Abstract
Much evidence suggests that dynamic laws of neurobehavioral coordination are sui generis: they deal with collective properties that are repeatable from one system to another and emerge from microscopic dynamics but may not (even in principle) be deducible from them. Nevertheless, it is useful to try to understand the relationship between different levels while all the time respecting the autonomy of each. We report a program of research that uses the theoretical concepts of coordination dynamics and quantitative measurements of simple, well-defined experimental model systems to explicitly relate neural and behavioral levels of description in human beings. Our approach is both top-down and bottom-up and aims at ending up in the same place: top-down to derive behavioral patterns from neural fields, and bottom-up to generate neural field patterns from bidirectional coupling between astrocytes and neurons. Much progress can be made by recognizing that the two approaches —reductionism and emergentism— are complementary. A key to understanding is to couch the coordination of very different things —from molecules to thoughts— in the common language of coordination dynamics.
Keywords: coordination dynamics, multiscale, brain, behavior, complementarity
I. The neural choreography challenge
New information about the brain is accruing at an astonishing rate at every level—from the molecular to the social. Though tremendous progress has been made, conspicuously lacking is a broad framework of ideas with which to interpret and integrate findings from so many different scales and levels of observation. We are confronted, as a former President of the Society for Neuroscience remarked in recent testimony to the US Congress, with the grand challenge of elucidating “neural choreography” (see also Akil, Martone & van Essen, 2011). No single focused level of analysis suffices to understand the brain and its disorders. We need to identify the dancers1, capture the essence of the dance and uncover how disease disrupts it. The task is daunting: the ‘functions’ of the brain and of brains interacting with each other, are manifold and nearly countless. Sift through, for example, typical issues of Neural Networks or The Journal of Neuroscience. The deep problem that won’t go away is the relationship between brains and minds, whether individual or collective. Much progress has been made, not least by the efforts of scientists and engineers in the field of neural networks, but an integrative picture is still lacking. The gap between the language of molecules and cellular machinery (genetics, neuroscience) and the language of mind and its various disorders (cognitive science, neurology and psychiatry) seems very large indeed and is fast widening. There is a belief that things will work out in the end, but at the moment it remains just that—a belief.
Might we take a different tack on the problem? Our intent here is to outline a conceptual and empirical framework (‘a general theory’) that aims to provide insight into how different levels of organization across multiple space and timescales are connected. Though it certainly relies upon them, on offer here is not a detailed model of neurons and neural machinery supporting hypothesized processes involved in cognition and behavior. The central idea is that all such processes –regardless of level of description— depend on coordination and the different forms it takes. Our approach is to identify the dynamic laws of coordination and reveal their mechanistic realizations level by level, using both a top-down and a bottom-up approach. By ascribing physiological meaning to the parameters and mathematical expressions in a (computationally implemented) phenomenological theory we aim to bridge the gap between behavioral phenomena and their neural underpinnings.
II. Connecting the micro- and the macro-
Twenty-five years ago, around the time that the journal Neural Networks was being founded, we reported empirical and theoretical results demonstrating that coordinated patterns of human behavior could be explained using the concepts of self-organization in open, nonequilibrium systems, particularly synergetics (Haken, 1983) and the mathematical tools of nonlinear dynamics (Schöner & Kelso, 1988, for review). We intimated then that similar principles are likely to be present also in elementary neural circuits called central pattern generators. In the intervening period, the evidence for mulitifunctionality in neural circuitry viewed as multistable dynamical systems is overwhelming (Briggman & Kristan, 2008; Prinz et al., 2004; see also Grillner & Graybiel, 2006). Moreover, in the last 30 years principles of self-organization have been shown to govern patterns of coordination (a) within a moving limb and between moving limbs; (b) between the articulators during speech production; (c) between limb movements and tactile, visual and auditory stimuli; (d) between people interacting with each other spontaneously or intentionally; (e) between humans and avatars; (f) between humans and other species, as in riding a horse; and (g) within and between the neural substrates that underlie the coupled behavior of human beings as measured using MEG, EEG and fMRI (Fuchs & Jirsa, 2006; Kelso, 1995; 2009, for reviews). How might these phenomena be understood?
There are strong hints that laws of coordination in neurobehavioral systems are generic and deal with collective properties that emerge from microscopic dynamics, but how to understand such emergent phenomena has proven difficult in the extreme. An argument can be made that such laws are truly sui generis and that it may not be possible, even in principle, to deduce higher level descriptions from lower level ones (Laughlin & Pines, 2000). The present approach is entirely conventional with respect to the history of science. Fundamentally, it begins with the identification of the macroscopic behavior of a system and attempts to derive it from a level below. Even for physical systems, however, the derivation of the ‘macro’ from the ‘micro’ is nontrivial. Only in the 70′s, for example, was it possible to derive the behavior of ferromagnets (as described by Landau’s mean field theory) from more fundamental grounds using the so-called renormalization group method that earned Kenneth Wilson the Nobel Prize in 1982. Thus, only some 70 years after atoms were discovered did it become possible to derive macroscopic properties of certain materials from a more microscopic basis, and only then using rather sophisticated and somewhat cumbersome mathematical techniques (cf. Laughlin & Pines, 2000).
One lesson to be learned from such successes in connecting the micro- and the macro- is that it is crucial to first have a precise description of the macroscopic behavior of a system in order to know what to derive. Without the phenomenological theory of phase transitions, for instance, with their critical exponents and so forth, microscopic approaches would be meaningless. Another lesson is that even in a system whose microscopic constituents are homogeneous (unlike, say the neurons, glia and neurotransmitters of the brain) special methods are needed to handle events and interactions that are occurring on many spatial and temporal scales. In an apparently heterogeneous, hierarchically organized system like the nervous system, it is necessary to proceed in a trans-level fashion with as intimate an interplay between theory and experiment as possible using multiple observables in line with what the existing technology allows—and keeping an eye out for new techniques that may open up additional levels of description. Figure 1 conveys some of the scales and methods we will consider here, along with examples of how they have been employed in various cognitive tasks.
III. Finding relevant variables
Our strategy for traversing levels is shown in Figure 2.
The basic idea is to identify relevant variables characterizing coordinated or collective states of the system and the collective variable’s dynamics (i.e. equations of motion for collective variables). Note that in most neurobehavioral systems these are not known in advance, but have to be found. The experimental method uses transition points or instabilities to clearly distinguish different coordinated behaviors. In coordination dynamics, phase transitions are exploited both as a dynamical mechanism for effecting change (‘switching’, decision-making’) and as a methodology to identify key collective variables and their dynamics. The reason is that in complex systems very many features can be measured but not all are relevant; coordination dynamics assumes that the variable that changes qualitatively is the most important one for system function (and, incidentally, for the scientist trying to understand it). It is these collective variables that are mapped on to a dynamical system. Further experiments are necessary to identify the component dynamics and further theory is needed to derive the collective variable dynamics from nonlinear interactions among components. Rhythmic coordination, an example of which is synchronization (cf.Fig.1) —with its long history in biology and behavior—proves to be an excellent starting point for developing a multi-level understanding of coordination. Experiments show, for example that the rhythmic motion of a single finger, which exhibits dynamic properties such as transient behavior and amplitude dependence on movement rate can be modeled as a Van der Pol-Rayleigh oscillator. Once the finger motion is coupled to other signals, e.g. the other hand starts to move or an external periodic stimulus is present--or even a virtual moving finger is present as in the human dynamic clamp or virtual partner interaction paradigm (Kelso, et al., 2009) -- then the relative phase between finger motion and the additional signal becomes a meaningful descriptor of coordination. However, not every relative phase will be selected. The coupling establishes a symmetry breaking by constraining the number of existing phase states. Moreover, not every existing relative phase is actually stable: an unstable state or repellor may separate stable states. When control parameters such as the movement rate are manipulated, coordination states may become unstable and the system may exhibit a transition from one state to another. In the vicinity of phase transitions, complex, open systems become low dimensional. Thus, in this picture, phase transitions are the core of self-organization. Since their original discovery in experiments and consequent theoretical modeling (Kelso, 1981; 1984; Haken, Kelso & Bunz, 1985; Kelso, Delcolle & Schöner, 1990; Schöner, Haken & Kelso, 1986), phase transitions and related dynamical phenomena have been observed in many different systems ranging from bimanual and sensorimotor coordination and their neural correlates, to interpersonal and interbrain coordination (for reviews, Fuchs & Kelso, 2009; Kelso, 1995; 2009; 2010; Tognoli, 2008; Kelso et al., 2009; Oullier & Kelso, 2009; Dumas, et al., 2010). A specific example of our strategic approach is provided in Figure 3.
IV. Phase transitions in coupled neural populations
To find generic mechanisms of dynamical systems is a powerful way to classify behavior on any level of description, but on its own makes no explicit connection to the underlying neural substrate and its dynamics. The key idea is to follow the same conceptual approach as on the behavioral level, namely to identify functionally relevant neural components (‘our dancers’) that must be coupled together to accomplish the coordination involved in cognitive processing. For such coordination, information flow is seldom of the sender-receiver, input-output unidirectional kind (Tognoli & Kelso, in press). On the contrary, the bidirectional nature of the coupling proves to be a crucial aspect of dynamic coordination, regardless of whether two hands, two people and two brains are interacting for social functions (Banerjee, et al., 2012; Dumas, et al., 2010; Tognoli, et al., 2007; Naeem, et al., 2012) or astrocytes and neurons are interacting for normal synaptic transmission (Wade et al., 2011).
The answer to the question of neuronal mechanisms depends again on the chosen level of description. Experimentally accessible observables are provided by non-invasive techniques such as electroencephalography (EEG), magnetoencephalography (MEG) and structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI, fMRI). The first two techniques, EEG and MEG, are direct measures of the functional organization of neuronal populations. EEG measures the electric potential on the skull surface; MEG usually measures the gradient of the magnetic field over the radial direction above the surface of the skull. Both signals are generated by the simultaneous neuroelectric activity of hundreds of thousands of neurons located mainly in neocortex. The spatial resolution of EEG and MEG is on the order of cm., its temporal resolution is msec. Structural MRI provides three-dimensional coordinates of neuronal tissue on the scale of mm. fMRI measures the metabolic activity of the brain, hence providing an indirect measure of neuronal activation on the spatial scale of mm and temporal scale of sec. In short, EEG and MEG provide measures of neuronal correlates on the same (or even faster) time scale as human behavior, but are confined to the skull surface, whereas fMRI is three-dimensional and spatially precise, but remains an indirect measure and a bit too slow to capture human behavior in real time. Taken together, these techniques measure complementary facets of the same neuronal processes. For illustrative purposes, we turn to Figure 4 where the same phase transition paradigm (Kelso, 1990) is carried out on behavioral, EEG, MEG and fMRI levels, albeit in different experiments. Not shown is recent work that focuses on the transition itself, for instance Jantzen, et al.’s (2009) study of BOLD changes in specific neural regions as a control parameter drives the system toward instabilities; remarkable work by Meyer-Lindenberg et al., (2002) which uses TMS as a perturbation to provoke instabilities in both brain recordings and behavior2; and Banerjee et al.’s (2012) research which captures transient recruitment of neural assemblies at the transition from antiphase to inphase.
V. Connecting behavioral and neural dynamics
Following the same strategy as the behavioral level, with the foregoing techniques it is possible to define an isolated spatiotemporal event such as a rhythmic finger movement, with the goal of identifying its neuronal correlates. Simple sensory and motor events are well represented in EEG and MEG signals and hence well-suited to investigating the neural dynamics. Obviously there is a relation between the spatiotemporal pattern in the EEG and MEG and the underlying neuronal generators. For present purposes, we bypass the so-called inverse problem (but see Murzin, et al., 2011) and seek to identify the dynamics of the patterns in the EEG and MEG. In the case of sensorimotor coordination, MEG experiments (Kelso, et al., 1998) showed that to get the finger to the right place at the right time, the velocity of movement is a key parameter that is highly correlated with the spatiotemporal pattern of the MEG. To model this simple but tight relationship, Fuchs et al (2000) were able to express the finger movement r(t) as a convolution of a memory function G(t – τ) with its neuronal signal Ψ(x, τ) over past times, τ. This expression was projected on a spatial template pattern β(x) which represents the spatial distribution of the neuronal areas involved in the behavioral task. Both, the memory function and the template can be identified from the experimental reconstructed the data. Then Fuchs et al (2000) reconstructed the finger movement r(t) as:
The linear relationship between the neuronal activation Ψ(x,t) and the finger movement r(t) is due to the rhythmic nature of the movement task. Experimentally, it also turns out that the memory function is an exponential function and the spatial template involves mainly contralateral primary motor areas and some minor contributions from ipsilateral cortex. Equivalently, the above relationship says that the finger movement is an oscillator that is driven by neuronal activity. Given experimental information about the memory function and the spatial template, finger movement can be “read out” directly from the neuronal activity Ψ(x,t) . In the case of bimanual coordination, the neuronal patterns Ψ1(t) and Ψ2(t) corresponding to the two finger motions, r1 (t) and r2 (t) have to interact. These patterns are obtained as projections of the neuronal activity onto the individual spatial templates βi(x)i,=1,2 Jirsa et al. (1998) applied a simple transformation Ψ+(t)=Ψ1(t)+Ψ2(t) and Ψ−(t)=Ψ1(t)-Ψ2(t) that expressed the neuronal pattern dynamics in terms of the patterns of behavioral coordination observed in the original Kelso experiments. At the critical frequency, this predicts that the neuronal pattern for antiphase behavior, Ψ−(t) becomes unstable and leads to the emergence of a new spatiotemporal organization, the in-phase pattern Ψ+(t). Experimental data from full-head MEG measurements supported this prediction, analysis revealing that a change in the symmetry of the brain pattern occurred across the behavioral transition (Jirsa, et al., 1998; see also, Banerjee, et al., 2012 for a different approach using EEG). The dynamical mechanism for both neuronal and behavioral levels proves to be of the same generic type, a Pitchfork bifurcation.
VI. Deriving behavioral and brain patterns from neural ensemble dynamics
How close are we to our goal of connecting neuronal dynamics to underlying biological mechanisms? According to the present strategy, in order to move beyond this still phenomenological level and identify underlying neural processes one has to go at least to the level of neural ensembles. Models of neural ensembles have a long history and differ in terms of connectivity and inclusion of physiological detail (see, for example, Amari, 1977; Ermentrout, 1998; Nunez, 1974; 1995; Robinson, et al., 1997; Wilson & Cowan, 1972; 1973; Wright & Liley, 1995). For our purposes, a good candidate is Jirsa and Haken’s (1996; 1997) neuronal field model which uses so-called ‘conversion operations’ (see Freeman, 1992 for review) to define mathematical relations between firing rates and local field potentials (cf. Fig.1). This is important because: a) Freeman’s and others’ experiments on a variety of cortical areas reveal a well-defined relation between neuronal firing rates and the local field potential; and b) it allows a connection to observable quantities in the EEG and MEG. Dendritic currents generated by active synapses are responsible for the extracellular local field potential measured in the former, and likewise intracellular dendritic currents are thought to generate the MEG signal. The wave-to-spike conversion from the local field potential is sigmoidal within a neuronal ensemble; the inverse spike-to-wave conversion from firing rate to the local field is also sigmoidal but is constrained to a small signal linear change3. Both short range intracortical fibers (excitatory and inhibitory) and long range corticocortical fibers (excitatory only) constitute the connectivity required to capture the relatively large spatial and temporal scales of EEG and MEG. Propagation along long range fibers may cause propagation delays up to 200 msec., consistent with the timescale of most behavior. In the neuronal field model, the notion of a functional unit--a spatiotemporal mapping relating external events to neuronal events-- is central (cf. Fig.2). For the specific case of bimanual coordination, the cortical sheet is divided into left and right hemispheric areas representing pre- and postcentral cortices containing the primary motor and sensorimotor areas of the left and right fingers. Localization of these areas obeys a reflection symmetry with respect to the plane between the two hemispheres. Sensorimotor units receive proprioceptive and kinesthetic information from the respective finger movements and motor areas may be thought of as driving the fingers. The spatiotemporal details of how the motor areas drive the finger oscillation is given by the functional mapping described above (Kelso, et al., 1999; Fuchs, et al., 2000). Mathematical analysis of these equations by Jirsa et al. (1998; 1999) predicts that the neuronal activity will show a spatially antisymmetric organization in the antiphase condition, but then undergo a transition to a symmetric organization simultaneously with the behavioral transition to in-phase. Numerical simulations of the neuronal ensemble dynamics confirm the mathematical predictions.
VII. Combining dynamical modeling with anatomical structure
In contrast to a purely phenomenological theory, notice that it is now possible to make statements about the relationship between behavioral dynamics and biological mechanisms. Phenomenological parameters on the behavioral level are now expressed in terms of the physiological and anatomical parameters in the neuronal field model. Callosal projections of motor and sensorimotor areas between the hemispheres are spatially symmetric with respect to the reflection at the plane separating the hemispheres. These connections couple the motor systems on both hemispheres directly; thus they strongly contribute to the neuronal dynamics during bimanual coordination in particular, because they contribute to the spatial overlap of functional units (see also Banerjee & Jirsa, 2007; Banerjee, et al., 2008; Daffertshofer, et al., 2005; Tuller & Kelso, 1989). The spatial overlap hypothesis was tested in the experimental MEG data and found to be consistent with neuronal field theory (see Jirsa et al., 1998). More recently, Banerjee and colleagues (2012) tracked spatiotemporal reorganization of the brain through the transition from antiphase to inphase bimanual coordination. Brain networks participating in unimanual movements were found to reorganize themselves during bimanual coordination patterns; recruitment of additional brain areas was mostly confined to the period surrounding the behavioral phase transition. The story told in the previous segments is a testament to the strategy of connecting levels using as tight a connection between experiment and theory as possible. Much more of course can be (and has been) done. Obviously, the level of anatomical and physiological detail can always be enhanced. For example, the geometry of the cortex can be defined in three dimensions to enable stronger experimental testing. The neuronal field model has to be elaborated to include functionally relevant subsystems, including subcortical components, so that their role in brain coordination can be addressed. The connectivity should contain additional structure. For instance, the density of connections of axonal fiber systems is not translationally invariant, i.e., is not everywhere the same. Intracortically speaking, the description of the cortex by a densely connected mesh is attractive and justifiable. However, the myelinated extracortical fiber system is sparse and selective. One would expect this aspect to have some functional significance for the spatial and temporal scales measured in EEG and MEG.
All of these aspects and more have been elaborated in further work by Jirsa and colleagues (Jirsa, et al., 2001; 2002). Neuronal field dynamics on the sphere along with the mapping of functional units onto the folded cortex and forward solutions (aided by fMRI) for EEG and MEG provide a platform for the further development of a multi-level model of human brain and behavioral function (see also Jirsa et al., 2010). Interconnected neural ensembles with homogeneous connections represent a neural level, while a network or systems level is defined by the interaction between heterogeneously connected cortical regions. The complementarity of structure and dynamics is at play here: while the anatomical structure constrains the dynamics, the dynamics simultaneously shapes the structure.The former relationship has been specifically approached through the study of how anatomical connectivity influences resting state activity and more specifically the so-called Default Mode Network (DMN) (Honey et al. 2009, Cabral et al. 2011). In coordination tasks, heterogeneous fiber tracts connect the cortical, subcortical and spinal subsystems involved. These pathways couple the subsystems and thus add to their cross-talk and the resulting coordination dynamics. Without getting overly technical, heterogeneities introduce additional entries in the connectivity matrix of the neural field (Assisi, et al., 2005; Jirsa & Kelso, 2000; Qubbaj & Jirsa, 2007) carrying information on distance and strength of coupling between areas. Developing technologies, such as diffusion tensor weighted imaging (DTI), provide information regarding the integrity of white matter tracts in the connectivity matrix of individual subjects (for an interesting application, see Jing, et al., 2011). The present approach thus allows for the integration of neurophysiological and anatomical facts regarding the coupling among neural ensembles for various behavioral functions, setting the stage on which non-invasive brain imaging, theory, experiment, modeling and data analysis can play out—a true TEAM approach (Theory, Experiment, Analysis and Modeling). From the perspective of coordination dynamics, the conceptual and methodological framework presented here allows for the development of a theoretical model of human brain function and behavior that operates at multiple levels of description. Laws of behavioral coordination may be connected to laws of neuronal pattern generation at the level of neuronal ensembles. Via the TEAM approach of coordination dynamics, it is now possible to link dynamical mechanisms and physiological quantities.
VIII. Down to the level of neural masses
It is obviously possible to continue the strategy of traversing scales further and further downward to levels that the field of neuroscience and neurocomputation consider quite basic for information processing, namely the level of spiking neurons (Fig.1). This is a place where the mathematical formalisms of neural networks and their applications have been quite successful. For example, in the field of fault tolerant computing, neural networks offer a fine-grained distributed computing architecture that enables fault tolerance to be realized at very low levels of granularity; computations may be mapped across many neuron clusters permitting a “scattering” of faults without significantly degrading computation (Harkin, et al., 2009). Interesting and challenging though such applications are, the goal here is slightly different. Above we reviewed evidence and theory indicating that ordinary behavioral coordination can be connected explicitly to neural fields: might it now be possible to reverse direction, as it were, and derive neural field descriptions from the bottom up? On the theoretical side, one of the bridges one has to cross is that large parameter spaces are involved: the interacting elements are differentiated and so numerous that combinatoric explosion is a humongous problem. Once again we are faced with the problem of preserving biophysical relevance at the same time as handling the large number of degrees of freedom involved. One way to decrease the complexity of models is by reducing the number of different types of neurons, synaptic connections and states. Such an approach simplifies simulations of large heterogeneous neural networks by mapping the collective state of similar types of neuron onto a probabilistic density distribution in the phase space (for an excellent review see Deco et al. 2008). Then, the dynamics of each class can be summarized in a single set of differential equations. These Fokker-Planck equations describe the temporal evolution of the probability density distributions over the state-space, representing the distribution of the ensemble of trajectories of all neurons within one class. Although a systematic exploration of the parameter space is still computationally expensive, two approximations are commonly used: the first consists of focusing only on the stationary solutions of the Fokker-Planck equations; the second consists in replacing the probability density distributions by their mass-center which provides a single number that represents the average activity of the population. The resulting model is then called a ‘neural mass’. Neural masses have already proven useful in modeling electrophysiological recordings such as EEG and MEG: ‘dynamic causal modeling’ uses them as a heuristic to infer the coupling among brain regions from experimental data (Friston et al. 2003; Marreiros et al. 2010). Despite fairly realistic results, neural mass models are a work in progress: further efforts are needed in order to accommodate the joint and often conflicting demands of biological realism and computational tractability. One technique is to subdivide neuronal populations according to their types (e.g. excitatory/inhibitory) and reduce complexity using mode decomposition techniques (Stefanescu and Jirsa, 2008). This gives access to a vaster range of complex dynamics observed in real data yet still employs a low dimensional description, thereby greatly easing computational costs. Figure 5 provides an image of the neural mass, an hypothesized unit of neurocomputation (Izhikevich and Edelman, 2008). Other recent developments introduce spatial constraints (Pinotsis and Friston 2011). Such spatial priors allow one to recover the dynamics of cortical activity even in the absence of explicit spatial information in experimental data (Pinotsis et al. 2012): they also can accommodate dynamic behaviors such as pattern formation and traveling waves (Coombes 2008). Even though the relation between neural masses (as nodes in a network) and neural fields seems conceptually clear, it is fair to say that the distance between neural masses, neural fields and human cognition and behavior still needs shortened. Nevertheless, the path advocated here to understand how neural fields arise from both cellular and behavioral constraints seems open.
IX. Bidirectional coupling: from neuro-glial masses to social interaction
The importance of bidirectional coupling in a general theory of neural and behavioral coordination can hardly be overemphasized. At the level of individual cells—the last rung in the ladder, at least for present purposes, bidirectional coupling is manifest in the tripartite synapse. The usual pre-post synaptic communication involves one-way (sender-receiver, Shannonian-like) chemical transmission. As is now well-known, in the tripartite synapse an astrocyte process connects with the axon and dendrite of both the pre- and post-synaptic neurons and is sensitive to neurotransmitters within the extracellular fluid in the synaptic cleft. When neurotransmitter, e.g. glutamate, is released into the synaptic cleft by the presynaptic terminal, some of it interacts with glutamate receptors on the astrocyte. This then initiates the release of so-called IP3 into the astrocytic cytoplasm. IP3 subsequently binds to IP3 receptors on the Endoplasmic Reticulum (ER), a long network of tubes and vesicles used to store calcium within the cell. The binding of IP3 with IP3 receptors opens channels that allow the release of Ca2+ from the ER into the cytoplasm (Ca2+ puff). While individual Ca2+ puffs are incapable of propagating intracellularly, several puffs can raise Ca2+ levels in the cytoplasm beyond a threshold and an oscillating Calcium Induced Calcium Release (CICR) propagation is observed. The increase in cytosolic Ca2+ then causes the release of neurotransmitter, more commonly called gliotransmitter, back into the synaptic cleft. The end result of all this is that the astrocyte can modulate synaptic transmission between pre- and post-synaptic neurons based on the previous activity of the synapse and the type of transmitter released--bidirectional neuronal~astrocyte coupling.
At this level of description the strategy is to construct biophysically realistic models of essential cellular processes such as calcium transport, presynaptic inhibition, etc. and ‘synergize’ them –just like Nature herself--to produce emergent coordinative effects such as synchronization and phase-locking, learning (Spike Timing Dependent Plasticity), repair, and so forth. This has already been demonstrated in the astrocyte-neuronal (AN) model of Wade et al (2011a,b) illustrated in Figure 6. The intriguing possibility (perhaps even more than that) is that these elementary models can be scaled up to the level of neuro-glial masses, that scale up to the level of neural fields composed of networks that produce patterns of neural activity in distributed cortical and subcortical areas of the brain, that (when exposed to external stimuli and internal influences such as neuromodulators) lead to the coordinated patterns of behavior involved in perceiving, attending, decision-making, learning, remembering, selecting and controlling movements—all the functions of mind-- and ultimately to the coordination between brains and people.
X. The brain: A geography of rhythms
The general theory of behavioral and brain coordination uses instabilities in behavior as an entry point and proceeds downward to the level of neural masses and (if Providence permits) back up again from the level of astrocyte-neuronal interactions. An approach entirely consistent with the theory has explored variability (1/f fluctuations) at behavioral and neural levels demonstrating the existence of similar scaling laws in psychophysical studies and neural data (Palva & Palva, 2011; see also Chen, et al., 1997; Chialvo, 2010; Ding, et al., 2002; Plenz & Thiagarian, 2007). Such work is highly suggestive that common dynamics are at play at multiple scales--without necessarily providing a picture about their local underpinnings (Beggs & Timme, 2012). In this respect, neuroscience has made considerable progress in the theorization of brain dynamics noticeably with the study of oscillatory activity. Interestingly, the collective effort of the computational neuroscience community toward the creation of a “virtual brain” (Jirsa et al. 2010) has also demonstrated the pervasive nature of such mesoscopic dynamical patterns. At this scale, the brain is indeed dominated by rhythmic activity covering frequencies from approximately near DC to several hundred Hertz, with specific frequency bands hypothesized to act as independent channels of communication and coordination (Roopun et al. 2008). Since even single neurons are also endowed with the capacity to express multiple frequencies, it has been proposed to view neural oscillations as the “critical middle ground” connecting neurons to behavior (Buzsaki and Draguhn, 2004). Understanding of neural oscillations and synchrony has progressively evolved from the early correlative observations with EEG to more functional accounts (Uhlhaas, et al., 2009). This shift has been accelerated in the 90′s when an international buzz appeared around gamma oscillations and their potential link with consciousness. Even if a bit hyped, this produced a number of insightful results and extended our knowledge of neurodynamics. Observations span across multiple scales in both space and time, from the percept-related modulation of gamma oscillations in neuronal microcircuits (e.g., Bressler and Freeman, 1980) to motor-related modulations of beta oscillations at scalp level (e.g., Boonstra et al., 2007; Houweling et al., 2010). In short, neural synchronization has been under the spotlight ever since it was proposed as a potential mechanism for the integration of information among distant brain regions (Varela et al., 2001; Fries, 2005).
A mathematical account of both oscillation and synchronization phenomena can be carried out with networks of oscillators, very much along the lines of the general theory proposed here. Whereas such a model may appear biophysically far from the individual neurons compared to neural masses, it fits the mesoscopic scale dominated by oscillatory activity. Though an idealization as far as the brain is concerned, the Kuramoto model of weakly phase-coupled oscillators is the most commonly used. As long as the coupling is weak and the subsystems nearly identical, this model has been demonstrated to approximate the long-term behavior of any ensemble of interacting oscillatory systems (Kuramoto, 1975; Acebron, et al., 2005). It has already been proposed for unifying brain oscillations and their relationship to basic computational processes including multistability, criticality, and information capacity (for review see Breakspear et al. 2010). Combined with ‘realistic’ connectivity, the Kuramoto model has shown similar efficiency to neural mass models for simulating the dynamical consequences of cortical lesions (Honey and Sporns 2008). Recent studies have also demonstrated how resting-state neural dynamics can originate from the interplay between the local neural dynamics and the large-scale structure of the brain (Honey et al. 2009; Cabral et al. 2011). Most of the studies thus far have focused on the isolated brain. Yet the Kuramoto model has been applied to complex systems in physics, biology, and social science, and is a useful tool for traversing multiple scales of brain and behavior. The inter-individual level has been approached recently through the modeling of perception-action coupling between two virtual brains using Kuramoto oscillators combined with realistic anatomical structure (Dumas et al. 2012). This study follows directly the experimental demonstration that behavioral synchrony correlates with the emergence of inter-brain synchronization (Dumas et al. 2010; cf Fig.1). Simulations describe how the anatomical structure affects both intra- and inter-individual neural dynamics: at the intra-individual level by favoring dynamical modes such as the alpha rhythm; at the inter-individual level, by facilitating inter-brain synchronization and thus partly accounting for our propensity to generate self-other dynamical couplings. By directly linking intra- and inter-brain synchronization, the work opens a way to draw a connection between neural, behavioral and social scales and thereby extend the general theory.
XI. From synchronization to metastability
As an hypothesized mechanism for integration in the brain, synchronization has captured by far the most attention among neuroscientists (see, e.g. the many contributions in von der Malsburg, et al., 2010). We all like to see order in our data and synchronization-- as a highly ordered form of phase- and frequency-locked coordination— not only catches the eye but is accessible through an armamentarium of analysis methods (cf. Fig.1). But the brain and the mind are always in constant flux, ensembles of neurons being assembled and annihilated continuously. Whereas synchronization (usually in-phase) naturally arises in Kuramoto-like models, it is viewed as only one regime of brain coordination dynamics. Elsewhere we have discussed in some detail also the metastable regime which arises when the system exhibits both an integrative tendency, i.e. a tendency to synchronize, and a tendency for the components to maintain their autonomy (e.g. Bressler & Kelso, 2001; Kelso, 1991; 1995; 2001; 2008; 2012; Kelso & Tognoli, 2007; Tognoli & Kelso, 2009). Metastability consists of a more subtle dwell and escape dynamic in which the brain is never quite stable and merely expresses a joint tendency or disposition for neural areas to synchronize together and to oscillate independently. In the general theory, metastability (with its slow-fast, dwell-escape dynamic) is the way the brain constantly creates and destroys neural assemblies. Measuring metastability in neurophysiological (and behavioral) data represents a methodological challenge since this dynamical phenomenon is more complex than phase locking (for possible metrics, see Kelso, 2008). The two main issues are to first determine if a system is metastable (in whole, in parts and at times); and then to quantify its properties to assess the kind of coordination dynamics that exists between the parts (for instance, the degree of integration~segregation, the particular phase relationships between transient subensembles, their spatial and temporal endurance, the temporal positioning of escape behaviors, and all other properties that contribute to the specifics of the system’s function). The former issue, determination of metastability, may be addressed with perturbation paradigms applied to properly prepared systems (Massimini et al. 2009; Stamoulis et al. 2011; Kitajo et al. 2011). As for the latter issue (what kind of metastability one is dealing with), a promising method would be model fitting performed on collective variables, with special emphasis on the location of dwells and escapes and their spread. When there are two tendencies, the most common metastability case, it is possible to relate empirically observed features (e.g. histograms of the relative phase) with the parameters of the extended-HKB model (Kelso et al., 1990). Importantly, such an approach combines both dwell and escape phenomena rather than treating them separately as instances of coordination behavior resembling states and transition (see also Kelso & Tognoli, 2007). Nevertheless, metastability could be associated with multistable or monostable regimes of coordination by alternation in time and co-existence in space as in dynamical chimera (Kelso & Tognoli, 2007; Shanahan, 2010; Kelso, 2012). Metastability emphasizes the transient nature of the ongoing coordination dynamics and is linked to intermittency (Pomeau & Manneville, 1980) and chaotic itinerancy (Tsuda et al. 1987; Tsuda, 2001). These aspects can been approached quantitatively with tools such as the entropy derived from the spectral density (Friston 1997) or connectivity indicators and metrics computed on brain functional networks (Sporns & Tononi, 2002; Sporns, 2004). Furthermore, as metastability is characterized by a stationary transient regime, its presence may also be associated with self-organized criticality (Bak et al. 1988). This last phenomenon is traditionally assessed through the discovery of power-laws and scale-free quantities (Chen, et al., 1997; Kitzbichler et al. 2009; Chialvo, 2010). At this point, it is important to notice how metastability is a phenomenon in its own right not just a sequence of transitions between multiple states (Kelso & Tognoli, 2007; Kelso, 2012).
Viewed as a generative dynamical mechanism, metastability is in line with observations that ensembles of neurons of various sizes come together and disband incessantly (Beggs & Timme, 2012; Plenz & Thiagarian, 2007). According to the present theory, the normal brain realizes its complexity at all scales not in its most ordered form (integration qua synchronization) or disordered form (segregation qua desynchronization), but in a subtle blend of both tendencies (see also Tononi, et al., 1994). The logic of the brain is not only either/or, but both~and. Dual tendencies for integration and segregation constitute a complementary pair (Kelso & Engstrøm, 2006) very much along the lines proposed by Stephen Grossberg, one of the pioneers of the field of Neural Networks: the brain is organized to obey principles of complementarity (Grossberg, 2000). Metastable coordination dynamics rationalizes William James (1890) beautiful metaphor of the stream of consciousness as the flight of a bird whose life journey consists of ‘perchings’ (phase gathering, integrative tendencies) and ‘flights’ (phase scattering, segregative tendencies). Both tendencies appear to be crucial: the former to summon and create thoughts; the latter to release individual brain areas to participate in other acts of cognition, emotion and action. Of course, the hypothesis of metastable coordination dynamics in all its manifestations as the organizing principle of brain and cognitive function--as the essence of ‘neural choreography’-- is the testbed of the general theory. If the brain does not work according to metastable coordination dynamics, and if brains working together do not work according to metastable coordination dynamics, and if metastable coordination does not break down in diseases of the brain, such as Parkinson’s and schizophrenia, the theory is wrong.
XII. Conclusion
In 1979, Francis Crick in a special issue of Scientific American on the brain said the following: “If a breakthrough in the study of the brain does come, it is perhaps likely to be at the level of the overall control of the system. If the system were as chaotic as it appears to be, it would not enable us to perform even the simplest tasks satisfactorily. To invent a possible, although unlikely example, the discovery that the brain was run phasically by some kind of periodic clock, as a computer is would probably constitute a major brakthrough” (p .137). According to the general theory outlined here, the brain is a self-organized, pattern forming dynamical system living in the metastable regime where fluctations play a key role. This is definitely not the way computers, at least as we know them, are organized. Nevertheless, on the basis of much research in coordination dynamics, Crick’s intuition has a ring of truth about it. Though the brain is not a clock, it is run phasically: coordination dynamics shows that relative phase is a key quantity that couples diverse regions of the brain whose neurons exhibit tendencies to oscillate. The primitives of the brain do appear to take the form of oscillations and the natural language of the brain is the way these oscillatory ensembles are coupled. The two “forces” that constitute the general theory deal fundamentally with (mostly bidirectional) information exchange. One force is the strength of coupling between the elements; this allows information to be distributed to all participating elements and is a key to integrative, collective action. The other is the ability of individual elements to express their autonomy, and thereby minimize the influence of others. Self-organization in the metastable regime is the interplay of both4. This brings us to a final point, perhaps more on the philosophical side but with quite cogent scientific consequences. Your neural network and my neural network are not the same. The nodes are different, the connections too; individual differences exist at all levels of structure and function. You walk differently than me, you think differently than me, you feel differently than me, you respond to the environment differently than me, you structure your world differently than me. A general theory, then, is not (or not only) about the contents of mind and emotions and their neural correlates. Rather it is about the dynamical processes of forming, breaking, uniting, dissolving, and harmonizing patterns of activity that occur at all levels. A general theory of behavioral and brain coordination applies just as much to the mind-world of ideas as it does to the brain-world of cells.
Highlights.
An account of brain and behavioral function that encompasses multiple levels in a coherent manner without reifying one level or another
Uses instabilities as an experimental window to identify behavioral and neural dynamics
General theory employs both a top-down and bottom-up approach to connect phenomenological models to physiology and biophysics
Highlights.
A theory of brain and behavior is proposed based on the principles of coordination dynamics
The theory connects multiple levels of description
Instabilities are used experimentally to identify relevant quantities and their dynamics
Bidirectional coupling is emphasized at all levels, both within and across brains
Acknowledgements
The research described herein is supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (MH080838), the National Science Foundation (BCS0826897), the US Office of Naval Research (N000140910527) and the Chaire d’Excellence Pierre de Fermat. We are grateful to Jose Luis Perez-Velazquez and an anonymous reviewer for their comments.
Footnotes
And to pursue the metaphor, there are dancers inside the dancer, like Russian dolls.
Obviously, the combination of these techniques brings many advances. For instance, the introduction of structural priors with MRI allows the reconstruction of cortical sources from EEG and MEG signals (e.g. Murzin, et al., 2011). Axonal white matter can now be tracked using diffusion MRI or DTI providing a snapshot of the structural connectivity of the brain. And recent progress permits access to EEG and fMRI simultaneously (e.g. Laufs et al. 2003)
Learning is approached via local changes of synaptic weights resulting in a temporal dependence of the connectivity function, i.e., the integral kernel. Attention and intention are approached via local changes in the sigmoidal response curves of neural ensembles, the so-called conversion operations. These conversion operations have been investigated in quantitative detail as a function of attention (e.g., Freeman,1975). The main result is that the slope and the height of the sigmoid vary by a factor of 2.5 between minimal and maximal attention. The sigmoidal variation of the ensemble response is realized biochemically by different concentrations of neurotransmitters such as dopamine and norepinephrine. Mathematically, the neural dynamics described by the spatiotemporal integral equations can be coupled to a one-dimensional concentration field in which elevated values designate increased values of slope and height of the sigmoidal response curve of neural ensembles. An increased slope and height of the sigmoid typically causes increased amplitude and excitability of the neural sheet (Jirsa, personal communication).
Symmetry breaking as observed in living things is deeply connected with the stabilization of a structural organization in a fluctuating environment. As Pattee (1987) remarks, “the physicist’s instability-based concepts and the biologist’s information-based concepts of self-organization are also closely related to the two sides of the structure-function complementarity” (see also Kelso & Engstrom, 2006). The brain’s anatomical structure may create statistical regularities that shape how information is processed by the nervous system (Sporns et al., 2005). The related “structural couplings” (Maturana and Varela, 1987) are expressed at multiple scales of observation: at the first order with living cell (e.g. neurons or astrocytes), at the second-order with the meta-cellular organism and its nervous system, and even at the third order of the social interactions between two embodied agents (cf. Fig.7). Mind is indeed not brain-bound and rather encompasses the body, through the regulation of its organismic cycles, the environment with sensorimotor couplings and other cognitive agents through interpersonal interactions (Thompson and Varela, 2001).
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Acebron JA, Bonilla LL, Vicente CJP, Ritort F, Spigler R. The Kuramoto model: A simple paradigm for synchronization phenomena. Reviews of Modern Physics. 2005;77(1):137–185. [Google Scholar]
- Akil H, Martone M, Van Essen DC. Challenges and opportunities in mining neuroscience data. Science. 2011;301:708–712. doi: 10.1126/science.1199305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amari S. Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral-inhibition type neural fields. Biological Cybernetics. 1977;27:77–87. doi: 10.1007/BF00337259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Assisi CG, Jirsa VK, Kelso JAS. Synchrony and clustering in heterogeneous networks with global coupling and parameter dispersion. Physical Review Letters. 2005;94:018106. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.94.018106. (on line) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bak P, Tang C, Wiesenfeld K. Self-organized criticality. Physical review A. 1988;38:364–374. doi: 10.1103/physreva.38.364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banerjee A, Jirsa VK. How do neural connectivity and time delays influence bimanual coordination? Biological Cybernetics. 2007;96:265–278. doi: 10.1007/s00422-006-0114-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banerjee A, Tognoli E, Assisi CG, Kelso JAS, Jirsa VK. Mode-level cognitive subtraction (MLCS) identifies spatiotemporal reorganization in large scale brain topographies. NeuroImage. 2008;42:663–674. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banerjee A, Tognoli E, Kelso JAS, Jirsa VK. Spatiotemporal reorganization of large-scale neural assemblies mediates bimanual coordination. NeuroImage. 2012 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.046. doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beggs JM, Timme N. Being critical of criticality in the brain. Frontiers in Physiology. 2012 doi: 10.3389/fphys.2012.00163. doi:10.3389/phys.2012.00163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boonstra TW, Daffertshofer A, Breakspear M, Beek PJ. Multivariate time-frequency analysis of electromagnetic brain activity during bimanual motor learning. Neuroimage. 2007;36:370–377. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breakspear M, Heitmann S, Daffertshofer A. Generative models of cortical oscillations: neurobiological implications of the Kuramoto model. Front Hum Neurosci. 2010;4:190. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bressler SL, Kelso JAS. Cortical coordination dynamics and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2001;5:26–36. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01564-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bressler SL, Freeman WJ. Frequency analysis of olfactory system EEG in cat, rabbit, and rat. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1980;50:19–24. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(80)90319-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briggman KL, Kristan WB. Multifunctional pattern-generating circuits. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;31:271–294. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buzsáki G, Draguhn A. Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science. 2004;304:1926–1929. doi: 10.1126/science.1099745. doi:10.1126/science.1099745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen Y, Ding M, Kelso JAS. Long term memory processes (1/fa type) in human coordination. Physics Review Letters. 1997;79:4501–4504. [Google Scholar]
- Chialvo D. Emergent complex neural dynamics. Nature Physics. 2010;6:744–750. [Google Scholar]
- Coombes S. Neural Fields. Scholarpedia. 2006;1(6):1373. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.1373. [Google Scholar]
- Crick FHC. Scientific American. W.H. Freeman; New York: 1979. Thinking about the brain. (September issue) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daffertshofer A, Peper CE, Beek PJ. Stabilization of bimanual coordination due to active interhemispheric inhibition: a dynamical account. Biological Cybernetics. 2005;92:101–109. doi: 10.1007/s00422-004-0539-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deco G, Jirsa VK, Robinson PA, Breakspear M, Friston K. The Dynamic Brain: From Spiking Neurons to Neural Masses and Cortical Fields. PLoS Comp Biol. 2008;4:e1000092. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000092. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000092.t001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ding M, Chen Y, Kelso JAS. Statistical analysis of timing errors. Brain and Cognition. 2002;48:98–106. doi: 10.1006/brcg.2001.1306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumas G, Chavez M, Nadel J, Martinerie J. Anatomical Connectivity Influences both Intra- and Inter-Brain Synchronizations. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(5):e36414. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036414. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumas G, Nadel J, Soussignan R, Martinerie J, Garnero L. Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e12166. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012166. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0012166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ermentrout B. Neural networks as spatio-temporal pattern-forming systems. Rep. Prog. Phys. 1998;61:353–430. [Google Scholar]
- Freeman WJ. Mass action in the nervous system. Academic Press; New York: 1975. [Google Scholar]
- Freeman WJ. Tutorial on neurobiology: From single neurons to brain chaos. Inter. Journ. Bif. Chaos. 1992;2:451–482. [Google Scholar]
- Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;9:474–480. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ. Transients, metastability, and neuronal dynamics. NeuroImage. 1997;5:164–171. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0259. doi:10.1006/nimg.1997.0259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, Harrison L, Penny W. Dynamic causal modelling. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1273–1302. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00202-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs A, Jirsa VK, editors. Coordination: Neural, behavioral and social dynamics. Springer; Berlin, Heidelberg: [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs A, Kelso JAS. Movement coordination. In: Meyers RA, editor. Encyclopedia of Complexity and System Science. Springer; Heidelberg: 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs A, Jirsa VK, Kelso JAS. Theory of the relation between human brain activity (MEG) and hand movements. NeuroImage. 2000;11(5):359–69. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grillner S, Graybiel AM. Microcircuits. The MIT Press; Cambridge: MA: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Grossberg S. The complementary brain: A unifying view of brain specialization and modularity. Trends Cog. Sci. 2000;4:233–246. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01464-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haken H. Synergetics, an introduction: Non-equilibrium phase transitions and self-organization in physics, chemistry and biology. Springer; Berlin: 1977/1983. [Google Scholar]
- Haken H, Kelso JAS, Bunz H. A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biol. Cybern. 1985;51:347–356. doi: 10.1007/BF00336922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harkin J, Morgan F, McDaid L, Hall S, McGinley B, Cawley S. A Reconfigurable and Biologically Inspired Paradigm for Computation Using Networks-on-chip and Spiking Neural Networks. International Journal of Reconfigurable Computing. 2009:1–13. Article ID 908740. [Google Scholar]
- Honey CJ, Sporns O. Dynamical consequences of lesions in cortical networks. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;29:802–809. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20579. doi:10.1002/hbm.20579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honey CJ, Sporns O, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Thiran J-P, Meuli R, Hagmann P. Predicting human resting-state functional connectivity from structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106:2035–2040. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811168106. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811168106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houweling S, Beek P, Daffertshofer A. Spectral changes of inter-hemispheric crosstalk during movement instabilities. Cereb. Cortex. 2010;20:2605–2613. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izhikevich EM, Edelman GM. Large-scale model of mammalian thalamocortical systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:3593–3598. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0712231105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James W. The Principles of Psychology. Vol.1. Dover; New York: 1890. [Google Scholar]
- Jantzen KJ, Steinberg FL, Kelso JAS. Coordination dynamics of large-scale neural circuitry underlying sensorimotor behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21:2420–2433. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21182. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.21182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jing M, McGinnity TM, Coleman S, Zhang H, Fuchs A, Kelso JAS. Enhancement of fibre orientation distribution reconstruction in diffusion weighted imaging by single channel blind source separation. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering. 2011 doi: 10.1109/TBME.2011.2172793. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/TBME.2011.2172793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jirsa VK, Haken H. Field theory of electromagnetic brain activity. Phys. Rev. Let. 1996;77:960. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.77.960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jirsa VK, Haken H. A derivation of a macroscopic field theory of the brain from the quasi-microscopic neural dynamics. Physica D. 1997;99:503–526. [Google Scholar]
- Jirsa VK, Kelso JAS. Spatiotemporal Pattern Formation in Continuous Systems with Heterogeneous Connection Topologies. Phys. Rev. E. 2000;62:8462–8465. doi: 10.1103/physreve.62.8462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jirsa VK, Fuchs A, Kelso JAS. Connecting cortical and behavioral dynamics: Bimanual coordination. Neur. Comp. 1998;10:2019–2045. doi: 10.1162/089976698300016954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jirsa VK, Fuchs A, Kelso JAS. Traversing scales of brain and behavioral organization. III. Theoretical modeling. In: Uhl C, editor. Analysis of neurophysiological brain functioning. Springer-Verlag; Berlin: 1999. pp. 107–125. [Google Scholar]
- Jirsa VK, Fuchs A, Jantzen KJ, Kelso JAS. Spatiotemporal forward solution of the EEG and MEG using network modeling. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging. 2002;21:493–504. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2002.1009385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jirsa VK, Jantzen KJ, Fuchs A, Kelso JAS. Neural field dynamics on the folded three dimensionalcortical sheet and its forward EEG and MEG. In: Insana MF, Leahy RM, editors. Information Processing in Medical Imaging. Springer; 2001. pp. 286–299. Lecture Notes in Computer Science. [Google Scholar]
- Jirsa VK, Sporns O, Breakspear M. Towards the virtual brain: network modeling of the intact and the damaged brain. Archives Italiennes de Biologie. 2010;148:189–205. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS, Tognoli E. Toward a complementary neuroscience: Metastable coordination dynamics of the brain. In: Kozma R, Perlovsky L, editors. Neurodynamics of Cognition and Consciousness. Springer; Heidelberg: 2007. pp. 39–60. [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS. On the oscillatory basis of movement. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 1981;18:63. [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS. Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. Am.J. Physiol. 1984;15:R1000–R1004. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1984.246.6.R1000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS. Phase transitions: Foundations of behavior. In: Haken H, editor. Synergetics of cognition. Springer-Verlag; Berlin: 1990. pp. 249–268. [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS. Behavioral and neural pattern generation: The concept of Neurobehavioral Dynamical System (NBDS) In: Koepchen HP, Huopaniemi T, editors. Cardiorespiratory and Motor Coordination. Springer-Verlag; Berlin: 1991. pp. 224–238. [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS. Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. The MIT Press; Cambridge, Massachusetts: 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS. An essay on understanding the mind. Ecological Psychology. 2008;20:180–208. doi: 10.1080/10407410801949297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS. Coordination Dynamics. In: Meyers RA, editor. Encyclopedia of Complexity and System Science. Springer; Heidelberg: 2009. pp. 1537–1564. [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS. Instabilities and phase transitions in human brain and behavior. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2010;4:23. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00023. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS. Multistability and metastability: Understanding dynamic coordination in the brain. Phil. Trans. Royal Society B. 2012;367:906–918. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS, Engstrøm DA. The Complementary Nature. The MIT Press; Cambridge, MA: 2006. Paperback Edition, March 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS, DeGuzman GC, Reveley C, Tognoli E. Virtual Partner Interaction (VPI): Exploring novel behaviors via coordination dynamics. PLoSONE. 2009;4(6):e5749. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS, DelColle J, Schöner G. Action-Perception as a pattern formation process. In: Jeannerod M, editor. Attention and Performance XIII. Erlbaum; Hillsdale, NJ: 1990. pp. 139–169. [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS, Fuchs A, Jirsa VK. Traversing scales of brain and behavioral organization. I. Concepts and experiments. In: Uhl C, editor. Analysis of Neurophysiological Brain Functioning. Springer-Verlag; Berlin: 1999. pp. 73–89. [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS, Fuchs A, Lancaster R, Holroyd T, Cheyne D, Weinberg H. Dynamic cortical activity in the human brain reveals motor equivalence. Nature. 1998;392:814–818. doi: 10.1038/33922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelso JAS. Metastable coordination dynamics of brain and behavior. Brain and Neural Networks (Japan) 2001;8:125–130. [Google Scholar]
- Kitajo K, Miyota R, Shimono M, Yamanaka K, Yamaguchi Y. State-Dependent Cortical Synchronization Networks Revealed by TMS-EEG Recordings. In: Wang R, Gu F, editors. Advances in Cognitive Neurodynamics (II) Springer; 2011. pp. 145–148. doi:10.1007/978-90-481-9695-1_23. [Google Scholar]
- Kitzbichler MG, Smith ML, Christensen SR, Bullmore E. Broadband Criticality of Human Brain Network Synchronization. PLoS Comp Biol. 2009;5:e1000314. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000314. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000314.t001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuramoto Y. Self-entrainment of a population of coupled non-linear oscillators. In: Araki H, editor. Lecture Notes in Physics. Vol. 39. Springer-Verlag; Berlin/Heidelberg: 1975. pp. 420–422. doi:10.1007/BFb0013365. [Google Scholar]
- Laufs H, Kleinschmidt A, Beyerle A, Eger E, Salek-Haddadi A, Preibisch C, Krakow K. EEG-correlated fMRI of human alpha activity. NeuroImage. 2003;19(4):1463–1476. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00286-6. ISSN 1053-8119, doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00286-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laughlin RB, Pines D. The theory of everything. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 2000;97:28–31. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.1.28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marreiros AC, Stephan KE, Friston KJ. Dynamic causal modeling. Scholarpedia. 2010;5(7):9568. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.9568. [Google Scholar]
- Massimini M, Boly M, Casali A, Rosanova M, Tononi G. A perturbational approach for evaluating the brain’s capacity for consciousness. Progress in Brain Research. 2009;177:201–214. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17714-2. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17714-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maturana H, Varela F. The tree of knowledge: The biological roots of human understanding. New Science Library/Shambhala Publications; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Meyer-Lindenberg A, Zieman U, Hajak G, Cohen L, Faith Berman K. Transition between dynamical states of differing stability in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2002;99:10948–10953. doi: 10.1073/pnas.162114799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murzin V, Fuchs A, Kelso JAS. Anatomically constrained minimum variance beamforming applied to EEG. Experimental Brain Research. 2011;214:515–528. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2850-5. DOI: 10.1007/s00221-011-2850-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naeem M, Prasad G, Watson DR, Kelso JAS. Electrophysiological signatures of intentional social coordination in the 10-12Hz range. NeuroImage. 2012;59:1795–1803. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.010. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nunez PL. The brain wave equation: A model for the EEG. Mathematical Biosciences. 1974;21:279–297. [Google Scholar]
- Nunez PL. Neocortical dynamics and human EEG rhythms. Oxford University Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Oullier O, Kelso JAS. Social coordination from the perspective of coordination dynamics. In: Meyers RA, editor. Encyclopedia of Complexity and Systems Science. Springer; Heidelberg: 2009. pp. 8198–8212. [Google Scholar]
- Oullier O, Lagarde J, Jantzen KJ, Kelso JAS. Dynamiques comportementale et cérébrale des coordinations sensorimotrices : (in)stabilite et metastabilite rythmiques. Journal de la Société de Biologie. 2006;200(2):145–167. doi: 10.1051/jbio:2006017. [Medline Version : Coordination Dynamics : (in)stability and metastability in behavioural and neural systems. J Soc Biol, 200(2), 145-167] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palva JM, Palva S. Roles of multiscale brain activity fluctuations in shaping the variability and dynamics of psychophysical performance. Prog. Brain. Res. 2011;193:335–350. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53839-0.00022-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pattee HH. Instabilities and Information in Biological Self-organization. In: Yates FE, editor. Self-Organizing Systems: The Emergence of Order. Plenum Press; New York: 1987. pp. 325–338. [Google Scholar]
- Pinotsis DA, Friston KJ. Neural fields, spectral responses and lateral connections. NeuroImage. 2011;55:39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.081. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinotsis DA, Moran RJ, Friston KJ. Dynamic causal modeling with neural fields. NeuroImage. 2012;59(2):1261–1274. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.020. ISSN 1053-8119, 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plenz D, Thiagarian T. The organizing principles of neuronal avalanche activity: Cell assemblies and cortex. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:101–110. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prinz AA, Bucher D, Marder E. Similar network activity from disparate circuit parameters. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:1345–1352. doi: 10.1038/nn1352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pomeau Y, Manneville P. Intermittent transition to turbulence in dissipative dynamical systems. Communications in Mathematical Physics. 1980;74:189–197. [Google Scholar]
- Qubbaj MR, Jirsa VK. Neural field dynamics with heterogeneous connection topology. Phys. Rev. Letts. 2007;98:238102-1–4. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.98.238102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson PA, Rennie CA, Wright JJ. Propagation and stability of waves of electrical activity in the cerebral cortex. Phys. Rev. E. 1997;56:826–840. doi:10.1103/PhysRevE.56.826. [Google Scholar]
- Roopun AK. Temporal interactions between cortical rhythms. Front. Neurosci. 2008;2:145–154. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.034.2008. doi:10.3389/neuro.01.034.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schöner G, Kelso JAS. Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems. Science. 1988;239:1513–1520. doi: 10.1126/science.3281253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]; Kelner KL, Koshland DE Jr., editors. Molecules to Models: Advances in Neuroscience. :311–325. Reprinted in. [Google Scholar]
- Schöner G, Haken H, Kelso JAS. A stochastic theory of phase transitions in human hand movement. Biological Cybernetics. 1986;53:247–257. doi: 10.1007/BF00336995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shanahan M. Metastable chimera states in community-structured oscillator networks. Chaos. 2010;20:013108. doi: 10.1063/1.3305451. doi:10.1063/1.3305451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sporns O, Tononi G. Classes of network connectivity and dynamics. Complexity. 2002;7:28–38. [Google Scholar]
- Sporns O. Complex neural dynamics. In: Jirsa VK, Kelso JAS, editors. Coordination dynamics: Issues and trends. Springer-Verlag; Berlin: 2004. pp. 197–215. [Google Scholar]
- Sporns R. The human connectome: a structural description of the human brain. PLoS Computational Biology. 2005;1(4):e42. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0010042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stamoulis C, Oberman L, Praeg E, Bashir S, Pascual-Leone A. Single Pulse TMS-Induced Modulations of Resting Brain Neurodynamics Encoded in EEG Phase. Brain Topography. 2011;24(2):105–113. doi: 10.1007/s10548-010-0169-3. doi:10.1007/s10548-010-0169-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stefanescu RA, Jirsa VK. A low dimensional description of globally coupled heterogeneous neural networks of excitatory and inhibitory neurons. PLoS Comp. Biol. 2008;4:e1000219. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000219. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000219.g014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson E, Varela FJ. Radical embodiment: neural dynamics and consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2001;5(10):418–425. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01750-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tognoli E, Kelso JAS. Brain coordination dynamics: true and false faces of phase synchrony and metastability. Progress in Neurobiology. 2009;87:31–40. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.014. Available online doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tognoli E. EEG coordination dynamics: neuromarkers of social coordination. In: Fuchs A, Jirsa VK, editors. Coordination: Neural, Behavioral and Social Dynamics. Springer; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Tognoli E, Kelso JAS. On the brain’s dynamical complexity: coupling and causal influences across spatiotemporal scales. In: Yamaguchi Y, editor. Advances in Cognitive Neurodynamics (III) Springer Netherlands; Dordrecht: (in press) [Google Scholar]
- Tognoli E, Lagarde J, DeGuzman GC, Kelso JAS. The phi complex as a neuromarker of human social coordination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2007;104:8190–8195. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0611453104. (from the cover; see also Scientific American Mind, August, 2007) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tononi G, Sporns O, Edelman GM. A measure for brain complexity: Relating functional segregation and integration in the nervous system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1994;91:5033–5037. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.11.5033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsuda I, Koerner E, Shimizu H. Memory dynamics in asynchronous neural networks. Progress of Theoretical Physics. 1987;78:51–71. [Google Scholar]
- Tsuda I. Toward an interpretation of dynamic neural activity in terms of chaotic dynamical systems. Behav Brain Sci. 2001;24:793–810. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x01000097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tuller B, Kelso JAS. Environmentally-specified patterns of movement coordination in normal and split-brain subjects. Exp. Brain Res. 1989;75:306–316. doi: 10.1007/BF00247936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uhlhaas PJ, Pipa G, Lima B, Melloni L, Neuenschwander S, Nikolić D, Singer W. Neural synchrony in cortical networks: History, concept and current status. Front Integr Neurosci. 2009;3:17. doi: 10.3389/neuro.07.017.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varela F, Lachaux J-P, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J. The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001;2:229–239. doi: 10.1038/35067550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Von der Malsburg C, Phillips WA, Singer W, editors. Dynamic Coordination in the Brain: From Neurons to Mind, Strüngmann Forum Report. vol. 5. MIT Press; Cambridge, MA: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Wade JJ, McDaid LJ, Harkin JG, Crunelli V, Kelso JAS. Bidirectional coupling between astrocytes and neurons mediates learning and dynamic coordination in the brain: A multiple modeling approach. PLoSONE. 2011a;6(12):e29445. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029445. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0029445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wade JJ, McDaid LJ, Harkin JG, Crunelli V, Kelso JAS. Exploring retrograde signaling via astrocytes as a mechanism for self-repair. IEEE Proceedings of International Joint Conference on Neural Networks.2011b. pp. 3149–3155. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson HR, Cowan JD. Excitatory and inhibitory interactions in localized populations of model neurons. Biophys. Journ. 1972;12:1–24. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(72)86068-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson HR, Cowan JD. A mathematical theory of the functional dynamics of cortical and thalamic nervous tissue. Kybernetik. 1973;13:55–80. doi: 10.1007/BF00288786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright JJ, Liley DTJ. Simulation of Electrocortical Waves. Biol. Cybern. 1995;72:347–356. doi: 10.1007/BF00202790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]