Abstract
The purpose of this paper was to examine the relationship of sex and pain-related fear in pain intensity reports to thermal stimuli and whether sex differences in reported pain intensity were mediated by pain-related fear. 177 participants, 124 female (23.5 ± 4.5 years old), filled out a demographic and fear of pain questionnaire (FPQ-III). Experimental pain testing was performed using thermal stimuli applied to the lower extremity. Participants rated the intensity of pain using the numerical pain rating scale (NPRS). Independent t-tests, Sobel's test, and linear regression models were performed to examine the relationships between sex, fear of pain, and pain sensitivity. We found significant sex differences for thermal pain threshold temperatures (t = 2.04, P = 0.04) and suprathreshold pain ratings for 49°C (t = −2.12, P = 0.04) and 51°C (t = −2.36, P = 0.02). FPQ-severe score mediated the effect of suprathreshold pain ratings of 49° (t = 2.00, P = 0.05), 51° (t = 2.07, P = 0.04), and pain threshold temperatures (t = −2.12, P = 0.03). There are differences in the pain sensitivity between sexes, but this difference may be mediated by baseline psychosocial factors such as fear of pain.
1. Introduction
Pain is a prevalent, debilitating condition that has serious health and economic consequences. Approximately 116 million Americans suffer from chronic pain conditions and the costs of pain range from $560 billion to $635 billion annually; this amount is equal to approximately 2,000 dollars for everyone living in the United States [1]. The prevalence of pain in primary care settings is estimated at approximately 30%, with nearly two-thirds of those pain reports attributed to musculoskeletal pain [2].
Evidence suggests men and women experience and report pain differently [3–6]. Clinically this is relevant because research has demonstrated that a greater percentage of chronic pain sufferers are women [4, 7]. Women also report more areas of bodily pain [8, 9] and more pain-related disability compared to men [10]. Given that pain is such a prevalent and debilitating condition with serious health and economic consequences, the Institute of Medicine has stressed the need to improve healthcare delivery of pain management, including individualized treatment approaches [1]. Although an individualized approach to the treatment of pain is recommended, should healthcare providers tailor their treatments based on the sex of the patient or are there other factors that could be important as well?
The etiology of sex differences in pain reports is still not clear. Evidence suggests that different biological [11–14] and psychosocial factors [15–17] may account for these differences. Psychosocial factors influence the perception and evaluation of pain. One commonly studied psychosocial factor is pain-related fear. Pain-related fear includes fear of the sensation of pain, fear of movement or reinjury, and fear of physical activities which are assumed to cause pain [18]. Pain-related fear is believed to contribute to the shift from acute low back pain (LBP) to chronic LBP [19] and numerous studies have demonstrated the association of pain-related fear with disability in patients with chronic [18, 20, 21] and acute [22] LBP, hip and knee osteoarthritis [23, 24], and foot and ankle dysfunction [25]. Camacho-Soto et al. examined the relationship between fear avoidance and disability in older adults and found that higher fear avoidance beliefs were associated with slower gait speeds and higher disability [26]. The results of these studies provide important information about how pain-related fear affects pain perception and functional mobility.
Thus differences in psychosocial factors, such as pain-related fear, across sexes may contribute to differences in the pain reports between men and women. Several lines of research using experimentally induced pain have begun to disentangle these sex differences. For example, an experimental pain study using electrical stimuli found the increased pain experienced by women during a movement task was accounted for by higher reports of fear among women compared to men [27]. In addition to pain-related fear, Robinson et al. [28] have found women to be more willing to report pain and consider themselves to be more sensitive to pain compared to males. Conversely, some males believe that they have higher pain endurance than women and compared to the typical male. Interestingly, after controlling for these gender role differences and anxiety, the previous sex differences in temporal summation, a proxy measure of central sensitization, were attenuated [29]. These findings indicate that sex differences in reported pain may be attributed to underlying differences in psychosocial factors.
This study aimed to investigate the role of pain-related fear and sex on experimentally induced pain using threshold and suprathreshold thermal stimuli. To our knowledge, the influence of pain-related fear as a mediator of sex differences in reported pain intensity to standardized thermal temperatures has not been previously reported. Therefore, the purpose of this paper was to examine sex differences in pain intensity reports to threshold and suprathreshold thermal stimuli, the influence of pain-related fear on pain intensity responses, and whether sex differences in pain intensity reports were mediated by pain-related fear.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
A convenience sample of pain-free participants was pooled from three previously reported studies that tested pain sensitivity and measured pain-related fear [30–32]. Pooling these studies was appropriate for addressing our purposes because the studies had the same eligibility criteria and testing procedures and were all conducted with similar samples (participants were pain-free at the time of testing). All data were collected in the same laboratory and used the same quantitative sensory testing (QST) procedures. Each study had Institutional Review Board approval and all participants provided informed consent before being included in the studies.
Participants provided demographic information and completed a validated psychosocial questionnaire. Participants underwent standardized QST for thermal pain sensitivity at the lower extremity. The QST protocol has been employed in previous studies [30, 31] and is further described below.
2.2. Questionnaire
The fear of pain questionnaire (FPQ-III) uses a 30-item, 5-point rating scale to measure fear about specific situations that would normally produce pain [33]. The FPQ-III is a commonly used and well-validated instrument that is appropriate for use in nonclinical and clinical populations [33–35]. The FPQ-III measures fears about pain as a trait-like phenomenon, assessing enduring behavioral patterns across pain situations [33]. The FPQ-III is a multifactor instrument; it can be used to assess fear in a specific area or to evaluate generalization of fear across domains: fear of severe pain, fear of medical pain, and fear of minor pain [33]. Scoring the FPQ-III subscales involves summing the 10 items that comprise each subscale. Items for each subscale are as follows: severe pain = 1, 3, 5, 6, 9, 10, 13, 18, 25, and 27; minor pain = 2, 4, 7, 12, 19, 22, 23, 24, 28, and 30; and medical pain = 8, 11, 14, 15, 16, 17, 20, 21, 26, and 29. The possible range is 10–50 for each subscale. The total score is the sum of all 30 items. The possible range is 30–150 for the total score [33].
2.3. Pain Intensity
Intensity of thermally evoked pain was rated using a numerical pain rating scale (NPRS) anchored at one end with 0 “no pain” and at the other end with 100 being “worst imaginable.” Subjects verbally rated their pain after each thermal pulse.
2.4. Quantitative Sensory Testing (QST)
All thermal stimuli were delivered to the skin of subjects using a computer-controlled Medoc Neurosensory Analyzer (TSA-2001, Ramat Yishai, Israel).
Before the testing session, each subject underwent a practice session. During this practice session subjects experienced the temperatures to which they were to be exposed. Subjects practiced using the NPRS to rate the intensity of the pain experienced in response to each stimulus. In order to standardize the scaling instructions and to clarify the distinction between the sensory intensity and affective dimensions, a standardized instructional set was used for all subjects during every exposure to the thermal stimuli. The scale instructions were repeated for every set of ratings within each session [36].
2.4.1. Heat Thresholds
After the practice session, heat threshold was measured. The temperature began from a baseline of 32°C. The probe temperature increased at a rate of 0.5°C/s until participants responded that the stimulus was painful. Subjects were then asked to rate that painful sensation using the NPRS. The heat stimuli were applied to the posterior surface of the upper calf below the popliteal fossa, with the subject sitting.
2.4.2. Suprathreshold Pain Ratings
Subjects experienced a sequence of four thermal pulses that included 45, 47, 49, and 51°C presented randomly. Subjects were cued to provide a verbal pain rating of any pain experienced immediately after the peak of each thermal pulse. This procedure was performed twice. The interval between trials was at least 60 seconds to avoid carryover effects from one stimulus to another, to prevent changes in receptor responses and to prevent tissue changes. Temperature levels were monitored by a contactor-contained thermistor and returned to a preset baseline of 35°C by active cooling at a rate of 10°C/sec [37, 38].
2.5. Data Analysis
All statistics were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics Data Editor 20. Descriptive statistics were calculated for demographic variables. Independent samples t-tests were performed to examine the sex differences in FPQ-III scores, threshold temperature, and pain ratings for threshold and suprathreshold pain thermal stimuli. Correlation analyses were performed for FPQ-III questionnaire and pain intensity at threshold and suprathreshold temperatures.
To address whether pain-related fear accounted for sex differences in pain sensitivity measures, we employed classic mediation analyses in accordance with the methods described by Baron and Kenny (1986) [39] and refined by Preacher and Hayes (2004) [40] using the PROCESS macro for SPSS provided by Hayes [41]. Significance of all indirect effects was assessed with Sobel's statistical test. In our mediation model, sex was the independent variable, pain sensitivity measures were the dependent variable, and fear of pain was the mediator (Figure 1). Prerequisite criteria were established prior to testing for mediation. The three criteria were (1) the mediator needed to be correlated with the independent variable, (2) the outcome variable needed to be correlated with the independent variable, and (3) the mediator needed to be correlated with the outcome.
Separate linear regression analyses were performed to examine the role of sex and fear of pain on experimental pain. Suprathreshold pain ratings at 49°C and 51°C, threshold temperature, and pain ratings at threshold temperature were dependent variables in separate models. Sex, FPQ-Severe, and age were entered as predictors in each of the models.
3. Results
177 participants were included in this analysis. 124 of the participants were female and the mean age of the participants was 23.5 ± 4.5 years old. Participant's FPQ scores are presented as a total score and the subcategories of FPQ-Severe pain, FPQ-minor pain, and FPQ-medical pain. There were no significant differences between sexes for total FPQ-III score (t = −1.71, P = 0.08), FPQ-minor pain score (t = −0.18, P = 0.85), or FPQ-medical pain score (t = −1.87, P = 0.06), but there was a significant difference between sexes on FPQ-severe pain scores (t = −2.29, P = 0.02), with females reporting higher fear of severe pain than males (see Figure 2).
We found significant sex differences for thermal pain thresholds temperatures (t = 2.04, P = 0.04) and suprathreshold pain ratings for 49°C (t = −2.12, P = 0.04) and 51°C (t = −2.36, P = 0.02). Females demonstrated lower threshold to thermal stimuli and reported higher pain ratings than males, indicating greater pain sensitivity. However we did not find significant sex differences for pain intensity ratings at 45°C (t = −1.43, P = 0.15), 47°C (t = −1.74, P = 0.08), or rating of threshold pain (t = −1.90, P = 0.06) (see Figure 3).
Correlation analyses revealed that female sex was highly correlated with pain ratings at 49°C (r = 0.16, P ≤ 0.05) and 51°C (r = 0.18, P ≤ 0.05) and FPQ-severe score (r = 0.17, P ≤ 0.05). These correlations indicate higher ratings of thermal stimuli experienced at both 49°C and 51°C and reported fear of pain is correlated with sex. When the correlations of reported fear of pain and pain ratings to thermal stimuli were examined, total FPQ-score and FPQ-severe were correlated with all thermal stimuli pain ratings, with magnitude of the correlation increasing with increasing temperature ratings. FPQ-minor and FPQ-medical were only correlated with suprathreshold ratings at thermal stimuli 49°C and 51°C (see Table 1).
Table 1.
45°C | 47°C | 49°C | 51°C | FPQ-score | FPQ-severe | FPQ-minor | FPQ-medical | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sex-female | 0.11 | 0.13 | 0.16* | 0.18* | 0.13 | 0.17* | 0.01 | 0.14 |
FPQ-score | 0.17* | 0.19* | 0.24** | 0.25** | 1 | 0.91** | 0.87** | 0.90** |
FPQ-severe | 0.19* | 0.25** | 0.28** | 0.30** | 0.91** | 1 | 0.69** | 0.72** |
FPQ-minor | 0.12 | 0.15 | 0.18* | 0.19* | 0.87** | 0.69** | 1 | 0.70** |
FPQ-medical | 0.15 | 0.11 | 0.15* | 0.17* | 0.90** | 0.72** | 0.70** | 1 |
**Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
*Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
Mediation analyses were carried out for the three pain sensitivity measures demonstrating significant sex differences, which were suprathreshold pain intensity ratings of 49° and 51° and thermal pain threshold temperatures. Prerequisite criteria for mediation were established for fear of severe pain (FPQ-severe). We found that FPQ-fear of severe pain mediated the effect of sex for all three pain sensitivity measures; suprathreshold ratings of 49° (Sobel's t = 2.00, SE = 1.24, and P = 0.05), suprathreshold ratings of 51° (Sobel's t = 2.07, SE = 1.35, and P = 0.04), and thermal pain threshold temperatures (Sobel's t = −2.12, SE = 0.15, and P = 0.03) (see Figures 4(a)–4(c)).
Separate linear regression analyses were performed to explain the variance related to thermal pain ratings and thresholds. These analyses revealed that models containing age, sex, and FPQ-severe score accounted for between 4.5 and 12% of the variance in pain ratings at 45, 47, 49, and 51°C. There was a linear relationship between the magnitude of the effect of the models and temperature of thermal stimulus, where magnitude increased with increasing temperature of the thermal stimulus (45°C (F 3, 171 = 2.65, P = 0.51), 47°C (F 3, 171 = 5.18, P = 0.002), 49°C (F 3, 170 = 6.20, P = 0.001), and 51°C (F 3, 172 = 7.673, P < 0.001)). The magnitude of the effect of the models for threshold temperate (F 3, 172 = 4.42, P = 0.005) and pain rating at threshold temperature (F 3, 172 = 4.99, P = 0.002) were similar (see Table 2).
Table 2.
Variables in the model | Beta | t-value | P-value | R 2 | Total variance explained by the model (%) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1: pain rating at 45°C | Sex | 0.09 | 1.15 | 0.25 | 0.010 | 4.5% |
FPQ-severe | 0.18 | 2.35 | 0.02 | 0.030 | ||
Age | 0.06 | 0.80 | 0.43 | 0.003 | ||
| ||||||
Model 2: pain rating at 47°C | Sex | 0.011 | 1.48 | 0.14 | 0.018 | 8.5% |
FPQ-severe | 0.24 | 3.24 | 0.001 | 0.052 | ||
Age | 0.13 | 1.66 | 0.09 | 0.015 | ||
| ||||||
Model 3: pain rating at 49°C | Sex | 0.13 | 1.72 | 0.09 | 0.026 | 10% |
FPQ-severe | 0.27 | 3.62 | <0.001 | 0.067 | ||
Age | 0.89 | 1.19 | 0.24 | 0.007 | ||
| ||||||
Model 4: pain rating at 51°C | Sex | 0.15 | 1.96 | 0.05 | 0.032 | 12% |
FPQ-severe | 0.29 | 3.96 | <0.001 | 0.076 | ||
Age | 0.11 | 1.52 | 0.13 | 0.012 | ||
| ||||||
Model 5: threshold pain rating | Sex | 0.17 | 2.27 | 0.03 | 0.021 | 8.1% |
FPQ-severe | 0.08 | 1.05 | 0.30 | 0.003 | ||
Age | 0.25 | 3.26 | 0.001 | 0.057 | ||
| ||||||
Model 6: threshold temperature | Sex | −0.10 | −1.36 | 0.17 | 0.024 | 7.3% |
FPQ-severe | −0.20 | −2.59 | 0.01 | 0.041 | ||
Age | −0.09 | 1.20 | 0.22 | 0.008 |
When specifically examining the effect of sex on dependent variables, sex was not a unique predictor for pain ratings at 45, 47, 49°C, or threshold temperature but was for pain ratings at 51°C (t = 1.96, P = 0.05) and pain rating at threshold temperature (t = 2.27, P = 0.03). Sex only accounted for a small amount of variance in these models, 3.2% of the variance in pain rating at 51°C and 2.4% of the variance in threshold temperature pain rating.
FPQ-severe score was a unique predictor in pain ratings at 45°C (t = 2.35, P = 0.02), 47°C (t = 3.24, P = 0.001), 49°C (t = 3.62, P < 0.001), 51°C (t = 3.96, P ≤ 0.001), and threshold temperature (t = −2.59, P = 0.01) but not pain rating at threshold temperature (t = −1.05, P = 0.30). FPQ-Severe accounted for between 3 and 7.6% of variance in each of these models. Age was included in models on a theoretical premise and was not found to be a significant predictor in any model except pain rating at threshold (t = 3.26, P < 0.001).
4. Discussion
The purpose of this study was to investigate sex differences in thermal pain sensitivity measures and the extent to which pain-related fear accounted for these differences. This study adds to the current body of literature as we investigated suprathreshold pain ratings at four standard thermal temperatures and supports that sex differences in pain sensitivity may be accounted for by other factors such as fear of pain. We found that females reported higher fear of severe pain than males and that this difference mediated the difference in pain sensitivity where we found sex differences. Past research examining sex and FPQ scores found similar results where females had significantly higher mean scores than males on the FPQ-severe pain scores [35].
In addition to these findings, females reported statistically significant lower heat thresholds with similar pain intensity ratings at the threshold compared to males. Another interesting observation was that there were no significant differences at the two lower temperatures used in our study. This finding could be interpreted as sex differences are magnified as the intensity of perceived pain increases. This finding may be relevant as the only difference in fear of pain we found in this study was in the severe pain subscale of the FPQ-III.
When controlling for FPQ-severe score and age, sex was only significant in predicting pain ratings at the highest temperature (51°C) and threshold pain rating and sex only accounted for a small amount of variance in these models. These findings indicate that although sex plays a role in suprathreshold and threshold pain ratings, the magnitude is rather small after controlling psychosocial factors. Our data suggest that although sex is not a significant predictor in most models, fear of severe pain is a significant predictor. A greater proportion of variance in all models except pain rating at threshold was explained by FPQ-severe score than by sex. Thus, this finding supports our hypothesis that when painful stimuli are more intense (e.g., highest suprathreshold temperatures), fear and fear of severe pain specifically may play a larger role in pain rating than sex.
This paper highlights the role of fear of pain and sex on pain sensitivity. Although a limitation of this paper is that the findings are in a nonclinical population, the principles can be applicable in conjunction with current knowledge to clinical populations of persons with pain conditions. We know that women and men differ in how they process pain in the experimental setting [42, 43] as well as in the clinical setting [44], but there is still a lack of consistent evidence on the contributions of psychosocial variables, including fear of pain, in the pain experience [45–49]. Past research has found that experimental pain responses were related to clinical pain [8] and treatment outcomes [17]. Thus physicians interested in tailoring pain treatments may be better served by assessing intermediate psychosocial factors that influence an individual's pain perception rather than the sex of the patient alone.
5. Conclusion
In summary we found that there are differences in the pain sensitivity between sexes, but this difference may be mediated by baseline psychosocial factors such as fear of pain. Specifically, the fear of severe pain may mediate the effect that sex has on pain intensity ratings in response to thermal stimuli.
Acknowledgments
These data were collected with support of funding from National Institutes of Health (K01AR054331, T32HD043730, and R01AT006334) and The Foundation for Physical Therapy.
Conflict of Interests
The authors have no conflict of interests to report.
References
- 1.Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Advancing Pain Research, C., and Education. Relieving Pain in America: A Blueprint for TransForming Prevention, Care, Education, and Research. 2011. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hasselstrom J, Liu-Palmgren J, Rasjo-Wraak G. Prevalence of pain in general practice. European Journal of Pain. 2002;6(5):375–385. doi: 10.1016/s1090-3801(02)00025-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Fillingim RB, Maixner W. Gender differences in the responses to noxious stimuli. Pain Forum. 1995;4(4):209–221. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Berkley KJ. Sex differences in pain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1997;20(3):371–380. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x97221485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Riley JL, III, Robinson ME, Wise EA, Myers CD, Fillingim RB. Sex differences in the perception of noxious experimental stimuli: a meta-analysis. Pain. 1998;74(2-3):181–187. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(97)00199-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Racine M, Tousignant-Laflamme Y, Kloda LA, Dion D, Dupuis G, Choinire M. A systematic literature review of 10 years of research on sex/gender and experimental pain perception—part 1: are there really differences between women and men? Pain. 2012;153(3):602–618. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.11.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Chang L, Heitkemper MM. Gender differences in irritable bowel syndrome. Gastroenterology. 2002;123(5):1686–1701. doi: 10.1053/gast.2002.36603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Fillingim RB, Edwards RR, Powell T. The relationship of sex and clinical pain to experimental pain responses. Pain. 1999;83(3):419–425. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00128-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Edwards RR, Haythornthwaite JA, Sullivan MJ, Fillingim RB. Catastrophizing as a mediator of sex differences in pain: differential effects for daily pain versus laboratory-induced pain. Pain. 2004;111(3):335–341. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Stubbs D, Krebs E, Bair M, et al. Sex differences in pain and pain-related disability among primary care patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain. Pain Medicine. 2010;11(2):232–239. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2009.00760.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Fillingim RB, Maixner W, Girdler SS, et al. Ischemic but not thermal pain sensitivity varies across the menstrual cycle. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1997;59(5):512–520. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199709000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Riley JL, III, Robinson ME, Wise EA, Price D. A meta-analytic review of pain perception across the menstrual cycle. Pain. 1999;81(3):225–235. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00258-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Teepker M, Peters M, Vedder H, Schepelmann K, Lautenbacher S. Menstrual variation in experimental pain: correlation with gonadal hormones. Neuropsychobiology. 2010;61(3):131–140. doi: 10.1159/000279303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Wise EA, Riley JL, III, Robinson ME. Clinical pain perception and hormone replacement therapy in postmenopausal women experiencing orofacial pain. Clinical Journal of Pain. 2000;16(2):121–126. doi: 10.1097/00002508-200006000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Wise EA, Price DD, Myers CD, Heft MW, Robinson ME. Gender role expectations of pain: relationship to experimental pain perception. Pain. 2002;96(3):335–342. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(01)00473-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Jensen MP, Turner JA, Romano JM. Changes in beliefs, catastrophizing, and coping are associated with improvement in multidisciplinary pain treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2001;69(4):655–662. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.69.4.655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Edwards RR, Doleys DM, Lowery D, Fillingim RB. Pain tolerance as a predictor of outcome following multidisciplinary treatment for chronic pain: differential effects as a function of sex. Pain. 2003;106(3):419–426. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2003.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Vlaeyen JWS, Kole-Snijders AMJ, Boeren RGB, Van Eek H. Fear of movement/(re)injury in chronic low back pain and its relation to behavioral performance. Pain. 1995;62(3):363–372. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(94)00279-N. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Vlaeyen JWS, Linton SJ. Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: a state of the art. Pain. 2000;85(3):317–332. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00242-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Crombez G, Vlaeyen JWS, Heuts PHTG, Lysens R. Pain-related fear is more disabling than pain itself: evidence on the role of pain-related fear in chronic back pain disability. Pain. 1999;80(1-2):329–339. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(98)00229-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Asmundson GJG, Norton PJ, Norton GR. Beyond pain: the role of fear and avoidance in chronicity. Clinical Psychology Review. 1999;19(1):97–119. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00034-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Swinkels-Meewisse IEJ, Roelofs J, Verbeek ALM, Oostendorp RAB, Vlaeyen JWS. Fear of movement/(re)injury, disability and participation in acute low back pain. Pain. 2003;105(1-2):371–379. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(03)00255-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Somers TJ, Keefe FJ, Pells JJ, et al. Pain catastrophizing and pain-related fear in osteoarthritis patients: relationships to pain and disability. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management. 2009;37(5):863–872. doi: 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2008.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Heuts PHTG, Vlaeyen JWS, Roelofs J, et al. Pain-related fear and daily functioning in patients with osteoarthritis. Pain. 2004;110(1-2):228–235. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.03.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lentz TA, Sutton Z, Greenberg S, Bishop MD. Pain-related fear contributes to self-reported disability in patients with foot and ankle pathology. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation. 2010;91(4):557–561. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2009.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Camacho-Soto A, Sowa GA, Perera S, Weiner DK. Fear avoidance beliefs predict disability in older adults with chronic low back pain. PM&R. 2012;4(7):493–497. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2012.01.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Meulders A, Vansteenwegen D, Vlaeyen JWS. Women, but not men, report increasingly more pain during repeated (un)predictable painful electrocutaneous stimulation: evidence for mediation by fear of pain. Pain. 2012;153(5):1030–1041. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Robinson ME, Riley JL, III, Myers CD, et al. Gender role expectations of pain: relationship to sex differences in pain. Journal of Pain. 2001;2(5):251–257. doi: 10.1054/jpai.2001.24551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Robinson ME, Wise EA, Gagnon C, Fillingim RB, Price DD. Influences of gender role and anxiety on sex differences in temporal summation of pain. Journal of Pain. 2004;5(2):77–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2003.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Bishop MD, Horn ME, George SZ, Robinson ME. Self-reported pain and disability outcomes from an endogenous model of muscular back pain. BMC Musculoskeletal Disorders. 2011;12, article 35 doi: 10.1186/1471-2474-12-35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bishop MD, Craggs JG, Horn ME, George SZ, Robinson ME. Relationship of intersession variation in negative pain-related affect and responses to thermally-evoked pain. Journal of Pain. 2010;11(2):172–178. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bishop M, Horn M, Gay C, George S. Comparison of hypoalgesic responses to manual therapy interventions in healthy volunteers. Journal of Orthopaedic & Sports Physical Therapy. 2012;42(1):A20–A21. [Google Scholar]
- 33.McNeil DW, Rainwater AJ., III Development of the fear of pain questionnaire—III. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 1998;21(4):389–410. doi: 10.1023/a:1018782831217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Albaret M-C, Muñoz Sastre MT, Cottencin A, Mullet E. The fear of pain questionnaire: factor structure in samples of young, middle-aged and elderly European people. European Journal of Pain. 2004;8(3):273–281. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2003.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Osman A, Breitenstein JL, Barrios FX, Gutierrez PM, Kopper BA. The fear of pain questionnaire-III: further reliability and validity with nonclinical samples. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2002;25(2):155–173. doi: 10.1023/a:1014884704974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Rosier EM, Iadarola MJ, Coghill RC. Reproducibility of pain measurement and pain perception. Pain. 2002;98(1-2):205–216. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(02)00048-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Staud R, Vierck CJ, Cannon RL, Mauderli AP, Price DD. Abnormal sensitization and temporal summation of second pain (wind-up) in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome. Pain. 2001;91(1-2):165–175. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(00)00432-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wright A, Graven-Nielsen T, Davies I, Arendt-Nielsen L. Temporal summation of pain from skin, Muscle and joint following nociceptive ultrasonic Stimulation in humans. Experimental Brain Research. 2002;144(4):475–482. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1062-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Baron RM, Kenny DA. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research. Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51(6):1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers. 2004;36(4):717–731. doi: 10.3758/bf03206553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hayes AF. PROCESS: a versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation and conditional process modeling [White Paper] 2012, http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf.
- 42.Fillingim RB, King CD, Ribeiro-Dasilva MC, Rahim-Williams B, Riley JL., III Sex, gender, and pain: a review of recent clinical and experimental findings. Journal of Pain. 2009;10(5):447–485. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Robinson ME, Riley JL, III, Brown FF, Gremillion H. Sex differences in response to cutaneous anesthesia: a double blind randomized study. Pain. 1998;77(2):143–149. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(98)00088-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kindler LL, Valencia C, Fillingim RB, George SZ. Sex differences in experimental and clinical pain sensitivity for patients with shoulder pain. European Journal of Pain. 2011;15(2):118–123. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2010.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Bingefors K, Isacson D. Epidemiology, co-morbidity, and impact on health-related quality of life of self-reported headache and musculoskeletal pain—a gender perspective. European Journal of Pain. 2004;8(5):435–450. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.George SZ, Wittmer VT, Fillingim RB, Robinson ME. Sex and pain-related psychological variables are associated with thermal pain sensitivity for patients with chronic low back pain. Journal of Pain. 2007;8(1):2–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2006.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Robinson ME, Dannecker EA, George SZ, Otis J, Atchison JW, Fillingim RB. Sex differences in the associations among psychological factors and pain report: a novel psychophysical study of patients with chronic low back pain. Journal of Pain. 2005;6(7):463–470. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.George SZ, Fritz JM, Childs JD, Brennan GP. Sex differences in predictors of outcome in selected physical therapy interventions for acute low back pain. Journal of Orthopaedic and Sports Physical Therapy. 2006;36(6):354–363. doi: 10.2519/jospt.2006.2270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Rustøen T, Wahl AK, Hanestad BR, Lerdal A, Paul S, Miaskowski C. Gender differences in chronic pain—findings from a population-based study of Norwegian adults. Pain Management Nursing. 2004;5(3):105–117. doi: 10.1016/j.pmn.2004.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]