Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2013 Dec;47(12):1837–1842. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.06.016

The interaction of affective with psychotic processes: A test of the effects of worrying on working memory, jumping to conclusions, and anomalies of experience in patients with persecutory delusions

Daniel Freeman a,, Helen Startup a, Graham Dunn b, Emma Černis a, Gail Wingham c, Katherine Pugh a, Jacinta Cordwell c, David Kingdon c
PMCID: PMC3905189  PMID: 23871449

Abstract

Worry has traditionally been considered in the study of common emotional disorders such as anxiety and depression, but recent studies indicate that worry may be a causal factor in the occurrence and persistence of persecutory delusions. The effect of worry on processes traditionally associated with psychosis has not been tested. The aim of the study was to examine the short-term effects of a bout of worry on three cognitive processes typically considered markers of psychosis: working memory, jumping to conclusions, and anomalous internal experience. Sixty-seven patients with persecutory delusions in the context of a non-affective psychotic disorder were randomised to a worry induction, a worry reduction, or a neutral control condition. They completed tests of the cognitive processes before and after the randomisation condition. The worry induction procedure led to a significant increase in worry. The induction of worry did not affect working memory or jumping to conclusions, but it did increase a range of mild anomalous experiences including feelings of unreality, perceptual alterations, and temporal disintegration. Worry did not affect the occurrence of hallucinations. The study shows that a period of worry causes a range of subtle odd perceptual disturbances that are known to increase the likelihood of delusions. It demonstrates an interaction between affective and psychotic processes in patients with delusions.

Keywords: Delusions, Worry, Paranoia, Hallucinations, Schizophrenia

1. Introduction

Worry is conventionally studied within the anxiety disorders, but it has recently been given prominence in a theoretical account of persecutory delusions (Freeman, 2007). Worry brings more implausible paranoid ideas to mind, keeps them there, and escalates the distress. There is increasing evidence to support this view. In a national epidemiological survey, individuals reporting concerns of plots to harm them had almost ten times higher odds of reporting worry than individuals without paranoid fears (Freeman et al., 2011). Cross-sectional studies with patients with persecutory delusions have shown that high levels of worry are common, comparable to those seen in generalised anxiety disorder, and that higher levels of worry are associated with more distressing paranoia (Freeman and Garety, 1999, Freeman et al., 2001, Startup et al., 2007, Morrison and Wells, 2007, Bassett et al., 2009, Freeman et al., 2010). Worry is not simply a consequence of paranoid thoughts however. Longitudinal studies have shown that worry predicts the later development and persistence of paranoid fears (Startup et al., 2007, Freeman et al., 2012b), and an experimental study has shown that higher levels of worry predict the occurrence of paranoid thoughts (Freeman et al., 2008). In the strongest tests of the causal role of worry in persecutory delusions, two clinical intervention studies have shown that directly targeting the cognitive style of worry significantly lessens paranoia (Foster et al., 2010, Hepworth et al., 2011). This series of studies indicates that worry in psychosis may well require the level of attention it receives in the anxiety disorders.

Theoretical accounts of psychosis increasingly highlight both affective and cognitive routes to delusions (e.g. Garety et al., 2001, Myin-Germeys et al., 2003, Bentall et al., 2009). This is not meant to imply that affect has no cognitive mechanisms but to make the distinction between what have traditionally been considered as emotional and psychotic processes. Non-affective cognitive disturbance in psychosis has had the greater focus (e.g. Hemsley, 1993, Frith, 1992, Kapur, 2003, Green et al., 2004), but the emotional route to delusions is gaining evidential support (e.g. Lincoln et al., 2010b, Ben-Zeev et al., 2011, Thewissen et al., 2011). The study of worry in psychosis is an example of the latter affective route, since worry is traditionally considered central to the experience of anxiety problems and it is not listed in descriptions of psychosis, but is there an impact of worry on cognitive processes traditionally associated with psychosis? Is there an interaction between emotional and psychotic processes? To answer this question we examined the immediate effects of a bout of worrying on three processes studied in psychosis: working memory, jumping to conclusions, and perceptual anomalies. Meta-analyses have consistently shown that working memory performance is impaired in individuals with a diagnosis of schizophrenia (e.g. Lee and Park, 2005, Forbes et al., 2009), leading to the view that it may be an endophenotype for the diagnosis (e.g. Horan et al., 2008). Jumping to conclusions, a tendency to seek less data before reaching a decision, has been specifically linked with delusional beliefs, and it has been reliably found in patients with psychosis (see Garety et al., 2007, Freeman, 2007, Fine et al., 2007). Particular cognitive dysfunctions in psychosis are considered to produce anomalies of experience (such as perceptual and attentional disturbances, altered experience of self, aberrant salience) that lead to delusional misinterpretations (e.g. Maher, 1988, Kapur, 2003, Uhlhaas and Mishara, 2007); the presence of such anomalies have been repeatedly found in patients with psychosis (e.g. Chapman, 1966, Phillipson and Harris, 1985, Bunney et al., 1999, Parnas et al., 2003) and to be associated with delusional ideation (e.g. Bell et al., 2006, Freeman et al., 2008, Freeman et al., 2010). We therefore studied worry in relation to three processes commonly considered important cognitive markers of psychosis.

Study of a bout of worry in patients with psychosis has not been reported before. However, a bout of worry in non-clinical worriers has been found to be associated with a reduction in working memory capacity (Hayes et al., 2008, Leigh and Hirsch, 2011), and an anxious mood induction has been found to increase the jumping to conclusions bias in a non-clinical sample (Lincoln et al., 2010a), although not in a delusions group (So et al., 2008). Whether a mood induction increases perceptual anomalies is unknown, but levels of anxiety positively correlate with the presence of perceptual distortions (e.g. Bell et al., 2011, Tone et al., 2011). In the current study it was hypothesized that psychotic processes would be exacerbated when patients engage in a period of worry: that worry makes it harder to process information and reason and that it creates a more subjectively odd state. It was predicted that a decrease in working memory, an exacerbation of the jumping to conclusions reasoning style, and the occurrence of anomalous experiences would be greatest in a worry induction group and least in a worry reduction condition. In order to enhance the clinical relevance of the study, we chose to study a bout of worry in patients with persecutory delusions in which worry was identified as at a high level; the effects of worry were studied in those who were prone to adopt this cognitive style.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

67 patients with persecutory delusions completed the study during the baseline assessment, prior to randomisation, of a clinical trial (ISRCTN23197625) (Freeman et al., 2012a). The participants were recruited from two mental health NHS Trusts: Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Southern Health NHS Foundation Trust. The inclusion criteria were: a current persecutory delusion as defined by Freeman and Garety (2000); scoring at least 3 on the conviction scale of the PSYRATS delusions scale (i.e. at least 50% conviction in the delusion) (Haddock et al., 1999); that the delusion had persisted for at least three months; a clinical diagnosis of schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder or delusional disorder; a clinically significant level of worry, as indicated by scores above 44 on the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (see Startup and Erickson, 2006); aged between 18 and 65; and no changes to medication in the past month. Criteria for exclusion were: a primary diagnosis of alcohol or substance dependency or personality disorder; organic syndrome or learning disability; a command of spoken English inadequate for engaging in therapy; and currently having individual CBT.

2.2. Baseline assessments

2.2.1. Psychotic Symptom Rating Scales – delusions (PSYRATS; Haddock et al., 1999)

The PSYRATS – delusions is a six item multidimensional measure. It assesses the conviction, preoccupation, distress and disruption associated with delusions. Symptoms over the last week are rated. Higher scores indicate greater severity.

2.2.2. Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Kay, 1991)

The PANSS is a 30-item rating instrument developed for the assessment of patients with schizophrenia. Symptoms over the last week were rated (i.e. currently present). Higher scores indicate the greater presence of psychiatric symptoms.

2.2.3. Green et al. Paranoid Thoughts Scale (GPTS; Green et al., 2008)

The GPTS is a thirty-two item measure of paranoid thinking. Part A assesses ideas of reference (e.g. ‘It was hard to stop thinking about people talking about me behind my back’) and Part B assesses ideas of persecution (e.g. ‘I was convinced there was a conspiracy against me’). Each item is rated on a 5-point scale. Higher scores indicate greater levels of paranoid thinking. The scale was completed for the period of the previous fortnight.

2.2.4. Penn State Worry Questionnaire (Meyer et al., 1990)

The PSWQ is the most established measure of trait worry style and has been used in non-clinical and clinical populations (see review by Startup and Erickson, 2006). Each of the sixteen items is rated on a 5-point scale. Higher scores indicate a greater tendency to worry.

2.2.5. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, 1999)

The WASI is a standardised short and reliable measure of intelligence. The Vocabulary and Matrix Reasoning subtests were used to obtain an estimate of IQ.

2.3. Tasks completed before and after the randomisation condition

2.3.1. Visual analogue rating scales (VAS)

In order to test the effects of the three conditions on worry and mood state, the participants marked on three 0 (‘Not at all’) to 100 (‘totally’) visual analogue scales the degree to which ‘right now’ they felt worried, anxious, or happy.

2.3.2. Working memory: tasks from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III (WAIS-III) (Wechsler, 1997)

Three working memory tasks were used: digit span forwards, digit span backwards, and letter–number sequencing. These are the most commonly used working memory tasks in schizophrenia research (see Nuechterlein et al., 2004).

2.3.3. Jumping to Conclusions (JTC): the beads task (Garety et al., 2005)

Data-gathering was assessed with a probabilistic reasoning task that has been extensively used with people with delusions. Participants are asked to request as many pieces of evidence (coloured beads) as they would like before making a decision (from which of two hidden jars the beads are drawn). The two jars have beads of two different colours in opposite ratios. The ratio of beads for the version used in the current study was 60:40. The colours of the beads in the jars were altered for the repeat of the task. The key variable is the number of beads requested before making a decision.

2.3.4. Anomalous experiences I: Cardiff Anomalous Perceptions Scale (CAPS; Bell et al., 2006)

In the current study the CAPS was adapted into a state measure by asking whether any of the perceptual anomalies had occurred in the past 5 min, with participants simply responding with a Yes or No. A higher score represents the reporting of a greater number of perceptual anomalies. The scale covers a variety of anomalies including changes in levels of sensory intensity (e.g. ‘Are sounds much louder than they normally would be?’), distortion of own body or the external world (e.g. ‘Have you found the appearance of things or people seeming to change in a puzzling way e.g. distorted shapes or sizes or colour?’), sensory flooding (e.g. ‘Have you found that sensations happened all at once and flooded you with information?’), temporal lobe experiences (e.g. ‘Have you had the feeling that of being uplifted, as if driving or rolling over a road while sitting quietly?’) and hallucinations (e.g. ‘Have you heard noises or sounds when there was nothing about to explain them?’). In the current study we analysed the total score, but also tested separately the hallucination items and the non-hallucination perceptual anomalies.

2.3.5. Anomalous experiences II: Cambridge Depersonalisation Scale (CDS; Sierra and Berrios, 2000)

We included a second scale to help capture the wide variety in anomalous experiences. The CDS was based on the view that ‘depersonalisation constitutes a syndrome which, in addition to ineffable feelings of ‘unreality’, also includes emotional numbing, heightened self-observation, changes in body experience, distortions in the experiencing of time and space, changes in the feeling of agency, feelings of having the mind empty of thoughts, memories and/or images, and an inability to focus and sustain attention’ (Sierra and Berrios, 2000). Two studies have shown that the scale assesses five distinct types of anomalous experiences (Sierra et al., 2005, Simeon et al., 2008). We used an adapted state version by asking about the occurrence of such experiences in the past few minutes. We used 19 items from the CDS, since ten items were not suitable for a state version (e.g. ‘previously familiar places look unfamiliar, as if I had never seen them before’). Higher scores indicate greater occurrence of anomalies. We examined the total score but also used four factors from the largest study of the CDS structure (Simeon et al., 2008): unreality of self (e.g. ‘Familiar voices (including my own) sound remote and unreal’), perceptual alterations (e.g. ‘I have the feeling that my hands or my feet have become larger or smaller’), unreality of surroundings (e.g. ‘What I see looks ‘flat’ or ‘lifeless’, as if I were looking at a picture’), and temporal disintegration (e.g. ‘It seems as if things that I have done had taken place a long time ago.’).

2.4. Procedure

The study was approved by an NHS research ethics committee. Participants completed the baseline assessments and the tasks assessing working memory, jumping to conclusions, and anomalous experiences. They then either received a worry induction, worry reduction, or a neutral condition. Randomisation to condition was carried out using www.randomisation.com. The tasks assessing psychotic processes were then repeated. The worry induction procedure was designed to encourage worry about topics that each participant already spent considerable time thinking about. It consisted of three stages. In the first stage the Worry Domains Questionnaire (WDQ; Tallis et al., 1992) was used to elicit common worries (that are not paranoid). In the second stage, participants took their two main worries identified from the WDQ and completed the catastrophising procedure for each (Vasey and Borkovec, 1992). The catastrophising procedure produces a ‘worry chain’ by the experimenter repeatedly responding to answers with ‘What is it that worries you about X?’ Participants were led to produce at least ten catastrophising steps for each worry. The third stage was 5 min of further engagement with worry, using instructions from McLaughlin et al. (2007): “During this period we would like you to engage with your worrisome thoughts. Please refer to your list of worrisome topics. When you are asked to begin, please close your eyes and worry about your most worrisome topic in the way that you usually worry about it, but as intensely as you can, until the experimenter asks you to stop and open your eyes. If you normally worry about one topic at a time, please try to do the same during this period. However, if your thoughts change to another worry topic during this period feel free to allow these thoughts to continue. It is alright to change topics during this period if the changes occur naturally during the worry process.” We have previously shown in non-clinical individuals that worry is increased using this procedure (Southgate, 2009). The worry reduction procedure consisted of a 10 min mindfulness relaxation exercise (‘Mindfulness of the breath’) (Kabat-Zinn, 2006), which could be accompanied by positive music if the participant chose (Delibes: Coppélia Act 1 Number 3 Mazurka) (Mayer et al., 1995). The neutral condition consisted of the person simply reading from a selection of magazines for ten minutes. Participants who received the worry induction condition also received if they wished the worry reduction condition at the end of testing.

2.5. Analysis

All analyses were carried out using SPSS Version 20 (IBM, 2011). To test change after the randomisation condition, analysis of covariance was used, with post-randomisation score as the dependent variable, group as a fixed factor, and baseline score as the covariate. When there was a main effect of group, least significant difference pairwise comparisons for the estimated marginal means were tested. All hypothesis testing was two-tailed.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic and clinical information

Basic demographic and clinical information for each of the randomisation groups is presented in Table 1. The participants predominately had clinical diagnoses of schizophrenia, were unemployed, and were prescribed anti-psychotic medication. They had levels of paranoia as assessed by the GPTS comparable to other studies of patients with persecutory delusions (e.g. Freeman et al., 2010) and levels of worry as assessed by the PSWQ comparable to patients with generalised anxiety disorder (e.g. Behar et al., 2003).

Table 1.

Basic demographic and clinical information.

Worry induction
(n = 20)
Neutral condition
(n = 22)
Worry reduction
(n = 25)
Mean age in years (SD) 39.9 (12.2) 42.2 (11.6) 43.5 (11.3)
Sex:
 Male 11 13 12
 Female 9 9 13
Ethnicity:
 White 19 20 24
 Black Caribbean 0 0 1
 Black African 0 0 0
 Black Other 0 0 0
 Indian 0 1 0
 Pakistani 0 0 0
 Other 1 1 0
Unemployed 15 14 15
Mean IQ (SD) 104.8 (17.6) 96.2 (19.4) 103.9 (17.3)
Diagnosis:
 Schizophrenia 17 16 15
 Schizo-affective disorder 1 1 3
 Delusional disorder 1 3 4
 Psychosis NOS 1 3 3
Medication (Chlorpromazine equiv.):
 None 1 1 2
 Low (1–200 mg) 2 5 5
 Medium (200–400 mg) 4 7 10
 High (≥400 mg) 13 9 8
Mean number of admissions in past 5 years (SD) 0.8 (1.3) 0.5 (0.7) 0.5 (1.0)
Mean PSYRATS delusions score (SD) 19.5 (1.8) 19.2 (2.7) 18.3 (3.0)
Mean PSYRATS delusions conviction (SD) 3.5 (0.5) 3.6 (0.5) 3.3 (0.7)
Mean PANSS score (SD) 84.6 (11.8) 82.4 (14.8) 77.5 (15.4)
Mean GPTS – Part A score (SD) 53.7 (14.3) 54.6 (14.7) 51.7 (16.8)
Mean GPTS – Part B score (SD) 61.1 (12.6) 58.9 (16.1) 54.8 (18.9)
Mean PSWQ score (SD) 66.1 (10.1) 66.1 (14.6) 64.9 (9.0)

3.2. The effects of the randomisation conditions

It can be seen in Table 2 that the worry induction condition significantly increased levels of worry compared with the other two conditions. The worry reduction condition reduced levels of worry. The worry induction condition also produced an increase in anxiety and a decrease in happiness compared with the other two conditions. The worry reduction condition did not significantly alter levels of anxiety or happiness.

Table 2.

Estimated post-randomisation scores on the VAS scales (adjusted for baseline scores).

Worry induction
(n = 20)
Neutral condition
(n = 22)
Worry reduction
(n = 25)
Test of group effect
Mean SE Mean SE Mean SE F (2, 65) p-Value
Worry 67.2a 4.3 44.2b 4.1 31.6c 3.8 19.5 <.001
Anxiety 65.8a 4.0 41.5b 3.8 34.1b 3.6 18.5 <.001
Happiness 29.2a 3.9 46.0b 3.8 45.5b 3.6 6.1 .004

Significant group differences (p < .05) are denoted by differing superscript letters.

3.3. Effects on psychotic processes

The post-randomisation performance of each group on the tasks assessing psychotic processes is summarized in Table 3. It can be seen that there are no differences between the groups in performance on working memory or jumping to conclusions tasks (controlling for baseline scores). However there are increases in the occurrence of anomalies of experience in the worry induction condition compared with the other two conditions. The only exception for anomalous experiences is that the occurrence of hallucinations is not affected by randomisation condition. (The same significant effects are found if PANSS total score is added as a covariate.)

Table 3.

Estimated post-randomisation scores on the psychotic processes (adjusted for baseline scores).

Worry induction
(n = 20)
Neutral condition
(n = 22)
Worry reduction
(n = 25)
Test of group effect
Mean SE Mean SE Mean SE F (2, 65) p-Value
Working memory
 Forward digit span 9.0 0.5 9.6 0.5 9.6 0.4 0.5 .618
 Backward digit span 6.2 0.4 5.8 0.3 5.8 0.3 0.5 .637
 Letter-number 8.2 0.4 8.7 0.4 8.4 0.3 0.6 .563



Jumping to conclusions
 Number of beads 6.4 0.6 6.5 0.6 7.8 0.6 1.8 .175



Anomalous experiences
CAPS 4.7 0.5 4.3 0.5 3.3 0.5 2.3 .107
 Hallucinations 1.2 0.2 1.1 0.2 1.3 0.2 0.3 .777
 Non-hallucination anomalies 2.2a 0.3 1.9a 0.3 0.9b 0.3 5.5 .006



CDS 5.0a 0.5 2.5b 0.5 2.4b 0.4 10.0 <.001
 Unreality 1.7a 0.2 1.1a,b 0.2 0.7b 0.2 4.8 .012
 Perceptual 0.9a 0.1 0.4b 0.1 0.2b 0.1 6.3 .003
 Surroundings 0.5a 0.1 0.2b 0.1 0.2b 0.1 3.8 .028
 Temporal 1.1a 0.1 0.6b 0.1 0.5b 0.1 4.5 .015

Significant group differences (p < .05) are denoted by differing superscript letters.

4. Discussion

Patients with persecutory delusions who worried were the focus of this study. Previous research has shown that this is the most common presentation of paranoia (e.g. Freeman and Garety, 1999, Morrison and Wells, 2007, Freeman et al., 2010). We examined the effects of a bout of worry in these individuals on processes customarily considered as markers of psychosis. Given that worry was known to occur frequently in these patients we wanted to know whether it impacted on psychotic processes. The results were very clear. The occurrence of worry did not affect working memory or jumping to conclusions, but it did lead to an increase in a range of low-level anomalous experiences. Increases in sensations of the unreality of self and surroundings (e.g. feeling of being a detached observer), perceptual alterations (e.g. body feeling very light as it if were floating), and temporal disintegration (e.g. seeming as if things done recently had taken place a long time ago) were all more likely to occur after a period of worry. Worrying less reduced the occurrence of anomalies of sensory intensity (e.g. lights or colours seeming brighter or more intense than usual) and sensory flooding (e.g. difficulty distinguishing one sensation from another). Our assessment covered a wide variety of anomalous experiences and it was only the occurrence of hallucinations that was unaffected. The point of interest learned from the experiment is that a period of worry in these patients was creating a range of potentially confusing and puzzling perceptual experiences that are known to predict paranoid thoughts. Worry was causing odd subjective states that are known to lead to odd ideas. This is an illustration of an interaction of affective with psychotic processes, and adds to our understanding of how worry may contribute to delusional experience. This information can be incorporated into the emerging therapies that target worry in psychosis (Foster et al., 2010, Hepworth et al., 2011); clinicians can assess for and normalise the occurrence of such anomalies after a period of worry in patients with persecutory delusions.

Working memory and jumping to conclusions were not altered by a period of worry. This fails to replicate the finding of worry impeding working memory in high non-clinical worriers (e.g. Leigh and Hirsch, 2011). However these previous studies assessed working memory and worry concurrently, while in the current study we simply assessed the effects after a period of encouraging worry. We do not know the extent to which individuals in the worry induction condition were continuing to worry, which is a limitation of the study. It is also clear that the worry induction was having effects more generally on mood; this is to be expected but limits the precision with which the occurrence of anomalous experiences can be attributed specifically to worry. Previous studies have had mixed findings on jumping to conclusions when manipulating levels of anxiety (So et al., 2008, Lincoln et al., 2010a), and the current study does not support the idea that JTC changes with anxious mood state. We examined three key processes and did not alter significance levels for multiple testing, agreeing with the view that ‘simply describing what tests of significance have been performed, and why, is generally the best method of dealing with multiple comparisons’ (Perneger, 1998). The final key limitation is that we did not assess levels of paranoia before and after each randomisation condition which means that we could not test whether and how paranoia increased after worrying; we simply tested the effects of worry on psychotic processes, and a larger experimental study that can examine mediation is now warranted (Emsley et al., 2010). Our contention is that the role of worry will receive increasing attention in the study and treatment of delusions.

Role of the funding source

The study was supported by a grant (09/160/06) from the Efficacy and Mechanism Evaluation (EME) Programme, which is funded by the UK Medical Research Council and UK NHS National Institute for Health Research. This study was included in the grant application for the clinical trial. The funder has not altered or reviewed this paper.

Contributors

The study was designed by Daniel Freeman, Helen Startup, Graham Dunn and David Kingdon. Daniel Freeman drafted the paper, which was commented upon by all authors. Emma Černis and Gail Wingham collected the data. Daniel Freeman and Graham Dunn carried out the analysis.

Conflict of interest

None.

Acknowledgements

The study was supported by a grant (09/160/06) from the Efficacy and Mechanism Evaluation (EME) Programme, which is funded by the UK Medical Research Council and UK NHS National Institute for Health Research. DF is supported by a UK Medical Research Council Senior Clinical Fellowship.

References

  1. Bassett M., Sperlinger D., Freeman D. Fear of madness and persecutory delusions. Psychosis. 2009;1:39–50. [Google Scholar]
  2. Behar E., Alcaine O., Zuellig A., Borkovec T. Screening for generalised anxiety disorder using the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2003;34:25–43. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7916(03)00004-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bell V., Halligan P.W., Ellis H.D. The Cardiff Anomalous Perceptions Scale (CAPS) Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2006;32:366–377. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbj014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bell V., Halligan P., Pugh K., Freeman D. Correlates of perceptual distortions in clinical and non-clinical populations using the Cardiff Anomalous Perceptions Scale (CAPS) Psychiatry Research. 2011;189:451–457. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.05.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bentall R.P., Rowse G., Shryane N., Kinderman P., Howard R., Blackwood N. The cognitive and affective structure of paranoid delusions. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66:236–247. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Ben-Zeev D., Ellington K., Swendsen J., Granholm E. Examining a cognitive model of persecutory ideation in the daily life of people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2011;37:1248–1256. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bunney W.E., Hetrick W.P., Bunney B.G., Patterson J.V., Jin Y., Potkin S.G. Structured interview for assessing perceptual anomalies (SIAPA) Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1999;25:577–592. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chapman J. The early symptoms of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1966;112:225–251. doi: 10.1192/bjp.112.484.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Emsley R., Dunn G., White I.R. Modelling mediation and moderation of treatment effects in randomised controlled trials of complex interventions. Statistical Methods in Medical Research. 2010;19:237–270. doi: 10.1177/0962280209105014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Fine C., Gardner M., Craigie J., Gold I. Hopping, skipping or jumping to conclusions? Clarifying the role of the JTC bias in delusions. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 2007;12:46–77. doi: 10.1080/13546800600750597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Forbes N., Carrick L., McIntosh A., Lawrie S. Working memory in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine. 2009;39:889–905. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Foster C., Startup H., Potts L., Freeman D. A randomised controlled trial of a worry intervention for individuals with persistent persecutory delusions. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2010;41:45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2009.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Frith C.D. LEA; Hove: 1992. The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. [Google Scholar]
  14. Freeman D. Suspicious minds: the psychology of persecutory delusions. Clinical Psychology Review. 2007;27:425–457. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Freeman D., Dunn G., Startup H., Kingdon D. The effects of reducing worry in patients with persecutory delusions: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials. 2012;13:223. doi: 10.1186/1745-6215-13-223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Freeman D., Garety P.A. Worry, worry processes and dimensions of delusions. Behavioural & Cognitive Psychotherapy. 1999;27:47–62. [Google Scholar]
  17. Freeman D., Garety P.A. Comments on the content of persecutory delusions: does the definition need clarification? British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2000;39:407–414. doi: 10.1348/014466500163400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Freeman D., Garety P.A., Kuipers E. Persecutory delusions: developing the understanding of belief maintenance and emotional distress. Psychological Medicine. 2001;31:1293–1306. doi: 10.1017/s003329170100455x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Freeman D., McManus S., Brugha T., Meltzer H., Jenkins R., Bebbington P. Concomitants of paranoia in the general population. Psychological Medicine. 2011;41:923–936. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Freeman D., Pugh K., Antley A., Slater M., Bebbington P., Gittins M. A virtual reality study of paranoid thinking in the general population. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;192:258–263. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.044677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Freeman D., Pugh K., Vorontsova N., Antley A., Slater M. Testing the continuum of delusional beliefs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2010;119:83–92. doi: 10.1037/a0017514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Freeman D., Stahl D., McManus S., Meltzer H., Brugha T., Wiles N. Insomnia, worry, anxiety and depression as predictors of the occurrence and persistence of paranoid thinking. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2012;47:1195–1203. doi: 10.1007/s00127-011-0433-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Garety P.A., Bebbington P., Fowler D., Freeman D., Kuipers E. Implications for neurobiological research of cognitive models of psychosis. Psychological Medicine. 2007;37:1377–1391. doi: 10.1017/S003329170700013X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Garety P.A., Freeman D., Jolley S., Dunn G., Bebbington P.E., Fowler D. Reasoning, emotions and delusional conviction in psychosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:373–384. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.3.373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Garety P.A., Kuipers E., Fowler D., Freeman D., Bebbington P.E. A cognitive model of the positive symptoms of psychosis. Psychological Medicine. 2001;31:189–195. doi: 10.1017/s0033291701003312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Green C., Freeman D., Kuipers E., Bebbington P., Fowler D., Dunn G. Measuring ideas of persecution and reference: the Green et al Paranoid Thought Scales (G-PTS) Psychological Medicine. 2008;38:101–111. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Green M.F., Nuechterlein K.H., Gold J.M., Barch D.M., Cohen J., Essock S. Approaching a consensus cognitive battery for clinical trials in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;56:301–307. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Haddock G., McCarron J., Tarrier N., Faragher F.B. Scales to measure dimensions of hallucinations and delusions: the psychotic symptom rating scales (PSYRATS) Psychological Medicine. 1999;29:879–889. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799008661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hayes S., Hirsch C., Mathews A. Restriction of working memory capacity during worry. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2008;117:712–717. doi: 10.1037/a0012908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hemsley D.R. A simple (or simplistic?) cognitive model for schizophrenia. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1993;31:633–645. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(93)90116-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hepworth C., Startup H., Freeman D. Developing treatments for persistent persecutory delusions: the impact of an emotional processing and metacognitive awareness (EPMA) intervention. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2011;199:653–658. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e318229cfa8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Horan W., Braff D., Neuchterlein K., Sugar C., Cadenhead K., Calkins M. Verbal working memory impairments in individuals with schizophrenia and their first-degree relatives. Schizophrenia Research. 2008;103:218–228. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. IBM . IBM Corporation; 2011. SPSS statistics version 20. Release 20.0.0. [Google Scholar]
  34. Kabat-Zinn J. Sounds True Inc; Boulder, Colorado: 2006. Mindfulness for beginners. [Google Scholar]
  35. Kapur S. Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:13–23. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kay S.R. Brunner; New York: 1991. Positive and negative syndromes in schizophrenia. [Google Scholar]
  37. Lee J., Park S. Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:599–611. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Leigh E., Hirsch C. Worry in imagery and verbal form: effect on residual working memory capacity. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2011;49:99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lincoln T.M., Lange J., Burau J., Exner C., Moritz S. The effect of state anxiety on paranoid ideation and jumping to conclusions. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2010;36:1140–1148. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lincoln T.M., Mehl S., Ziegler M., Kesting M.-L., Exner C., Reif W. Is fear of others linked to an uncertain sense of self? The relevance of self-worth, interpersonal self-concepts, and dysfunctional beliefs to paranoia. Behavior Therapy. 2010;41:187–197. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2009.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Maher B.A. Anomalous experience and delusional thinking: the logic of explanations. In: Oltmanns T.F., Maher B.A., editors. Delusional beliefs. Wiley; New York: 1988. pp. 15–33. [Google Scholar]
  42. Mayer J., Allen J., Beauregard K. Mood inductions for four specific moods. Journal of Mental Imagery. 1995;19:133–150. [Google Scholar]
  43. McLaughlin K., Borkovec T., Sibrava N. The effects of worry and rumination on affect states and cognitive activity. Behavior Therapy. 2007;38:23–38. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Meyer T.J., Miller M.L., Metzger R.L., Borkovec T.D. Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1990;28:487–495. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Morrison A.P., Wells A. Relationships between worry, psychotic experiences and emotional distress in patients with schizophrenia spectrum diagnoses and comparisons with anxious and non-patient groups. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2007;45:1593–1600. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Myin-Germeys I., Krabbendam L., Delespaul P., van Os J. Can cognitive deficits explain differential sensitivity to life events in psychosis? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2003;38:262–268. doi: 10.1007/s00127-003-0633-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Nuechterlein K., Barch D., Gold J., Goldberg T., Green M., Heaton R. Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2004;72:29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Parnas J., Handest P., Sæbye D., Jansson L. Anomalies of subjective experiences in schizophrenia and psychotic bipolar disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2003;108:126–133. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.00105.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Perneger T. What's wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. British Medical Journal. 1998;316:1236–1238. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7139.1236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Phillipson O., Harris J. Perceptual changes in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine. 1985;15:859–866. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700005092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Sierra M., Berrios G. The Cambridge Depersonalisation Scale. Psychiatry Research. 2000;93:153–164. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(00)00100-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Sierra M., Baker D., Medford N., David A. Unpacking the depersonalization syndrome. Psychological Medicine. 2005;35:1523–1532. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705005325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Simeon D., Kozin D., Segal K., Lerch B., Dujour R., Giesbrecht T. De-constructing depersonalization. Psychiatry Research. 2008;157:303–306. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. So S., Freeman D., Garety P. Impact of state anxiety on the jumping to conclusions delusion bias. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2008;42:879–886. doi: 10.1080/00048670802345466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Southgate L. University of London; 2009. Does worry make paranoia worse? An investigation of the effects of worry on persecutory ideation. DClinPsy thesis. [Google Scholar]
  56. Startup H.M., Erickson T.M. The Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ) In: Davey G.C.L., Wells A., editors. Worry and its psychological disorders. Wiley; Chichester: 2006. pp. 101–120. [Google Scholar]
  57. Startup H., Freeman D., Garety P.A. Persecutory delusions and catastrophic worry in psychosis: developing the understanding of delusion distress and persistence. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2007;45:523–537. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Tallis F., Eysenck M.W., Mathews A. A questionnaire for the measurement of nonpathological worry. Personality and Individual Differences. 1992;13:161–168. [Google Scholar]
  59. Thewissen V., Bentall R., Oorschot M., á Campo J., van Lierop T., van Os J. Emotions, self-esteem, and paranoid episodes. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2011;50:178–195. doi: 10.1348/014466510X508677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Tone E., Goulding S., Compton M. Associations among perceptual anomalies, social anxiety and paranoia in a college student sample. Psychiatry Research. 2011;188:258–263. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Uhlhaas P., Mishara A. Perceptual anomalies in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2007;33:142–156. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Vasey M., Borkovec T.D. A catastrophising assessment of worrisome thoughts. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1992;16:505–520. [Google Scholar]
  63. Wechsler D. 3rd ed. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1997. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. [Google Scholar]
  64. Wechsler D. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio TX: 1999. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES