Significance
Neurons exhibit distinct homeostatic and Hebbian forms of plasticity that stabilize firing rate and adaptively alter synaptic input patterns, respectively. How these mechanisms are recruited in response to sensory experience in vivo is unclear. We studied plasticity in rodent somatosensory cortex, where the neural map of the whiskers adaptively changes to reflect sensory statistics but only after a prominent initial delay. By recording synaptic excitation and inhibition in vivo, we found that the delay reflects not a lack of plasticity but a rapid homeostasis, in which inhibition is powerfully reduced to offset the initial Hebbian reduction of whisker-evoked excitation. Thus, rapid homeostasis by disinhibition stabilizes the whisker map and precedes classical whisker map plasticity.
Keywords: conductance
Abstract
How homeostatic processes contribute to map plasticity and stability in sensory cortex is not well-understood. Classically, sensory deprivation first drives rapid Hebbian weakening of spiking responses to deprived inputs, which is followed days later by a slow homeostatic increase in spiking responses mediated by excitatory synaptic scaling. Recently, more rapid homeostasis by inhibitory circuit plasticity has been discovered in visual cortex, but whether this process occurs in other brain areas is not known. We tested for rapid homeostasis in layer 2/3 (L2/3) of rodent somatosensory cortex, where D-row whisker deprivation drives Hebbian weakening of whisker-evoked spiking responses after an unexplained initial delay, but no homeostasis of deprived whisker responses is known. We hypothesized that the delay reflects rapid homeostasis through disinhibition, which masks the onset of Hebbian weakening of L2/3 excitatory input. We found that deprivation (3 d) transiently increased whisker-evoked spiking responses in L2/3 single units before classical Hebbian weakening (≥5 d), whereas whisker-evoked synaptic input was reduced during both periods. This finding suggests a transient homeostatic increase in L2/3 excitability. In whole-cell recordings from L2/3 neurons in vivo, brief deprivation decreased whisker-evoked inhibition more than excitation and increased the excitation–inhibition ratio. In contrast, synaptic scaling and increased intrinsic excitability were absent. Thus, disinhibition is a rapid homeostatic plasticity mechanism in rodent somatosensory cortex that transiently maintains whisker-evoked spiking in L2/3, despite the onset of Hebbian weakening of excitatory input.
During deprivation-induced sensory map plasticity in cerebral cortex, changes in sensory input trigger both homeostatic plasticity mechanisms that maintain stable cortical firing rates and Hebbian mechanisms, in which inactive inputs lose (and active inputs gain) representation in sensory maps (1). Diverse mechanisms for homeostasis exist, including synaptic scaling (2–4), plasticity of intrinsic excitability (5, 6), and changes in sensory-evoked inhibition and excitation–inhibition (E-I) ratio (7–11). How homeostatic and Hebbian mechanisms interact to control map stability and plasticity remains unclear.
One key unknown is the relative dynamics of homeostatic and Hebbian plasticity. Homeostasis mediated by synaptic scaling is slow, occurring over hours in vitro and days in vivo. This process is evident in visual cortex, where eyelid closure during the critical period classically drives Hebbian weakening of closed eye spiking responses (after 2 d of deprivation) followed several days later by a slower homeostatic increase in visual responses (12, 13) mediated by excitatory synaptic scaling (3, 14, 15). We investigated whether more rapid forms of homeostasis also exist that shape the earliest stages of cortical plasticity. Recent results in visual cortex show that eyelid closure rapidly weakens inhibitory circuits (within 1 d), and this process increases network excitability and, therefore, is an initial homeostatic response to deprivation (10, 16). This disinhibition correlates with rapid structural plasticity in inhibitory axons and dendrites (17) and is mediated by a reduction in excitatory drive to parvalbumin-positive interneurons (10). Whether rapid homeostasis by disinhibition or other mechanisms is a general feature of cortical plasticity outside the visual cortex is unknown. Theoretical work shows that rapid homeostasis by inhibitory and/or intrinsic plasticity can guide development of realistic sensory tuning and sparse sensory coding in cortical networks, suggesting broad relevance (18).
We tested for rapid homeostasis during the onset of whisker map plasticity in the rodent primary somatosensory (S1) cortex, a major model of cortical plasticity. Each cortical column in the S1 whisker map corresponds to one facial whisker, termed its principal whisker (PW). Trimming or plucking a subset of whiskers in young adults weakens spiking responses to deprived PWs in layer 2/3 (L2/3) of deprived columns (19, 20). This process is mediated by Hebbian synaptic weakening at L4–L2/3 and L2/3–L2/3 excitatory synapses (21–23). No homeostatic restoration or strengthening of deprived whisker responses is known. However, PW response weakening is often preceded by an unexplained initial delay of ∼7 d, in which deprived whisker-evoked spiking responses remain stable (24, 25). We hypothesized that this initial delay reflects not a lack of plasticity but rapid homeostasis that (i) masks initial Hebbian weakening of L2/3 excitatory input and (ii) is mediated by loss of inhibition and/or increased intrinsic excitability in L2/3 neurons. Such rapid homeostasis would be a unique component of whisker map plasticity.
Results
Early Homeostatic Phase of Plasticity Precedes Classical Hebbian Depression.
To detect a rapid homeostatic phase of plasticity, we measured whisker-evoked extracellular spiking in urethane-anesthetized rats following increasing durations of D-row whisker deprivation and in age-matched whisker-intact controls (Fig. 1). D-row deprivation was begun at postnatal day (P)21 ± 1 (mean ± SD). In control rats, L2/3 spiking responses to PW deflection increased from P22 (the first day of recording) to P31, reflecting circuit maturation. In deprived rats, 3 d of deprivation caused a previously unknown 46% increase in deprived PW responses in L2/3 of D columns of deprived rats (mean age = P24) versus age-matched controls [control (CTL): 0.77 ± 0.09 spikes/stimulus, n = 42 units; deprived (DEP): 1.11 ± 0.12 spikes/stimulus, n = 30 units; P < 0.02, two-tailed t test]. This increase occurred specifically in long latency spikes (25–77 ms) (Fig. S1 A and B), and spontaneous firing rate was unaffected (CTL: 1.84 ± 0.26 spikes/s; DEP: 1.66 ± 0.21 spikes/s; P = 0.59), suggesting a possible reduction in PW-evoked inhibition. The increased spiking was replaced by classical Hebbian depression at 5–10 d of deprivation (5 d CTL: 1.26 ± 0.17 spikes/stimulus, n = 23; DEP: 0.48 ± 0.10 spikes/stimulus, n = 25; P < 0.01; 10 d CTL: 1.02 ± 0.14 spikes/stimulus, n = 24; DEP: 0.55 ± 0.08 spikes/stimulus, n = 30; P < 0.01) (Fig. 1 A–D). All recording sites were matched in columnar location and depth, and the greatest response enhancement at 3 d was observed in middle and deep L2/3 (Fig. S1). L4 responses were unaffected (3 d CTL: 1.14 ± 0.13 spikes/stimulus, n = 21; DEP: 1.32 ± 0.16 spikes/stimulus, n = 23; P = 0.38) (Fig. 1 E and F). Thus, deprivation drives an apparent compensatory enhancement of L2/3 spiking responses to the deprived PW before classical Hebbian weakening. At 3 d, responses to spared surround whiskers were reduced rather than enhanced, indicating whisker-specific rather than global homeostatic plasticity (Fig. 1G).
To confirm that Hebbian response weakening is absent at 3 d of deprivation, we recorded whisker responses at 3 vs. 7–10 d using intrinsic signal optical imaging (ISOI), which is an indirect hemodynamic measure of L2/3 activity (20, 26). ISOI avoids potential single-unit selection bias but has relatively low sensitivity for small changes in neural activity. D whisker-evoked peak signal intensity and cortical response area were unaltered by 3 d of deprivation vs. age-matched controls (DEP: n = 21 rats; CTL: n = 32; P = 0.57, t test) but reduced after 7–10 d of deprivation (DEP: n = 23; CTL: n = 34; P < 0.0003) (Fig. 2 A–C). The ratio of D1 response area to the mean of E1 and C1 responses areas within each animal (which controls for cross-animal differences in overall responsiveness) was not altered at 3 d but sharply reduced at 7–10 d (Fig. S2). Together with the single-unit data, this finding indicates that cortical responses are generally preserved at 3 d and decrease at 7–10 d of deprivation, as observed in adult rats and mice (24). ISOI did not detect the increase in late spiking at 3 d of deprivation, reflecting either low sensitivity or offsetting effects of the subthreshold contribution to the intrinsic signal.
Reduced Whisker-Evoked Local Field Potential During the Early Homeostatic Phase.
To probe the basis for response enhancement at 3 d, we measured whisker-evoked local field potentials (wLFPs) in L2/3, which primarily reflect sensory-evoked subthreshold synaptic input (27). Current source density analysis from a 16-site linear probe confirmed that the wLFP in mid-L2/3 (400-μm depth) reflects a discrete L2/3 current sink (Fig. S3A). We compared L2/3 wLFPs between deprived D and spared B columns in D row-deprived rats. After 3 d of deprivation, wLFP amplitude was 43 ± 3% lower in D than B columns (D: −369 ± 45 μV; B: −602 ± 63 μV, n = 9 penetrations each; P < 0.01, two-tailed t test) (Fig. 2 D and E). This reduction occurred between 1 and 3 d of deprivation, and D-column wLFPs remained stably smaller after 5 and 10 d of deprivation (5 d D: −357 ± 58, n = 9; B: −533 ± 29 μV, n = 6; P < 0.04; 10 d D: −437 ± 43, n = 8; B: −620 ± 51, n = 7; P < 0.02). The same pattern was observed for wLFP initial slope (Fig. S2) and wLFP ratio (D/B column) computed in single animals (Fig. 2F). In contrast, L4 wLFPs were not significantly affected (Fig. 2F). These findings suggest that 3 d of deprivation weakens PW-evoked synaptic drive to L2/3, even as PW-evoked spiking is increased, suggestive of a homeostatic increase in L2/3 excitability.
PW-Evoked Excitation and Inhibition.
To directly test how subthreshold synaptic input is altered by 3 d of deprivation, we measured whisker-evoked synaptic currents in single L2/3 neurons using blind whole-cell voltage clamp recordings in vivo (28). Recordings were targeted by intrinsic signal optical imaging to deprived D1 or D2 columns in deprived animals and control D1, D2, E1, C1, and δ-columns in whisker-intact littermates. Recordings were made in L2/3 at 396 ± 10 μm (range = 273–590 μm) subpial depth, with Cs gluconate, N-(2,6-dimethylphenylcarbamoylmethyl)triethylammonium (QX-314) bromide, and 1,2-bis(o-aminophenoxy)ethane- N,N,N′,N′-tetraacetic acid (BAPTA) to promote voltage clamp. We presume that most cells are pyramidal neurons, which are 90% of L2/3 neurons in this depth range (29). For each cell, we determined the PW and measured PW-evoked postsynaptic currents (wPSCs) at multiple holding potentials (−90 to + 30 mV) using a standardized whisker stimulus (2° up–down deflection, 550°/s) (Fig. S4). Inclusion criteria included linear current–voltage (I–V) relationship (Fig. S4) and initial resting membrane potential (Vrest) of <−60 mV. We estimated whisker-evoked total synaptic conductance (Gtotal), inhibitory synaptic conductance (Gin), and excitatory synaptic conductance (Gex) based on the parallel conductance equation using standard methods (30, 31). Conductance waveforms were calculated from 0 to 77 ms poststimulus, where I–V relations were most consistently linear. Because of incomplete dendritic voltage clamp (32), these measurements reflect apparent synaptic conductance at the soma. We showed previously that this method (used in S1 slices) detects the majority of L4-evoked excitation and somatic inhibition in L2/3 pyramidal cells (33).
We first compared 3-d deprived cells with age-matched controls (Fig. 3). In control cells (n = 19; P24 ± 1), PW-evoked wPSCs and apparent synaptic conductances were small (median peak Gtotal = 0.92 nS; median peak Gex = 0.67 nS; median peak Gin = 0.42 nS), paralleling the weak spiking responses in L2/3 at this age (34). Gex waveforms often showed slow dynamics, which may reflect a predominance of recurrent excitation or NMDA receptor currents. Three days of deprivation (n = 19 cells; P24 ± 1) reduced PW-evoked Gtotal, Gex, and Gin relative to control animals (Fig. 3 and Table 1). This reduction was true for both integrated conductance (0–77 ms; reduced 51.5%, 55.0%, and 81.4% below control for Gtotal, Gex, and Gin, respectively) and peak conductance (60.8%, 53.7%, and 71.4% for Gtotal, Gex, and Gin, respectively). Thus, 3 d of deprivation depressed both whisker-evoked excitation and whisker-evoked inhibition in L2/3 neurons. Recording depth, input resistance (Rinput), and series resistance (Rseries) did not differ between control and deprived cells (Table S1), but Vrest was slightly elevated in deprived cells (see below).
Table 1.
Condition | Cells | Measurement | Gtotal (nS) | Gex (nS) | Gin (nS) |
CTL P23–P25 | 19 | Peak (nS) | 0.92 (0.72, 1.6) | 0.67 (0.45, 1.0) | 0.42 (0.30, 0.88) |
Integral (nS × ms) | 35.2 (29.2, 67.9) | 25.3 (18.1, 34.4) | 16.1 (6.6, 32.7) | ||
DEP 3 d (P23–P25) | 19 | Peak (nS) | 0.36 (0.14, 1.1)* | 0.31 (0.20, 0.89)† | 0.12 (0.05, 0.43)* |
Integral (nS × ms) | 7.1 (5.0, 51)* | 11.4 (7.0, 34)† | 3.0 (0.92, 13.4)* | ||
CTL P27–P32 | 12 | Peak (nS) | 1.3 (0.88, 4.6) | 0.73 (0.46, 2.1) | 1.0 (0.44, 3.0) |
Integral (nS × ms) | 60.4 (36.9, 200) | 25.4 (20.3, 70.4) | 37.8 (13.7, 138) | ||
DEP 7–10 d (P27–P32) | 18 | Peak (nS) | 0.73 (0.22, 1.9)† | 0.50 (0.17, 1.3)‡ | 0.36 (0.07, 0.82)* |
Integral (nS × ms) | 28.9 (7.0, 83.5)† | 16.1 (5.7, 51.2)‡ | 8.3 (0.91, 29.7)* |
Significantly different from CTL: P < 0.01.
Significantly different from CTL: P < 0.05.
Trend: P < 0.07 (Wilcoxon rank test).
To examine effects on E-I ratio, we plotted peak Gex vs. peak Gin for each cell and observed an increase in excitation relative to inhibition after deprivation (Fig. 4A). To quantify this effect, we calculated Gex fraction [Gex/(Gex + Gin)] for each individual neuron (Fig. 4B). Gex fraction calculated from integrated conductance was significantly increased after 3 d of deprivation relative to age-matched controls [median (25th, 75th percentile); CTL: 0.56 (0.42, 0.81); DEP: 0.85 (0.65, 0.94); P = 0.015, Wilcoxon rank test]. Gex fraction calculated from peak conductance also increased [CTL: 0.57 (0.43, 0.67); DEP: 0.77 (0.61, 0.88); P = 0.009]. When Gex fraction was calculated millisecond by millisecond during the response, a significant increase was found from 25 to 77 ms poststimulus (the same late epoch when whisker-evoked spikes were increased) (Fig. 4C). Thus, deprivation rapidly reduced whisker-evoked Gex in deprived columns but reduced Gin even more strongly, leading to an increase in E-I ratio. This effect, which co-occurred with increased whisker-evoked spiking, indicates rapid disinhibition, which would function to homeostatically increase network excitability after loss of PW-evoked sensory drive (9).
To better characterize the deprivation effect, we separately analyzed early (0–25 ms) and late (25–77 ms) epochs in the PW response (Fig. S5). In the early epoch, deprivation reduced integrated Gex and Gin similarly (median conductance value reduced to 0.26 and 0.09 of control, respectively; P < 0.01), and Gex fraction was unchanged [CTL: 0.48 (0.37, 0.56); DEP: 0.55 (0.34, 0.81); P = 0.16]. In the late epoch, deprivation reduced Gex modestly (0.60 of control; P < 0.05) and Gin substantially (0.21 of control; P < 0.01), and Gex fraction was increased [CTL: 0.58 (0.45, 0.83); DEP: 0.86 (0.68, 0.97); P = 0.014]. Thus, both excitation and inhibition were reduced throughout the response, with a nonsignificant trend for greater reduction of early vs. late Gex (P = 0.10). The early epoch showed parallel reduction of Gex and Gin, which was associated with no change in PW-evoked spiking, whereas the late epoch showed a preferential reduction in Gin, which was associated with increased PW-evoked spiking.
Disinhibition Persists After 7 d of Deprivation.
Disinhibition could occur transiently during the early homeostatic phase of plasticity or persist during the later Hebbian weakening phase of plasticity. To distinguish these possibilities, we measured whisker-evoked synaptic conductance after 7–10 d of deprivation vs. in age-matched whisker-intact control rats (P27–P32) (Fig. S6). In control animals (n = 12 cells), Gtotal, Gex, and Gin were slightly but not significantly greater than at P24 ± 1 (P > 0.07) (Table 1). In rats with 7–10 d of deprivation (n = 18 cells), PW-evoked Gtotal and Gin were reduced (integral: 52.2% and 78.0% less than age-matched controls; peak: 43.8% and 64%; P < 0.05), and there was a trend for reduction of Gex (integral: 36.6% less then control; peak: 31.5%; P < 0.07) (Table 1). The effect on Gex fraction was similar to 3 d of deprivation but only significant at trend level (Gex from integrated conductance: P = 0.052; from peak: P = 0.073) (Fig. S6D). Gex fraction calculated millisecond by millisecond during the whisker response showed significant disinhibition late in the response, again similar to 3 d of deprivation (Fig. S6E). Recording depth, Rinput, and Rseries were not different between deprived and control cells (Table S1). Vrest was unchanged after 7–10 d of deprivation (CTL: −71.2 ± 0.7 mV, n = 24 cells; DEP: −70.4 ± 1.2 mV, n = 18 cells; P = 0.21, Wilcoxon rank test). Thus, disinhibition was largely sustained during the later Hebbian phase of plasticity, and both early homeostatic and later Hebbian phases of plasticity involved weakening of both excitation and inhibition. Although the relative latency of Gex and Gin strongly impacts sensory-evoked spiking (35), deprivation did not alter the relative timing of Gex vs. Gin onset (mean Gex − Gin latency difference, CTL: 1.9 ± 0.8 ms; DEP: 2.3 ± 1.7 ms for 3 and 7–10 d data combined).
Synaptic Scaling and Intrinsic Excitability.
To determine whether rapid homeostasis also involves synaptic scaling (2) or increased intrinsic excitability in L2/3 pyramidal cells (8), we made whole-cell recordings from visually identified L2/3 pyramidal cells in ex vivo S1 brain slices prepared from D row-deprived rats (onset: P19–P21, duration: 3 d) and age-matched, sham-deprived littermates. We targeted recordings to visualized D columns (21, 36). To examine synaptic scaling, AMPA receptor-mediated miniature excitatory postsynaptic currents (mEPSCs; 200–300 per cell) were isolated using TTX (0.5 µM), picrotoxin (100 µM), and D-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid (50 µM) and recorded at 30 °C at −90 mV. mEPSC amplitude and frequency distributions were identical between D columns in deprived (n = 8 cells) and sham (n = 10 cells) slices (P = 0.28 and P = 0.92, respectively), and mEPSC kinetics were also identical (Fig. 5A). Example mEPSCs are shown in Fig. S7. Identical results were obtained in additional cells at 22–25 °C (n = 9 deprived and 10 sham cells) (Fig. S7). Thus, synaptic scaling was not detectable with brief deprivation. NMDA current kinetics were also unchanged (Fig. S7).
Three-day deprivation depolarized Vrest for both L2/3 neurons in vivo (CTL: −70.1 ± 0.9 mV, n = 35 cells; DEP: −67 ± 1.1 mV, n = 26 cells; P = 0.016, Wilcoxon rank test) and L2/3 pyramidal cells in D columns in brain slices, which were measured either with synaptic transmission intact (SHAM: −74.0 ± 0.9 mV, n = 19 cells; DEP: −70.7 ± 1.0 mV, n = 22 cells; P < 0.02) or in the presence of synaptic blockers of fast GABAergic and glutamatergic transmission (picrotoxin, 100 µM; 2,3-dioxo-6-nitro-1,2,3,4-tetrahydr\x{200b}obenzo[f]quinoxaline-7-sulfonamide, 10 µM; D-APV, 50 µM; SHAM: −72.4 ± 0.68 mV, n = 16 cells; DEP: −69.4 ± 1.1 mV, n = 22 cells; P = 0.04) (Fig. 5B). Deprivation did not alter input resistance (this measurement and all subsequent measurements in synaptic blockers: SHAM: 74.8 ± 4.7 MΩ, n = 16 cells; DEP: 75.2 ± 5.2 MΩ, n = 22 cells; P = 0.95, t test) or Ih as measured by Vm sag ratio (P = 0.92) (Fig. S7).
Despite elevated Vrest, the somatic current required to elicit a single spike (rheobase) was unchanged after deprivation (SHAM: 251.2 ± 14 pA, n = 13 cells; DEP: 256.9 ± 14 pA, n = 12 cells; P = 0.77). Above rheobase, however, the relationship between firing rate and injected current was depressed in deprived D columns relative to sham D columns (ANOVA P = 0.004) (Fig. 5 C and D). Reduced intrinsic excitability has also been observed previously after whisker deprivation (37) and peripheral denervation (38). The reduced firing rate was associated with increased spike width (ANOVA P = 0.012) but no change in spike threshold (Fig. S7) or amplitude of afterhyperpolarization after each spike in the train (P = 0.7).
Thus, brief deprivation did not induce synaptic scaling but did induce three rapid effects in L2/3 of deprived columns: a substantial reduction in whisker-evoked inhibition, a smaller reduction in excitation, and a modest reduction in intrinsic spiking excitability.
Discussion
Prior studies reported an unexplained delay (up to 7 d) between the onset of whisker row deprivation and the Hebbian loss of spiking responses to deflection of deprived whiskers (20, 24). Our results show that this delay represents not an absence of plasticity but a period of active homeostasis, in which network excitability is rapidly increased in L2/3 of deprived S1 columns to compensate for the Hebbian loss of whisker-evoked excitation. This homeostasis is substantial enough to maintain (and even slightly increase) whisker-evoked L2/3 firing rate and maintain normal spontaneous activity, despite a significant concurrent reduction in synaptic excitation in L2/3. Thus, rapid homeostasis maintains constant cortical activity for several days after altered sensory use, as proposed theoretically (1).
Brief (3-d) deprivation weakened whisker-evoked inhibition more than excitation, thus increasing E-I ratio in L2/3 neurons. Thus, disinhibition is a major component of rapid homeostasis in S1. Disinhibition and other inhibitory circuit modifications are well-known after sustained deprivation in visual cortex (7, 8, 11, 39–41) and S1 (33, 42–44). Rapid disinhibition (i.e., before the onset of classical Hebbian response plasticity) is proposed as an early step in lesion-induced adult plasticity in S1 (45) and tone-shock conditioning in auditory cortex (46), and it was recently observed in visual cortex after visual deprivation (10, 16). Our results show that this same phenomenon acts to maintain stable spiking in S1. Disinhibition was previously observed in S1 in cortical columns corresponding to spared whiskers, where it promotes subsequent steps in whisker map plasticity (9), but the current results demonstrate disinhibition in deprived columns, where it mediates sensory response homeostasis. The rapid functional reduction in sensory-evoked inhibition is consistent with rapid pruning of GABAergic neuron dendrites and axons after sensory deprivation or a delay in GABAergic synapse maturation (17, 47, 48).
A likely circuit locus for disinhibition in L2/3 of deprived S1 columns is within the local L2/3 recurrent network, where sustained deprivation (>5 d) is known to weaken recurrent inhibition more than excitation (43). In contrast, feedforward L4–L2/3 excitation and inhibition are weakened equally, on average, by sustained deprivation (33). Preferential disinhibition in the recurrent network would explain the current finding that whisker-evoked Gex fraction and spiking are selectively increased at long poststimulus latencies (Figs. S1 and S6). In visual cortex, rapid disinhibition is mediated by reduced excitation onto L2/3 parvalbumin-positive interneurons (10, 16), which may also be the case in S1 (33).
Brief deprivation modestly decreased intrinsic excitability in L2/3 pyramidal cells, despite elevating Vrest. These effects were small (5–10% change relative to controls) relative to the reduction in whisker-evoked inhibition (∼70–80% reduced from controls), suggesting that disinhibition is the dominant effect. Homeostatic synaptic scaling was absent at 3 d of deprivation, consistent with the relatively slow dynamics of scaling observed in V1 (4, 49). How the initial homeostatic increase in whisker-evoked spiking responses after deprivation is transformed to subsequent Hebbian depression is unclear, because Gex and Gin were reduced similarly after 3 vs. 7–10 d of deprivation (Fig. S6). Possibilities include additional reduction of intrinsic excitability at 7–10 d and changes in distal excitatory or inhibitory inputs that could not be detected in our whole-cell recordings in vivo.
We note several functional advantages of disinhibition as a mechanism of network homeostasis. First, disinhibition will achieve higher network firing rates with less total synaptic current than global homeostatic strengthening of excitatory synapses and thus, may be more efficient metabolically. Second, because inhibitory neurons innervate nearly all local pyramidal cells (50, 51), decreased excitatory drive to a few interneurons may regulate an entire local network uniformly. Third, disinhibition powerfully promotes dendritic depolarization and spiking and therefore, may gate subsequent Hebbian synaptic potentiation during map plasticity (9, 10). Indeed, this gating of plasticity, rather than acute restoration of sensory representation, may be the primary role of disinhibition (52). Fourth, in cases where specific interneurons are tuned to specific sensory features (53), feature-specific homeostasis could be implemented by disinhibition within the specific interneuron class. Consistent with this idea, 3 d of deprivation homeostatically increased spiking responses to deprived PWs but reduced responses to spared surround whiskers (Fig. 1G). This finding is consistent with prior results (24) and not consistent with global homeostasis mediated by synaptic scaling (2, 13).
Materials and Methods
Long–Evans rats (P19–P36) were used. Procedures were approved by the University of California at Berkeley Animal Care and Use Committee and meet National Institutes of Health guidelines. Whiskers (D1–D6 and γ) were deprived unilaterally by trimming, and control animals were sham-trimmed littermates. In vivo recording was performed under urethane anesthesia. Standardized whisker deflections were applied 3 mm from the face in both control and deprived rats. Single-unit LFP and whole-cell recordings were targeted by ISOI to the center of a D1 whisker functional column. Whole-cell recording in vivo used internal with cesium, QX-314, and BAPTA, and Rseries was not corrected. Whisker-evoked excitatory and inhibitory synaptic conductances were estimated from wPSCs at five holding potentials using standard methods (31, 54). Slice physiology was performed in across-row S1 slices that allow visualization of A–E whisker columns (21, 36). Recordings were made from visually identified L2/3 pyramidal neurons in D-barrel columns. Rseries was compensated for by voltage clamp experiments. Numbers are mean ± SE or median (25th, 75th percentile). Conductance values were nonnormally distributed and compared by nonparametric tests. Detailed methods are in SI Materials and Methods.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Brooke Angel for analysis, and M. R. DeWeese and laboratory members for comments. This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants R01NS073912 and R01NS046652-07A1.
Footnotes
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1312455111/-/DCSupplemental.
References
- 1.Turrigiano GG, Nelson SB. Homeostatic plasticity in the developing nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(2):97–107. doi: 10.1038/nrn1327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Turrigiano GG, Leslie KR, Desai NS, Rutherford LC, Nelson SB. Activity-dependent scaling of quantal amplitude in neocortical neurons. Nature. 1998;391(6670):892–896. doi: 10.1038/36103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kaneko M, Stellwagen D, Malenka RC, Stryker MP. Tumor necrosis factor-alpha mediates one component of competitive, experience-dependent plasticity in developing visual cortex. Neuron. 2008;58(5):673–680. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Desai NS, Cudmore RH, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG. Critical periods for experience-dependent synaptic scaling in visual cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5(8):783–789. doi: 10.1038/nn878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Aizenman CD, Akerman CJ, Jensen KR, Cline HT. Visually driven regulation of intrinsic neuronal excitability improves stimulus detection in vivo. Neuron. 2003;39(5):831–842. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00527-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Breton J-D, Stuart GJ. Loss of sensory input increases the intrinsic excitability of layer 5 pyramidal neurons in rat barrel cortex. J Physiol. 2009;587(Pt 21):5107–5119. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.180943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Maffei A, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG. Selective reconfiguration of layer 4 visual cortical circuitry by visual deprivation. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7(12):1353–1359. doi: 10.1038/nn1351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Maffei A, Turrigiano GG. Multiple modes of network homeostasis in visual cortical layer 2/3. J Neurosci. 2008;28(17):4377–4384. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5298-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gambino F, Holtmaat A. Spike-timing-dependent potentiation of sensory surround in the somatosensory cortex is facilitated by deprivation-mediated disinhibition. Neuron. 2012;75(3):490–502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kuhlman SJ, et al. A disinhibitory microcircuit initiates critical-period plasticity in the visual cortex. Nature. 2013;501(7468):543–546. doi: 10.1038/nature12485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ma W-P, Li Y-T, Tao HW. Downregulation of cortical inhibition mediates ocular dominance plasticity during the critical period. J Neurosci. 2013;33(27):11276–11280. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5598-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Frenkel MY, Bear MF. How monocular deprivation shifts ocular dominance in visual cortex of young mice. Neuron. 2004;44(6):917–923. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Mrsic-Flogel TD, et al. Homeostatic regulation of eye-specific responses in visual cortex during ocular dominance plasticity. Neuron. 2007;54(6):961–972. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gao M, et al. A specific requirement of Arc/Arg3.1 for visual experience-induced homeostatic synaptic plasticity in mouse primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2010;30(21):7168–7178. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1067-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Lambo ME, Turrigiano GG. Synaptic and intrinsic homeostatic mechanisms cooperate to increase L2/3 pyramidal neuron excitability during a late phase of critical period plasticity. J Neurosci. 2013;33(20):8810–8819. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4502-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hengen KB, Lambo ME, Van Hooser SD, Katz DB, Turrigiano GG. Firing rate homeostasis in visual cortex of freely behaving rodents. Neuron. 2013;80(2):335–342. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Keck T, et al. Loss of sensory input causes rapid structural changes of inhibitory neurons in adult mouse visual cortex. Neuron. 2011;71(5):869–882. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Zylberberg J, Murphy JT, DeWeese MR. A sparse coding model with synaptically local plasticity and spiking neurons can account for the diverse shapes of V1 simple cell receptive fields. PLoS Comput Biol. 2011;7(10):e1002250. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Glazewski S, Fox K. Time course of experience-dependent synaptic potentiation and depression in barrel cortex of adolescent rats. J Neurophysiol. 1996;75(4):1714–1729. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.4.1714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Drew PJ, Feldman DE. Intrinsic signal imaging of deprivation-induced contraction of whisker representations in rat somatosensory cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2009;19(2):331–348. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Allen CB, Celikel T, Feldman DE. Long-term depression induced by sensory deprivation during cortical map plasticity in vivo. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6(3):291–299. doi: 10.1038/nn1012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Shepherd GMG, Pologruto TA, Svoboda K. Circuit analysis of experience-dependent plasticity in the developing rat barrel cortex. Neuron. 2003;38(2):277–289. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00152-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cheetham CEJ, Hammond MSL, Edwards CEJ, Finnerty GT. Sensory experience alters cortical connectivity and synaptic function site specifically. J Neurosci. 2007;27(13):3456–3465. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5143-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Jacob V, Petreanu L, Wright N, Svoboda K, Fox K. Regular spiking and intrinsic bursting pyramidal cells show orthogonal forms of experience-dependent plasticity in layer V of barrel cortex. Neuron. 2012;73(2):391–404. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Li L, et al. Endocannabinoid signaling is required for development and critical period plasticity of the whisker map in somatosensory cortex. Neuron. 2009;64(4):537–549. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Grinvald A, Lieke E, Frostig RD, Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Functional architecture of cortex revealed by optical imaging of intrinsic signals. Nature. 1986;324(6095):361–364. doi: 10.1038/324361a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Heynen AJ, Bear MF. Long-term potentiation of thalamocortical transmission in the adult visual cortex in vivo. J Neurosci. 2001;21(24):9801–9813. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-24-09801.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Margrie TW, Brecht M, Sakmann B. In vivo, low-resistance, whole-cell recordings from neurons in the anaesthetized and awake mammalian brain. Pflugers Arch. 2002;444(4):491–498. doi: 10.1007/s00424-002-0831-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Meyer HS, et al. Inhibitory interneurons in a cortical column form hot zones of inhibition in layers 2 and 5A. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011;108(40):16807–16812. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113648108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Borg-Graham LJ, Monier C, Frégnac Y. Visual input evokes transient and strong shunting inhibition in visual cortical neurons. Nature. 1998;393(6683):369–373. doi: 10.1038/30735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wehr M, Zador AM. Balanced inhibition underlies tuning and sharpens spike timing in auditory cortex. Nature. 2003;426(6965):442–446. doi: 10.1038/nature02116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Williams SR, Mitchell SJ. Direct measurement of somatic voltage clamp errors in central neurons. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11(7):790–798. doi: 10.1038/nn.2137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.House DRC, Elstrott J, Koh E, Chung J, Feldman DE. Parallel regulation of feedforward inhibition and excitation during whisker map plasticity. Neuron. 2011;72(5):819–831. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Brecht M, Roth A, Sakmann B. Dynamic receptive fields of reconstructed pyramidal cells in layers 3 and 2 of rat somatosensory barrel cortex. J Physiol. 2003;553(Pt 1):243–265. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.044222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Wilent WB, Contreras D. Dynamics of excitation and inhibition underlying stimulus selectivity in rat somatosensory cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(10):1364–1370. doi: 10.1038/nn1545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Finnerty GT, Roberts LS, Connors BW. Sensory experience modifies the short-term dynamics of neocortical synapses. Nature. 1999;400(6742):367–371. doi: 10.1038/22553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Maravall M, Stern EA, Svoboda K. Development of intrinsic properties and excitability of layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons during a critical period for sensory maps in rat barrel cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92(1):144–156. doi: 10.1152/jn.00598.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hickmott PW. Changes in intrinsic properties of pyramidal neurons in adult rat S1 during cortical reorganization. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94(1):501–511. doi: 10.1152/jn.00924.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Yazaki-Sugiyama Y, Kang S, Câteau H, Fukai T, Hensch TK. Bidirectional plasticity in fast-spiking GABA circuits by visual experience. Nature. 2009;462(7270):218–221. doi: 10.1038/nature08485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Gandhi SP, Yanagawa Y, Stryker MP. Delayed plasticity of inhibitory neurons in developing visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105(43):16797–16802. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0806159105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Maffei A, Nataraj K, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG. Potentiation of cortical inhibition by visual deprivation. Nature. 2006;443(7107):81–84. doi: 10.1038/nature05079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Jiao Y, Zhang C, Yanagawa Y, Sun Q-Q. Major effects of sensory experiences on the neocortical inhibitory circuits. J Neurosci. 2006;26(34):8691–8701. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2478-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Shao YR, et al. Plasticity of recurrent l2/3 inhibition and gamma oscillations by whisker experience. Neuron. 2013;80(1):210–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Hickmott PW, Merzenich MM. Local circuit properties underlying cortical reorganization. J Neurophysiol. 2002;88(3):1288–1301. doi: 10.1152/jn.00994.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Tremere L, Hicks TP, Rasmusson DD. Role of inhibition in cortical reorganization of the adult raccoon revealed by microiontophoretic blockade of GABA(A) receptors. J Neurophysiol. 2001;86(1):94–103. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.86.1.94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Letzkus JJ, et al. A disinhibitory microcircuit for associative fear learning in the auditory cortex. Nature. 2011;480(7377):331–335. doi: 10.1038/nature10674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Marik SA, Yamahachi H, McManus JNJ, Szabo G, Gilbert CD. Axonal dynamics of excitatory and inhibitory neurons in somatosensory cortex. PLoS Biol. 2010;8(6):e1000395. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.van Versendaal D, et al. Elimination of inhibitory synapses is a major component of adult ocular dominance plasticity. Neuron. 2012;74(2):374–383. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Goel A, Lee H-K. Persistence of experience-induced homeostatic synaptic plasticity through adulthood in superficial layers of mouse visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2007;27(25):6692–6700. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5038-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Fino E, Yuste R. Dense inhibitory connectivity in neocortex. Neuron. 2011;69(6):1188–1203. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Packer AM, Yuste R. Dense, unspecific connectivity of neocortical parvalbumin-positive interneurons: A canonical microcircuit for inhibition? J Neurosci. 2011;31(37):13260–13271. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3131-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Sale A, et al. Environmental enrichment in adulthood promotes amblyopia recovery through a reduction of intracortical inhibition. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10(6):679–681. doi: 10.1038/nn1899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Adesnik H, Bruns W, Taniguchi H, Huang ZJ, Scanziani M. A neural circuit for spatial summation in visual cortex. Nature. 2012;490(7419):226–231. doi: 10.1038/nature11526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Monier C, Fournier J, Frégnac Y. In vitro and in vivo measures of evoked excitatory and inhibitory conductance dynamics in sensory cortices. J Neurosci Methods. 2008;169(2):323–365. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.