Abstract
Objective: Increased vulnerability to extraneous distraction is a key symptom of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), which may have particularly disruptive consequences. Here we apply Load Theory of attention to increase understanding of this symptom, and to explore a potential method for ameliorating it. Previous research in nonclinical populations has highlighted increased perceptual load as a means of improving the ability to focus attention and avoid distraction. The present study examines whether adults with ADHD can also benefit from conditions of high perceptual load to improve their focused attention abilities. Method: We tested adults with ADHD and age- and IQ-matched controls on a novel measure of irrelevant distraction under load, designed to parallel the form of distraction that is symptomatic of ADHD. During a letter search task, in which perceptual load was varied through search set size, participants were required to ignore salient yet entirely irrelevant distractors (colorful images of cartoon characters) presented infrequently (10% of trials). Results: The presence of these distractors produced a significantly greater interference effect on the search RTs for the adults with ADHD compared with controls, p = .005, ηp2 = .231. Perceptual load, however, significantly reduced distractor interference for the ADHD group and was as effective in reducing the elevated distractor interference in ADHD as it was for controls. Conclusions: These findings clarify the nature of the attention deficit underlying increased distraction in ADHD, and demonstrate a tangible method for overcoming it.
Keywords: attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, ADHD, distraction, perceptual load, inattention
Symptoms of inattention and distractibility are key diagnostic criteria for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and appear to form the most pervasive component of the disorder; inattentive symptoms persist into adulthood more commonly than symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity (Wilens, Faraone & Biederman, 2004). Such symptoms can be highly disruptive in many daily life contexts from the workplace to the drive home from work. Indeed, many disadvantages reported by adults with ADHD, such as academic failure, workplace problems, and increased risk of car accidents (Faraone et al., 2000), have also been associated with increased daily life distractibility in nonclinical populations (Forster & Lavie, 2008b, for review), suggesting that symptoms of distraction may (at least partially) mediate the risk of functional impairments in ADHD.
The present study applies the Load Theory of Attention and Cognitive Control (Lavie, 1995; Lavie et al., 2004) to enhance understanding of the increased daily life distraction in ADHD, and to examine ways to ameliorate this. According to Load Theory, a key determinant of the ability to focus attention while avoiding distraction is whether the task being performed involves a sufficiently high “perceptual load” to fill perceptual capacity. Perceptual load has been operationally defined as either the quantity of stimuli requiring perceptual processing, or the complexity of perceptual judgments (Lavie, 2005). Tasks involving low load (e.g., involving few items, and simple judgments) are argued to leave spare capacity which spills over, resulting in involuntary processing of distractors. In this respect low load tasks confer vulnerability to distraction and necessitate reliance on executive “late selection” mechanisms to minimize interference. On the other hand, when the task processing involves high load (e.g., requiring complex perceptual judgments or searching among many items), using up the available perceptual capacity, perception of distractors is reduced or even eliminated. Thus, higher levels of perceptual load engender more efficient “early selection” of the task stimuli and reduce vulnerability to distraction.
In support of Load Theory are abundant demonstrations that increased perceptual load in the task results in reduced distractor processing throughout visual cortex, leading to reduced distractor interference across a range of distractor types (Lavie, 2005, for review). Thus, increasing perceptual load appears to be a powerful method for reducing distraction in nonclinical populations. However, it remains to be established whether increasing perceptual load would be equally effective in reducing the increased level of distraction experienced in ADHD.
The neural mechanisms underlying ADHD are heterogeneous and remain poorly characterized, although deficits in frontal-executive processes are proposed as one of the major sources of symptoms and impairments seen in ADHD (e.g., Bush, Valera & Seidmanm, 2005). Underrecruitment of the frontal-executive mechanisms, thought to underlie late selection (Lavie et al., 2004), may therefore play a key role in the disruption underlying distraction in ADHD. It remains unknown, however, whether deficits in ADHD may also involve early selection mechanisms or whether these remain intact.
Two previous studies (Huang-Pollock, Nigg, & Carr, 2005; Chan et al., 2009) examining perceptual load effects within response-competition tasks provide encouraging preliminary support for the hypothesis that early selection is intact in ADHD. Both studies found that perceptual load reduced distractor interference in children with ADHD and controls alike. However, neither study reported increased overall distractor interference in the ADHD group, even under low perceptual load (when risk of distraction is greatest). Huang-Pollock et al. (2005) found no group differences, whereas Chan et al. (2009) found a laterality bias, whereby children with ADHD experienced either more or less interference than controls depending on whether distractors were presented in the right or left visual field. Thus, as perceptual load effects have not yet been tested within a paradigm sensitive to reveal heightened distraction in ADHD, it is unclear whether elevated distraction in ADHD can be efficiently reduced by a potentially intact early selection mechanism.
To this end, the present study sought first to identify a laboratory measure sufficiently sensitive to reveal increased distraction in ADHD. Although the response-competition task is an established and widely used measure of distractor interference, we argue that this task may not be the most appropriate index for the symptom of irrelevant distraction in ADHD. Within this task, distraction is indexed by the performance decrement in the presence of a distractor associated with a competing response to the target. This is assessed against the baseline of performance in the presence of a response-compatible or response-neutral distractor. In this respect distraction in this paradigm does not reflect the extent of noticing and paying attention to an irrelevant distractor (in other words the extent of inattention), but instead the extent to which response associations of different distractor stimuli affect the target response. The defining role of distractor–response associations in this measure presents two limitations in indexing irrelevant distraction in ADHD. First, in the light of proposals that disruption to inhibitory mechanisms is one of the deficits underlying ADHD (e.g., Halperin & Schulz, 2006; Johnson, Wiersema, & Kuntsi, 2009), it is unclear whether any findings of inflated response-competition effects in ADHD can be attributed to failures of attention, as opposed to failures of response inhibition. In addition, the fact that response-competition effects indicate slowing of target responses attributable to competing response-incompatible (vs. response-compatible or response-neutral) distractors confines the scope of this measure to a very particular class of distractors, namely those that have specific associations with the task responses. This does not seem to capture the clinical symptoms of distraction by “extraneous stimuli” (as per DSM criterion), which are by definition task-unrelated and hence not particularly associated with one task response or the other.
Indeed, despite its prevalent use, previous studies of ADHD using variants of the response-competition task have produced somewhat inconsistent results; although some studies find evidence of increased interference in the ADHD group (e.g., Crone, Jennings, & van der Molen, 2003; Jonkman et al., 1999; Konrad et al., 2006; Vaidya et al., 2005), others did not (e.g., Booth et al., 2007; Brodeur & Pond, 2001; Chan et al., 2009; Chang, Davie, & Gavin, 2009; Geurts et al., 2008; Herrmann et al., 2010; Huang-Pollock et al., 2005; McLoughlin et al., 2009). Studies in natural settings, however, support the clinical picture that sources of distraction in ADHD tend to have little relevance to any current task. For example, while following a route through a zoo, children with ADHD were more likely than controls to veer off the designated route toward animals in other routes (Lawrence et al., 2002). Another study found children with ADHD to be significantly more distracted from watching TV by the presence of irrelevant yet appealing toys (Landau, Lorch, & Milich, 1992). Note also that in both of these examples, as is often the case in daily life, the stimuli that people fail to ignore despite their irrelevance to the task are highly salient (both “eye-catching,” and meaningful). In contrast, response-competition tasks typically use rather less salient distractor stimuli, such as letters, or simple shapes. Thus we anticipated that a more general measure of interference from salient yet entirely task-irrelevant distractors might prove more sensitive to reveal the distraction deficit in ADHD.
Finally, we note that the majority of previous investigations into attention in ADHD have studied child populations. As both early and late attentional selection processes are subject to significant developmental changes (Huang-Pollock, Carr & Nigg, 2002), it is important to establish whether any disruption to these processes reflects a core deficit (consistent with the persistence of inattentive symptoms into adulthood, Wilens, Biederman, & Spencer, 2002) as opposed to simply a developmental delay.
We therefore compared the performance of adults with ADHD with that of age-matched control participants on a recently established measure of irrelevant distraction (Forster & Lavie, 2008a, 2008b), designed to reflect a more general form of distraction than that measured in the response-competition task. Participants were asked to identify which of two possible “target” letters was present in a central letter search display, while ignoring any images presented outside the display. A colorful distractor image depicting a famous cartoon character (e.g., Spiderman) was presented in the periphery on the minority (10%) of trials. Note that, like the animals and toys that distracted ADHD groups in the naturalistic studies described above, the distractor in our measure was entirely irrelevant to the letter-search task: being visually dissimilar to the search-task stimuli, presented in an irrelevant location, and bearing no relation to the task through content and meaning, or task responses. Distractor interference was indexed simply by the increase in reaction times (RTs) for the presence versus absence of the images (following Forster & Lavie, 2008a, 2008b).
To test our claim that the elevated distraction in ADHD can be ameliorated under conditions of high perceptual load, we manipulated perceptual load by changing the similarity between the target and the other search letters. In the low load condition the other letters were five small ‘o’s, whereas in the high load condition they were five angular letters (see Figure 1). This manipulation changed the complexity of perceptual judgments as per one of the main operational definitions of perceptual load described earlier. We predicted that, in accordance with the diagnostic criteria of increased distraction, compared with age-matched controls the ADHD group will show increased RT interference from the task-irrelevant distractors. Importantly, we examined whether perceptual load would be as effective in reducing this heightened distraction for adults with ADHD, as in reducing distraction for the control group.
Method
Participants
Thirty-four age-matched adults participated in the study: 17 adults with ADHD and 17 volunteer controls. Participants in the ADHD group were recruited via the Maudsley Hospital Adult ADHD Clinic and local ADHD support groups. All ADHD participants were diagnosed in the U.K. by a psychiatrist who prescribed stimulant medication treatment (or in the case of one participant, had offered to prescribe stimulant treatment but this was declined). The U.K. diagnostic standards are conservative, with the diagnosis requiring evidence of ADHD in childhood and persistence of sufficient symptoms and impairment into adulthood. The standard diagnostic procedure uses a clinical diagnostic interview and detailed developmental account, with informants included if they are available. Fifteen of the ADHD participants were taking prescribed stimulant medication (methylphenidate, atomoxetine, amphetamine and dextroamphetamine)—these participants were tested after a 24-hour medication washout period.
On the day of testing, participants completed the Barkley and Murphy (2006) Current Symptoms Rating Scale—this is a Likert-type rating scale in which each Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM–IV) ADHD that symptom occurs is rated as occurring from never or rarely (0) to very often (3). As per the DSM–IV criteria, half of the items relate to inattentive symptoms, and half relate to hyperactive-impulsive symptoms. Scores can be computed using two methods (Barkley & Murphy, 2006): First, the number of symptoms reported as occurring “often” or “very often” may be counted. Using this method, all participants in the ADHD group met the threshold of six or more current inattentive symptoms at the time of testing. Eleven of these participants additionally met the threshold of six of more hyperactive-impulsive current symptoms. To obtain a continuous measure of the degree of current symptoms, responses to each item can be also summed (Summary Scores). Mean Symptom Counts and Summary Scores are presented in Table 1.
Table 1. Mean Age, WASI T-Scores, and Scores on the Barkley and Murphy’s (2006) Current Symptoms Rating Scale Symptom Count and Summary Score by Group (SE).
Current symptoms | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Symptom count | Summary Score | IQ | ||||||
Age | Inattentive | Hyperactive-impulsive | Inattentive | Hyperactive-impulsive | Matrix reasoning | Vocabulary | Total | |
ADHD | 34.65 (2.67) | 8.11 (0.22) | 6.47 (0.64) | 21.12 (.70) | 18.00 (1.42) | 57.62 (2.62) | 54.38 (2.37) | 112.00 (4.12) |
Control | 32.88 (2.24) | 1.18 (0.31) | 1.06 (0.31) | 6.31 (.86) | 6.06 (.78) | 60.24 (1.52) | 52.41 (1.97) | 112.64 (3.06) |
Control participants were recruited via the University College London volunteer participant pool. Participants were recruited by age, to match the ADHD group. The ADHD and control groups showed similar IQ scores (t < 1 for group differences, see Table 1; IQ scores were unavailable for four of the ADHD group—excluding these participants from analysis produced an identical pattern of results), assessed using the Matrix Reasoning and Vocabulary subtests of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale for Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, 1999). Participants in both groups had normal range IQ and normal or corrected to normal vision. Additional exclusion criteria were any diagnosed learning disability, history of neurological disorder, severe head injury or diagnosed neuropsychological impairment, current diagnosed axis I or axis II DSM–IV psychiatric disorder (other than ADHD for the ADHD group), or current use of psychoactive medication (other than prescribed ADHD medication for the ADHD group). All control participants reported no history of ADHD and reported fewer than the threshold of six symptoms on both the hyperactive-impulsive and inattentive scales of Barkley and Murphy’s Current Symptoms Rating Scale. The data of one control participant were omitted from analysis for having a percentage error rate over three standard deviations greater than the mean among control participants in the high load condition.
Stimuli and Procedure
Participants were tested individually in a quiet room, at viewing distance of approximately 57 cm (maintained using a chinrest) from a 15 in. monitor. Each trial began with a 500-ms presentation of a fixation cross, immediately followed by a 200-ms presentation of six letters arranged to form a circle (see Figure 1 for example stimulus displays). The task was to identify which of two possible target letters (X or N) was present, pressing the 0 key for ‘X’ and the 2 key for ‘N.’ Participants were emphatically instructed to respond as fast as possible while still being accurate, ignoring any stimuli except for the letter-search set.
On “no distractor” trials (90% of trials) the letter circle display appeared alone. On 10% of trials a distractor cartoon image was presented either above or below the letter circle display, remaining onscreen until the end of the trial. Each trial ended either upon response, or (if no response was made) after 2000 ms. A tone sounded for incorrect or missed responses. Participants completed two blocks of 12 practice trials, followed by eight blocks of 60 trials. Load was manipulated between blocks in the order Low-High-Low-High-Low-High-Low-High or High-Low-High-Low-High-Low-High-Low (counterbalanced between participants). The first three trials of each block were treated as warm up trials and never contained a distractor.
Results
Mean RTs and percentage error rates were calculated as a function of group and experimental condition (see Table 2).
Table 2. Mean RTs and Percentage Error Rates (SE in Parentheses) as a Function of Group and Experimental Conditions.
Distractor condition | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Distractor | No distractor | Distractor cost (Distractor−No distractor) | ||||
ADHD | Control | ADHD | Control | ADHD | Control | |
Low load | ||||||
RT (ms) | 681 (36) | 558 (20) | 580 (25) | 515 (16) | 101 (20) | 43 (10) |
% Error | 11.94 | 9.75 | 8.00 | 6.38 | ||
High load | ||||||
RT (ms) | 905 (38) | 794 (28) | 845 (31) | 778 (23) | 60 (15) | 16 (9) |
%Error | 29.82 | 17.31 | 22.29 | 17.56 |
RTs
RTs of less than 100 ms or greater than 1500 ms (<6% responses for the ADHD group, < 2% responses for controls), and incorrect responses, were excluded from RT analysis. Mixed model ANOVA (group × load × distractor condition) revealed a main effect of load, F(1, 31) = 208.64, MSE = 9665.84, p < .001, ηp2 = .871, 95% CI [212.91–281.57] (see Figure 2), confirming that our manipulation of load was effective in increasing processing demands. There was no interaction between load and group, F<1, ηp2 = .001, thus the increase in processing demands was similar for ADHD and control groups. There was also a main effect of distractor presence, F(1, 31) = 42.22, MSE = 2355.03, p < .001, ηp2 = .577, 95% CI [36.38–75.01], reflecting significantly slowed RTs in the presence (vs. absence) of the distractor. Critically, there was a significant interaction between group and distractor condition, F(1, 31) = 9.29, p = .005, ηp2 = .231. As can be seen in Figure 2, the ADHD group showed significantly greater RT interference from the distractors, compared with controls. This indicates that our task is sensitive to detect inflated distraction in ADHD.
Distractor presence also significantly interacted with load, F(1, 31) = 7.94, MSE = 1206.39, p = .008, ηp2 = .204, reflecting that distractor interference was reduced under high load, t(32) = 4.04, SEM = 9.55, p < .001, d = 1.43, 95% CI [19.09–58.00], compared with low load, t(32) = 6.01, SEM = 12.65, p < .001, d = 2.12, 95% CI [47.07–98.62]. Importantly, there was no three-way load × distractor × group interaction, indicating that load was equally effective in decreasing distractor interference for all participants, F < 1, ηp2 = .010.
In keeping with previous findings of slower RTs in ADHD (e.g., Klein et al., 2006) the ADHD group showed slower RTs overall, F(1, 31) = 6.751, MSE = 40635.62, p = .014, ηp2 = .179. However, differences in overall RT did not account for the increased distractor costs. An additional ANOVA on the percentage increase in RT for distractor trials compared with no distractor trials, in high and low load, confirmed significantly greater percentage distractor costs in the ADHD group, F(1, 31) = 7.85, MSE = 105.55, p = .009, ηp2 = .202, 95% CI [1.60–11.46]. It is striking to note that even when baseline RT was controlled for, the distractor costs of the ADHD group were more than twice those seen for control group. Most importantly, however, despite their greatly increased levels of distraction, even when overall RT slowing is accounted for it is clear that the ADHD group were able to benefit from a significant reduction in distraction under high load, t(16) = 3.13, SEM = 3.38, p = .006, d = 1.57, 95% CI [3.412–17.75]. As can be seen in Figure 2, load was just as effective in reducing distraction in adults with ADHD as for controls (for interaction of load by group F(1, 31) = 1.12, p > .29, ηp2 = .035). In fact increasing load compensated for the increased distractor costs of the ADHD participants, to the extent that the high load distractor costs of the ADHD group were of a similar level to those experienced by the control group under low load (t < 1 for the difference, d = 0.20).
Finally, we examined the extent to which specific symptoms of ADHD predicted distractor interference. A multiple regression, conducted on inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive Summary Scores for both ADHD and control participants, revealed that inattentive scores alone significantly predicted overall distractor cost (see Figure 3): For the model r2 = .203, F(1, 31) = 3.83, p = .033 (inattentive b = .709, p = .023; hyperactive-impulsive b = −.367, p = .225).
Percentage Error Rates
Although our use of a RT task, with an emphasis on performance speed, meant that RT was our main measure, the error rates also showed a main effect of distractor presence, F(1, 3) = 4.28, MSE = 54.03, p = .047, ηp2 = .121, 95% CI [.09–5.28], reflecting more errors in the presence of a distractor. A main effect of load on error rates was also found, F(1, 3) = 58.87, MSE = 105.61, p < .001, ηp2 = .655, 95% CI [9.95–17.78]. There was no interaction between load and distractor presence, although the trend mirrored the pattern found on RTs, F(1, 31) = 1.12, MSE = 29.87, p = .30, ηp2 = .023.
A significant main effect of group was found on errors, F(1, 31) = 6.31, MSE = 205.10, p = .017, ηp2 = .169, 95% CI [1.18–11.35], qualified by a load x group interaction, F(1, 31) = 5.93, p = .021, ηp2 = .160, reflecting more errors among ADHD participants compared with controls, especially under high load. There was no interaction between group and distractor presence, or three-way load × distractor presence × group interaction, Fs < 1, ηp2 = .023 and ηp2 = .022, respectively.
Discussion
The present study establishes two key findings: i) Adults with ADHD show more than double the distractor interference compared with controls; the increase in RT in the presence of distractors is more than twice the increase seen in the control group. Furthermore, the difference in distractor costs was retained after controlling for the overall slower RTs seen in the ADHD group. This establishes our task as a sensitive measure of a distraction deficit in adults with ADHD. The magnitude of distractor costs was specifically related to the degree of inattentive symptoms reported, suggesting that our task may provide a valid measure of the ADHD inattentive symptoms related to distractibility. ii) The greater levels of distraction in ADHD can be reduced with higher perceptual load in the task. Indeed, perceptual load was equally effective in reducing distraction for adults with ADHD as it was for controls. This finding points to an intact early selective attention mechanism and suggests that distraction in ADHD results from disruption to later attention mechanisms, such as the efficiency of executive cortical control. As mentioned in the Introduction, Load Theory proposes that high perceptual load can reduce distractor interference simply by filtering distractor stimuli from perception. Efficient cortical executive control is only needed then when this early attentional selection fails. It therefore follows that facilitating early selection with high perceptual load can compensate for late selection/executive control deficits that otherwise lead to increased distraction in ADHD.
The load effects we identified may also account for the “hyper-focusing” presentation that has been observed in the clinical picture of ADHD (e.g., see Schecklmann et al., 2008; Asherson, 2005). Patients with ADHD often describe periods of intensive concentration on certain interesting tasks (e.g., video games, Internet searching, fast competitive sports) associated with diminished perception of the environment. We note that in addition to potentially increased interest, the aforementioned tasks also typically involve high level of perceptual load (i.e., the rapid presentation of multiple moving stimuli). Our findings indicate that under such conditions people with ADHD may be fully engaged in task processing, with reduced perception of other stimuli. Thus, although these two clinical manifestations of distractibility on some tasks and hyper-focusing on other tasks may appear seemingly paradoxical, our study clarifies one factor that may determine which manifestation will occur—namely, perceptual load. Of course, this does not preclude the potential role of other factors such as the rewarding nature of the stimulus and related motivational factors. However, we speculate that the phenomenon of hyper-focus in ADHD might relate specifically to tasks with a high level of load, where distraction is reduced.
Our findings also have potential implications for developing interventions to manage the highly disruptive symptom of distraction. As mentioned above, increased daily life distraction has been directly linked to a number of the negative outcomes, such as failure at school or work, or being accident prone, which are also strongly associated with the inattention symptoms (including distractibility) of ADHD. Our results suggest that by increasing perceptual load, for example in educational material (e.g., presenting periodic tables with a higher visual load), the levels of distraction in individuals with ADHD could be reduced to nonclinical levels; although note that non-ADHD individuals would also be expected to benefit to an equivalent degree from the use of higher visual load materials.
With respect to our first key findings of substantially higher distractor interference effects in adults with ADHD, compared with matched controls, which are correlated with ratings of inattentive symptoms, these suggest our RT interference measure can serve as a distractor assessment tool that provides an objective proxy for less tangible symptoms of inattention, such as failing to give close attention to work, being easily distracted, and engaging in mind-wandering (Forster & Lavie, under revision). Several aspects of our task may have increased its applicability to clinical forms of distraction reported in ADHD. Unlike previous paradigms (which have failed to produce consistent evidence of elevated distraction), our paradigm is designed to measure distraction from the presence of distractors that are entirely irrelevant to the task and can therefore generalize across different tasks and contexts (see Forster & Lavie, 2008b, 2011). Indeed it has been found that although children with ADHD do not show greater interference (vs. controls) from response-incongruent versus neutral pictorial distractors, they do show increased interference from the presence versus absence of any distractor (i.e., including task-irrelevant ones; Brodeur & Pond, 2011).
In addition, our distractors’ potency may have been increased by their high salience, in terms of their visual appearance (e.g., color, size, visual complexity), cultural meaning, and positive emotional associations (e.g., because of our choice of sympathetic protagonist characters rather than villains). The infrequency of distractor presentation in our task may also play a key role. Notably, similar cartoon characters presented as high-frequency response-competition distractors have previously failed to produce inflated distraction in ADHD (Geurts et al., 2008). Future research should identify which of these distractor features are necessary to differentiate ADHD participants and controls.
We note that, although our sample size had sufficient statistical power to detect group differences in the level of distraction, larger scale studies are required to establish our measure as a valid and reliable distractibility test. In addition, future studies should clarify whether our measure predicts informant ratings of inattentive symptoms as closely as self-ratings. Finally, further investigations are still required to clarify the causes of distractibility in ADHD, including the role of both executive (e.g., inhibition) and nonexecutive (e.g., state regulation) factors (Johnson et al., 2009).
In summary, the present study makes three key contributions to the study of ADHD. Our findings introduce the irrelevant distractor paradigm as an objective and sensitive method for assessing distraction in ADHD. This method should prove useful in future research (e.g., assessing distraction in a variety of contexts, evaluating the efficacy of pharmacological and nonpharmacological treatments in reducing distraction in ADHD and so forth). In addition, we clarify that increased distraction in ADHD reflects disruption to only one of two mechanisms underlying the avoidance of distraction. Finally, we demonstrate a potentially powerful method for reducing the heightened levels of distraction associated with ADHD, by increasing perceptual load. This finding, with the somewhat counterintuitive implication that increasing, rather than decreasing, the perceptual difficulty of tasks for people with ADHD may in fact facilitate their performance, provides a promising direction for the development of novel interventions to overcome this disruptive component of the inattention of ADHD.
References
- Asherson P. (2005). Clinical assessment and treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 5, 525–539 doi: 10.1586/14737175.5.4.525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barkley R. A., & Murphy K. R. (2006). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A clinical workbook. New York, NY: Guilford Press [Google Scholar]
- Booth J. E., Carlson C. L., & Tucker D. M. (2007). Performance on a neurocognitive measure of alerting differentiates ADHD combined and inattentive subtypes: A preliminary report. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 423–432 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brodeur D. A., & Pond M. (2001). The development of selective attention in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 229–239 doi: 10.1023/A:1010381731658 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bush G., Valera E. M., & Seidman L. J. (2005). Functional neuroimaging of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A review and suggested future directions. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1273–1284 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan E., Mattingley J. B., Huang-Pollock C., English T., Hester R., Vance A., & Bellgrove M. A. (2009). Abnormal spatial asymmetry of selective attention in ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 1064–1072 doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02096.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang W. P., Davie P. L., & Gavin W. J. (2009). Error monitoring in college students with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of Psychophysiology, 23, 113–125 doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.23.3.113 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Crone E. A., Jennings J. R., & van der Molen M. (2003). Sensitivity to interference and response contingency in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 214–226 doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faraone S. V., Biederman J., Spencer T., Wilens T., Seidman L. J., Mick E., & Doyle A. E. (2000). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults: An overview. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 9–20 doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00889-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forster S., & Lavie N. (2008a). Attentional capture by entirely irrelevant distractors. Visual Cognition, 16, 200–214 doi: 10.1080/13506280701465049 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Forster S., & Lavie N. (2008b). Failures to ignore entirely irrelevant distractors: The role of load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 73–83 doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.14.1.73 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forster S., & Lavie N. (2011). Entirely irrelevant distractors can capture and captivate attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1064–1070 doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0172-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forster S., & Lavie N. (2013). Distracted by your mind? Individual differences in distractibility predict mind-wandering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. doi: 10.1037/a0034108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geurts H. M., van Meel C. S., & Luman M. (2008). What’s in a game: The effect of social motivation on interference control in boys with ADHD and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 848–857 doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01916.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halperin J. M., & Schulz K. P. (2006). Revisiting the role of the prefrontal cortex in the pathophysiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 560–581 doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.4.560 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrmann M. J., Mader K., Schreppel T., Jacob C. P., Heine M., Boreatti-Hümmer A., et al. Fallgatter A. J. (2010). Neural correlates of performance monitoring in adult patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 11, 457–464 doi: 10.3109/15622970902977552 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang-Pollock C. L., Carr T. H., & Nigg J. T. (2002). Development of selective attention: Perceptual load influences early versus late attentional selection in children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 38, 363–375 doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.3.363 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang-Pollock C. L., Nigg J. T., & Carr T. H. (2005). Deficient attention is hard to find: Applying the perceptual load model of selective attention to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder subtypes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 1211–1218 doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.00410.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson K. A., Wiersema J. R., & Kuntsi J. (2009). What would Karl Popper say? Are current psychological theories of ADHD falsifiable? Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5, 15 doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-5-15 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jonkman L. M., Kemner C., Verbaten M. N., van Engeland H., Kenemans J. L., Camferman G., et al. Koelega H. S. (1999). Perceptual and response interference in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and the effects of methylphenidate. Psychophysiology, 36, 419–429 doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3640419 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein C., Wendling K., Huettner P., Ruder H., & Peper M. (2006). Intra-subject variability in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 1088–1097 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konrad K., Neufang S., Hanisch C., Fink G. R., & Herpertz-Dahlmann B. (2006). Dysfunctional attentional networks in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Evidence from an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 643–651 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.08.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landau S., Lorch E. P., & Milich R. (1992). Visual attention to and comprehension of television in attention-deficit hyperactivity disordered and normal boys. Child Development, 63, 928–937 doi: 10.2307/1131244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavie N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 451–468 doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.21.3.451 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavie N. (2005). Distracted and confused? Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 75–82 doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lavie N., Hirst A., de Fockert J. W., & Viding E. (2004). Load Theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339–354 doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence V., Houghton S., Tannock R., Douglas G., Durkin K., & Whiting K. (2002). ADHD outside the laboratory: Boys’ executive function performance on tasks in videogame play and on a visit to the zoo. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 447–462 doi: 10.1023/A:1019812829706 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLoughlin G., Albrecht B., Banaschewski T., Rothenberger A., Brandeis D., Asherson P., & Kuntsi J. (2009). Performance monitoring is altered in adult ADHD: A familial event-related potential investigation. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3134–3142 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.07.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schecklmann M., Ehlis A. C., Plichta M. M., Romanos J., Heine M., Boreatti-Hummer A., et al. Fallgatter A. J. (2008). Diminished prefrontal oxygenation with normal and above-average verbal fluency performance in adult ADHD. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43, 98–106 doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaidya C. J., Bunge S. A., Dudukovic N. M., Zalecki C. A., Elliott G. R., & Gabrieli J. D. (2005). Altered neural substrates of cognitive control in childhood ADHD. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 1605–1613 doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.9.1605 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment [Google Scholar]
- Wilens T. E., Biederman J., & Spencer T. J. (2002). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Annual Review of Medicine, 53, 113–131 doi: 10.1146/annurev.med.53.082901.103945 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilens T. E., Faraone S. V., & Biederman J. (2004). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 292, 619–623 doi: 10.1001/jama.292.5.619 [DOI] [Google Scholar]