Abstract
Craving is a key aspect of drug dependence that is thought to motivate continued drug use. Numerous brain regions have been associated with craving, suggesting that craving is mediated by a distributed brain network. Whether an increase in subjective craving is associated with enhanced interactions among brain regions was evaluated using resting state functional magnetic imaging (fMRI) in nicotine dependent participants. We focused on craving-related changes in the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex (OMPFC) network, which also included the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (sgACC) extending into the ventral striatum. Brain regions in the OMPFC network are not only implicated in addiction and reward, but, due to their rich anatomic interconnections, may serve as the site of integration across craving-related brain regions. Subjective craving and resting state fMRI were evaluated twice with an ∼1 hour delay between the scans. Cigarette craving was significantly increased at the end, relative to the beginning of the scan session. Enhanced craving was associated with heightened coupling between the OMPFC network and other cortical, limbic, striatal, and visceromotor brain regions that are both anatomically interconnected with the OMPFC, and have been implicated in addiction and craving. This is the first demonstration confirming that an increase in craving is associated with enhanced brain region interactions, which may play a role in the experience of craving.
Introduction
Craving, or the strong desire to use an abused substance, is a key component of drug addiction, and is a motivator for drug use [1], [2]. To understand craving, research has focused on identifying the neurobiological substrates underlying the desire to use drugs. However, craving is linked with a number of brain regions, such as those involved in primary drug reward, habitual drug use, memory, and reward based-decision making [3]–[8]. Thus, it is hypothesized that a distributed brain network mediates craving instead of a single localized region [9]. An increase in craving may, therefore, involve enhanced brain region interactions that facilitate integration of information across these disparate brain areas.
The relationship between craving and brain-network interactions can be evaluated using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data collected at rest. While previous task-related fMRI research has identified discrete brain regions associated with craving [6], [8], resting-state fMRI allows for the evaluation of brain function at a more distributed network level. During rest, brain regions with highly correlated fluctuations in blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signals are defined as functional networks [10], called resting state networks (RSNs), which are thought to reflect intrinsic functional brain organization [11], [12]. Subsequent research has revealed that RSNs are associated with known brain systems related to cognition, perception, and reward [13], [14]. To evaluate associations between craving and network-level brain changes, we collected resting-state fMRI and subjective craving data in nicotine-dependent smokers at two time points approximately one hour apart. Over this one-hour time period, participants reported a significant rise in craving as measured by the brief questionnaire of smoking urges (QSU) [15].
We focused on the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex (OMPFC) network, which is a previously defined RSN [13], [16] comprised of anatomically interconnected brain regions [17] involved in the mesocorticolimbic reward circuit. Specifically, the OMPFC network contains the medial and orbital prefrontal cortex (mPFC, OFC), and the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex (sgACC) extending into the ventral striatum. We focused on this network as these prefrontal and striatal regions are implicated in craving [4], [7], [18] drug reinforcement [19], [20], and reward processing [21]. Additionally, the OFC is thought to be a multimodal integration area leading to hedonic experience [22]. In addition to the direct link between OMPFC brain regions and craving, this network may act as a hub [23] where information is integrated to facilitate the subjective experience of craving. To confirm the idea that a distributed brain network is associated with craving, we hypothesize that interactions between the OMPFC network and other craving-related brain regions will increase along with a rise in the subjective experience of craving.
Methods
Participants
Seventeen nicotine-dependent smokers (8 men/9 women) were studied: 25.4±4.6 (mean ± standard deviation) years old with 15.3±2.1 years of education and 6.7±4.7 pack-years of smoking use (pack years = number of packs of cigarettes smoked/day x years as a smoker). Participants had an average Fagerström test for nicotine dependence (FTND) [24] score of 6.3±1.0, which confirmed moderate to severe nicotine dependence. Participants also reported smoking ≥10 cigarettes/day over the past 6 months. The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) was used to assess all participants for current nicotine dependence and to exclude those with a lifetime diagnosis of the following conditions: organic mental disorder, bipolar or unipolar depression, or schizophrenia spectrum disorder. Participants also were excluded for pregnancy, current psychotropic drug use or recent alcohol use (Alco-Sensor FST, Intoximeters, Inc.). No participants consumed any alcohol prior to the study as indicated by a blood alcohol level of 0. Additionally, no subjects met criteria for alcohol abuse or dependence. Recruitment was conducted using online advertisements and fliers posted in the Boston area. All participants gave written informed consent prior to participating in the study and the institutional review board at McLean Hospital approved this study and consent procedure.
Functional Neuroimaging
All participants smoked one of their own cigarettes immediately following signing the informed consent to standardize the time since a cigarette was last smoked. MRI scanning began approximately 1.5 h after smoking this cigarette. Scans were acquired on a Siemens Trio 3 Tesla scanner (Erlangen, Germany) with a 32-channel head coil. Multiecho multiplanar rapidly acquired gradient-echo (ME-MPRAGE) structural images were acquired with the following parameters (TR = 2.1 s, TE 3.3 ms, slices = 128, matrix = 256×256, flip angle = 7°, resolution = 1.0 mm×1.0 mm×1.33 mm), and gradient echo echo-planar images were acquired using the following parameters (TR = 2.5 s, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, slices = 42, voxel size = 3.5 mm isotropic). Slices were acquired aligned to the anterior and posterior commissure and the phase encode direction was set to acquire from the posterior to anterior direction to prevent prefrontal signal loss. During the 6-minute resting state fMRI scans participants were asked to remain awake with their eyes open. Two resting state scans were acquired approximately 1 h apart.
fMRI Pre-processing
All data analysis was conducted using tools from the Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Brain (FMRIB) Software Library (FSL; www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Functional data pre-processing included: motion correction with MCFLIRT, brain extraction using BET, slice timing correction, spatial smoothing with a Gaussian kernel of full-width half-maximum 6 mm, and high-pass temporal filter with Gaussian-weighted least-squares straight-line fitting with σ = 100 s. Subject specific data was registered to the MNI152 2 mm3 standard space template (Montreal Neurological Institute, Montreal, QC, Canada) using FLIRT and the fMRI data was transformed into standard space at 2×2×2 mm resolution using the registration transformation matrices.
fMRI Resting-State Independent Components Analysis
To identify resting state networks common to all participants, the data from all subjects were temporally concatenated and a multivariate group probabilistic ICA (PICA) was conducted using FSL MELODIC [25], [26]. Consistent with our prior work [16], the dimensionality was fixed to 35 to investigate large-scale RSNs. To ensure stable convergence of the ICA, the ICA was run 8 times followed by a meta-level ICA fed by all of the spatial maps from the 8 decompositions [14]. This meta ICA was conducted to identify the set of independent components common to all subjects. These components included, common resting state networks, which have been identified elsewhere [13], [14] such as the default mode, salience, fronto-parietal, motor, and visual networks. Artifact-related components were also identified. Through comparison with brain networks reported previously [13], [14], [16] the resulting independent component maps were visually inspected to identify the OMPFC network (see Fig. 1, green shading).
Dual Regression
To calculate subject specific time courses and spatial maps, we used a dual regression approach [27]–[29]. In the first stage of dual regression, the full set of group ICs, which include all RSNs and artifact components, are used in a multiple spatial regression against each individual subject's dataset to estimate the average time course of voxels in each RSN. By including all of the ICs in the multiple regression, any voxel with contributions from multiple signal sources (for example, from coupling with an RSN and from motion effects) will have these effects partialled out into their separate contributions by the multiple regression prior to the averaging. The subject-specific time courses were normalized to unit variance and then used in a second multiple regression against the individual subject's dataset, to identify voxels correlated with each of the RSN time courses, thus identifying the spatial map of each RSN unique to the subject. To evaluate a change in functional connectivity between the two resting state acquisitions, difference maps were calculated by subtracting the individual subject specific spatial maps for the second minus the first resting state session.
Resting State Associations with Craving and Carbon Monoxide
Just prior to the first resting state scan and just after the second resting state scan, craving was measured by the brief 10-item Questionnaire of Smoking urges [15]. Expired carbon monoxide (CO; Micro Smokerlyzer II, Bedfont Scientific Instruments) was also measured at these two time periods. Significant differences in craving and CO at these two time periods were assessed using a paired t-test. Changes in craving and CO were calculated by subtracting the second (post-scan) minus the first (pre-scan) measurements. Changes in craving and CO were correlated with the RSN difference maps using non-parametric permutation testing using 5,000 permutations (FSL Randomize) [30]. Multiple comparisons were corrected to p<0.05 using cluster-based thresholding where the cluster-forming threshold was Z = 2.3 [31].
Results
Craving and Carbon monoxide
Craving, measured by the QSU, significantly increased following the second resting state scan (t16 = −3.3, p<0.01; pre 22.5±8.2, post 30.2±10.2). Expired CO levels significantly dropped from pre (26.9±12.3 ppm) to post (18.6±8 ppm) scanning (t16 = 6.3, p<0.01). There was no relationship between the change in craving and CO, nor was the difference in craving or CO associated with age, pack-year, or FTND score.
Identified resting state network
The OMPFC network common to all subjects included the ventromedial PFC (Broadmann area (BA) 10), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC; BA 11), subgenual ACC (BA 24, 32), and the ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens extending into the adjacent caudate. (Figure 1, green shading). This definition of the OMPFC network visually overlapped with our previous work [16] and the work of others [13].
Craving associations with RSNs
No associations were found between the first assessment of craving and the first RSN measure, nor were associations found between the second craving assessment and the second RSN measure. A positive correlation was found between the change in craving (second – first) and the OMPFC RSN difference maps (second – first). As craving rose, increased connectivity was found between the OMPFC network difference map and several brain regions including: left dorsal regions of the superior frontal gyrus (BA 10) extending into the dACC (BA 24, 32) and left frontal pole (BA 10), bilateral supplementary motor area (BA 6), bilateral ventral striatum, bilateral caudate, bilateral ventral occipital cortex (BA 18, 19), right thalamus, right hippocampus and parahippicampal gyrus, and left superior cerebellum (left crus 1 and left lobule VI; Figure 1, orange shading; related correlation plot, Figure 2; Table 1).
Table 1. Brain regions showing enhanced network coupling with the OMPFC network as craving increases.
Brain Area | Brodmann Area | X | Y | Z | P-Max | Voxels |
Bilateral parahippocampal gyrus, bilateral occipital cortex, left cerebellum, right hippocampus | 18, 19 | −38 | −62 | −36 | 0.02 | 2507 |
Bilateral anterior ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens extending into the caudate), right mid to posterior caudate | 12 | 22 | −20 | 0.03 | 2242 | |
Supplementary motor area, left frontal pole, superior frontal gyrus, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex | 6, 10, 24, 32 | −10 | 54 | −18 | 0.03 | 2190 |
Brain areas and Brodmann area refer to the location of each cluster of contiguous voxels. MNI coordinates (X, Y, Z) refer to the region of maximum coupling for each cluster. P-max refers to the maximum P-statistic in each cluster (pcluster corrected <0.05). Voxels refers to the total number of voxels within the cluster.
Carbon Monoxide associations with RSNs
No associations were found between CO and any of the RSN measurements.
Discussion
The current results identify that a relationship exists between increased subjective craving and enhanced coupling between the OMPFC network and several other brain regions. This association was noted only when evaluating the change in craving as no interaction was found between craving and RSN measures at either time point one or two alone. This suggests that stable levels of craving are not related to network functional connectivity strength, but experiencing a rise in craving is accompanied by greater OMPFC network coupling. While others have evaluated the relationship between nicotine withdrawal symptoms and the default mode and executive control networks [28], this is the first focused evaluation of craving on reward-related brain networks. As we found no correlation between the decrease in expired CO and OMPFC network coupling, the relationship between craving and changes in connectivity cannot be attributed to physiological changes due to a drop in CO.
There is a strong link between the OMPFC network and craving as regions within this network, which include the vmPFC, OFC, sgACC, and striatum, activate during craving [4], [32]. Several, but not all studies also report deactivation of these regions during the regulation of craving [4], [32]–[35]. Supporting the idea that OMPFC regions act as a craving-related functional unit, Hanlon and colleagues [4] showed that this entire cluster of brain regions activates during tobacco craving. More broadly the OMPFC network may be involved in reward evaluation during the experience of craving, as OMPFC network regions are active during both reward anticipation and reward delivery [36]–[40].
In the present study, increased subjective craving paralleled enhanced coupling between the OMPFC network and brain regions that typically activate when drug users are exposed to drug-associated cues; including dorsal regions of the mPFC [41], the hippocampus [42], visual areas [41], [43], sensory motor regions [44], the striatum [7], [45], and cerebellum [46]. This co-activation of prefrontal and other cortical, limbic, striatal, and visceromotor areas led London and colleagues [18] to speculate that there is an “interplay of related networks” during drug-cue exposure that may correspond with craving. Our findings confirm that a rise in subjective craving is correlated with enhanced coupling between brain regions associated with craving, which are also anatomically connected with the OMPFC [17]. These rich interconnections with OMPFC brain regions are thought to form a sensory-visceromotor link to guide reward-related behavior and give rise to hedonic experience [17], [22]. Thus, it is possible that the integration of information between these regions may play a role in the experience of craving as well as guiding smoking behavior.
In the context of the current study, we are unable to directly determine whether there is a link between increased brain network coupling and behavior. However, as craving intensity peaks prior to relapse [47], it is tempting to speculate that that the enhanced coupling between brain regions associated with increased craving facilitates smoking-related behavior. Increased craving-related coupling between the OMPFC network and regions such as the supplementary motor area (SMA) supports the notion that the observed network-interactions may have an impact on behavior. Not only does the SMA activate to smoking-related cues [44], but SMA neurons fire prior to hand movements, which is thought to facilitate psychomotor responses to an object [48]. Thus, the interaction between these brain regions implicated in craving and behavior may actually be involved in the process of “preparing to smoke”. Alternatively, as smokers were in the scanner and unable to smoke ad libitum, when they had the immediate desire, the involvement of brain regions such as the SMA may regulate smoking behavior as the SMA also is implicated in the inhibition of action [49].
Future directions not only include linking network changes with behavior, but also defining the neurotransmitter systems mediating brain network connectivity. For instance, greater midbrain dopamine D3 receptor availability is positively correlated with enhanced coupling between the OMPFC network and brain structures such as the striatum and OFC [50]. In addition, D3 receptors play a strong role in nicotine-seeking and are a promising target for nicotine cessation treatment [51]–[53]. These studies suggest that future research should focus on the role of D3 receptors in craving-related OMPFC network coupling. Given that D3 receptors influence other addictive disorders [54], these future studies should be expanded to include craving-related network changes for nicotine and other abused substances. While we found no relationship between craving and factors such as age and level of nicotine dependence, these factors should also be studied more directly as our work focused specifically on relatively young and heavily nicotine dependent smokers. Finally, while we speculate that OMPFC brain regions may act as an integrative hub [23], future studies involving alternative connectivity methods should focus on identifying the directionality of information flow between craving-related brain structures.
Our results confirm that increased tobacco craving is associated with enhanced interactions between reward- and craving-related brain regions. This rise in craving over time was not related to baseline measures of smoking history, nor was the change in brain interactions due to a decrease in expired CO, indicating that changes in OMPFC coupling are specifically related to enhanced subjective craving. The relationship between changes in craving and RSN connectivity indicate that brain network interactions are associated with changes in subjective state.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse Grant K01DA029645. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1. Robinson TE, Berridge KC (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 18: 247–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Sinha R (2013) The clinical neurobiology of drug craving. Curr Opin Neurobiol 23: 649–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Fanous S, Goldart EM, Theberge FRM, Bossert JM, Shaham Y, et al. (2012) Role of orbitofrontal cortex neuronal ensembles in the expression of incubation of heroin craving. J Neurosci 34: 11600–11609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Hanlon CA, Hartwell KJ, Canterberry M, Li X, Owens M, et al. (2013) Reduction of cue-induced craving through realtime neurofeedback in nicotine users: The role of region of interest selection and multiple visits. Psychiatry Res 213: 79–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Heinz A, Siessmeier T, Wrase J, Hermann D, Klein S, et al. (2004) Correlation between dopamine D2 receptors in the ventral striatum and central processing of alcohol cues and craving. Am J Psychiatry 161: 1783–1789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. McClernon JF, Kozink RV, Lutz AM, Rose JE (2009) 24-h smoking abstinence potentiates fMRI-BOLD activation to smoking cues in cerebral cortex and dorsal striatum. Psychopharmacology 204: 25–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, et al. (2006) Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: Mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction. J Neurosci 26: 6583–6588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Wang Z, Faith M, Patterson F, Tang K, Kerrin K, et al. (2007) Neural substrates of abstinence-induced cigarette cravings in chronic smokers. J Neurosci 27: 14035–1404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Koob GF, Volkow ND (2009) Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 35: 217–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Fox MD, Raichel ME (2007) Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci 8: 70–711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Greicius M (2008) Resting-state functional connectivity in neuropsychiatric disorders. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21: 424–430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Vincent JL, Patel GH, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Baker JT, et al. (2007) Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature 447: 83–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Laird AR, Fox PM, Eickhoff SB, Turner JA, Ray KL, et al. (2011) Behavioral interpretations of intrinsic connectivity networks. J Cog Neurosci 23: 4022–4037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, et al. (2009) Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A 106: 13040–13045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Cox LS, Tiffany ST, Christen AG (2001) Evaluation of the brief questionnaire of smoking urges (QSU-brief) in laboratory and clinical settings. Nicotine Tob Res 3: 7–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Janes AC, Nickerson LD, Frederick BdeB, Kaufman MJ (2012) Prefrontal and limbic resting state brain network functional connectivity differs between nicotine-dependent smokers and non-smoking controls. Drug Alcohol Depend 125: 252–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Ongur D, Price JL (2000) The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb Cortex 10: 206–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. London ED, Ernst M, Grant S, Bonson K, Weinstein A (2000) Orbitofrontal cortex and human drug abuse: Functional imaging. Cereb Cortex 10: 334–342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Di Chiara G, Imperato A (1988) Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 85: 5274–5278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Koob GF, Bloom FE (1988) Cellular and molecular mechanisms of drug dependence. Science 242: 715–723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Bray S, Shimojo S, O'Doherty JP (2010) Human medial orbitofrontal cortex is recruited during experience of imagined and real rewards. J Neurophysiol 103: 2506–2512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Kringelbach ML (2005) The human orbitofrontal cortex: Linking reward to hedonic experience. Nat Rev Neurosci 6: 691–702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Buckner RL, Sepulcre J, Talukdar T, Krienen FM, Liu H, et al. (2009) Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: Mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer's Disease. J Neurosci 29: 1860–1873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Fagerström KO (1978) Measuring degree of physical dependence to tobacco smoking with reference to individualization of treatment. Addict Behav 3: 235–241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Beckmann CF, DeLuca M, Devlin JT, Smith SM (2005) Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B: Biol Sci 360: 1001–1013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Beckmann CF, Smith SM (2004) Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 23: 137–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Beckmann CF, Mackay CE, Filippini N, Smith SM (2009) Group comparison of resting-state FMRI data using multi-subject ICA and dual regression. Neuroimage 47: S148. [Google Scholar]
- 28. Cole DM, Beckmann CF, Long CJ, Matthews PM, Durcan MJ, et al. (2010) Nicotine replacement in abstinent smokers improves cognitive withdrawal symptoms with modulation of resting brain network dynamics. Neuroimage 52: 590–599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Filippini N, MacIntosh BJ, Hough MG, Goodwin GM, Frisoni GB, et al. (2009) Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carries of the APOE-epsilon4 allele. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106: 7209–7214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Nichols TE, Holmes AP (2002) Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum Brain Mapp 15: 1–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Hayasaka S, Nichols TE (2003) Validating cluster size inference: random field and permutation methods. NeuroImage 20: 2343–2356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang G, Telang F, Logan J, et al. (2010) Cognitive control of drug craving inhibits brain reward regions in cocaine users. Neuroimage 49: 2536–2543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Brody AL, Mandelkern MA, Olmstead RE, Jou J, Tiongson E, et al. (2007) neural substrates of resisting craving during cigarette cue exposure. Biol Psychiatry 62: 642–651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Hartwell KJ, Johnson KA, Li X, Myrick H, LeMatty T, et al. (2011) Neural correlates of craving and resisting craving for tobacco in nicotine dependent smokers. Addict Biol 16: 654–666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Kober H, Mende-Siedlecki P, Kross EF, Weber J, Mischel W, et al. (2010) Prefrontal-striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107: 14811–14816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Breiter HC, Aharon I, Kahnerman D, Dale A, Shizgal P (2001) Functional imaging of neural responses to expectancy and experience of monetary gains and losses. Neuron 30: 619–639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Delgado MR, Miller MM, Inati S, Phelps EA (2005) An fMRI study of reward-related probability learning. Neuroimage 24: 862–873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Liu X, Powell DK, Wang H, Gold BT, Corbly CR, et al. (2007) Functional dissociation in frontal and striatal areas for processing of positive and negative reward information. J Neurosci 27: 4587–4597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Rogers DR, Rammani N, Mackay C, Wilson JL, Jezzard P, et al. (2004) Distinct portions of anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex are activated by reward processing in separable phases of decision-making cognition. Biol Psychiatry 55: 594–602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Shultz W, Tremblay L, Hollerman JR (2000) Reward processing in primate orbitofrontal cortex and basal ganglia. Cereb Cortex 10: 272–284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Janes AC, Smoller JW, David SP, Frederick BdeB, Haddad S, et al. (2012b) Association between CHRNA5 genetic variation at rs16969968 and brain reactivity to smoking images in nicotine dependent women. Drug Alcohol Depend 120: 7–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Grant S, London ED, Newlin DB, Villemagne VL, Liu X, et al. (1996) Activation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93: 12040–12045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Due DL, Huettel SA, Hall WG, Rubin DC (2002) Activation in mesolimbic and visuospatial neural circuits elicited by smoking cues: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Am J Psychiatry 159: 954–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Yalachkov Y, Kaiser J, Naumer MK (2012) Functional neuroimaging studies in addiction: multisensory drug stimuli and neural cue reactivity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36: 825–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. David SP, Munafo MR, Johansen-Berg H, Smith SM, Rogers RD, et al. (2005) Ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens activation to smoking-related pictorial cues in smokers and non-smokers: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 15: 488–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Maas LC, Lukas SE, Kaufman MJ, Weiss RD, Daniels SL, et al. (1998) Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. Am J Psychiatry 155: 124–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Allen SS, Bade T, Hatsukami D (2008) Center B (2008) Craving, withdrawal, and smoking urges on days immediately prior to smoking relapse. Nicotine Tob Res. 10: 35–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Nachev P, Kennard C, Husain M (2008) Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nat Rev Neurosci 9: 856–869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Boy F, Husain M, Singh KD, Summer P (2010) Supplementary motor area activations in unconscious inhibition of voluntary action. Exp Brain Re 206: 441–448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Cole DM, Beckman CF, Searle GC, Plisson C, Tziortzi AC, et al. (2012) Orbitofrontal connectivity with resting-state networks is associated with midbrain dopamine D3 receptor availability. Cereb Cortex 22: 2784–2793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Andreoli M, Tessaro M, Pilla M, Valerio E, Hagan J, et al. (2003) Selective antagonism at dopamine D3 receptors prevents nicotine-triggered relapse to nicotine-seeking behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 28: 1272–1280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Heidbreder CA, Newman AH (2010) Current perspectives on selective dopamine D3 receptor antagonists as pharmacotherapeutics for addictions and related disorders. Ann NY Acad Sci 1187: 4–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Le Foll B, Diaz J, Sokoloff P (2003) Increased dopamine D3 receptor expression accompanying behavioral sensitization to nicotine in rats. Synapse 47: 176–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Sokoloff P, Le Foll B, Perachon S, Bordet R, Ridray S, et al. (2001) The Dopamine D3 receptor and drug addiction. Neurtox Res 3: 433–441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]