For the past 35 years, clinicians and researchers in the United States have utilized essentially the same diagnostic system for the purposes of describing patients' symptomatic presentations.
Having common diagnostic definitions for both research and clinical practice has had a number of advantages. It has made possible the transfer of information between the ever growing clinical research literature and clinical practice. Because the same criteria are used for diagnosing patients in both settings, it is easier to translate findings of a research paper to the diagnosis and treatment of the next patient that one might see in an office practice. This approach also ensures greater clarity of communication within and among areas of psychiatric practice. Most importantly, this approach facilitates the necessary dialogue and mutual influence between clinicians and researchers.
Recognizing the value of operationalized diagnostic criteria for facilitating communication among clinicians and researchers and improving the reliability of diagnostic assessment, in 1980 the American Psychiatric Association adopted diagnostic criteria as the centerpiece of the DSM-III classification. The expectation was that, in addition to improving clinical assessment, they would be widely adopted by the research community.
Subsequently, most of the psychiatric research literature since DSM-III has been keyed to DSM categories, thus facilitating its application to clinical practice. The hope was that iterative refinement of the diagnostic criteria sets through successive validation studies would eventually elucidate their underlying etiologies 1,2. However, despite years of intensive investigation, researchers using the current DSM paradigm have “failed to identify a single neurobiological phenotypic marker or gene that is useful in making a diagnosis of a major psychiatric disorder” (3, p. 33). While much of this lack of success reflects the enormous complexity and relative inaccessibility of the human brain 4, undoubtedly a major contributor is the fact that the DSM categories are a poor mirror of nature.
Although it has become increasingly evident to researchers over the past 20 years that the DSM categories do not represent valid disease entities, the entrenched hegemony of the DSM system and the conservative nature of review processes has led to researchers being pressured to use the DSM-IV categories “in order to satisfy most grant-making bodies, journal reviewers and editors, and organizers of scientific meetings” (5, p. 156).
One of the main goals of the National institute of Mental Health's RDoC project is to release the research community from the shackles of the DSM/ICD categorical system by providing an alternative framework for conducting research in terms of fundamental circuit-based behavior dimensions. Given its role as the premier governmental body funding psychiatric research in the United States, the NIMH is uniquely positioned to incentivize researchers to adopt such a framework and thus it is likely that most NIMH-funded research over the next decade will adopt the RDoC framework.
While this has the potential to be a positive step that facilitates the development of the requisite research literature “to attain groundbreaking nosological approaches in the future that are based upon genetics, other aspects of neurobiology, and behavioral science” 6, it has the potential drawback of impeding clinicians' ability to make clinical sense of such research and apply it to their patients, whose clinical presentations will likely continue for the foreseeable future to be thought of in terms of the DSM/ICD-type categories.
Indeed, one of the central thrusts of RDoC is to discourage the use of the DSM/ICD syndromal constructs by researchers in either research design or subject selection, except insofar as is necessary during the research community's “transition” from the DSM/ICD to RDoC. As noted by Cuthbert, many if not most of the symptoms that form the basis for DSM psychiatric assessment and treatment do not appear in the RDoC matrix, impeding clinicians' ability to relate to RDoC-themed research studies.
So what can be done to mitigate this situation? Although in his paper Cuthbert repeatedly discusses the need for a “transition” from DSM/ICD to RDoC and provides concrete suggestions for how this may be done (e.g., incorporating “various combinations of RDoC constructs and DSM/ICD disorder categories in experiments”), according to Cuthbert such “transitional research designs are best regarded as temporary heuristics for a limited number of studies”.
Rather than viewing the retention of elements of the DSM/ICD system as heuristics to be phased out as soon as possible, it should be a required part of any RDoC-oriented research project to provide linkages or crosswalks between the RDoC design and the DSM/ICD classifications. At a minimum, study populations used in RDoC-themed protocols should also be described in terms of DSM-5/ICD-11 diagnoses, if for no other reason than to provide a touchstone to the clinician for appreciating the types of subjects included in the study.
For example, according to Cuthbert, a “prototypical RDoC design… would include subjects with a wide range of normal-to-impaired functioning with respect to the dimensional constructs of interest”. It would be relatively straightforward to diagnostically assess these subjects, not for the purposes of the experimental design but to characterize the study population in terms understandable by clinicians.
Only by explicitly building bridges between the DSM/ICD and RDoC worlds can the field continue to promote some level of communication and interaction between clinicians and researchers.
References
- 1.Robins E, Guze S. Establishment of diagnostic validity in psychiatric illness: its application to schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1970;126:983–7. doi: 10.1176/ajp.126.7.983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Spitzer R, Williams J. Having a dream: a research strategy for DSM-IV. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;46:959–60. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800330105013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Charney D, Barlow D, Botteron K. Neuroscience research agenda to guide development of a pathophysiologically based classification system. In: Kupfer D, First M, Regier D, et al., editors. A research agenda for DSM-V. Washington: American Psycihiatric Association; 2002. pp. 31–84. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hyman S. Mental illnesses: genetically complex disorders of neural circuitry and neural communication. Neuron. 2000;28:321–3. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00110-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hyman SE. The diagnosis of mental disorders: the problem of reification. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;27:155–79. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Cuthbert BN. The RDoC framework: facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry. 2014;13:28–35. doi: 10.1002/wps.20087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]