Abstract
Written and verbal languages are neurobehavioral traits vital to the development of communication skills. Unfortunately, disorders involving these traits—specifically reading disability (RD) and language impairment (LI)—are common and prevent affected individuals from developing adequate communication skills, leaving them at risk for adverse academic, socioeconomic and psychiatric outcomes. Both RD and LI are complex traits that frequently co-occur, leading us to hypothesize that these disorders share genetic etiologies. To test this, we performed a genome-wide association study on individuals affected with both RD and LI in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. The strongest associations were seen with markers in ZNF385D (OR = 1.81, P = 5.45 × 10−7) and COL4A2 (OR = 1.71, P = 7.59 × 10−7). Markers within NDST4 showed the strongest associations with LI individually (OR = 1.827, P = 1.40 × 10−7). We replicated association of ZNF385D using receptive vocabulary measures in the Pediatric Imaging Neurocognitive Genetics study (P = 0.00245). We then used diffusion tensor imaging fiber tract volume data on 16 fiber tracts to examine the implications of replicated markers. ZNF385D was a predictor of overall fiber tract volumes in both hemispheres, as well as global brain volume. Here, we present evidence for ZNF385D as a candidate gene for RD and LI. The implication of transcription factor ZNF385D in RD and LI underscores the importance of transcriptional regulation in the development of higher order neurocognitive traits. Further study is necessary to discern target genes of ZNF385D and how it functions within neural development of fluent language.
Keywords: ALSPAC, dyslexia GWAS, language impairment, PING, reading disability, ZNF385D
The development of reading and verbal language skills through early childhood and into adolescence is vital to a child's academic performance, self-perception of cognitive abilities and development of sociability. Reading disability (RD) and language impairment (LI) are two common language-based learning disabilities with prevalence estimates of 5–17% and 5–8%, respectively (Pennington & Bishop 2009; Peterson & Pennington 2012). RD and LI are characterized by unexplained difficulties in written and verbal language, respectively, despite adequate intelligence, educational and socioeconomic opportunity (Pennington & Bishop 2009; Peterson & Pennington 2012). RD and LI have lifelong detrimental effects on communication and language skills, particularly without early intervention. RD and LI are frequently comorbid; e.g. children diagnosed with LI are more likely to develop RD later in childhood (Pennington 2006). Additionally, children with RD and/or LI exhibit deficits in many of the same neurocognitive domains, including phonological processing, comprehension, fluency and phonological short-term memory (Catts et al. 2005; Gathercole & Baddeley 1990; Pennington 2006; Pennington & Bishop 2009; Wise et al. 2007).
The relatedness between RD and LI goes deeper than similarity in clinical presentation. RD and LI share numerous risk factors and associated genes, as both are complex disorders with substantial genetic contributors (Pennington & Bishop 2009; Scerri & Schulte-Korne 2010). Linkage, candidate gene association and rare variant studies have identified genes that contribute to RD and/or LI (Graham & Fisher 2013; Newbury et al. 2009, 2011; Pinel et al. 2012; Rice et al. 2009; Scerri et al. 2011). Some of these risk genes, including DCDC2, KIAA0319, FOXP2, CNTNAP2 and CMIP, contribute to both RD and LI (Newbury et al. 2011; Peter et al. 2011; Powers et al. 2013; Scerri et al. 2011; Wilcke et al. 2011). These studies suggest that RD and LI share certain risk genes that influence core language processes. However, genome-wide association studies (GWAS) on reading and language are limited. Recently, Luciano et al. (2013) completed a GWAS on quantitative performance on reading- and language-related measures. The strongest associations were seen between ABCC13 and nonword repetition. These analyses identified novel genes and loci for performance on written and verbal language tasks, but do not address disorder states (i.e. RD or LI) nor the common comorbidity of RD and LI.
Neuroimaging studies of written and verbal language have identified various brain regions and measures important for fluent language and altered in impaired individuals (Shaywitz & Shaywitz 2008; Vandermosten et al. 2012). Some argue that these imaging differences may represent a mediatory step between genetic risk variants and the ultimate clinical phenotype (Eicher & Gruen 2013). Thus, recent studies have used these neuroimaging measures as endophenotypes in their analyses. These imaging-genetic studies have associated RD and LI risk genes—including FOXP2, CNTNAP2, KIAA0319, DCDC2 and C2orf3—with various brain imaging phenotypes—including brain activation patterns, white and grey matter volumes and fiber tract volumes (Cope et al. 2012; Darki et al. 2012; Eicher & Gruen 2013; Liegeois et al. 2003; Pinel et al. 2012; Scott-Van Zeeland et al. 2010; Scerri et al. 2012; Tan et al. 2010; Wilcke et al. 2011).
The goal of this investigation is to identify novel genes that contribute to the overlap of RD and LI by performing a GWAS on subjects with both RD and LI in an extensively phenotyped birth cohort: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). The large number of neurocognitive assessments in the ALSPAC allows for the simultaneous analysis of RD and LI. By doing so, we aim to identify new genes that contribute to both RD and LI. We then replicate our results in the Pediatric Imaging Neurocognition Genetics (PING) study using oral reading and receptive vocabulary measures. For replicated markers, we test for associations with fiber tract volumes previously implicated in language.
Materials and methods
Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children
Subject recruitment and collection of phenotype and genetic data for the ALSPAC cohort were completed by the ALSPAC team. The ALSPAC is a prospective population-based, birth cohort based on the Avon region of the UK. It consists mainly of children of northern European descent, born in 1991 and 1992. Children were recruited before birth; recruitment of their pregnant mothers resulted in a total of 15 458 fetuses, of whom 14 701 were alive at 1 year of age. Details regarding the participants, recruitment and study methodologies are described in detail elsewhere (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac) (Boyd et al. 2012; Golding et al. 2001). The children of the ALSPAC have been extensively phenotyped from before birth to early adulthood. Ethical approval was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee, Local UK Research Ethics Committees, and the Yale Human Investigation Committee.
Reading and language measures
The reading, language and cognitive measures used for this study were collected at ages 7, 8 and 9 years. Subjects with IQ ≤ 75 on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III) Total IQ, completed at age 8 years, were excluded from the presented analyses (Wechsler et al. 1992). Reading measures in the ALSPAC include a phoneme deletion task at age 7, single-word reading at ages 7 and 9 years, single nonword reading at age 9 years, and reading passage comprehension at age 9 years. The phoneme deletion task measures phoneme awareness, widely considered to be a core deficit in both RD and LI (Pennington 2006; Pennington & Bishop 2009). For the phoneme deletion task, also known as the Auditory Analysis Test, the child listens to a word spoken aloud, and is then asked to remove a specific phoneme from that word to make a new word (Rosner & Simon 1971). Single-word reading was assessed at age 7 using the reading subtest of the Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions (WORD). At age 9, single-word and nonword reading were assessed by asking the child to read 10 real words and 10 nonwords aloud from a subset of a larger list of words and nonwords taken from research conducted by Terezinha Nunes and colleagues (Rust et al. 1993). Reading comprehension scores were ascertained at age 9, using the Neale Analysis of Reading Ability (NARA-II) (Neale 1997). Two additional language measures, nonword repetition and verbal comprehension tasks, were collected during clinical interviews at age 8 years. An adaptation of the Nonword Repetition Task (NWR), in which subjects repeated recordings of nonwords, was used to assess short-term phonological memory and processing (Gathercole & Baddeley 1996). Children also completed the Wechsler Objective Language Dimensions (WOLD) verbal comprehension task, where they answered questions about a paragraph read aloud by an examiner describing a presented picture (Wechsler 1996). z-Scores were calculated for each subject on each individual measure.
Case definitions
We aimed to capture persistently poor performers in various reading and verbal language domains as RD and LI cases in our case definitions (Table 1). Therefore, we defined RD cases as having a z-score less than or equal to −1 on at least 3 of the 5 following tasks: single-word reading at age 7 years, phoneme deletion at age 7 years, single-word reading at age 9 years, nonword reading at age 9 years, and reading comprehension at age 9 years. There were 527 subjects defined as RD cases. We defined LI cases as having a z-score less than or equal to −1 on at least 2 of the 3 following tasks: phoneme deletion at age 7 years, verbal comprehension at age 8 years, and nonword repetition at age 8 years. There were 337 subjects defined as LI cases. As phoneme awareness is important in both RD and LI, we chose to include it as a part of the case definition for both RD and LI to reflect clinical presentation. There were 174 individuals affected with both RD and LI, with a male to female ratio of 1.7:1. In the further characterization of observed associations, we created subsets of cases with no comorbidity. There were 163 LI cases excluding those with comorbid RD, and 353 RD cases excluding those with comorbid LI (Fig. 1). For all analyses, controls were defined as ALSPAC subjects of European ancestry who completed all the necessary neurobehavioral assessments but did not meet the criteria for case status.
Table 1.
RD (n = 527)* | LI (n = 337)† |
---|---|
Phoneme deletion age 7 years | Phoneme deletion age 7 years |
Single-word reading age 7 years | Verbal comprehension age 8 years |
Single-word reading age 9 years | Nonword repetition age 8 years |
Nonword reading age 9 years | |
Reading comprehension age 9 years |
RD cases had a z-score of less than or equal to −1 on at least 3 of the 5 reading measures.
LI cases had a z-score of less than or equal to −1 on at least 2 of the 3 language measures.
Genotyping and analysis
Subjects were genotyped on Illumina HumanHap 550 bead arrays (San Diego, CA, USA). Subjects were excluded if the percentage of missing genotypes was greater than 2% (n = 6). To prevent possible population stratification, only subjects of European ancestry were included. In our primary analysis of RD and LI individuals, there were 174 cases and 4117 controls. There were a total of 500 527 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) genotyped before quality assessment and quality control. Markers were removed if Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium P ≤ 0.0001 (n = 93) or if missingness was greater than 10% (n = 19). All markers had a minor allele frequency greater than 0.01. All genetic analyses were performed using logistic regression in plink v1.07 (Purcell et al. 2007). To correct for multiple testing, we set a Bonferroni corrected threshold of α = 1.00 × 10−7 = 0.05/500 000 markers tested.
Following our initial analyses examining cases with both RD and LI, we further examined RD and LI case definitions individually (i.e. LI excluding those with comorbid RD, and RD excluding those with comorbid LI). These analyses were completed to determine whether a single disorder (RD or LI) was driving association signals in the comorbid RD and LI analysis (Fig. 1). We also examined the associations of markers within several previously identified RD and/or LI risk genes, including those recently reported in Luciano et al. (2013), in order to present their results with these phenotypic definitions. These genes included: ABCC13, ATP2C2, BC0307918, CMIP, CNTNAP2, DAZAP1, DCDC2, DYX1C1, FOXP2, KIAA0319, KIAA0319L, PRKCH, ROBO1 and TDP2.
Gene-based analyses were performed on each phenotype (comorbid RD and LI, as well as RD and LI individually) using the VEGAS program, similar to the Luciano et al. study (Liu et al. 2010; Luciano et al. 2013). To correct for multiple testing, we set a Bonferroni corrected threshold of α = 2.84 × 10−6 = 0.05/17 610 genes tested.
PING replication analyses
Replication analyses were completed in the PING study. Details on the recruitment, ascertainment, neurobehavioral, genetic and neuroimaging methods and data acquisition in the PING study are described in detail elsewhere, but are summarized briefly below (Akshoomoff et al. in press, Brown et al. 2012; Fjell et al. 2012; Walhovd et al. 2012). The PING study is a cross-sectional cohort of typically developing children between the ages of 3 and 20 years. Subjects were screened for history of major developmental, psychiatric, and/or neurological disorders, brain injury or medical conditions that affect development. However, subjects were not excluded due to learning disabilities such as RD and LI. The human research protections programs and institutional review boards at the 10 institutions (Weil Cornell Medical College, University of California at Davis, University of Hawaii, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Massachusetts General Hospital, University of California at Los Angeles, University of California at San Diego, University of Massachusetts Medical School, University of Southern California and Yale University) participating in the PING study approved all experimental and consenting procedures. For individuals under 18 years of age, parental informed consent and child assent (for those 7–17 years of age) were obtained. All participants age 18 years and older gave their written informed consent.
Subjects completed the validated study version of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery, in which two language- and reading-related tasks were completed: the Oral Reading Recognition Test and Picture Vocabulary Test (Akshoomoff et al. in press; Weintraub et al. 2013). In the Oral Reading Recognition Test, a word or letter is presented on the computer screen and the participant is asked to read it aloud. Responses are recorded as correct or incorrect by the examiner, who views accepted pronunciations on a separate computer screen. The Picture Vocabulary Test is a measure of receptive vocabulary and administered in a computerized adaptive format. The participant is presented with an auditory recording of a word and four images on the computer screen; the task is to touch the image that most closely represents the meaning of the word.
Subjects were genotyped on the Illumina Human660W-Quad BeadChip (San Diego, CA, USA), with markers used for replication analyses passing quality control filters (sample call rate > 98%, SNP call rate > 95%, minor allele frequency > 5%). We constructed a reference panel as described elsewhere (Brown et al. 2012; Fjell et al. 2012; Walhovd et al. 2012). To assess ancestry and admixture proportions in the PING participants, we used a supervised clustering approach implemented in the admixture software (Alexander et al. 2009) and clustered participant data into six clusters corresponding to six major continental populations: African, Central Asian, East Asian, European, Native American and Oceanic. Implementation of ancestry and admixture proportions in the PING subjects is described in detail elsewhere (Brown et al. 2012; Fjell et al. 2012; Walhovd et al. 2012). To prevent possible population stratification, only subjects with a European genetic ancestry factor (GAF) of 1 were included in genetic analysis of behavior. These 440 individuals of European ancestry [mean age of 11.5 (SD = 4.8) years, 53.0% male] were analyzed using quantitative performance on the Oral Reading Recognition and Picture Vocabulary scores with plink v1.07, with age included as a covariate (Purcell et al. 2007). To correct for multiple testing (20 total tests = 10 SNPs × 2 language measures), we set statistical thresholds using the false discovery rate with α = 0.05 (Benjamini & Hochberg 1995).
PING imaging analysis
PING imaging techniques, data acquisition and analyses are discussed in depth elsewhere and briefly below (Brown et al. 2012; Fjell et al. 2012; Walhovd et al. 2012). Across the 10 sites and 12 scanners, a standardized multiple modality high-resolution structural MRI protocol was implemented, involving 3D T1- and T2-weighted volumes and a set of diffusion-weighted scans. At the University of California at San Diego, data were obtained on a GE 3T SignaHD× scanner and a 3T Discovery 750× scanner (GE Healthcare, Waukesha, WI, USA) using eight-channel phased array head coils. The protocol included a conventional three-plane localizer, a sagittal 3D inversion recovery spoiled gradient echo T1-weighted volume optimized for maximum gray/white matter contrast (echo time = 3.5 milliseconds, repetition time = 8.1 milliseconds, inversion time = 640 milliseconds, flip angle = 8°, receiver bandwidth = ± 31.25 kHz, FOV = 24 cm, frequency = 256, phase = 192, slice thickness = 1.2 mm), and two axial 2D diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) pepolar scans (30-directions b value = 1000, TE = 83 milliseconds, TR = 13 600 milliseconds, frequency = 96, phase = 96, slice thickness = 2.5 mm). Acquisition protocols with pulse sequence parameters identical or near identical to those protocols used at the University of California at San Diego were installed on scanners at the other nine sites. Data were acquired on all scanners to estimate relaxation rates and measure and correct for scanner-specific gradient coil nonlinear warping. Image files in DICOM format were processed with an automated processing stream written in MATLAB (Natick, MA, USA) and C++ by the UCSD Multimodal Imaging Laboratory. T1-weighted structural images were corrected for distortions caused by gradient nonlinearities, coregistered, averaged and rigidly resampled into alignment with an atlas brain. Image postprocessing and analysis were performed using a fully automated set of tools available in the FreeSurfer software suite (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) as well as an atlas-based method for delineating and labeling WM fiber tracts (Fischl 2012).
Diffusion tensor imaging
Diffusion-weighted images were corrected for eddy current distortion using a least square inverse and iterative conjugate gradient descent method to solve for the 12 scaling and translation parameters describing eddy current distortions across the entire diffusion MRI scan, explicitly taking into account the orientations and amplitudes of the diffusion gradient (Zhuang et al. 2006). Head motion was corrected by registering each diffusion-weighted image to a corresponding image synthesized from a tensor fit to the data (Hagler et al. 2009). Diffusion MRI data were corrected for spatial and intensity distortions caused by B0 magnetic field in-homogeneities using the reversing gradient method (Holland et al. 2010). Distortions caused by gradient nonlinearities were corrected by applying a predefined, scanner-specific, nonlinear transformation (Jovicich et al. 2006). Diffusion-weighted images were automatically registered to T1-weighted structural images using mutual information (Wells et al. 1996) and rigidly resampled into a standard orientation relative to the T1-weighted images with isotropic 2-mm voxels. Cubic interpolation was used for all resampling steps. Conventional DTI methods were used to calculate diffusion measures (Basser et al. 1994; Pierpaoli et al. 1996). Scanning duration for the DTI sequence was 4:24 min. White matter fiber tracts were labeled using a probabilistic-atlas-based segmentation method (Hagler et al. 2009). Voxels containing primarily gray matter or cerebral spinal fluid, identified using FreeSurfer's automated brain segmentation, were excluded from analysis (Fischl et al. 2002). Fiber tract volumes were calculated as the number of voxels with probability greater than 0.08, the value that provided optimal correspondence in volume between atlas-derived regions of interest and manually traced fiber tracts.
Statistical analyses
Imaging-genetics analyses were performed in individuals of European genetic ancestry. Scanner, age, handedness, socioeconomic status and sex were included as covariates in all analyses (Akshoomoff et al. in press; Brown et al. 2012; Fjell et al. 2012; Walhovd et al. 2012). 332 subjects of European genetic ancestry had completed imaging measures that passed PING quality control. Fiber tract volumes in 16 tracts of interest were tested by multiple regression analyses in R using the PING data portal (https://mmil-dataportal.ucsd.edu).
Results
SNP and gene-based associations
The 10 strongest GWAS associations with comorbid RD and LI in ALSPAC are presented in Table 2. The strongest associations were observed with ZNF385D (OR = 1.81, P = 5.45 × 10−7) and COL4A2 (OR = 1.71, P = 7.59 × 10−7) (Table 2). Next, we examined RD and LI individually—with no comorbid cases included—determining whether one disorder was driving these associations. The 10 strongest associations for RD cases and LI cases individually are presented in Tables3 and 4, respectively. The strongest associations with LI were with markers in NDST4 (OR = 1.83, P = 1.40 × 10−7) (Table 3). Markers on chromosome 10 (OR = 1.43, P = 5.16 × 10−6), chromosome 8 (OR = 1.70, P = 5.85 × 10−6) and the OPA3 gene (OR = 1.53, P = 6.92 × 10−6) had the strongest associations with RD (Table 4). Markers with P < 0.01 within genes previously implicated in RD and/or LI are presented in Table S1, Supporting Information for each phenotype. The strongest associations with these markers were seen for KIAA0319 with comorbid RD and LI (rs16889556, P = 0.0005177), FOXP2 with comorbid RD and LI (rs1530680, P = 0.0001702), CNTNAP2 with LI (rs6951437, P = 0.0000462) and DCDC2 with LI (rs793834, P = 0.0002679) (Table S1a–S1c). Gene-based analyses were completed on each phenotype (comorbid RD and LI, RD individually and LI individually), and the 10 strongest gene-based associations are presented in Table S2. None of the gene-based associations survived correction for multiple testing; however, the strongest associations were seen with: (1) OR5H2, OR5H6 and RRAGA with comorbid RD and LI, (2) NEK2, DLEC1 and NARS with LI and (3) MAP4, OR2L8 and CRYBA4 with RD. Markers with the strongest P-values in discovery analyses in ZNF385D, COL4A2 and NDST4 were carried forward for replication analysis in PING. We observed replication of two markers within ZNF385D and performance on the Picture Vocabulary Test (P = 0.00245 and 0.004173) (Table 5). However, markers did not replicate with the Oral Reading Recognition Test (P > 0.05).
Table 2.
Marker | Chr | Base pair | Minor allele | MAF Aff | MAF Unaff | Gene | Odds ratio | P value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
rs12636438 | 3 | 22038281 | G | 0.3017 | 0.1927 | ZNF385D | 1.811 | 5.45 × 10−7 |
rs1679255 | 3 | 22022938 | C | 0.3006 | 0.1923 | ZNF385D | 1.805 | 6.87 × 10−7 |
rs9521789 | 13 | 109917621 | C | 0.5201 | 0.3879 | COL4A2 | 1.71 | 7.59 × 10−7 |
rs1983931 | 13 | 109916103 | G | 0.5201 | 0.3896 | COL4A2 | 1.698 | 1.06 × 10−6 |
rs9814232 | 3 | 21948179 | A | 0.2931 | 0.1886 | ZNF385D | 1.784 | 1.30 × 10−6 |
rs7995158 | 13 | 109909718 | A | 0.5201 | 0.3911 | 1.687 | 1.44 × 10−6 | |
rs6573225 | 14 | 58354640 | C | 0.1965 | 0.1122 | 1.935 | 1.56 × 10−6 | |
rs4082518 | 10 | 17103032 | T | 0.3103 | 0.2049 | CUBN | 1.746 | 2.17 × 10−6 |
rs442555 | 14 | 58365937 | C | 0.1983 | 0.1149 | 1.905 | 2.38 × 10−6 | |
rs259521 | 3 | 21942154 | T | 0.2902 | 0.1885 | ZNF385D | 1.761 | 2.42 × 10−6 |
Chr, chromosome; MAF Aff, minor allele frequency in affected subjects; MAF Unaff, minor allele frequency in unaffected subjects.
Table 3.
Marker | Chr | Base Pair | Minor Allele | MAF Aff | MAF Unaff | Gene | Odds Ratio | P value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
rs482700 | 4 | 116286939 | G | 0.3896 | 0.2588 | NDST4 | 1.827 | 1.40 × 10−7 |
rs7695228 | 4 | 116309516 | T | 0.3920 | 0.2636 | NDST4 | 1.801 | 2.94 × 10−7 |
rs1940309 | 4 | 116306410 | T | 0.3865 | 0.2606 | NDST4 | 1.788 | 4.14 × 10−7 |
rs505277 | 4 | 116248257 | T | 0.3773 | 0.2528 | NDST4 | 1.791 | 4.35 × 10−7 |
rs476739 | 4 | 116248997 | A | 0.3773 | 0.2529 | NDST4 | 1.79 | 4.41 × 10−7 |
rs867036 | 4 | 116381578 | C | 0.3957 | 0.2696 | NDST4 | 1.774 | 5.31 × 10−7 |
rs867035 | 4 | 116381423 | C | 0.3957 | 0.2697 | NDST4 | 1.773 | 5.45 × 10−7 |
rs2071674 | 4 | 2366882 | T | 0.0920 | 0.0389 | ZFYVE28 | 2.503 | 1.90 × 10−6 |
rs7694946 | 4 | 116413588 | C | 0.3620 | 0.2526 | NDST4 | 1.678 | 8.95 × 10−6 |
rs4823324 | 22 | 44616787 | C | 0.2914 | 0.4143 | ATXN10 | 0.581 | 9.30 × 10−6 |
Chr, chromosome; MAF Aff, minor allele frequency in affected subjects; MAF Unaff, minor allele frequency in unaffected subjects.
Table 4.
Marker | Chr | Base pair | Minor allele | MAF Aff | MAF Unaff | Gene | Odds ratio | P value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
rs180950 | 10 | 115697957 | G | 0.456 | 0.369 | 1.431 | 5.16 × 10−6 | |
rs2590673 | 8 | 126037337 | G | 0.133 | 0.083 | 1.697 | 5.85 × 10−6 | |
rs892100 | 19 | 50772522 | C | 0.228 | 0.162 | OPA3 | 1.526 | 6.92 × 10−6 |
rs1792745 | 18 | 51955991 | T | 0.187 | 0.129 | 1.558 | 1.22 × 10−5 | |
rs12546767 | 8 | 126151747 | C | 0.152 | 0.099 | KIAA0196 | 1.618 | 1.32 × 10−5 |
rs12634033 | 3 | 146524529 | C | 0.135 | 0.087 | 1.646 | 1.80 × 10−5 | |
rs892270 | 12 | 105002956 | G | 0.534 | 0.451 | NUAK1 | 1.395 | 2.16 × 10−5 |
rs10887149 | 10 | 124156994 | A | 0.278 | 0.357 | PLEKHA1 | 0.690 | 2.25 × 10−5 |
rs10041417 | 5 | 33218502 | T | 0.226 | 0.164 | 1.489 | 2.58 × 10−5 | |
rs6792971 | 3 | 68468217 | C | 0.111 | 0.068 | FAM19A1 | 1.703 | 2.59 × 10−5 |
Chr, chromosome; MAF Aff, minor allele frequency in affected subjects; MAF Unaff, minor allele frequency in unaffected subjects.
Table 5.
Oral Reading Test | Picture Vocabulary Test | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Marker | Minor allele | MAF | Gene | Beta | P value | Beta | P value |
rs12636438 | G | 0.161 | ZNF385D | −0.1867 | 0.9452 | −2.88 | 0.004173* |
rs1679255 | G | 0.292 | ZNF385D | −1.84 | 0.5016 | −3.048 | 0.002445** |
rs9521789 | G | 0.4370 | COL4A2 | −0.3411 | 0.7332 | 0.8647 | 0.3877 |
rs476739 | A | 0.265 | NDST4 | 0.5406 | 0.5891 | 0.5159 | 0.6062 |
rs505277 | A | 0.280 | NDST4 | 0.5406 | 0.5891 | −0.3452 | 0.7301 |
rs482700 | G | 0.278 | NDST4 | 0.5498 | 0.5828 | −0.05341 | 0.9574 |
rs7695228 | A | 0.295 | NDST4 | 0.6258 | 0.5318 | 0.09991 | 0.9205 |
rs867036 | G | 0.378 | NDST4 | 0.2605 | 0.7946 | −0.1414 | 0.8876 |
rs867035 | G | 0.377 | NDST4 | 0.2961 | 0.7673 | −0.1565 | 0.8757 |
rs1940309 | A | 0.281 | NDST4 | 0.6049 | 0.5456 | 0.1296 | 0.8969 |
MAF, minor allele frequency in full PING sample.
P value less than FDR-adjusted statistical threshold (FDR-adjusted threshold = 0.05 × (2/19) = 0.00526.
P value less than FDR-adjusted statistical threshold (FDR-adjusted threshold = 0.05 × (1/20) = 0.00250.
Imaging-genetics of ZNF385D
To follow-up on the replicated associations of ZNF385D, we examined the effects of these variants on fiber tract volumes previously implicated in written and verbal language. Before doing so, we determined that fiber tract volume was a predictor of performance on Oral Reading Recognition and Picture Vocabulary Tests (Fig. 2a,b). Within subjects of only European genetic ancestry, ZNF385D genotypes were predictors of overall fiber tract volume as well as fiber tract volumes in the right and left hemispheres (Table 6). ZNF385D SNPs were also predictors bilaterally within the inferior longitudinal fasiculus (ILF), inferior fronto-occipto fasiculus (IFO) and temporal superior longitudinal fasiculus (tSLF) in this subset (Table 6). To discern whether these associations between ZNF385D and fiber tract volumes reflect global brain volume differences among genotype, we next examined the relationship of ZNF385D with both total brain segmentation and total cortical volumes. We found associations for both measures with rs1679255 (P = 0.00072 and 0.00027, respectively) and rs12636438 (P = 0.000259 and 0.000069, respectively). The effects appeared to be additive in nature, with heterozygous individuals having intermediate phenotypes relative to those homozygous for the major allele and to those homozygous for the minor allele. In fact, when these total brain volume measures were inserted into the model as a covariate, ZNF385D associations with DTI fiber tract volumes were no longer present.
Table 6.
rs1679255 | rs12636438 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Fiber tract | Slope | P value | Slope | P value |
All | −3329.9 | 0.044* | −3717.9 | 0.023* |
Right All | −1731.4 | 0.039* | −1965 | 0.017* |
Left All | −1616.3 | 0.055 | −1775.6 | 0.033* |
Right ILF | −251.3 | 0.011* | −234.4 | 0.016* |
Left ILF | −256.9 | 0.0088** | −254.6 | 0.009** |
Right IFO | −200.8 | 0.032* | −190 | 0.041* |
Left IFO | −221 | 0.012* | −226.3 | 0.009** |
Right SLF | −168.1 | 0.06 | −206 | 0.02* |
Left SLF | −199.5 | 0.022* | −212.9 | 0.013* |
Right tSLF | −170.8 | 0.011* | −180.7 | 0.0068** |
Left tSLF | −163.1 | 0.023* | −169.9 | 0.016* |
Right pSLF | −153.1 | 0.079 | −182.4 | 0.034* |
Left pSLF | −112.2 | 0.18 | −125.3 | 0.131 |
Right SIFC | −148.8 | 0.052 | −165.6 | 0.029* |
Left SIFC | −34.54 | 0.66 | −54.3 | 0.48 |
CC | −977.1 | 0.15 | −1181.6 | 0.081 |
All, all fiber tracts; CC, corpus callosum; pSLF, parietal superior longitudinal fasiculus; SLF, superior longitudinal fasiculus; SIFC, striatal inferior frontal cortex.
P ≤ 0.05.
P ≤ 0.01.
Discussion
In this investigation, we sought to identify genes that contribute to the common co-occurrence of RD and LI. In our discovery analyses, we found associations of ZNF385D and COL4A2 in comorbid cases, and of NDST4 with LI. Next, we observed associations of ZNF385D with performance on a vocabulary measure, but not on an oral reading measure, in PING. Association with performance on a vocabulary measure, although not exactly recapitulating the comorbidity phenotype, does provide further evidence for the contribution of ZNF385D to language. To gain functional understanding, we interrogated the effects of replicated ZNF385D markers on the volumes of language-related fiber tracts. ZNF385D markers associated bilaterally with overall fiber tract volumes and overall brain volume.
Studies have shown that RD and LI share genetic contributors (Trzaskowski et al. 2013). However, specific genes that contribute to both RD and LI have only recently begun to be examined. These studies have used a candidate gene approach to examine this shared genetic etiology. Such an approach has been successful in showing the shared contribution of DCDC2, KIAA0319, FOXP2, CNTNAP2, among others, to both RD and LI (Eicher & Gruen 2013; Graham & Fisher 2013; Newbury et al. 2009, 2011; Pinel et al. 2012; Rice et al. 2009; Scerri et al. 2011). In fact, markers within KIAA0319, FOXP2 and CNTNAP2 (along with BC0307918) showed nominal association with comorbid RD and LI in our analyses (P < 0.01). RD/LI risk genes also showed a tendency to associate with LI individually (DCDC2, KIAA0319 and CNTNAP2) and with RD individually (CNTNAP2 and CMIP) (P < 0.01). The lack of replication for other RD/LI risk genes and differences between this study and those of Scerri et al. (2011) and Luciano et al. (2013) are likely a result of different case definitions and numbers, as we designed our case classifications to capture a wide range of reading- and language-impaired subjects, as opposed to using highly specific neurocognitive measures.
A glaring omission in the genetic investigations of RD and LI is the lack of hypothesis-free methods. These methods allow for discovery of new genes because they do not rely on pre-selected candidates. Here, our GWAS analyses indicate that ZNF385D contributes to comorbid RD and LI. Our study is not the first GWAS on reading- and language- related traits. Luciano et al. (2013) recently reported a GWAS of quantitative measures of written and verbal language measures in two population-based cohorts, including ALSPAC. They found strong evidence that ABCC13, BC0307918, DAZAP1, among others contribute to performance on these measures, although our analyses did not provide strong evidence for them. The analytical strategies differed in two ways: (1) the use of dichotomous rather than quantitative measures to condition genetic associations and (2) examining reading and language together as opposed to individually. Past association studies of RD and LI have shown differences in results depending on whether associations were conditioned on dichotomous or quantitative phenotypes. For instance, KIAA0319 tends to associate more readily with quantitative measures, while DCDC2 associates more often with dichotomized variables (Paracchini et al. 2008; Powers et al. 2013; Scerri et al. 2011). The present study, which examines comorbidity, and that of Luciano et al., which examined performance on reading and language tasks individually, conditioned genetic associations on different traits, which can lead to different statistical associations. Both analytical strategies are valid and have gleaned separate, yet related insight into the genetic underpinnings of written and verbal language. They demonstrate the importance of creative and careful examination of phenotypes when examining neurocognitive and other complex traits.
Following our primary analysis of comorbid RD and LI, we next examined RD and LI individually to determine whether a single disorder was driving the association signals. ZNF385D did not associate with either RD or LI individually, indicating that ZNF385D contributes to processes related to both RD and LI, as opposed to only one of these disorders. Within PING, we observed associations of ZNF385D markers with performance on the Picture Vocabulary Test and not the Oral Reading Recognition Test. Measures of receptive vocabulary (e.g. the Picture Vocabulary Test) are related to both written and verbal language tasks (Scarborough 1990; Wise et al. 2007), while performance on decoding measures (e.g. the Oral Reading Recognition Test) appear to be specific to reading. Therefore, the Picture Vocabulary Test may reflect the comorbid RD and LI phenotype used in ALSPAC better than the Oral Reading Recognition Test and explain the association pattern of ZNF385D in PING. In addition to ZNF385D, we observed suggestive associations of COL4A2 with comorbid RD/LI and NDST4 with LI. Neither of these associations replicated in PING, but future studies should attempt to replicate these associations, particularly due to the known involvement of COL4A2 in porencephaly and white matter lesions (Verbeek et al. 2012; Yoneda et al. 2012).
Gene-based analyses did not reveal any associations that survived correction for multiple testing. Nonetheless, there were intriguing gene associations that should be investigated in future studies. For instance, with LI, there were suggestive associations with genes on chromosome 19—IL4I1, ATF5, NUP62 and SIGLEC11—which may correspond to the SLI2 linkage peak (Monaco 2007; SLI Consortium 2002), Luciano et al. (2013) found a similar accumulation of suggestively associated genes approximately 5 Mb away from our genes. Additionally, MAP4, a microtubule assembly gene, was the strongest associated gene with RD. There is evidence microtubule function plays a key role in reading development as aberrant neuronal migration is thought to contribute to the etiology of RD and other RD candidate genes are thought to interact with microtubules (e.g. DCDC2 and ACOT13) (Cheng et al. 2006). Although intriguing, these suggestive findings must be validated in an independent cohort.
The strongest observed associations in this study were with markers within ZNF385D. ZNF385D has previously been implicated in schizophrenia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Poelmans et al. 2011; Xu et al. 2013). Both schizophrenia and ADHD are neurobehavioral disorders thought to have core impairments in common with RD and LI, including comprehension and semantic processing (Gilger et al. 1992; Li et al. 2009; Willcutt et al. 2005). Additionally, our observed association of ZNF385D on global brain volume may indicate that ZNF385D influences various neurocognitive traits through its effect on the entire brain.
There is little known regarding the function of ZNF385D, although its zinc finger domain suggests it is a transcriptional regulator. The importance of transcriptional regulation in written and verbal language is not a new concept. The most widely studied language gene, FOXP2, is a potent transcription factor that has been shown to regulate another language gene, CNTNAP2 (Vernes et al. 2007; Vernes et al. 2011). Additionally, in the DYX2 locus, two risk variants, READ1 within DCDC2 and the KIAA0319 risk haplotype, appear to have the capacity to regulate gene expression (Couto et al. 2010; Dennis et al. 2009; Meng et al. 2011) and possibly interact (Ludwig et al. 2008; Powers et al. 2013), although more evidence is needed to demonstrate these functionalities. ZNF385D variants now join this list of putative transcriptional variants that influence written and verbal language skills. The characterization of target genes of ZNF385D and of its transcriptional effects on these targets will be an important next step. Additionally, the identification of target genes may generate therapeutic candidates for treatment and remediation of RD and LI. To gain further insight into ZNF385D, we performed imaging-genetics analyses of ZNF385D and fiber tract volumes of language-related tracts. ZNF385D appears to modulate fiber tract and total brain volumes, which may subsequently affect the connectivity and functionality of brain regions important in the efficient, fluent integration of written and verbal language. Thus, identification of target genes and how the modulation of their expression during neural development yields differences in fiber tract and total brain volumes will be vital for dissecting not only the mechanism of ZNF385D, but also for the development of core language skills in children.
This study is subject to several limitations. First, although the overall sample size of the ALSPAC is formidable, the number of cases for each definition is relatively small. This is expected in a cross-sectional cohort of the general population as the prevalence of these disorders ranges between 5% and 17% (Pennington & Bishop 2009). The ALSPAC cohort would not be expected to be enriched for RD and/or LI cases. Small sample size could have hindered our statistical power and ability to identify risk genes with small effect size. Second, the reading and language measures performed in the ALSPAC and PING studies were not identical. Phenotypes in PING were treated as a quantitative trait rather than a dichotomous variable as in ALSPAC. Therefore, attempts to replicate associations observed in the ALSPAC cohort may have been hampered as reading/language measures in PING may have captured different skills than those in ALSPAC. However, the associations observed in the PING indicate that ZNF385D plays a substantial, consistent role in overall language processes. Third, atlas-derived tract volume measures, like volumes derived from manually traced fiber tracts, are likely underestimates of true fiber volume for most tracts. However, fiber tract volumes were derived consistently for all subjects and likely reflect inter-individual differences. Nonetheless, the strength and independent replication of our associations and the relationship with brain imaging phenotypes strongly implicate ZNF385D in core language processes underlying RD and LI.
In conclusion, we identify ZNF385D as a novel gene contributing to both RD and LI, as well as fiber tract and overall brain volume. The implication of another transcription factor in communication disorders underscores the importance of transcriptional regulation in neural development of language domains in the brain. Future studies should aim to further characterize the molecular functionality of ZNF385D and replicate this association, as well as our nonreplicated associations—NDST4 and COL4A2—in RD, LI and other related disorders.
Acknowledgments
We thank all the families and participants who took part in these studies. We also wish to acknowledge the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses. The UK medical research council and the Wellcome Trust (Grant ref: 092731) and the University of Bristol provide core support for the ALSPAC. The GWAS genotyping of the ALSPAC children samples was supported by 23andMe. The authors gratefully thank the children, adolescents, adults and parents who participated in the PING study. Data collection and sharing for this project were funded by the PING Study (National Institutes of Health Grant RC2DA029475). PING is funded by the National Institute on Drug Abuse and the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development. PING data are disseminated by the PING Coordinating Center at the Center for Human Development, University of California at San Diego. This research was specifically funded by the National Institutes of Health (Grant ref: R01 NS043530 awarded to J.R.G., and F31 DC012270 awarded to J.D.E.).
upporting Information
Additional supporting information may be found in the online version of this article at the publisher's web-site:
Table S1: Associations of markers within genes previously implicated in RD and/or LI with (a) Comorbid RD and LI, (b) LI individually and (c) RD individually.
Table S2: Gene-based analyses of comorbid RD and LI, LI individually and RD individually. The top 10 gene-based associations for each are shown.
References
- Akshoomoff N, Newman E, Thompson WK, McCabe C, Bloss CS, Chang L, Amaral DG, Casey BJ, Ernst TM, Frazier JA, Gruen JR, Kaufmann WE, Kenet T, Kennedy DN, Libiger O, Mostofsky S, Murray SS, Sowell ER, Schork N, Dale AM, Jernigan TL. The NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery: Results from a large normative Developmental Sample (PING) Neuropsychology. 2013 doi: 10.1037/neu0000001. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander DH, Novembre J, Lange K. Fast model-based estimation of ancestry in unrelated individuals. Genome Res. 2009;19:1655–1664. doi: 10.1101/gr.094052.109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Basser PJ, Mattiello J, LeBihan D. MR diffusion tensor spectroscopy and imaging. Biophys J. 1994;66:259–267. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(94)80775-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc B. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
- Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, Lawlor DA, Fraser A, Henderson J, Molloy L, Ness A, Ring S, Davey Smith G. Cohort profile: the ‘Children of the 90s’—the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;42:111–127. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown TT, Kuperman JM, Chung Y, et al. Neuroanatomical assessment of biological maturity. Curr Biol. 2012;22:1693–1698. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catts HW, Adlof SM, Hogan TP, Weismer SE. Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2005;48:1378–1396. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2005/096). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng Z, Bao S, Shan X, Xu H, Gong W. Human thioesterase superfamily member 2 (hTHEM2) is co-localized with beta-tubulin onto the microtubule. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2006;350:850–853. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2006.09.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cope N, Eicher JD, Meng H, Gibson CJ, Hager K, Lacadie C, Fulbright RK, Constable RT, Page GP, Gruen JR. Variants in the DYX2 locus are associated with altered brain activation in reading-related brain regions in subjects with reading disability. Neuroimage. 2012;63:148–156. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Couto JM, Livne-Bar I, Huang K, Xu Z, Cate-Carter T, Feng Y, Wigg K, Humphries T, Tannock R, Kerr EN, Lovett MW, Bremner R, Barr CL. Association of reading disabilities with regions marker by actylated H3 histones in KIAA0319. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2010;153B:447–462. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darki F, Peyrard-Janvid M, Matsson H, Kere J, Klingberg T. Three dyslexia susceptibility genes, DYX1C1, DCDC2, and KIAA0319, affect temporo-parietal white matter structure. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;72:671–676. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dennis MY, Paracchini S, Scerri TS, Prokunina-Olsson L, Knight JC, Wade-Martins R, Coggill P, Beck S, Green ED, Monaco AP. A common variant associated with dyslexia reduces expression of the KIAA0319 gene. PLoS Genet. 2009;5:e1000436. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1000436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eicher JD, Gruen JR. Imaging-genetics in dyslexia: connecting risk genetic variants to brain neuroimaging and ultimately to reading impairments. Mol Genet Metab. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.ymgme.2013.07.001. DOI: 10.1016/j.ymgme.2013.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B. FreeSurfer. Neuroimage. 2012;62:774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, van der Kouwe A, Killiany R, Klaveness S, Montillo A, Makris N, Rosen B, Dale AM. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33:41–55. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00569-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Brown TT, Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, and Genetics Study et al. Multimodal imaging of the self-regulating developing brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:19620–19625. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1208243109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole S, Baddeley AD. Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? J Mem Lang. 1990;29:336–360. [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole SE, Baddeley AD. The Children's Test of Nonword Repetition. London: The Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Gilger JW, Pennington BF, DeFries JC. A twin study of the etiology of comorbidity: attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and dyslexia. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1992;31:343–348. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199203000-00024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golding J, Pembrey M, Jones R, ALSPAC StudyTeam. ALSPAC—the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. I. Study methodology. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2001;15:74–87. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-3016.2001.00325.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graham SA, Fisher SE. Decoding the genetics of speech and language. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23:43–51. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagler DJ, Jr, Ahmadi ME, Kuperman J, Holland D, McDonald CR, Halgren E, Dale AM. Automated white-matter tractography using a probabilistic diffusion tensor atlas: application to temporal lobe epilepsy. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:1535–1547. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland D, Kuperman JM, Dale AM. Efficient correction of inhomogeneous static magnetic field-induced distortion in Echo Planar Imaging. Neuroimage. 2010;50:175–183. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jovicich J, Czanner S, Greve D, Haley E, van der Kouew A, Gollub R, Kennedy D, Schimitt F, Brown G, Macfall J, Fischl B, Dale A. Reliability in multi-site structural MRI studies: effects of gradient non-linearity correction on phantom and human data. Neuroimage. 2006;30:436–443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li X, Branch CA, DeLisi LE. Language pathway abnormalities in schizophrenia: a review of fMRI and other imaging studies. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2009;22:131–139. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e328324bc43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liegeois F, Baldeweg T, Connelly A, Gadian DG, Mishkin M, Vargha-Khadem F. Language fMRI abnormalities associated with FOXP2 gene mutation. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:1230–1237. doi: 10.1038/nn1138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu JZ, McRae AF, Nyholt DR, Medland SE, Wray NR, Brown KM, Hayward NK, Montgomery GW, Visscher PM, Martin NG, Macgregor S. A versatile gene-based test for genome-wide association studies. Am J Hum Genet. 2010;87:139–145. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luciano M, Evans DM, Hansell NK, Medland SE, Montgomery GW, Martin NG, Wright MJ, Bates TC. A genome-wide association study for reading and language abilities in two population cohorts. Genes Brain Behav. 2013 doi: 10.1111/gbb.12053. DOI: 10.1111/gbb.12053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludwig KU, Roeske D, Schumacher J, Schulte-Korne G, Konig IR, Warnke A, Plume E, Ziegler A, Remschmidr H, Muller-Hyhsok B, Nothen MM, Hoffmann P. Investigation of interaction between DCDC2 and KIAA0319 in a large German dyslexia sample. J Neural Transm. 2008;115:1587–1589. doi: 10.1007/s00702-008-0124-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meng H, Powers NR, Tang L, Cope NA, Zhang PX, Fuleihan R, Gibson C, Page GP, Gruen JR. A dyslexia-associated variant in DCDC2 changes gene expression. Behav Genet. 2011;41:58–66. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9408-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monaco AP. Multivariate linkage analysis of specific language impairment (SLI) Ann Hum Genet. 2007;71(Pt 5):660–673. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-1809.2007.00361.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neale MD. Windsor, United Kingdom: NFER-Nelson; 1997. Neale Analysis of Reading Ability—Revised: Manual for Schools. [Google Scholar]
- Newbury DF, Winchester L, Addis L, et al. CMIP and ATP2C2 modulate phonological short-term memory in language impairment. Am J Hum Genet. 2009;85:264–272. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newbury DF, Paracchini S, Scerri TS, Winchester L, Addis L, RIcharson AJ, Walter J, Stein JF, Talcott JB, Monaco AP. Investigation of dyslexia and SLI risk variants in reading- and language-impaired subjects. Behav Genet. 2011;41:90–104. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9424-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paracchini S, Steer CD, Buckingham LL, Morris AP, Ring S, Scerri T, Stein J, Pembrey ME, Ragiussis J, Golding J, Monaco AP. Association of the KIAA0319 dyslexia susceptibility gene with reading skills in the general population. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:1576–1584. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07121872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington BF. From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition. 2006;101:385–413. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington BF, Bishop DV. Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:283–306. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterson RL, Pennington BF. Developmental dyslexia. Lancet. 2012;379:1997–2007. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60198-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peter B, Raskind WH, Matsushita M, Lisowski M, Vu T, Berninger VW, Wijsman EM, Brkanac Z. Replication of CNTNAP2 association with nonword repetition and support for FOXP2 association with timed reading and motor activities in a dyslexia family sample. J Neurodev Disord. 2011;3:39–49. doi: 10.1007/s11689-010-9065-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierpaoli C, Jezzard P, Basser PJ, Barnett A, Di Chiro G. Diffusion tensor MR imaging of the human brain. Radiology. 1996;201:637–648. doi: 10.1148/radiology.201.3.8939209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinel P, Fauchereau F, Moreno A, Barbot A, Lathrop M, Zelenika D, Le Bihan D, Poline JB, Bourgeron T, Dehaene S. Genetics variants of FOXP2 and KIAA0319TTRAPTHEM2 locus are associated with altered brain activation in distinct language-related regions. J Neurosci. 2012;32:817–825. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5996-10.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poelmans G, Pauls DL, Buitelaar JK, Franke B. Integrated genome-wide association study findings: identification of a neurodevelopmental network for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168:365–377. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10070948. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powers NR, Eicher JD, Butter F, Kong Y, Miller LL, Ring SM, Mann M, Gruen JR. Alleles of a polymorphic ETV6 binding site in DCDC2 confer risk of reading and language impairment. Am J Hum Genet. 2013;93:19–28. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2013.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, Thomas L, Ferreira MA, Bender D, Maller J, Sklar P, de Bakker PI, Daly MJ, Sham PC. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81:559–575. doi: 10.1086/519795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice ML, Smith SD, Gayan J. Convergent genetic linkage and associations to language, speech and reading measures in families of probands with Specific Language Impairment. J Neurodev Disord. 2009;1:264–282. doi: 10.1007/s11689-009-9031-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosner J, Simon DP. The auditory analysis test: an initial report. J Learn Disabil. 1971;4:40–48. [Google Scholar]
- Rust J, Golombok S, Trickey G. WORD: Wechsler Objective Reading Dimensions Manual. Sidcup: Psychological Corporation; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Scarborough HS. Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Dev. 1990;61:1728–1743. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scerri TS, Schulte-Korne G. Genetics of developmental dyslexia. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;19:179–197. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0081-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scerri TS, Morris AP, Buckingham LL, Newbury DF, Miller LL, Monaco AP, Bishop DV, Paracchini S. DCDC2, KIAA0319 and CMIP are associated with reading-related traits. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70:237–245. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scerri TS, Darki F, Newbury DF, Whitehouse AJ, Peyrard-Janvid M, Matsson H, Ang QW, Pennell CE, Ring S, Stein J, Morris AP, Monaco AP, Kere J, Talcott JB, Kingberg T, Paracchini S. The dyslexia candidate locus on 2p12 is associated with general cognitive ability and white matter structure. PLoS One. 2012;7:e50312. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott-Van Zeeland AA, Abrahams BS, Alvarez-Retuerto AI, Sinnenblick LI, Rudie JD, Ghahremani D, Mumford JA, Poldrack RA, Dapretto M, Geschwind DH, Bookheimer SY. Altered functional connectivity in frontal lobe circuits is associated with variation in the autism risk gene CNTNAP2. Sci Transl Med. 2010;2:56ra80. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3001344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. Paying attention to reading: the neurobiology of reading and dyslexia. Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20:1329–1349. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- SLI Consortium. A genomeside scan identified two novel loci involved in specific language impairment. Am J Hum Genet. 2002;70:384–398. doi: 10.1086/338649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan GC, Doke TF, Ashburner J, Wood NW, Frackowiak RS. Normal variation in fronto-occipital circuitry and cerebellar structure with an autism-associated polymorphism of CNTNAP2. Neuroimage. 2010;53:1030–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trzaskowski M, Davis OS, DeFries JC, Yang J, Visscher PM, Plomin R. DNA evidence for strong genome-wide pleiotropy of cognitive and learning abilities. Behav Genet. 2013;43:267–273. doi: 10.1007/s10519-013-9594-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vandermosten M, Boets B, Poelmans H, Sunaert S, Wouters J, Ghesquiere P. A tractography study in dyslexia: neuroanatomic correlates of orthographic, phonological and speech processing. Brain. 2012;135(Pt 3):935–948. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verbeek E, Meuwissen ME, Verheijen FW, Govaert PP, Licht DJ, Kuo DS, Poulton CJ, Schot R, Leguin MH, Dudink J, Halley DJ, de Coo RI, den Hollander JC, Oegema R, Gould DB, Mancini GM. COL4A2 mutation associated with familial porencephaly and small-vessel disease. Eur J Hum Genet. 2012;20:844–851. doi: 10.1038/ejhg.2012.20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vernes SC, Spiteri E, Nicod J, Groszer M, Taylor JM, Davies KE, Geschwind DH, Fisher SE. High-throughput analysis of promoter occupancy reveals direct neural targets of FOXP2: a gene mutated in speech and language disorder. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81:1232–1250. doi: 10.1086/522238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vernes SC, Oliver PL, Spiteri E, Lockstone HE, Puliyadi R, Taylor JM, Ho J, Mombereau C, Brewer A, Lowy E, Nicod J, Groszer M, Baban D, Sahgal N, Cazier JB, Ragoussis J, Davies KE, Geschwind DH, Fisher SE. Foxp2 regulates gene networks implicated in neurite outgrowth in the developing brain. PLoS Genet. 2011;7:e1002145. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1002145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walhovd KB, Fjell AM, Brown TT, Pediatric Imaging Neurocognition and Genetics Study et al. Long-term influence of normal variation in neonatal characteristics on human brain development. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:20089–20094. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1208180109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Objective Language Dimensions (WOLD) London: The Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D, Golombok S, Rust J. WISC-IIIUK: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Sidcup: Psychological Corporation; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Weintraub S, Dikmen SS, Heaton RK, et al. Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology. 2013;90(11 Suppl 3):S54–S64. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872ded. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wells WM, 3rd, Viola P, Atsumi H, Nakajima S, Kikinis R. Multi-modal volume registration by maximization of mutual information. Med Image Anal. 1996;1:35–51. doi: 10.1016/s1361-8415(01)80004-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilcke A, Ligges C, Burkhardt J, Alexander M, Wolf C, Quente E, Ahnert P, Hoffmann P, Becker A, Muller-Myhsok C, Cichon S, Boltze J, Kirsten H. Imaging genetics of FOXP2 in dyslexia. Eur J Hum Genet. 2011;20:224–229. doi: 10.1038/ejhg.2011.160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willcutt EG, Pennington BF, Olson RK, Chhabildas N, Hulslander J. Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between reading disability and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: in search of the common deficit. Dev Neuropsychol. 2005;27:35–78. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2701_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise JC, Sevcik RA, Morris RD, Lovett MW, Wolf M. The relationship among reception and expressive vocabulary, listening comprehension, pre-reading skills, word identification skills, and reading comprehension by children with reading disabilities. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2007;50:1093–1099. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/076). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu C, Aragam N, Li X, Villa EC, Wang L, Briones D, Petty L, Posada Y, Arana TB, Cruz G, Mao C, Camarillo C, Su BB, Escamilla MA, Wang K. BCL9 and C9orf5 are associated with negative symptoms in schizophrenia: meta-analysis of two genome-wide association studies. PLoS One. 2013;8:e51674. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoneda Y, Haginova K, Arai H, Tamoka S, Tsurusaki Y, Doi H, Miyake N, Yokochi K, Osaka H, Kato M, Matsumoto N, Saitsu H. De novo and inherited mutations in COL4A2, encoding the type IV collagen a2 chain cause porencephaly. Am J Hum Genet. 2012;90:86–90. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.11.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhuang J, Hrabe J, Kangarlu A, Xu D, Bansal R, Branch CA, Peterson BS. Correction of eddy-current distortions in diffusion tensor images using the known directions and strengths of diffusion gradients. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2006;24:1188–1193. doi: 10.1002/jmri.20727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.