressful life events and sensitivity to their depressogenic effects. Am J Psychiatry 158: 587–593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.StataCorp. 2007. Stata Statistical Software: Release 10. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.
  • 42. Vahtera J, Westerlund H, Hall M, Sjosten N, Kivimaki M, et al. (2009) Effect of retirement on sleep disturbances: the GAZEL prospective cohort study. Sleep 32: 1459–1466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Westerlund H, Kivimaki M, Singh-Manoux A, Melchior M, Ferrie JE, et al. (2009) Self-rated health before and after retirement in France (GAZEL): a cohort study. Lancet 374: 1889–1896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Clark AE, Diener E, Georgellis Y, Lucas RE (2008) Lags and leads in life satisfaction: A test of the baseline hypothesis. The Economic Journal 118: F222–F243. [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Luhmann M, Hofmann W, Eid M, Lucas RE (2012) Subjective well-being and adaptation to life events: a meta-analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol 102: 592–615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Hemminki K, Li X (2003) Lifestyle and cancer: effect of widowhood and divorce. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev 12: 899–904. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Borland DC (1982) A cohort analysis approach to the empty-nest syndrome among three ethnic groups of women: A theoretical position. J Marr Fam 44: 117–129. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Curlee J (1969) Alcoholism and the empty-nest. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic 33.. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Umberson D, Chen MD (1994) Effects of a parent's death on adult children: Relationship salience and reaction to loss. Amer Sociol Rev 59: 152–168. [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Shah SM, Carey IM, Harris T, Dewilde S, Victor CR, et al. (2013) The effect of unexpected bereavement on mortality in older couples. Am J Public Health 103: 1140–1145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Ross A, Sundaramurthi T, Bevans M (2012) A Labor of Love: The Influence of Cancer Caregiving on Health Behaviors. Cancer Nurs. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 52. Dawson DA, Grant BF, Chou SP, Stinson FS (2007) The impact of partner alcohol problems on women's physical and mental health. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 68: 66–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Moos RH, Schutte KK, Brennan PL, Moos BS (2011) Personal, family and social functioning among older couples concordant and discordant for high-risk alcohol consumption. Addiction 106: 324–334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Virtanen P, Vahtera J, Broms U, Sillanmaki L, Kivimaki M, et al. (2008) Employment trajectory as determinant of change in health-related lifestyle: the prospective HeSSup study. Eur J Public Health 18: 504–508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Shaw BA, Agahi N, Krause N (2011) Are changes in financial strain associated with changes in alcohol use and smoking among older adults? J Stud Alcohol Drugs 72: 917–925. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Ferreira MP, Weems MK (2008) Alcohol consumption by aging adults in the United States: health benefits and detriments. J Am Diet Assoc 108: 1668–1676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Hussong AM (2003) Further refining the stress-coping model of alcohol involvement. Addict Behav 28: 1515–1522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Frederick S, Loewenstein G (1999) Hedonic Adaptation; Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwarz N, editors. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.
  • 59. Taylor SE (1983) Adjustment to threatening events: A theory of cognitive adaptation. American Psychologist 38: 1161–1173. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Baumeister RF, Bratslavsky E, Finkenauer C, Vohs KD (2001) Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology 5.. [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Frederickson BL, Losada MF (2005) Positive affect and the complex dynamics of human flourishing. American Psychologist 60: 678–686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Larsen RJ, Prizmic Z (2008) Regulation of emotional well-being: Overcoming the hedonic treadmill; Eid M, Larsen RJ, editors. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • 63. Meyer IH (2003) Prejudice as stress: conceptual and measurement problems. Am J Public Health 93: 262–265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Institut national de prévention et d'éducation pour la santé (2007) Barometre Sante 2005. In: Guilbert P, Gautier A, editors. Saint Denis, FR.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

File S1

Tables S1 and S2. Table S1. Logistic regression predicting heavy alcohol use around stressful life events for women in the GAZEL cohort. Table S2. Logistic regression predicting heavy alcohol use around stressful life events for men in the GAZEL cohort.

(DOCX)


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES