Abstract
Introduction
Recent research suggests that diet quality influences depression risk; however, a lack of experimental evidence leaves open the possibility that residual confounding explains the observed relationships. The aim of this study was to document the cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between dietary patterns and symptoms of depression and to undertake a detailed examination of potential explanatory factors, particularly socioeconomic circumstances, in the diet-depression relationship.
Methods
Data were drawn from the Personality and Total Health (PATH) Through Life Study, a longitudinal community study following three age cohorts (20+; 40+; 60+yrs) from south-eastern Australia over three assessment periods (n = 3663). Regression analyses evaluated the cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships between dietary patterns, depressive symptoms, age, detailed measures of socioeconomic circumstances, other health behaviours, and cardiovascular risk factors.
Results
The lowest tertile of prudent (healthy) dietary pattern and the highest tertile of western (unhealthy) dietary pattern were associated with an increased likelihood of depressive symptoms. However, these contemporaneous associations were explained by adjustment for detailed measures of socioeconomic circumstances and physical activity. In prospective analyses, lower scores on the healthy dietary pattern and higher scores on the unhealthy dietary pattern independently predicted increased depressive symptoms across time, before and after adjustment for potential confounders and baseline depressive symptoms, but only for those in the oldest cohort. Dietary patterns did not explain the relationship between socioeconomic position and depressive symptoms.
Conclusion
The results of this study confirm that the relationship between habitual dietary intake and depressive symptoms is somewhat explained by socioeconomic circumstances and other health behaviours, but suggest that long term exposure to unhealthy dietary habits independently predisposes to depression over the lifecourse.
Introduction
In recent times there has been an increasing focus on the possible links between diet quality and the common mental disorders, depression and anxiety. To date, only observational data exist, although such studies have now been conducted in children [1], [2], adolescents [3]–[5] and adults [6]–[10] in multiple countries, with findings that are largely concordant with the hypothesis that poor diet quality is a risk factor for common mental disorders.
In these studies, diet quality is measured using numerous different methods, including by the use of a priori dietary quality indexes derived from recommended dietary guidelines, or by other composite measures of dietary intake. Examples of such dietary indexes include the United States Department of Agriculture Healthy Eating Index (HEI) [11]; validated scores based on national dietary guidelines [12]; validated methods to measure Mediterranean diets [13]; as well as various non-validated methods for assessing diet quality. Diet quality is also assessed by the use of dietary pattern analysis, which affords a determination of habitual dietary patterns [14]. Such studies commonly identify two main dietary patterns that reflect healthy dietary habits (often named ‘prudent’ or ‘wholesome’) and unhealthy (‘western’) dietary habits (e.g. [8], [15], [16]).
Overall diet quality, as measured by these dietary pattern scores, has been shown to be cross-sectionally related to clinical mood and anxiety disorders in a representative sample of Australian women. Higher scores on the healthy dietary pattern, characterized by vegetables, fruit, lean red meats, wholegrains and fish consumption, were associated with reduced odds for major depression, dysthymia, bipolar disorder and anxiety disorders, while higher scores on the ‘western’ dietary pattern, characterized by an increased consumption of unhealthy and processed foods, were associated greater odds for major depression, dysthymia and bipolar disorder [7], [17]. Similarly, Nanri et al. [10] found that a healthy Japanese pattern, comprising higher intakes of vegetables, fruit, mushrooms and soy products, was inversely related to depressive symptoms in Japanese public servants. In the one prospective study conducted to date using dietary pattern analysis, Akbaraly et al. [8] used data from the Whitehall II cohort to demonstrate that a ‘whole food’ dietary pattern, indicating greater consumption of nutrient-dense foods such as vegetables, fruits and fish, was inversely related to the risk for self-reported depression five years later. Conversely, a ‘processed food’ pattern, characterised by sweetened desserts, fried food, processed meat, and refined carbohydrates, was positively related to the risk for later depression.
Although the findings from observational studies have been largely consistent, evidence of mediation via biological mechanisms and data from intervention studies are currently lacking [18]. As such, it is still an unresolved question as to whether there is a true, biologically causal link between diet and mental health [18]. For example, it is plausible that differences in socioeconomic position, reflecting individuals' relative place within the structure of society and assessed by a range of measures of socioeconomic circumstances, such as educational level, economic disadvantage and occupational status, may explain the association observed between diet and depression risk. The fact that different measures of socioeconomic position have been shown to be inversely related to both diet quality ([19], [20] also reviewed in [21]) and risk for depression [22] is consistent with this hypothesis. While most previous studies in this field have included measures of socioeconomic circumstances in their analyses and largely excluded them as explanatory variables (e.g. marital and employment status [9]; education and area-level measures of advantage and disadvantage [7]; marital status, job title and position [10]; marital status, employment grade and education [8]; and income and education [6]), there is always the possibility that the variables used did not capture all salient aspects of socioeconomic position or even that diet quality provides an explanation of the association between socioeconomic position and depression (ie. low socioeconomic position leading to poorer diet quality which, in turn, predisposes to depression). It is also possible that the association between diet quality and mental health reflects the comorbidity between physical and mental health. Diet quality is a risk factor for CVD and other chronic physical health conditions, which are also correlated with depression [23].
Thus, the aim of this study was to examine in detail the contemporaneous and longitudinal associations between dietary intake, socioeconomic position, other health behaviours, cardiovascular risk factors and experience of depressive symptoms in order to investigate residual confounding, particularly by socioeconomic factors, as an explanation for the possible relationship between diet and depressive symptoms across the adult age range.
Methods
Ethics statement
All participants gave written informed consent prior to each wave of data collection in the Personality and Total Health (PATH) Through Life Study project. The study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of The Australian National University.
Study design
The PATH project is a longitudinal community study following three narrow age cohorts from Canberra and the neighbouring town of Queanbeyan in South-eastern Australia. The core aims of the PATH study are to delineate the course, risk and protective factors for depression, anxiety, substance use, and cognitive ability across adulthood. Detailed description of the study is available elsewhere [24]. The birth years of the three study cohorts are 1975–1979, 1956–1960, and 1937–1941, with baseline interviews conducted from 1999 to 2001 when respondents were aged between 20 and 24 years (20+; 1163 males, 1241 females), 40 and 44 years (40+: 1193 males, 1337 females) and 60 and 64 years (60+; 1319 males, 1232 females). The original sample was randomly drawn from the electoral roll (registration on the electoral roll is compulsory for Australian citizens) and the wave 1 response rate for each of the three cohorts was 59%, 65% and 58%. Overall, 90% of wave 1 respondents were re-interviewed at wave 2 (four years after baseline) and 90% of these were re-interviewed at wave 3 (a further four years later).
At each wave, participants took part in a structured interview (usually at their home or the Australian National University) that included a questionnaire completed using a hand-held computer, and physical and cognitive tests administered by a trained interviewer. Measures ranged from socio-demographic characteristics through to physical health, mental health, substance use, personality, and cognition. Only measures used in the present study are described below.
Wave 1 participants were also invited to take part in a Diet and Health substudy and given a self-completion questionnaire (including items related to food frequency), which was to be returned by mail when completed. Overall, 57% of respondents (n = 4250) completed and returned the booklet. Moderate levels of physical activity (vs mild), not participating in the workforce (vs being employed), having children, not speaking a language other than English, and not being a current smoker were all factors associated with increased likelihood of completing and returning the diet questionnaire (ORs = 1.16, 1.02–1.31; 1.39, 1.20–1.61; 1.36, 1.14–1.63; 2.05, 1.72–2.44; 1.52, 1.34–1.73 respectively). There were also cohort differences in the rate of completion of the diet questionnaire (20+ (ref); 40+ OR = 1.49, 1.25–1.78; 60+ OR = 2.76, 2.24–3.39). Although having more depressive symptoms was associated with decreased likelihood of completing the questionnaire, this association differed across the cohorts with the effect non-significant for the 20+ cohort (OR = 0.99, 0.95–1.03), stronger in the 40+ cohort (OR = 0.96, 0.93–1.00) and strongest in the 60+ cohort (OR = 0.91, 0.87–0.97).
This analysis is restricted to respondents who provided data at the three waves of the study. Of the 4250 respondents who completed the food frequency questionnaire, 3663 (86%) completed the study at wave 3 and represent the final sample for the current study. The rate of attrition was consistent for men and women (14.9 and 14.7; chi square = 0.03, df = 1, p = .868), but greater for those in the oldest cohort (20+ = 13.8, 40+ = 10.9. 60+ = 18.4; chi square = 35.87, df = 2, p<.001). Increased levels of education (OR = 0.91, 0.87–0.94) and moderate (vs mild) levels of physical activity at baseline (OR = 0.77, 0.87–0.74) were each associated with lesser risk of subsequent attrition, whereas baseline depressive symptoms (OR = 1.06, 1.01–1.10), not participating in the labour force (OR = 1.47. 1.17–1.86), and speaking English as a second language (OR = 1.57. 1.14–2.17) were associated with greater likelihood of attrition. Importantly, the dietary factors were not associated with subsequent attrition.
Measures
Diet
Dietary intake was assessed using a version of the validated Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation (CSIRO) Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ) [25], which was included in the self-completion Diet and Health booklet. The FFQ was developed to assess usual food and drink intake and, thereby, nutrient intake for all respondents. The FFQ included a list of foods and standard serving sizes and respondents indicated their habitual frequency of consumption on an 11-point scale (from never to 3 times a day) and indicated divergence from usual serving size. Other open-ended and multiple-choice questions collected more detailed information on food intake, including cooking methods, type of fat/oil used, seasonality of consumption, and consumption of fat- or salt-reduced products. Dietary analysis of the data produced estimates of daily nutrient intake and (critical for this analysis) daily grams of each food item consumed. While the estimate of absolute nutrient intake from the FFQ has modest agreement with other methods such as daily diet records [26], the method has high repeatability and is recommended for assessment of relative intake and, therefore, an appropriate tool for epidemiological analysis [25].
Mental health
Depressive symptoms were assessed using the Goldberg Depression scale at waves 1, 2 and 3 [27]. The scale has 9-items, each of which asks about the presence of a symptom over the past month with a binary (yes/no) response option, producing symptom score from 0 to 9. We focus our analysis on this total scale score, a count measure ranging from 0 to 9, but for descriptive purposes report a dichotomous measure of high depressive symptoms (scores of 6 or greater on the scale [28]).
Socioeconomic position
A time-invariant measure sought to capture the socioeconomic position of respondents drawing on multiple measures of respondents' socioeconomic circumstances from across all three waves. Baseline measures included labour force status (employed, unemployed or not participating in the labour force), occupation, and educational attainment (years of full-time education corresponding with highest level of educational attainment). Respondents' description of their job was classified using the Australian and New Zealand Standard Classification of Occupations (Australian Bureau of Statistics 1997) and for this analysis categorized as low-skilled occupations (elementary clerical, sales and service and labourers) vs all other jobs. Socioeconomic disadvantage in childhood was also assessed by a single item which asked respondents whether they had “grown up in poverty”.
Additional socioeconomic measures from later waves included whether respondents reported being welfare dependent (i.e., reported that their main source of income was from a government pension or payment), their reported weekly household income (from 6 categories presented in Table 1), and their experience of financial hardship. Four dichotomous items included from wave 2 of the study assessed lack of basic goods and opportunities due to a lack of financial resources (over the past year have the following happened because you were short of money: pawned or sold something; went without meals, unable to heat home, asked for help from welfare/community organisations). The items focus on objective aspects of deprivation, assessing the specific behavioural consequences of a lack of financial resources. Finally, the analysis considered area-level socioeconomic circumstances (the Index of Relative Socioeconomic Advantage and Disadvantage; Australian Bureau of Statistics 2004) reflecting respondents' suburb of residence at wave 3. We use this information to identify approximately 15% of respondents residing in the most disadvantaged suburbs.
Table 1. Baseline demographic, socioeconomic, lifestyle and diet characteristics of the sample.
N | High level of depressive symptoms | Test of association | |||
Yes (≥6) | No (< 6) | ||||
N | 3663 | 343 | 3304 | ||
Demographic characteristics | |||||
Age at baseline (%) | 20–24 | 987 | 14.9 | 85.1 | χ2 (2) = 80.5 p = .000 |
40–44 | 1223 | 10.9 | 59.1 | ||
60–64 | 1437 | 4.4 | 95.6 | ||
Sex (%) | Men | 1612 | 8.0 | 92.0 | χ2 (1) = 6.7 p = .01 |
Women | 2035 | 10.5 | 89.5 | ||
Measures of socioeconomic position | |||||
Years of completed education (w1) | Mean (se) | 3646 | 14.4 (.12) | 14.6 (.04) | F(1, 3646) = 2.9, p = .087 |
Weekly income (w2)(%) | Up to $300 | 143 | 17.5 | 82.5 | χ2 (6) = 21.3, p = .002 |
Up to $575 | 438 | 11.0 | 89.0 | ||
Up to $1075 | 825 | 9.1 | 90.9 | ||
Up to $1700 | 796 | 9.7 | 90.3 | ||
Up to $2400 | 615 | 10.1 | 89.9 | ||
More than $2400 | 677 | 7.4 | 92.6 | ||
Not reported | 153 | 3.9 | 96.1 | ||
Labour-force status (w1) | Employed | 2531 | 10.0 | 90.0 | χ2 (2) = 31.2, p = .000 |
Unemployed | 83 | 24.1 | 75.9 | ||
Not in labour force | 1032 | 6.7 | 93.3 | ||
Hardship (w2) | No | 3354 | 8.1 | 92.0 | χ2 (1) = 89.9, p = .000 |
Yes | 293 | 24.9 | 75.1 | ||
Childhood poverty | No | 3229 | 91.2 | 8.8 | χ2 (1) = 11.1, p = .001 |
Yes | 418 | 86.1 | 13.9 | ||
Occupational skill level (w1) | Moderate/High | 3122 | 8.7 | 91.3 | χ2 (1) = 12.2, p = .000 |
Low | 525 | 13.5 | 86.5 | ||
Income support dependent (w2) | No | 3217 | 9.0 | 91.0 | χ2 (1) = 4.9, p = .03 |
Yes | 430 | 12.3 | 87.7 | ||
Area disadvantage (w3) | Not | 3092 | 8.9 | 91.1 | χ2 (2) = 12.8, p = .002 |
Disadvantaged | 499 | 13.4 | 86.6 | ||
Not reported | 56 | 3.6 | 96.4 | ||
Overall SES factor (higher = greater disadvantage) | Mean (se) | 3647 | .17 (.06) | −.02 (.02) | F(1, 3647) = 10.0, p = .002 |
Lifestyle and cardiovascular risk factors | |||||
Physical activity | None/mild | 1716 | 10.8 | 89.2 | χ2 (2) = 8.8, p = .01 |
Moderate | 1077 | 8.7 | 91.3 | ||
Vigorous | 854 | 7.4 | 92.6 | ||
Current smoker | No | 3112 | 8.1 | 91.9 | χ2 (1) = 42.6, p = .000 |
Yes | 535 | 17.0 | 83.0 | ||
Hypertension | No | 2330 | 10.6 | 89.4 | χ2 (1) = 11.9, p = .001 |
Yes | 1287 | 7.1 | 92.9 | ||
Diabetes | No | 3547 | 9.4 | 90.6 | χ2 (1) = 0.3, p = .556 |
Yes | 99 | 11.1 | 88.9 | ||
Reported heart problems | No | 3410 | 9.4 | 90.6 | χ2 (1) = 0.1, p = .822 |
Yes | 234 | 9.8 | 90.2 | ||
Stroke or TIA | No | 3582 | 9.4 | 90.6 | χ2 (1) = 0.2, p = .672 |
Yes | 64 | 10.9 | 89.1 | ||
Diet | |||||
Prudent diet consumption | Low | 1214 | 13.0 | 87.0 | χ2 (2) = 28.1, p = .000 |
Medium | 1217 | 7.9 | 92.1 | ||
High | 1216 | 7.3 | 92.7 | ||
Western diet consumption | Low | 1218 | 8.7 | 91.3 | χ2 (2) = 4.7, p = .10 |
Medium | 1216 | 8.6 | 91.4 | ||
High | 1213 | 10.9 | 89.1 |
Covariates
Other covariates included in the analyses were cohort (age), gender, baseline smoking and level of physical activity, which categorised respondents as undertaking vigorous, moderate or none/mild physical activity, based on the approach used in the UK Whitehall II study [29]. Cardiovascular risk factors were assessed at each wave including hypertension (corresponding with NHMRC 2010 criteria), current diabetes, reported experience of heart problems, and experience of stroke or transient ischemic attack.
Statistical analysis
This analysis included respondents who provided data at all three waves of the study. FFQ data was only collected at wave 1. Our consideration of socioeconomic position sought to identify whether the association between diet and depression reflects structural aspects of society. Therefore, each of the individual measures can be considered a marker of this underlying construct (for similar example see Butterworth et al. [30]). We constructed a composite measure of socioeconomic position via factor analysis of these multiple measures described earlier, using the weighted least square estimation procedure in Mplus 4.2. The single factor solution demonstrated adequate model fit (CFI = 0.931, TLI = 0.902, RMSEA = 0.032). Missing data for the items included in the current analysis was minimal, with mean level of missingness across variables of 0.2%.
Consistent with the recommended methodologies in the field of nutritional epidemiology [14], principle components analysis (PCA) was used to summarize the information from the 188 distinct food items into meaningful scales representing dietary patterns. We do not claim that these factors capture all individual variation in dietary consumption but, rather, serve to identify major shared dietary patterns within the population. After conducting the initial PCA on the full set of dietary variables, we simplified the analysis and interpretation by excluding uncommon dietary items (items consumed by fewer than 25% of respondents and with median intake of less than 1 gram per week). This reduced the pool of individual dietary items to 86 without significantly altering the factor scores. Further, to minimize the potential influence of extreme levels of individual food item consumption on factor scores, estimated weekly consumption for each food item was converted to a z-score and extreme scores truncated to ±5. Thus, the potential influence of coding errors or outlying values (e.g., respondents who reported daily consumption of 2 kg of beer, 1850 g of cola drinks, or 330 g of pineapple) was reduced. Overall, 73% of respondents were not affected by these changes, and a further 19% had only a single dietary item truncated.
Consistent with previous similar research, the scree plot of eigenvalues suggested that a two-factor solution was more salient than alternatives, while being readily interpretable. The two orthogonal factors accounted for 14.4% of the variance in this large set of dietary items. The first factor, which we labelled ‘prudent’ (healthy) diet, reflected consumption of fresh vegetables (carrots, broccoli), salad (lettuce, cucumber) and fruit (peach, melon, nectarines), and grilled fish. The second factor labeled ‘western’ (unhealthy) was best characterised by the consumption of roast meat, sausages, hamburgers, steak, chips, crisps and soft drinks. For each of the factors, higher scores represent greater levels of consumption of these types of food. Although the construction of the factors for this analysis ensured orthogonality, even without this restriction the correlation between the two factors was weak at r = −.13. The continuous factor scores were used as outcome variables in preliminary analyses, while scale scores were categorised into tertiles (representing low, medium and high levels of consumption) when diet pattern was included as a predictor of depressive symptoms. The Pearson correlation between these reduced dietary pattern factor scores and the factors scores based on the complete and untruncated set of food items were .97 and .95. Sensitivity tests, repeating the analyses with these “raw” factors, also demonstrated the same results as reported in this manuscript. The rotated factor loadings are presented in table S1.
Descriptive statistics were generated, and chi-square and linear regression models used to evaluate the association between each measure and depressive symptoms. Cross-sectional negative binomial (given overdispersion) models were used to evaluate the contemporaneous association between diet pattern and depression scale scores at baseline, controlling for covariates and potential confounders. The exponentiated model coefficients are interpreted as incidence rate ratios (IRRs). In considering the prospective effects of dietary patterns on subsequent depressive symptoms we used a generalised estimating equation (GEE) approach to examine population average effects observed in longitudinal negative binomial models, with the outcome measures reflecting depressive symptoms at waves 2 and 3. Sensitivity analyses repeated these models excluding those with high depressive symptoms at baseline and excluding those reporting use of antidepressants. All analyses were conducted using STATA 12.
Results
Preliminary analyses
Table 1 presents data on the baseline characteristics of the sample, separately for those with high and low levels of depressive symptoms. The results confirm that higher depressive symptoms were more common amongst women, younger respondents and those in poorer social circumstances (e.g., experiencing hardship, unemployed, disadvantaged area; although the association between high depressive symptoms and years of education was marginal [31]). High levels of depressive symptoms were also more common amongst those who engaged in low levels of physical activity, current smokers and those identified as hypertensive. These results also provide an indication of an association between consumption of prudent diet and depressive symptoms, though the univariate association between western diet and the binary measure of depressive symptoms did not reach statistical significance.
Initial linear regression models showed that poorer socioeconomic position (higher scores on this factor) was associated with lower levels of prudent dietary consumption (Beta = −.06. se = .018, p = .001), and greater consumption of a western diet (Beta = .19, se = .018, p<.001). Analysis of all of the individual measures of socioeconomic circumstances (results not shown) demonstrated that not reporting income, fewer years of completed education, childhood poverty, and residing in a disadvantaged area were independently inversely associated with prudent dietary pattern scores, while not reporting income, fewer years of completed education, and experience of hardship were positively associated with western dietary pattern scores. Further linear regression models also showed that hypertension (Beta = .15, se = .036, p<.001) and diabetes (Beta = .40, se = .094, p<.001) were associated with greater consumption of a western diet. Similarly, smoking was associated with higher western dietary pattern scores (Beta = .32, se = .040, p<.001) and there was a trend for higher levels of physical activity to be related to lower western dietary pattern scores (Beta = −.07, se = .040, p = .078). Higher levels of physical activity were associated with higher scores on the prudent dietary pattern factor (Beta = .29, se = .040, p<.001), while smoking was associated with lower scores (Beta = −.27, se = .044, p<.001); however, none of cardiovascular risk factors were associated with prudent dietary pattern scores (results not shown).
Many diet studies control for energy intake as they seek to examine the influence of particular nutrients as a proportion of overall dietary intake. However, this is not the case in the current analysis of the health correlates of dietary patterns as the focus not upon the amount of particular foods but on different food intake patterns. For example, high levels of energy intake are an important characteristic of a western diet. Indeed, while Pearson's r correlations showed only a weak association between scores on prudent diet factor and energy intake (r = .21), they confirmed that energy intake was strongly related to western diet (r = .67). Further, analysis suggested that the inclusion of energy intake did not improve fit over a model with the categorical measure of western diet (log likelihood χ2 = 0.61, df = 1, p = .436) but did render the effect of high levels of western diet non-significant. Thus, the current analyses did not include energy intake in models as statistical control for energy intake may overly control the analysis and mask important associations involving western dietary pattern.
The cross-sectional (wave 1) association between diet and depressive symptoms
An initial cross-sectional negative binomial regression model controlling for age and sex showed a modest association between western dietary pattern scores and number of depressive symptoms (IRR for highest tertile versus lowest tertile of western diet = 1.10; see Table 2). Similarly, compared to those with the highest tertile of consumption of a prudent diet, those in the lowest consumption tertile reported a greater number of depressive symptoms (IRR = 1.15). However, adjustment for socioeconomic position attenuated the prudent diet-depressive symptoms association and fully explained the western-depression score association (see Model 2, Table 2). Adjusting for other lifestyle behaviours and cardiovascular risk factors eliminated any independent association between diet and depressive symptoms (Model 3, Table 2). Posthoc investigation showed that it was low level of physical activity that explained the association between low levels of prudent diet and depressive symptoms. There was no evidence that the effect of dietary pattern on depressive symptoms was moderated by age, sex or socioeconomic circumstances.
Table 2. Results from series of cross-sectional negative binomial regression models investigating the association between tertiles of prudent and western diet intake and number of depressive symptoms reported at baseline.
Model 1 * | Model 2 * | Model 3 * | Model 4 * | ||||||
Socioeconomic circumstances | Health Behaviours | CVD Risk factors | |||||||
Prudent diet | Low | 1.15 | 1.06–1.26 | 1.11 | 1.02–1.21 | 1.06 | 0.97–1.16 | 1.07 | 0.98–1.17 |
Medium | 1.00 | 0.92–1.09 | 1.00 | 0.92–1.09 | 0.98 | 0.91–1.07 | 0.99 | 0.91–1.07 | |
High | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | |||||
Western diet | Low | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | ||||
Medium | 1.02 | 0.93–1.11 | 1.00 | 0.92–1.09 | 1.00 | 0.92–1.08 | 0.99 | 0.91–1.08 | |
High | 1.10 | 1.01–1.20 | 1.03 | 0.95–1.13 | 1.01 | 0.93–1.10 | 1.01 | 0.92–1.10 | |
Socioeconomic position | 1.22 | 1.17–1.27 | 1.20 | 1.15–1.24 | 1.19 | 1.14–1.24 | |||
Physical activity | None/mild | 1.00 | 1.00 | ||||||
Moderate | 0.90 | 0.83–0.97 | 0.91 | 0.84–0.98 | |||||
Vigorous | 0.80 | 0.74–0.88 | 0.82 | 0.75–0.89 | |||||
Smoking status | Current | 1.17 | 1.06–1.28 | 1.18 | 1.08–1.30 | ||||
Hypertension | Current | 0.98 | 0.90–1.06 | ||||||
Diabetes | Current | 1.19 | 0.96–1.46 | ||||||
Heart problems | Current | 1.21 | 1.05–1.40 | ||||||
Stroke/TIA | Current | 1.30 | 1.01–1.67 |
*controlling for age & sex.
To evaluate the nature of the association amongst diet, depressive symptoms and socioeconomic circumstances a series of hierarchical negative binomial models, which varied the order in which the measure of socioeconomic position and dietary factor tertiles were added to the model, examined the degree to which the second variable explained the effect of the first (based on log-scale coefficients). The results showed that the inclusion of the dietary factor scores explained only 3.6% of the effect of the socioeconomic factor on depressive symptoms. By comparison, the inclusion of the socioeconomic factor explained 25.2% of the effect of prudent diet and 66.0% of the effect of western diet on depression symptom scores.
Longitudinal analyses
The initial longitudinal model, considering data from the three cohorts combined, demonstrated a significant association between dietary patterns and subsequent depressive symptoms, with those with the lowest intake of a prudent diet and those with the highest intake of a western diet showing elevated risk of later depressive symptoms. However, there was a significant interaction between age and dietary patterns (wald χ2 test of interaction terms = 17.5, df = 8, p = .025). Therefore, subsequent analyses were stratified by age group. Results showed no predictive effect of either prudent or western dietary pattern on depressive symptom incidence in the youngest or middle-aged cohorts (results not shown). However, in the 60+ age group, those in the lowest tertile of prudent diet had higher levels of depressive symptoms at waves 2 and 3 than those with highest consumption of prudent diet, and those in the highest tertile of the western dietary pattern had higher symptom scores than those with the lowest level of consumption of a western diet (see Table 3). Further adjustment for baseline depressive symptoms (Model 2), the time-invariant measure of socioeconomic position (Model 3), and other health behaviours (Model 4) attenuated the association between western diet and subsequent depressive symptoms to marginal significance and the final adjustment for CVD risk factors reduced the western diet-depression association to non-significance. However, these later variables are likely to be on the causal pathway between dietary intake and depressive symptoms. The prudent diet-depression association remained significant through all models, even after adjustment for CVD risk factors. There were no interactions evident between dietary patterns and any of the other covariates included in the model. Sensitivity analyses i) excluding participants with high levels of baseline depressive symptoms (rather than controlling for baseline symptoms) and ii) excluding those reporting antidepressant medication use produced an identical pattern of results to those reported here (results not shown).
Table 3. Results from a series of longitudinal negative binomial regression models investigating the association between tertiles of prudent and western diet intake at baseline and subsequent number of depressive symptoms reported at waves 2 and 3 for study respondents in the oldest cohort.
60s | Model 1* | Model 2 * + baseline depressive symptoms | Model 3 * + socioeconomic circumstances | Model 4 * + health behaviours | Model 5 * + CVD risk factors | ||||||
Prudent diet | Low | 1.31 | 1.12–1.53 | 1.21 | 1.04–1.41 | 1.19 | 1.02–1.38 | 1.19 | 1.01–1.40 | 1.18 | 1.01–1.39 |
Medium | 1.11 | 0.97–1.27 | 1.08 | 0.95–1.23 | 1.08 | 0.95–1.22 | 1.08 | 0.94–1.23 | 1.08 | 0.94–1.24 | |
High | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | ||||||
Western diet | Low | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | 1.00 | |||||
Medium | 1.03 | 0.90–1.18 | 0.95 | 0.83–1.09 | 0.95 | 0.83–1.08 | 0.93 | 0.80–1.06 | 0.92 | 0.80–1.06 | |
High | 1.39 | 1.20–1.62 | 1.22 | 1.06–1.41 | 1.20 | 1.03–1.39 | 1.17 | 1.00–1.36 | 1.14 | 0.98–1.34 | |
Wave 1 dep score | 1.34 | 1.30–1.38 | 1.33 | 1.29–1.37 | 1.33 | 1.28–1.37 | 1.32 | 1.27–1.36 | |||
Socioeconomic position | 1.07 | 1.01–1.14 | 1.04 | 0.98–1.11 | 1.03 | 0.97–1.10 | |||||
Baseline Physical activity | None/mild | 1.00 | 1.00 | ||||||||
Moderate | 0.90 | 0.78–1.02 | 0.90 | 0.79–1.03 | |||||||
Vigorous | 0.89 | 0.74–1.07 | 0.92 | 0.77–1.12 | |||||||
Baseline Smoking | 1.27 | 1.03–1.56 | 1.29 | 1.05–1.59 | |||||||
Baseline Hypertension | 1.16 | 1.01–1.34 | |||||||||
Baseline Diabetes | 1.00 | 0.72–1.39 | |||||||||
Baseline Heart Problems | 0.97 | 0.77–1.39 | |||||||||
Baseline Stroke/TIA | 1.05 | 0.72–1.54 | |||||||||
Time-varying Hypertension | 0.87 | 076–0.99 | |||||||||
Time-varying Diabetes | 1.17 | 0.92–1.48 | |||||||||
Time-varying Heart Problems | 1.10 | 0.92–1.33 | |||||||||
Time-varying Stroke/TIA | 1.12 | 0.87–1.45 |
controlling for sex, wave.
Discussion
In this study, both lower scores on a prudent dietary pattern and higher scores on a western dietary pattern were associated with current depressive symptoms cross-sectionally; however, these relationships were explained after adjustment for socioeconomic position and other health behaviours. Specifically, physical activity levels explained the relationship between prudent dietary patterns and depressive symptoms, while socioeconomic position explained the association between the western dietary pattern and depressive symptoms. In longitudinal analyses, however, both low scores on the prudent dietary pattern and higher scores on a western dietary pattern were associated with an increased number of subsequent depressive symptoms in older adults. These relationships were attenuated by the inclusion of socioeconomic and health behaviour variables, but remained significant. Dietary patterns did not explain the relationship between socioeconomic position and depressive symptoms, suggesting that this association is not mediated by poor diet. On the other hand, the inclusion of CVD risk factors in the model did attenuate the association between western diet and depressive symptoms suggesting that the relationship between unhealthy foods and depression risk may be mediated, at least partly, through risk factors for CVD.
The results of this study suggest that, for older people, a diet low in healthy foods and/or a diet high in unhealthy foods may increase the risk of depression over time. Importantly, the predictive effect of dietary patterns on later depressive symptoms persisted after controlling for a robust indicator of socioeconomic position, as well as baseline depressive symptoms and lifestyle and cardiovascular risk factors. This supports the contention that poor diet quality imposes its own risk for depression, independently of socioeconomic circumstances. This also receives support from the finding that dietary patterns did not explain the association between socioeconomic position and depressive symptoms [22]. Finally, there was no evidence in any of the analyses that the two variables representing western and prudent dietary patterns interacted to jointly influence depressive scores, which is consistent with the view that reduced intake of healthy foods is a risk factor for poor mental health in older adults, regardless of the intake of unhealthy foods, and vice versa.
Although we cannot be sure of why the predictive effect of dietary patterns on depressive scores was only evident in the older cohort and not in the younger and middle-aged cohorts, one possible explanation relates to the chronic impact of poor quality diet on the biological systems that affect depression risk, with the cumulative effects of poor diet only impacting on mood in later life. For example, while the long-term effects of poor diet lead to conditions associated with chronic inflammation (eg. visceral obesity), and vascular pathology (e.g. atherosclerosis), these effects are likely to develop over decades and their impact on brain function may not be detectable until later in life. This is concordant with what is seen in cognition, where risk factors in mid-life influence dementia risk in late-life, but are not observed to have a large effect in younger or middle aged adults [32]. As dietary information was not available for subsequent waves of assessment it was not possible to assess changes in dietary pattern scores across waves. However, it is reasonable to expect that diet assessed at baseline is reflective of habitual dietary patterns across the lifecourse, or at least extended periods of time. This is particularly the case for older adults, whose dietary habits are known to be relatively stable over time [33]. As such, the predictive relationship of poor quality diet, characterized by these dietary pattern scores, on the incidence of depressive symptoms may reflect the long-term exposure to the noxious effects of high fat, refined sugar foods and/or a diet insufficient in nutrients and fibre.
Chronic sub-clinical inflammation and attendant increases in oxidative stress are biological mechanisms that appear to be implicated in associations between diet and depression. Consumption of foods high in energy such as sugars, proteins and lipids has been found to be pro-inflammatory. Animal studies have shown that rodents fed a high fat diet had higher level of neuronal inflammation [34], while in humans, experimentally raised plasma glucose levels were associated with increased plasma cytokine (TNF-a, IL6, IL18) [35]. Intake of both lipids and proteins has been found to be associated with increased production of reactive oxygen species (ROS), which are known to be associated with low-grade pro-inflammatory responses [36], [37]. Increased ROS and pro-inflammatory responses are also associated with apoptosis and cerebral atrophy, particularly in the hippocampus, both in controlled experiments in rats and in observational studies in humans [38]–[40]. Since cortical grey matter and, even more so, hippocampal atrophy has been found to be linked with depression [41], it is possible that western diets high in fats, animal protein, and sugars lead to a state of chronic sub-clinical inflammation and subsequent brain atrophy which increases the risk of developing depression. Conversely, a number of foods such as fish, fruit and vegetables consistent with a prudent diet have been demonstrated to have anti-inflammatory and anti-oxidant effects and thus are likely to be protective against cerebral changes associated with depression [42], [43]. Finally, intake of lipids and pro-inflammatory foods are also known to be associated with atherosclerosis, diabetes, and cardio-vascular disease. These chronic diseases have all been found to be associated with depression, cerebro-vascular disease, and brain atrophy and thus suggest another pathway through which diet may modulate mental health. Concordant with this hypothesis, we found some evidence that the relationship between western dietary patterns and depression risk was mediated by hypertension, type 2 diabetes, heart problems and stroke.
Study strengths and limitations
The strengths of our study include the use of data drawn from a large, representative, population-based sample of adults, with well-validated tools used for the assessment of both diet and depression. Outcomes of the diagnostic measures used for depression in PATH are concordant with national prevalence rates for depression and with established age and gender differences [24]. Furthermore, the thoroughness of measures used to determine the socioeconomic position of participants in the PATH study is an important strength. Our current study extends previous investigations in this nascent field of mental health research by explicitly testing the hypothesis that the diet-depression association is explained by residual confounding by socioeconomic position, with the factor score based on measures from multiple domains. However, we were limited in some of our measures, with information on dietary intakes only available at the baseline assessment and a time-invariant measure of socioeconomic position used in the analysis. This limited our ability to track the contemporaneous associations over time, such as the potential association between change in diet and change in depressive symptoms. All longitudinal studies are limited to some extent by attrition and selection bias, however the follow-up rate in the PATH study is very high by current international standards. Further research is needed to establish and explicate biological mechanisms in the diet-mental health relationship. We assumed stability of dietary intakes over our measured time periods, however there is likely to be some variability that we are unable to monitor.
Conclusion
The results of this study confirm that the relationship between habitual dietary intake and depressive symptoms is somewhat explained by socioeconomic position and other health behaviours, but suggest that long term exposure to unhealthy dietary habits independently predisposes to depression over the lifecourse and that this risk is partly mediated through the influence of dietary habits on risk factors for CVD. Thorough investigations of the mechanisms by which poor diet may impact on depression risk are now required.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful to Anthony Jorm, Bryan Rodgers, Helen Christensen, Simon Easteal, Patricia Jacomb, Karen Maxwell, and the PATH interviewers and participants. Further information about the PATH through Life project is available at http://crahw.anu.edu.au/research/projects/personality-total-health-path-through-life. There is no open access to the data set, but strategic collaborations are welcome and contact information is available on the website for interested parties to learn more about formal application procedures.
Funding Statement
This study was supported by NHMRC Grant No. 973302, Program Grants No. 179805 and 350833, NHMRC project grant No. 157125. KJA is funded by NHMRC Research Fellowship No. 1002560. NC is funded by an ARC Future Fellowship No. 120100227. FNJ is funded by an NHMRC Postdoctoral Fellowship No. 628912. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Kohlboeck G, Sausenthaler S, Standl M, Koletzko S, Bauer CP, et al. (2012) Food Intake, Diet Quality and Behavioral Problems in Children: Results from the GINI-plus/LISA-plus Studies. Ann Nutr Metab 60: 247–256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Jacka F, Ystrom E, Brantsaeter AL, Karevold E, Roth C, et al. (2013) Maternal and Early Postnatal Nutrition and Mental Health of Offspring by Age 5 Years: A Prospective Cohort Study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 52(10): 1038–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Jacka FN, Kremer PJ, Leslie E, Berk M, Patton G, et al. (2010) Associations between diet quality and depressed mood in adolescents: results from the Healthy Neighbourhoods study. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 44: 435–442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Jacka F, Kremer PJ, Berk M, de Silva-Sanigorski A, Moodie M, et al. (2011) A prospective study of diet quality and mental health in adolescents. PLoS One 6: e24805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Weng TT, Hao JH, Qian QW, Cao H, Fu JL, et al.. (2011) Is there any relationship between dietary patterns and depression and anxiety in Chinese adolescents? Public Health Nutr: 1–10. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 6. Jacka FN, Mykletun A, Berk M, Bjelland I, Tell GS (2011) The association between habitual diet quality and the common mental disorders in community-dwelling adults: the Hordaland Health study. Psychosom Med 73: 483–490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Jacka FN, Pasco JA, Mykletun A, Williams LJ, Hodge AM, et al. (2010) Association between western and traditional diets and depression and anxiety in women. Am J Psychiatry 167: 305–311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Akbaraly TN, Brunner EJ, Ferrie JE, Marmot MG, Kivimaki M, et al. (2009) Dietary pattern and depressive symptoms in middle age. Br J Psychiatry 195: 408–413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Sanchez-Villegas A, Delgado-Rodriguez M, Alonso A, Schlatter J, Lahortiga F, et al. (2009) Association of the Mediterranean dietary pattern with the incidence of depression: the Seguimiento Universidad de Navarra/University of Navarra follow-up (SUN) cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry 66: 1090–1098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Nanri A, Kimura Y, Matsushita Y, Ohta M, Sato M, et al. (2010) Dietary patterns and depressive symptoms among Japanese men and women. Eur J Clin Nutr 64: 832–839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Guenther PM, Reedy J, Krebs-Smith SM, Reeve BB, Basiotis PP (2007) Development and Evaluation of the Healthy Eating Index-2005: Technical Report. U.S. Department of Agriculture.
- 12. Collins CE, Young AF, Hodge A (2008) Diet quality is associated with higher nutrient intake and self-rated health in mid-aged women. J Am Coll Nutr 27: 146–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Mila-Villarroel R, Bach-Faig A, Puig J, Puchal A, Farran A, et al. (2011) Comparison and evaluation of the reliability of indexes of adherence to the Mediterranean diet. Public Health Nutr 14: 2338–2345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Hu FB (2002) Dietary pattern analysis: a new direction in nutritional epidemiology. Curr Opin Lipidol 13: 3–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Chatzi L, Melaki V, Sarri K, Apostolaki I, Roumeliotaki T, et al. (2011) Dietary patterns during pregnancy and the risk of postpartum depression: the mother-child ‘Rhea’ cohort in Crete, Greece. Public Health Nutr 14: 1663–1670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Lopez-Garcia E, Schulze MB, Fung TT, Meigs JB, Rifai N, et al. (2004) Major dietary patterns are related to plasma concentrations of markers of inflammation and endothelial dysfunction. Am J Clin Nutr 80: 1029–1035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Jacka FN, Pasco JA, Mykletun A, Williams LJ, Nicholson GC, et al. (2011) Diet quality in bipolar disorder in a population-based sample of women. J Affect Disord 129: 332–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Jacka FN, Mykletun A, Berk M (2012) Moving towards a population health approach to the primary prevention of common mental disorders. BMC Medicine 10: 149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Galobardes B, Morabia A, Bernstein MS (2001) Diet and socioeconomic position: does the use of different indicators matter? Int J Epidemiol 30: 334–340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Henderson L, Gregory J, Swan G (2002) The National Diet and Nutrition Survey: Adults Aged 19–64 Years: Types and Quantities of Foods Consumed. London.
- 21. Darmon N, Drewnowski A (2008) Does social class predict diet quality? The American journal of clinical nutrition 87: 1107–1117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Butterworth P, Olesen SC, Leach LS (2012) The role of hardship in the association between socio-economic position and depression. The Australian and New Zealand journal of psychiatry 46: 364–373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Berk M, Jacka FN (2012) Preventive strategies in depression: gathering evidence for risk factors and potential interventions. British journal of psychiatry 201: 339–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Anstey KJ, Christensen H, Butterworth P, Easteal S, Mackinnon A, et al. (2012) Cohort Profile: The PATH through life project. Int J Epidemiol 41: 951–960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Lassale C, Guilbert C, Keogh J, Syrette J, Lange K, et al. (2009) Estimating food intakes in Australia: validation of the Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation (CSIRO) food frequency questionnaire against weighed dietary intakes. Journal of human nutrition and dietetics : the official journal of the British Dietetic Association 22: 559–566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Ambrosini GL, Mackerras D, de Klerk NH, Musk AW (2003) Comparison of an Australian food-frequency questionnaire with diet records: implications for nutrition surveillance. Public Health Nutr 6: 415–422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Goldberg D, Bridges K, Duncan-Jones P, Grayson D (1988) Detecting anxiety and depression in general medical settings. Bmj 297: 897–899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Butterworth P, Rodgers B, Windsor TD (2009) Financial hardship, socio-economic position and depression: results from the PATH Through Life Survey. Soc Sci Med 69: 229–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Marmot MG, Smith GD, Stansfeld S, Patel C, North F, et al. (1991) Health inequalities among British civil servants: the Whitehall II study. Lancet 337: 1387–1393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Butterworth P, Olesen SC, Leach LS (2013) Socioeconomic differences in antidepressant use in the PATH Through Life Study: evidence of health inequalities, prescribing bias, or an effective social safety net? J Affect Disord 149: 75–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Morris LJ, Prior SL, Anstey KJ, Butterworth P, Tait RJ, et al.. (2011) Mental Health and Wellbeing in the ACT. Canberra, ACT.
- 32. Anstey KJ, Cherbuin N, Budge M, Young J (2011) Body mass index in midlife and late-life as a risk factor for dementia: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. Obes Rev 12: e426–437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Mulder M, Ranchor AV, Sanderman R, Bouma J, van den Heuvel WJ (1998) The stability of lifestyle behaviour. Int J Epidemiol 27: 199–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Zhang X, Dong F, Ren J, Driscoll MJ, Culver B (2005) High dietary fat induces NADPH oxidase-associated oxidative stress and inflammation in rat cerebral cortex. Exp Neurol 191: 318–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Esposito K, Nappo F, Marfella R, Giugliano G, Giugliano F, et al. (2002) Inflammatory cytokine concentrations are acutely increased by hyperglycemia in humans: role of oxidative stress. Circulation 106: 2067–2072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Mohanty P, Ghanim H, Hamouda W, Aljada A, Garg R, et al. (2002) Both lipid and protein intakes stimulate increased generation of reactive oxygen species by polymorphonuclear leukocytes and mononuclear cells. Am J Clin Nutr 75: 767–772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Abramson JL, Hooper WC, Jones DP, Ashfaqc S, Rhodes SD, et al. (2005) Association between novel oxidative stress markers and C-reactive protein among adults without clinical coronary heart disease. Atherosclerosis 178: 115–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Gerecke KM, Kolobova A, Allen S, Fawer JL (2013) Exercise protects against chronic restraint stress-induced oxidative stress in the cortex and hippocampus. Brain Res. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 39. Baune BT, Ponath G, Rothermundt M, Roesler A, Berger K (2009) Association between cytokines and cerebral MRI changes in the aging brain. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 22: 23–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Rao JS, Kellom M, Kim HW, Rapoport SI, Reese EA (2012) Neuroinflammation and synaptic loss. Neurochem Res 37: 903–910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Du MY, Wu QZ, Yue Q, Li J, Liao Y, et al. (2012) Voxelwise meta-analysis of gray matter reduction in major depressive disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 36: 11–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Riediger ND, Othman RA, Suh M, Moghadasian MH (2009) A systemic review of the roles of n-3 fatty acids in health and disease. J Am Diet Assoc 109: 668–679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Maron D (2004) Flavonoids for reduction of atherosclerotic risk. Current Atherosclerosis Reports 6: 73–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.