Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Feb 10.
Published in final edited form as: J Consult Clin Psychol. 2012 Jul 9;80(6):1052–1061. doi: 10.1037/a0029098

When Diagnosing ADHD in Young Adults Emphasize Informant Reports, DSM Items, and Impairment

Margaret H Sibley 1, William E Pelham Jr 2, Elizabeth M Gnagy 3, James G Waxmonsky 4, Daniel A Waschbusch 5, Karen J Derefinko 6, Allison C Garefino 7, Brooke S G Molina 8, Brian T Wymbs 9, Dara E Babinski 10, Aparajita B Kuriyan 11
PMCID: PMC3919146  NIHMSID: NIHMS426087  PMID: 22774792

Abstract

Objective

This study examined several questions about the diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in young adults using data from a childhood-diagnosed sample of 200 individuals with ADHD (age M = 20.20 years) and 121 demographically similar non-ADHD controls (total N = 321).

Method

We examined the use of self-versus informant ratings of current and childhood functioning and evaluated the diagnostic utility of adult-specific items versus items from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manuel of Mental Disorders (DSM).

Results

Results indicated that although a majority of young adults with a childhood diagnosis of ADHD continued to experience elevated ADHD symptoms (75%) and clinically significant impairment (60%), only 9.6%–19.7% of the childhood ADHD group continued to meet DSM–IV–TR (DSM, 4th ed., text rev.) criteria for ADHD in young adulthood. Parent report was more diagnostically sensitive than self-report. Young adults with ADHD tended to underreport current symptoms, while young adults without ADHD tended to overreport symptoms. There was no significant incremental benefit beyond parent report alone to combining self-report with parent report. Non-DSM-based, adult-specific symptoms of ADHD were significantly correlated with functional impairment and endorsed at slightly higher rates than the DSM-IV-TR symptoms. However, DSM-IV-TR items tended to be more predictive of diagnostic group membership than the non-DSM adult-specific items due to elevated control group item endorsement.

Conclusions

Implications for the assessment and treatment of ADHD in young adults are discussed (i.e., collecting informant reports, lowering the diagnostic threshold, emphasizing impairment, and cautiously interpreting retrospective reports).

Keywords: adult ADHD, diagnosis, assessment


It is clear that when children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) become adults, many continue to display manifestations of inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity (Barkley, Fischer, Smallish, & Fletcher, 2006; Mannuzza, Gittelman-Klein, Bessler, Malloy, & LaPadula, 1993; Weiss & Hechtman, 1993). As such, ADHD in adults is characterized by a range of impairments in daily life functioning (Barkley, Murphy, & Fischer, 2008). For example, compared with their peers, young adults who were diagnosed with ADHD in childhood are far less likely to pursue higher education, hold a steady job, responsibly manage their finances, and maintain adaptive social relationships (Barkley et al., 2006, 2008; Weiss & Hechtman (1993). These individuals also are more likely to experience dangerous problems with driving, risky sexual behavior, substance abuse, intimate partner violence, and criminal behavior (Barkley et al., 2008; Derefinko & Pelham, in press; Flory, Molina, Pelham, Gnagy, & Smith, 2006; Mannuzza, Klein, & Moulton, 2008; Thompson, Molina, Pelham, & Gnagy, 2007; Weiss & Hechtman, 1993; Wymbs et al., in press). Despite these poor outcomes, ADHD in adulthood is poorly defined and somewhat controversial (Barkley, 2006). A major reason for this confusion is that the field lacks clear evidence-based methods for identifying ADHD in adulthood.

Studies applying strictly interpretation of Diagnostic and Statistical Manuel of Mental Disorders (DSM; American Psychiatric Association, 1980–2000)) diagnostic criteria report low to moderate persistence rates (4%-42%; Barkley, Fischer, Smallish, & Fletcher, 2002; Kessler et al., 2005; Mannuzza, Klein, Bessler, Malloy, & LaPadula, 1998; Mannuzza, Klein, & Moulton, 2003). Therefore, experts initially believed that ADHD tends to remit after adolescence (Barkley et al., 2002; Mannuzza et al., 1998). However, other studies report higher persistence rates (49%–66%) by defining diagnostic threshold according to the presence of significant impairment or elevated symptomatology (as compared with control-group norms; Barkley et al., 2002; Weiss & Hechtman, 1993). These data suggest that, as adults, individuals with childhood ADHD display the core symptoms of the disorder and serious dysfunction (Biederman, Mick, & Faraone, 2000) but meet criteria for fewer of the DSM ADHD symptoms than children.

Recent work on ADHD in adults aims to better characterize its symptom expression. However, the typical adult with ADHD may not be well represented in these samples. Namely, most diagnostic research involves self-identified clinical samples of adults with ADHD (Barkley et al., 2008; Biederman et al., 2006). Typically, up to 50% of adults in these samples are women (e.g., Biederman et al., 2006), and sample participants often are not required to possess a childhood history of significant ADHD-related impairment (e.g., Barkley et al., 2008), making their composition different from samples identified in childhood, who possess a standard ADHD diagnosis. Namely, adults with a lifetime history of ADHD tend to underestimate their problems (Barkley et al., 2002), rarely present for ADHD treatment in adulthood, and therefore are unlikely to be included in adult-referred clinical samples. As a result, confusion over the expression of ADHD in adulthood may stem from research with somewhat unrepresentative samples.

Beyond “who” is included in the aforementioned samples, correct characterization of ADHD in adulthood hinges upon “how” information about these individuals is obtained. As mentioned previously, the typical adult with a childhood history of ADHD tends to dramatically underreport his or her own problems (Barkley et al., 2002; Sibley et al., 2010). Yet, most research with adult-diagnosed samples relies solely on self-report, which only appears valid for these self-referred individuals (Barkley, Knouse, & Murphy, 2011). Specifically, reports from informants such as parents (Barkley et al., 2002), siblings (Loney, Ledolter, Kramer, & Volpe, 2007), and other adults (Barkley et al., 2008) appear to offer more valid ratings of adults with an established childhood history of ADHD. Furthermore, most adult-diagnosed samples either do not require the DSM “B” criterion be met (ADHD symptoms in childhood; Barkley et al., 2008; Biederman et al., 2006) or rely solely on retrospective self-report to assess childhood functioning (Faraone et al., 2006; Kessler et al., 2010). Research is mixed with regard to the ability of adults with ADHD to provide accurate retrospective report of their childhood functioning (Mannuzza, Klein, Klein, Bessler, & Shrout, 2002; Miller, Newcorn, & Halpern, 2010). Therefore, further work is needed in this area.

The recent studies with adult-diagnosed samples universally suggest that the DSM needs new developmentally appropriate items for ADHD in adulthood (Barkley et al., 2008; Faraone, Biederman, & Spencer, 2010; Kessler et al., 2010). Undoubtedly, some of the DSM–IV (DSM–4th ed., American Psychiatric Association, 1994) symptoms are inappropriate descriptors of adults (e.g., difficulty playing quietly, inappropriate running and/or climbing). A string of studies asserts that combining adult-specific items with several developmentally ubiquitous ones (e.g., easily distracted, difficulty organizing tasks, difficulty sustaining attention) creates an adult-ADHD algorithm that improves upon the DSM's diagnostic utility (Barkley et al., 2008; Faraone et al., 2010; Fedele, Hartung, Canu, & Wilkowski, 2010; Kessler et al., 2010). However, these findings are limited by factors discussed previously, namely, the use of adult-referred samples and self-report information. Consequently, there are now several recommended sets of adult-specific ADHD items that possess very little overlap with each other (Barkley et al., 2008; Faraone et al., 2010; Kessler et al., 2010; Wender, 1985). To date, adult-specific ADHD items have not been examined using a sample of adults with established ADHD in childhood that provides both self- and informant-report of functioning. Using these methods might elucidate the expression of ADHD in adulthood and the utility of adult-specific items.

In sum, further work is needed to understand and standardize the diagnosis of ADHD in adulthood. Thus, in the current study, we aimed to develop recommendations for an adult-ADHD diagnostic protocol by examining the symptoms and functioning of young adults in the Pittsburgh ADHD Longitudinal Study (PALS; Molina, Pelham, Gnagy, Thompson, & Marshal, 2007). The PALS includes a sample of young adults who were well diagnosed with ADHD in childhood using standard DSM criteria applied in a specialty clinic setting. We first compared estimates of ADHD persistence into young adulthood by examining rates of DSM–IV–TR (4th ed., text rev.; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) diagnosis (A criteria), elevated ADHD symptomatology, and clinically significant functional impairment. We hypothesized that a majority of the sample would continue to display elevated ADHD symptomatology and clinically significant functional impairment in young adulthood, but that fewer would meet DSM criteria for ADHD. With regard to the utility of informant report, we hypothesized that young adults with ADHD would underreport their current and childhood symptomatology and that parent report alone would be the most useful method of assessing ADHD. Next, we evaluated the performance of adult-specific item sets posited by several research teams (Barkley et al., 2008; Faraone et al., 2010; Kessler et al., 2010) relative to item sets based on DSM-IV criteria. To do so, we compared symptom endorsement rates, parent- and self-report agreement, and convergent validity for each of these item sets within the PALS ADHD and non-ADHD control groups. We hypothesized that across these indices, the adult-specific items would possess greater diagnostic utility than the DSM items.

Method

Participants

PALS ADHD group

The ADHD group was recruited from a pool of 516 study-eligible participants diagnosed with DSM–III–R (DSM, 3rd ed., rev.; American Psychiatric Association, 1987) or DSM–IV ADHD in childhood and treated at the Attention Deficit Disorder Clinic at Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic (WPIC) in Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, from 1987 to 1996. Of the 516 participants, 493 were recontacted an average of 8.35 years later (SD = 2.79) to participate in annual interviews. Of those contacted, 364 (70.5 %) enrolled in the PALS. At the first follow-up interview, the ADHD group ranged in age from 11 to 28, with 99% falling between 11 and 25 years old. They were admitted to the follow-up study on a rolling basis between the years 1999 and 2003 and completed their first follow-up interview immediately upon enrollment.

All probands participated in the summer treatment program for children with ADHD, an 8-week intervention that included behavioral modification, parent training, and psychoactive medication trials where indicated (Pelham & Hoza, 1996; Pelham et al., 2010). Diagnostic information for the probands was collected at initial referral to the clinic in childhood (baseline) using parent- and teacher-rated DSM–III–R and DSM–IV symptom scales (Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale, or DBD; Pelham, Evans, Gnagy, & Greenslade, 1992) and a semistructured diagnostic interview administered to parents by a PhD-level clinician. The interview consisted of the DSM–III–R or DSM–IV descriptors for ADHD, oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), and conduct disorder (CD) with supplemental probe questions regarding situational and severity factors. It also included queries about other comorbidities to determine whether additional assessment was needed. Following DSM guidelines, clinicians made diagnoses of ADHD, ODD, and CD if a sufficient number of symptoms were endorsed (considering information from both parents and teachers) to result in diagnosis. Two PhD-level clinicians independently reviewed all ratings and interviews to confirm DSM diagnoses; when disagreement occurred, a third clinician reviewed the file and the majority decision was used. Exclusion criteria for probands were assessed in childhood (baseline) and included a full-scale IQ < 80, a history of seizures, neurological problems, pervasive developmental disorder, schizophrenia, or other psychotic or organic mental disorders.

Participants in the follow-up study were compared with the eligible individuals who did not enroll on demographic (i.e., age at first treatment, race, and parental education level and marital status) and diagnostic (i.e., parent and teacher ratings of ADHD and related symptomatology) variables collected at baseline. Only one of 14 comparisons was statistically significant at the p < .05 significance level. Participants had a slightly lower average CD symptom rating on a 4-point scale as indicated by a composite of parent and teacher ratings (participants M = 0.43, SD = .31; nonparticipants M = 0.53, SD = .39).

PALS control group

Control participants were 240 individuals without ADHD recruited for the PALS from the greater Pittsburgh community between 1999 and 2001. These individuals were recruited from several sources including pediatric practices in Allegheny County (40.8%), advertisements in local newspapers (27.5%), local universities and colleges (20.8%), and other methods (10.9%) such as Pittsburgh Public Schools and word of mouth. Control recruitment lagged 3 months behind the ADHD group enrollment in order to facilitate efforts to obtain demographic similarity (discussed in later section). A telephone screening interview was administered to parents of potential control participants to gather basic demographic characteristics, history of diagnosis or treatment for ADHD and other behavior problems, presence of exclusionary criteria as previously listed for the ADHD group, and a checklist of ADHD symptoms. Young adults also provided self-report of ADHD symptoms (see Measures). ADHD symptoms were counted as present if reported by either the parent or the young adult. Participants who met DSM–III–R criteria for ADHD, either currently or historically, were immediately excluded from study consideration.

If a potential control participant passed the initial phone screen, senior research staff members met to determine whether he or she was demographically appropriate for the study. Each potential control participant was examined on four demographic characteristics: (a) age, (b) gender, (c) race, and (D) parent education level. A control participant was deemed study-eligible if his OR her enrollment increased the control group's demographic similarity to the participants diagnosed with ADHD. At the end of the recruitment process, the two groups were equivalent on the four demographic variables noted previously.

Current Subsample

In the current study, we utilized data from 200 ADHD participants and 121 controls who were age 18 or older upon recruitment into the follow-up study (range: 18–28 years old). ADHD participants in this subsample ranged from 5 to 16 years old at baseline (M = 10.31, SD = 2.32). Additionally, baseline and the first follow-up visit were an average of 9.89 (SD = 2.42) years apart for these participants. At baseline, 42.6% of these probands met criteria for comorbid ODD and an additional 38.3% met criteria for CD according to combined parent and teacher report. At follow-up, 69.0% of probands and 49.0% of the control group lived in their parents’ home. However, living at home was not significantly related to any demographic, symptom, or impairment variables after we accounted for the participant's age. Table 1 lists demographic characteristics of this subsample (total N = 321). The two groups did not differ on any demographic variables (p > .25).

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics of Young Adults at Follow-Up Recruitment

Demographic variables ADHD Control
Age (in years; M, SD) 20.20 (2.19) 19.77 (1.73)
Gender
    Male (%) 87.0 85.1
Racial minority (%) 18.6 12.7
    African American (%) 12.0 9.3
    Other (%) 6.6 3.4
Highest parent education
    High school graduate or GED (%) 9.7 9.6
    Part college or specialized training (%) 37.7 35.6
    College or university graduate (%) 26.0 27.9
    Graduate professional training (%) 26.6 26.9
Single-parent household (%) 31.1 30.9

Note. All comparisons nonsignificant p > .25. ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; GED = general equivalency degree.

Procedure

As noted, baseline diagnostic information was gathered for the ADHD group at initial referral to the clinic during childhood. Follow-up interviews in young adulthood were conducted by post-baccalaureate research staff. All questionnaires (paper and pencil or Web-based) in the current study were completed privately. During informed consent, participants were assured of the confidentiality of disclosed materials. In cases where distance prevented participant travel to WPIC, information was collected through mail, telephone correspondence, and home visits. PALS follow-up interviews were conducted yearly beginning in the year of enrollment. Data for the current study were from the first follow-up visit. Participants were permitted to take stimulant medication on the day of the follow-up visit; however, few of the ADHD group (<10%) were prescribed stimulant medication at the time of assessment.

Measures

Childhood ADHD symptomatology

Baseline and retrospective report of childhood ADHD symptomatology was measured using the DBD (Pelham, Gnagy, Greenslade, & Milich, 1992). The DBD lists the DSM–III–R and DSM–IV symptoms of ADHD, ODD, and CD. At baseline, parents and teachers of study participants were asked to provide ratings of (0) not at all, (1) just a little, (2) pretty much, or (3) very much for each symptom of ADHD, ODD, and CD. The psychometric properties of the DBD rating scale are very good in childhood and adolescent samples, with empirical support for distinguishing inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, ODD, and CD factors, and internally consistent subscales with alphas above .95 (Pelham, Evans, et al., 1992; Pelham, Gnagy, et al., 1992; Pillow, Pelham, Hoza, Molina, & Stultz, 1998; Wright, Waschbusch, & Frankland, 2007). Severity scores (i.e., inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, ADHD total) were obtained by summing the ratings for each symptom on the dimension and dividing that sum by the total number of items on the subscale. For retrospective reports, parents and young adults were instructed to rate the child's behavior at baseline (entry into the summer treatment program).

Adult ADHD symptomatology

To measure young adult symptomatology at follow-up, clinicians administered an unpublished measure to participants and their parents (provided by R. Barkley; Barkley et al., 2008). This measure includes 91 items assessing the core symptoms of ADHD and associated features in a number of adult-related settings using age-appropriate behaviors and wording. Eighteen of these items were selected for the current analyses based upon previous work asserting the superiority of these items for distinguishing individuals with ADHD from non-ADHD controls (Barkley et al., 2008; Faraone et al., 2010; Kessler et al., 2010). The scale also includes the DSM–IV–TR symptoms of ADHD. Comparable to the DBD, responses on the adult ADHD measure were on a 0–3 scale. A symptom was counted as present if the respondent endorsed (2) often or (3) very often. ADHD symptom severity scores were calculated as they were on the DBD for four previously recommended sets of adult ADHD items (discussed later; American Psychiatric Association, 2000; Barkley et al., 2008; Faraone et al., 2010; Kessler et al., 2010).

Functional impairment

To determine each young adult's level of functional impairment at follow-up, clinicians administered an age-appropriate version of the Impairment Rating Scale to parents and young adults (IRS; Fabiano et al., 2006). Respondents indicated the degree of impairment a young adult displayed in seven domains that included academics, relationships with others, job performance, and overall impairment. Respondents marked an “X” on a line representing the continuum from no problem to extreme problem. Responses to each of the seven items were coded 0–6, with 0 representing no impairment and 6 representing extreme impairment. In the current study, the overall impairment item, which was rated last in the scale, was used to measure clinically significant impairment. The IRS has been shown to demonstrate good concurrent, convergent, and discriminant validity in child, adolescent, and young adult samples (Evans et al., 2012; Fabiano et al., 2006). The IRS has been shown to be highly accurate in identifying impairment in ADHD samples across settings and informants, with a score of 3 indicating clinically significant functional impairment (Evans et al., 2012; Fabiano et al., 2006).

Results

Reporting on DSM “A” Criteria

At follow-up in young adulthood, the ADHD group met an average of 2.94 DSM inattention symptoms (SD = 3.04) and 2.05 DSM hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms (SD = 2.73) according to parent reports and 1.31 DSM inattention symptoms (SD = 2.08) and 1.52 DSM hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms (SD = 2.27) according to self-reports. In comparison, controls met an average of 0.17 symptoms of inattention (SD = 0.63) and 0.12 symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity (SD = 0.41) according to parent reports and 0.53 symptoms of inattention (SD = 1.17) and 0.55 symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity (SD = 1.03) according to self-reports.

To judge persistence of ADHD into young adulthood, we examined the proportion of ADHD participants by each informant method (parent-only, self-only, combined parent and self) who (a) met DSM–IV–TR “A” criteria for ADHD, (b) possessed clinically significant impairment, and (c) displayed an elevated ADHD severity score. As is common practice for identifying developmentally normative behavior (Achenbach, 1991), elevated severity was defined by scores that were two standard deviations above the mean of the control group for either DSM–IV–TR inattention symptom severity (parent = 0.96, self = 1.27) or hyperactivity/impulsivity severity (parent = 0.66, self = 1.21). Analyses (see Table 2) revealed that a majority of probands possessed elevated ADHD symptom severity and clinically significant functional impairment but did not meet DSM diagnostic criteria for ADHD. Persistence also varied significantly as a function of informant. Post hoc follow-up analyses (see Table 2) with a Bonferroni adjustment of p < .006 were used to test specific hypotheses regarding the incremental value of informant reporting. Results indicated that parent reports endorsed significantly higher symptom severity and impairment than self-reports and that there was no significant incremental benefit to combining self and parent reports.

Table 2.

Proportion of ADHD Sample With Persistent Symptomatology at Follow-Up Crossed With Report Source

Informant
Variable Parent Self Combined χ 2 df p OR
DSM symptom threshold 12.0% 9.6% 19.7%
    Parent vs self 0.28 1 .59 1.22
    Parent vs combined 4.09 1 .04 1.90
    Self vs combined 6.41 1 .01 2.32
Elevated symptom severity 71.8% 24.4% 75.6%
    Parent vs self 70.31 1 <.001 7.87
    Parent vs combined 0.60 1 .44 1.22
    Self vs combined 82.05 1 <.001 9.64
Clinically significant impairment 55.6% 14.6% 60.4%
    Parent vs self 58.96 1 <.001 7.41
    Parent vs combined .63 1 .43 1.20
    Self vs combined 70.36 1 <.001 8.85

Note. McNemar's nonparametric chi-square tests were used to assess significant differences in prevalence from data of 159 young-adult participants with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder for whom parent report was available. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) symptom threshold represents DSM–A-criteria of at least six symptoms of either inattention or hyperactivity/impulsiveness. Elevated symptom severity was calculated based on 2 standard deviations above the mean of control group inattention or hyperactivity/impulsiveness severity. OR = odds ratio.

Reporting on DSM “B Criteria”

We also investigated the ability of parents and probands to retrospectively report childhood functioning. We examined partial intercorrelations between baseline parent and teacher reports and retrospective (at PALS follow-up) parent and self-reports, controlling for the number of years since baseline. A Bonferroni adjustment set the pre-established alpha level to p < .01 for these analyses. Self-retrospective reports were significantly correlated with parent (r = .28) and teacher (r = .25) baseline reports of symptomatology. Parent retrospective reports were significantly correlated with parent (r = .46), but not with teacher (r = .02), reports of baseline functioning. Parent and self-retrospective reports were not significantly correlated with each other (r = .21). In addition, according to self-retrospective report, 53.6% of the sample met DSM–A criteria for ADHD in childhood. Parent retrospective report correctly identified 81.6% of the sample as meeting diagnostic criteria for ADHD.

To detect within-subject trends, we also conducted two (parent and self) repeated-measures analyses with ADHD symptomatology as the dependent variable and report source (retrospective report, parent baseline, teacher baseline) as the within-subjects variable. Examination of sample moments revealed that assumptions of normality and independence for the generalized linear model were met. Mauchley's test of sphericity was significant for both models, indicating that this assumption had been violated. As a result, the Huynh–Feldt F test was employed to detect univariate effects using a model that accounts for this violation. Within-subjects analysis revealed that for parent retrospective report, the model was nonsignificant, F(1.67, 197.44) = 0.14, p = .25, ηp2=.01, indicating that parent retrospective report of childhood ADHD symptomatology did not differ significantly from parent or teacher baseline reports. For self-retrospective report, the main effect of rating was significant, F(1.93, 341.80) = 82.87, p < .01, ηp2=.32. Follow-up analyses indicated that self-retrospective report suggested significantly less symptomatology than parent report, F(1, 181) = 136.92, p < .01, ηp2=.43, and teacher report, F(1, 178) = 39.58, p < .01, ηp2=.37, at baseline.

Adult-Specific ADHD Items

On average, the ADHD group met 6.76 of the 18 adult-specific symptoms of ADHD according to parent report (SD = 5.76) and 3.31 according to self-report (SD = 4.33). In comparison, the controls met 0.82 adult-specific symptoms of ADHD according to parent report (SD = 2.15) and 2.23 according to self-report (SD = 3.06).

For all chi-square and correlational analyses of adult-specific items (see Table 3), a Bonferroni adjustment set the pre-established alpha level to p < .001. To compare the 18 previously identified adult-specific symptoms of ADHD to the 18 DSM items, we first examined the percentage of participants in each group who displayed each symptom according to self and parent report. Two sets (self and parent report) of thirty-six 2 (item endorsed: yes vs no) × 2 (group: ADHD vs control) chi-square analyses were conducted (see Table 3). According to self-report, the average DSM item was endorsed by 15.7% of young adults with ADHD and 6.1% of controls. The average adult-specific item was endorsed by 17.9% of young adults with ADHD and 11.9% of controls. For parent-report, the average DSM item was endorsed for 27.8% of probands and 1.6% of controls. The average adult-specific item was endorsed for 37.8% of probands and 4.6% of controls. Table 3 displays endorsement rates for each item individually. For parent-report, average odds ratio for the between group comparisons was higher for the DSM items (ORM = 26.62)1 than for the adult-specific items (ORM = 18.82). For self-report, the DSM items (ORM = 5.20) also tended to outperform the adult-specific items (ORM = 1.89) in discriminating diagnostic group. According to parent report, the ADHD symptoms that best discriminated young adults with and without ADHD were the following: easily distracted (OR = 65.26), trouble organizing thoughts (OR = 45.50), loses things (OR = 44.61), fidgets (OR = 43.24), and cannot hold things in memory (OR = 41.90). For self-report, the most predictive symptoms were difficulty doing things quietly (OR = 19.17) and difficulty remaining seated (OR = 16.74). Next, we examined the correlations between parent report of each DSM and adult-specific item and parent report of overall impairment within the ADHD sample. The DSM items (rM = .45) and the adult-specific items (rM = .47)2 were similarly correlated with overall impairment (see Table 3).

Table 3.

Comparison of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (4th ed., Text Rev.) and Adult-Specific Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Items

Self-report
Parent report
Symptom ADHD % Control % χ2(1) OR ADHD % Control % χ2(1) OR Correlation (r) with impairmenta
DSM–IV–TR symptoms
    Makes careless mistakesb 9.2 5.8 1.19 1.65 37.4 5.4 31.18* 10.52 .47*
    Difficulty sustaining attentionb 11.8 5.8 3.14 2.18 24.5 1.1 23.95* 29.84 .48*
    Does not listen 9.3 1.7 7.29 6.09 18.4 0.0 19.25* .44*
    Difficulty following instructions 14.9 1.7 14.75* 10.40 37.4 2.2 39.12* 27.20 .57*
    Difficulty organizing tasksc 10.2 6.6 1.20 1.61 38.4 2.2 40.09* 28.00 .46*
    Dislikes tasks requiring attention 12.8 4.2 6.39 3.36 37.2 2.2 35.99* 17.80 .57*
    Loses things 14.9 6.6 4.93 2.47 32.7 1.1 35.96* 44.61 .34*
    Easily distracted 29.1 17.4 5.55 1.95 41.5 1.1 48.58* 65.26 .54*
    Forgetful in daily activities 15.3 5.0 7.96 3.46 29.9 1.1 31.14* 39.30 .42*
    Fidgets 29.6 17.4 5.99 2.00 32.0 1.1 33.99* 43.24 .35*
    Difficulty remaining seated 12.2 0.8 13.43* 16.74 12.6 1.1 10.09* 13.25 .48*
    Restless 23.0 6.6 14.36* 4.21 28.8 0.0 32.46* .44*
    Difficulty doing things quietlyc 13.8 0.8 15.58* 19.17 13.7 1.1 11.30* 14.60 .43*
    On the go, driven by motor 19.0 8.3 6.63 2.58 25.7 0.0 28.32* .34*
    Talks excessively 18.9 10.0 4.48 2.09 24.5 3.2 18.93* 9.73 .33*
    Blurts out answers 12.8 6.6 3.03 2.07 17.0 1.1 14.97* 18.85 .41*
    Difficulty waiting turn 15.4 2.5 13.15* 7.09 22.4 1.1 21.40* 26.63 .45*
    Interrupts or intrudes 10.3 2.5 6.75 4.52 25.9 3.2 20.58* 10.46 .50*
Adult-specific symptoms
    Easily distracted during concentrationc 27.6 19.0 2.70 1.62 42.2 7.6 32.93* 8.86 .50*
    Makes decisions impulsivelyc 24.5 18.2 1.73 1.46 40.8 5.4 36.23* 12.14 .47*
    Difficulty doing things in proper orderc 12.8 9.1 1.00 1.46 23.3 1.1 22.43* 27.93 .42*
    Starts projects without directionsc 25.0 17.4 2.54 1.59 46.3 6.5 42.32* 12.48 .46*
    Difficulty stopping activitiesc 10.8 9.1 .25 1.21 29.7 2.2 28.13* 19.25 .46*
    Poor follow-throughc 7.7 8.3 .03 .93 33.3 3.2 30.42* 15.00 .57*
    Prone to daydreamingd 25.5 23.1 .23 1.14 27.9 3.2 23.15* 11.60 .34*
    Lacks self-disciplined 11.2 9.0 .13 1.15 52.4 6.5 53.00* 15.97 .58*
    Trouble doing what tells selfd 15.3 5.8 6.58 2.94 25.9 1.1 25.69* 32.07 .50*
    Cannot defer gratificationd 17.3 9.1 4.19 2.10 38.8 7.5 28.44* 7.78 .48*
    Trouble thinking clearlyd 15.4 4.1 9.60 4.22 29.5 2.2 27.10* 19.00 .48*
    Cannot hold things in memoryd 16.8 8.3 4.69 2.25 31.3 1.1 33.03* 41.90 .44*
    Difficulty persisting on uninteresting tasksd 32.1 29.8 .20 1.12 57.8 11.8 50.21* 10.22 .41*
    Trouble organizing thoughtsd 17.4 9.1 4.26 2.11 33.3 1.1 35.57* 45.50 .52*
    Difficulty prioritizing workb 14.3 7.4 3.40 2.07 46.3 3.2 50.64* 25.82 .54*
    Trouble planning aheadb 15.8 6.6 5.88 2.65 40.8 5.4 36.23* 12.14 .46*
    Cannot complete tasks on timeb 16.3 6.6 6.40 2.76 34.0 4.3 28.84* 11.47 .52*
    Cannot work unless under deadlineb 16.4 14.0 .32 1.20 47.6 8.6 39.53* 9.66 .39*

Note. OR = odds ratio.

a

Impairment r represents the relationship between parent endorsed symptom severity and parent rated overall impairment (Impairment Rating Scale; Fabiano et al., 2006).

b

Items suggested by Kessler et al., 2010.

c

Items suggested by Barkley et al., 2008.

d

Items suggested by Faraone et al., 2010.

*

p < .001.

Comparing Previously Researched Adult Item Sets

To investigate whether a previously recommended set of adult-ADHD symptoms optimally classifies young adults according to their childhood diagnostic status, we compared three previously reported ADHD item sets for adults (see Table 3; Barkley et al., 2008–nine items; Faraone et al., 2010–nine items; Kessler et al., 2010–six items) to the DSM item set on several dimensions. First, we examined whether the three empirically identified sets were superior to the DSM–IV–TR criteria for classifying probands and controls into the correct diagnostic group. For each of the four sets referenced, we conducted a binary logistic regression analyses with childhood diagnostic status (ADHD vs control) as the dichotomous dependent variable and parent report of the severity of each item in the set as continuous predictors. Correct classification represents the percentage of participants who were classified into the correct diagnostic group using a logistic regression model that contained the severity of each item in the set as predictors. The overall test of each binary logistic regression analysis was significant—DSM–IV–TR: χ2(18) = 151.65, p < .001; Barkley's nine items: χ2(9) = 120.18, p < .001; Faraone's nine items: χ2(9) = 119.74, p < .001; Kessler's six items: χ2(6) = 101.96, p < .001—indicating that all four item sets were able to assign the correct diagnostic classification to a significant number of cases (DSM–IV–TR = 87.5%; Barkley's nine items = 80.3%; Faraone's nine items = 82.1%, Kessler's six items = 78.8%).

Second, we examined whether parent–proband agreement was enhanced by the alternative sets of items by examining Pearson's r between parent report and self-report of symptom severity for each of the four sets. The relationship between parent and self-report of ADHD symptom severity was nonsignificant for all four item sets: DSM–IV–TR r = .16; Barkley's nine items r = .16; Faraone's nine items r = .13, and Kessler's six items r = .14).

Finally, within the ADHD group, we examined Pearson's r between parent-reported functional impairment and parent-reported symptom severity for each set in order to determine each set of items’ relationship with overall impairment in the lives of young adults with ADHD. For all correlational analyses, a Bonferroni adjustment set the pre-established alpha level to p < .01. The relationship between parent-reported ADHD symptom severity and overall impairment was significant for all four item sets: DSM–IV–TR r = .60; Barkley's nine items r = .64; Faraone's nine items r = .61, and Kessler's six items r = .59.

Discussion

This study used a prospectively followed sample of young adults with childhood-diagnosed ADHD to examine methodological and symptomatological issues related to the diagnosis of ADHD in young adults. Findings were that (a) 75% of young adults who were diagnosed with ADHD as children possessed elevated ADHD symptomatology and 60% possessed clinically significant impairment, yet only 20% qualified for a DSM–IV–TR–based ADHD diagnosis, (b) current parent reports as well as parent reports made in childhood detected far more symptoms and impairment than self-report, and (c) items written to assess adult-specific ADHD symptoms were endorsed more frequently for both the young adults with and without ADHD histories, and DSM items better discriminate these groups. Thus, in a sample of young adults with childhood-diagnosed ADHD, it does not appear that the previously identified adult-specific item sets (Barkley et al., 2008; Faraone et al., 2010; Kessler et al., 2010) outperform the DSM items. We will discuss each of these findings in turn.

The results of our study (see Table 2) suggest that according to self- and parent report, only 10%–20% of individuals with a childhood diagnosis of ADHD continue to meet the DSM–IV–TR symptom count threshold in young adulthood. Despite this low diagnostic persistence rate, 72%–76% of young adults diagnosed with ADHD in childhood continued to display elevated ADHD symptomatology and 55%–60% continued to experience clinically significant impairment in daily life functioning. This discrepancy between diagnosis and functioning is also consistent with other longitudinal follow-up studies of children with ADHD (Barkley et al., 2002; Mannuzza et al., 1998; Weiss & Hechtman, 1993). The mismatch between continued ADHD-related problems and adulthood diagnostic persistence is particularly concerning because under-identification of adult ADHD may hamper the referral and treatment of impaired adults with this disorder. Failing to meet diagnostic criteria may disqualify individuals from receiving services such as medication, post–high school educational accommodations, or insurance reimbursement for psychosocial treatment. The insufficient diagnostic criteria and methods applied to adults with ADHD no doubt contribute to the still-widespread notion that children with ADHD “grow out of it” over the course of development. Most professionals who work with adults are not familiar with ADHD as a problem in their patients (Kessler et al., 2006), leading to the dearth of effective treatments available to this population (Weiss et al., 2008).

Consistent with previous studies, our data suggest that young adults with a lifetime history of ADHD reported lower levels of symptomatology and impairment than informants (Barkley et al., 2002; Loney et al., 2007). In our sample, this trend was apparent for impairment ratings, DSM–IV–TR symptom ratings, and adult-specific ADHD items (see Tables 2 and 3). The discrepancy between self- and parent report may reflect persistence into young adulthood of a characteristic self-perception bias (Hoza, Pelham, Dobbs, Owens, & Pillow, 2002) or may be directly related to the ADHD cognitive profile (i.e., the tendency to respond carelessly or rush through rating scales; Sibley et al., 2010). However, it is important to note that in a handful of cases (3%–8% of the sample; see Table 2), self-reported information made a meaningful contribution to the diagnostic assessment. Furthermore, it is also possible that some parents overreported their son's or daughter's impairments. With respect to retrospective report of childhood functioning, our results also caution against relying upon self-report. Although self-retrospective reports were modestly correlated with baseline ratings (r = .25–.28), young adults with ADHD tended to underreport their childhood symptomatology. Parent retrospective report possessed a stronger, albeit imperfect, association with baseline parent report on the same measure (r = .46). Furthermore, there is a consistent finding in the literature suggesting a tendency for adults without ADHD to overendorse ADHD symptoms (Murphy, Gordon, & Barkley, 2002; Murphy & Schachar, 2000; Sollman, Ranseen, & Berry, 2010). Consistent with this finding, control self-report endorsed higher levels of ADHD symptoms than parent-report (see Table 3). These findings suggest that it is important to obtain informant report during the assessment of a young adult's current and childhood ADHD symptoms. In fact, given these results, we suggest that the utility of self-report be considered more for the perception of awareness, development of treatment plans, and consideration of alternative concurrent mental health problems, rather than as a sole source of ADHD diagnostic information. In our sample, there was no incremental benefit to combining self-reports with parent reports.

Regardless of diagnostic status, young adults and their parents tended to endorse the adult-specific symptoms of ADHD at higher rates than the DSM symptoms. This finding suggests that some adult-specific items may describe somewhat normative behaviors that are not specific to ADHD. For example, an equally high proportion of probands and controls (see Table 3) endorsed difficulty in persisting on uninteresting tasks (32.1% vs. 29.8%) and being prone to daydreaming (25.5% vs 23.1%). Consequently, DSM–IV–TR items tended to more accurately predict diagnostic group membership than the adult-specific items, despite sometimes possessing lower symptom prevalence rates. However, DSM items were still endorsed at far lower rates than they typically are in childhood or even in adolescence (Sibley et al., in press). In addition, adult-specific item sets (at least those evaluated here) were not incrementally useful in the identification of childhood-diagnosed young adults with ADHD. Therefore, it may be the case that a reduced diagnostic threshold combined with developmentally appropriate descriptions of the existing DSM items might possesses better diagnostic utility than an entirely new set of items. This is consistent with previous recommendations to reduce the adult diagnostic threshold to four symptoms (Barkley et al., 2008) and with the proposed changes to the DSM–V (see http://www.dsm5.org). For example, post hoc analyses of parent-reported symptoms suggested that reducing the DSM–A criteria diagnostic threshold to four symptoms increased ADHD prevalence from 12.0% to 39.9% in young adults. A threshold of three symptoms increased this prevalence to 48.9%, while prevalence of ADHD in control group remained below 3%.

Unlike previous researchers, we failed to find incremental benefit in the inclusion of adult-specific ADHD items (Barkley et al., 2008; Faraone et al., 2010; Kessler et al., 2010). One reason for these inconsistent results may be differential item utility in childhood (i.e., the PALS) versus adult-diagnosed samples (Barkley et al., 2008; Faraone et al., 2010; Kessler et al., 2010). Considering ADHD dimensionally, we believe that some studies of adult-diagnosed individuals may oversample populations with subthreshold symptomatology and higher functioning than individuals with a lifetime history of ADHD. Like the non-ADHD controls in our sample and others (Murphy et al., 2002; Murphy & Schachar, 2000; Sollman et al., 2010), these individuals may even overendorse symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity. Thus, it is hoped that future work on the expression of ADHD in adulthood will be conducted with childhood-diagnosed samples, which may help to represent the entirety of the ADHD population.

The results of this study should be considered within the context of its limitations. While our sample was demographically representative of the county in which the study occurred, many of our participants came from middle-class families. As a result, our findings may be most generalizable to middle-class, racial-majority males. Furthermore, given the small proportion of girls in our sample (<10%), we could not independently examine differential diagnostic trends by gender. As a clinic-referred sample, the outcomes found in the PALS may not generalize to epidemiological samples of individuals with ADHD. We did not obtain report from non-parent adult informants, who may play an important role in ADHD diagnosis for adults, especially for those participants who no longer lived at home. It is possible that a greater number of symptoms would be detected for participants if an additional informant was used. Our measure of impairment was limited to parent report; however, future research should validate symptoms with objective measures of impairment to protect against method variance. The IRS also does not specify that impairment must be solely due to ADHD, so it is possible that comorbid problems impacted severity ratings. Finally, our sample of adults with ADHD was relatively young (M age = 20.20 years) and our findings may not generalize to older adults with ADHD. Therefore, it will be important for us to reconsider these diagnostic issues as outcome data become available from the 25- and 30-year-old PALS assessments.

Despite these limitations, we believe that our study offers several important recommendations for the diagnosis of ADHD in young adults. First, our data suggest that informant reporting should be integral to the adult ADHD diagnostic process. Some work suggests siblings and other adults who are familiar with the client may also be useful informants (Barkley et al., 2008; Loney et al., 2007). Despite the potential inconvenience of contacting informants, they are far more likely than the target individual to provide valid information about current and childhood functioning. Second, rather than abandoning the DSM–IV–TR ADHD items for adults, our data suggest that a less stringent symptom threshold (e.g., four symptoms) and emphasis on the presence of clinically significant impairment may be the optimal algorithm for identifying ADHD in young adults without increasing the rate of false positives.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by National Institute on Drug Abuse Grants DA12414 to William E. Pelham, Jr., DA05605 to Ralph Tarter, and F31 DA017546 to Brooke S. G. Molina and National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Grant AA11873 to Brooke S. G. Molina. Research was also supported in part by National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Grants AA08746 to Ralph Tarter, AA00202 and AA12342 to Brooke S. G. Molina, and AA06267 to William E. Pelham, Jr.; National Institute of Mental Health Grants MH12010, MH48157, MH45576, MH50467, and MH069614 to William E. Pelham, Jr., MH53554 to Benjamin Lahey, and MH47390 to Betsy Hoza; National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences Grant ES0515-08 to H. Needleman; Institute of Education Sciences Grants IESLO3000665A and IESR324B060045 to William E. Pelham, Jr.; and the American Psychological Foundation (Elizabeth Munsterberg Koppitz Fellowship to Margaret H. Sibley).

Footnotes

1

Abbreviation ORM indicates the mean odds ratio.

2

Abbreviation rM indicates the mean Pearson's correlation.

Contributor Information

Margaret H. Sibley, Center for Children and Families, Florida International University

William E. Pelham, Jr., Center for Children and Families, Florida International University

Elizabeth M. Gnagy, Center for Children and Families, Florida International University

James G. Waxmonsky, Center for Children and Families, Florida International University

Daniel A. Waschbusch, Center for Children and Families, Florida International University

Karen J. Derefinko, Center for Children and Families, Florida International University

Allison C. Garefino, Department of Psychology, Kennesaw State University

Brooke S. G. Molina, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center

Brian T. Wymbs, Seattle Children's Research Institute, Seattle, Washington

Dara E. Babinski, Center for Children and Families, Florida International University.

Aparajita B. Kuriyan, Center for Children and Families, Florida International University.

References

  1. Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; Burlington, VT: 1991. [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 3rd ed., rev. Author; Washington, DC: 1987. [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Author; Washington, DC: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  4. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed., text rev. Author; Washington, DC: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  5. Barkley RA. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and treatment. 3rd ed. Guilford; New York, NY: 2006. [Google Scholar]
  6. Barkley RA, Fischer M, Smallish L, Fletcher K. The persistence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into young adulthood as a function of reporting source and definition of disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111:279–289. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.279. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Barkley RA, Fischer M, Smallish L, Fletcher K. Young adult outcome of hyperactive children: Adaptive functioning in major life activities. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45:192–202. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000189134.97436.e2. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000189134.97436.e2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barkley RA, Knouse LE, Murphy KR. Correspondence and disparity in the self- and other ratings of current and childhood ADHD symptoms and impairment in adults with ADHD. Psychological Assessment. 2011;23:437–446. doi: 10.1037/a0022172. doi:10.1037/a0022172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barkley RA, Murphy KR, Fischer M. ADHD in adults: What the science says. Guilford; New York, NY: 2008. [Google Scholar]
  10. Biederman J, Faraone SV, Spencer TJ, Mick E, Monuteaux MC, Aleardi M. Functional impairments in adults with self-reports of diagnosed ADHD: A controlled study of 1001 adults in the community. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2006;67:524–540. doi: 10.4088/jcp.v67n0403. doi:10.4088/JCP.v67n0403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Biederman J, Mick E, Faraone SV. Age-dependent decline of symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Impact of remission definition and symptom type. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;157:816–818. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.816. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Derefinko KD, Pelham WE. ADHD and substance use. In: Sher K, editor. Oxford handbook of substance use and substance dependence. Oxford University Press; New York, NY: in press. [Google Scholar]
  13. Evans SW, Brady CE, Harrison JR, Bunford N, Kern L, Andrews C. Measuring ADHD symptoms and impairment based on high school teachers’ ratings. 2012. Manuscript submitted for publication. [DOI] [PubMed]
  14. Fabiano GA, Pelham WE, Jr., Waschbusch DA, Gnagy EM, Lahey BB, Chronis AM, Burrows-MacLean L. A practical measure of impairment: Psychometric properties of the Impairment Rating Scale in samples of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and two school-based samples. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 2006;35:369–385. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3503_3. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3503_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Spencer T, Mick E, Murray K, Petty C, Monuteaux MC. Diagnosing adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Are late onset and subthreshold diagnoses valid? American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163:1720–1729. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.10.1720. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.10.1720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Faraone SV, Biederman J, Spencer T. Diagnostic efficiency of symptom items for identifying adult ADHD. Journal of ADHD & Related Disorders. 2010;1:38–48. [Google Scholar]
  17. Fedele DA, Hartung CM, Canu WH, Wilkowski BM. Potential symptoms of ADHD for emerging adults. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2010;32:385–396. doi:10.1007/s10862-009-9173-x. [Google Scholar]
  18. Flory K, Molina B, Pelham W, Gnagy E, Smith B. Childhood ADHD predicts risky sexual behavior in young adulthood. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2006;35:571–577. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3504_8. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3504_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hoza B, Pelham WE, Dobbs J, Owens J, Pillow DR. Do boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder have positive illusory self-concepts? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002;111:268–278. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.111.2.268. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kessler RC, Adler L, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners CK, Demler O, Zaslavsky AM. The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey replication. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;163:716–723. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.4.716. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.4.716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kessler RC, Adler LA, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners C, Faraone SV. Patterns and predictors of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder persistence into adulthood: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:1442–1451. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.001. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kessler RC, Green J, Adler LA, Barkley RA, Chatterji S, Faraone SV, Van Brunt DL. Structure and diagnosis of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Analysis of expanded symptom criteria from the adult ADHD clinical diagnostic scale. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67:1168–1178. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.146. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Loney J, Ledolter J, Kramer JR, Volpe RJ. Retrospective ratings of ADHD symptoms made at young adulthood by clinic-referred boys with ADHD-related problems, their brothers without ADHD, and control participants. Psychological Assessment. 2007;19:269–280. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.3.269. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.19.3.269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Mannuzza S, Gittelman-Klein R, Bessler A, Malloy P, LaPadula M. Adult outcome of hyperactive boys: Educational achievement, occupational rank, and psychiatric status. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1993;50:565–576. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820190067007. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820190067007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Bessler A, Malloy P, LaPadula M. Adult psychiatric status of hyperactive boys grown up. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1998;155:493–498. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.4.493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Klein DF, Bessler A, Shrout P. Accuracy of adult recall of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;159:1882–1888. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.11.1882. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.159.11.1882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Moulton J. Persistence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into adulthood: What have we learned from the prospective follow-up studies? Journal of Attention Disorders. 2003;7:93–100. doi: 10.1177/108705470300700203. doi:10.1177/108705470300700203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Mannuzza S, Klein RG, Moulton J. Lifetime criminality among boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A prospective follow-up study into adulthood using official arrest records. Psychiatry Research. 2008;160:237–246. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.11.003. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Miller CJ, Newcorn J, Halperin J. Fading memories: Retrospective recall inaccuracies in ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2010;14:7–14. doi: 10.1177/1087054709347189. doi:10.1177/1087054709347189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Molina BG, Pelham WE, Gnagy EM, Thompson AL, Marshal MP. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder risk for heavy drinking and alcohol use disorder is age specific. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2007;31:643–654. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00349.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Murphy KR, Gordon M, Barkley RA. To what extent are ADHD symptoms common? A reanalysis of standardization data from the DSM–IV checklist. ADHD Report. 2002;8:1–5. [Google Scholar]
  32. Murphy P, Schachar R. Use of self-ratings in the assessment of symptoms of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2000;157:1156–1159. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.7.1156. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.7.1156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Pelham WE, Jr., Evans SW, Gnagy EM, Greenslade KE. Teacher ratings of DSM-III–R symptoms for the disruptive behavior disorders: Prevalence, factor analyses, and conditional probabilities in a special education sample. School Psychology Review. 1992;21:285–299. [Google Scholar]
  34. Pelham WE, Jr., Gnagy E, Greenslade K, Milich R. Teacher ratings of DSM–III–R symptoms for the disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1992;31:210–218. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199203000-00006. doi:10.1097/00004583-199203000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Pelham WE, Jr., Gnagy EM, Greiner AR, Waschbusch DA, Fabiano GA, Burrows-MacLean L. Summer treatment programs for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In: Kazdin AE, Weisz JR, editors. Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and adolescents. 2nd ed. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  36. Pelham WE, Hoza B. Intensive treatment: A summer treatment program for children with ADHD. In: Hibbs ED, Jensen PS, editors. Psychosocial treatments for child and adolescent disorders: Empirically based strategies for clinical practice. American Psychological Association; Washington, DC: 1996. pp. 311–340. [Google Scholar]
  37. Pillow DR, Pelham WE, Hoza B, Molina BSG, Stultz CH. Confirmatory factor analyses examining attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms and other childhood disruptive behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1998;26:293–309. doi: 10.1023/a:1022658618368. doi:10.1023/A:1022658618368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Sibley MH, Pelham WE, Molina BSG, Gnagy EM, Waschbusch DA, Garefino A, Karch KM. Diagnosing ADHD in adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 80:139–150. doi: 10.1037/a0026577. in press doi:10.1037/a0026577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Sibley MH, Pelham WE, Molina BSG, Waschbusch DA, Gnagy E, Babinski DE, Biswas B. Inconsistent self-report of delinquency by adolescents and young adults with ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2010;38:645–656. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9404-3. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9404-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Sollman MJ, Ranseen JD, Berry DR. Detection of feigned ADHD in college students. Psychological Assessment. 2010;22:325–335. doi: 10.1037/a0018857. doi:10.1037/a0018857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Thompson AL, Molina B, Pelham W, Gnagy E. Risky driving in adolescents and young adults with childhood ADHD. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2007;32:745–759. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsm002. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsm002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Weiss G, Hechtman L. Hyperactive children grown up. 2nd ed. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 1993. [Google Scholar]
  43. Weiss M, Safren SA, Solanto MV, Hechtman L, Rostain AL, Ramsay J, Murray C. Research forum on psychological treatment of adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2008;11:642–651. doi: 10.1177/1087054708315063. doi:10.1177/1087054708315063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Wender PH. Wender AQCC (Adult Questionnaire–Childhood Characteristics) Scale. Psychopharmacology Bulletin. 1985;21:927–928. [Google Scholar]
  45. Wright KD, Waschbusch DA, Frankland BW. Combining data from parent ratings and parent interview when assessing ADHD. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2007;29:141–148. doi:10.1007/s10862-006-9039-4. [Google Scholar]
  46. Wymbs BT, Molina BSG, Pelham WE, Cheong J, Gnagy EM, Belendiuk KA, Waschbusch DA. Risk of intimate partner violence among young adult males with childhood ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders. doi: 10.1177/1087054710389987. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES