Abstract
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is known to have an integral role in establishing stable memories after learning events. The neuroplasticity induced by Pavlovian fear conditioning has likewise been shown to rely on interactions between BDNF and its principal receptor, tyrosine kinase receptor B (TrkB), in the amygdala after training. Although the necessity of amygdala bdnf expression and TrkB activation for associative learning within aversive contexts has been explored, it is unclear to what extent this interaction is involved in appetitive learning. It is also unclear whether the noted increases in amygdala BDNF after fear conditioning are due to local gene transcription and translation or anterograde transmission from cortical regions. To address both of these questions, we used two lentiviral approaches in mice, using both fear conditioning and cocaine-conditioned place preference (CPP), during acquisition and extinction. First, we decreased expression of bdnf mRNA in the amygdala of homozygous floxed mice with a Cre-expressing virus. In a second set of studies, we infused a virus that expressed a dominant-negative TrkB isoform into the same region. These approaches significantly impaired consolidation of fear conditioning and cocaine-CPP, as well as extinction of CPP. Together, these data suggest that BDNF-TrkB signaling is critical for amygdala-dependent learning of both appetitive and aversive emotional memories.
Keywords: BDNF, TrkB, amygdala, learning
Introduction
Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) has a well-established role in synaptic plasticity within the context of long-term potentiation (LTP) in the adult CNS (Korte et al., 1995; Pang et al., 2004; Bramham and Messaoudi, 2005). In addition to its demonstrated role in hippocampal-dependent learning, interest in the influence of BDNF and its primary receptor, tyrosine kinase receptor B (TrkB), in amygdala-dependent learning has also grown rapidly in recent years. Cell bodies in the basolateral amygdala (BLA) strongly express bdnf mRNA, and the TrkB receptor is expressed throughout the amygdala, including the BLA and the central nucleus (CeA) (Krause et al., 2008). The BLA is known to facilitate the acquisition and consolidation of appetitive and aversive learning events, and recent studies have shown that bdnf mRNA is significantly upregulated in this region of the amygdala following Pavlovian fear conditioning (Rattiner et al., 2004; Ou and Gean, 2006).
The role of the BLA in forming associations between conditioned (CS) and unconditioned (US) stimuli has been extensively studied, and the neurocircuitry involved in amygdala-dependent learning is well defined (Davis, 1992; Fanselow and LeDoux, 1999). Amygdala LTP is induced by fear conditioning, and this plasticity supports the acquisition, consolidation, and expression of aversive associative memories (Rogan et al., 1997; Goosens and Maren, 2002). Furthermore, neural plasticity in the amygdala has been shown to be dependent on BDNF (Rattiner et al., 2005; Cowansage et al., 2010), and point mutations at the primary TrkB phosphorylation sites can alter acquisition and consolidation of fear learning as well as synaptic plasticity (Musumeci et al., 2009). Others have found that local infusion of a TrkB antagonist in the amygdala inhibits fear consolidation in rats (Rattiner et al., 2004) and that in vitro application of either BDNF or 7,8-dihydroxyflavone, another TrkB agonist, lowers the threshold for LTP induction in the BLA (Li et al., 2011, Meis et al., 2012).
Despite ample evidence supporting the necessity of amygdala BDNF–TrkB interactions for the stable formation of fear memories, there is a notable paucity of data addressing the role of this local neurotrophic activity in other forms of amygdala-dependent learning. In addition to supporting stimulus-response learning in aversive contexts, the BLA is also integrally involved in appetitive learning (Everitt et al., 2003). For instance, pretraining excitotoxic lesions of the BLA impair the acquisition of cocaine-conditioned place preference (CPP), whereas post-training ablation at the same site impairs extinction of the task (Fuchs et al., 2002); these data strongly support a regulatory function of the BLA in incentive-based memory formation. Other research has also implicated BDNF in drug-seeking behavior (Lu et al., 2004), making the contribution of amygdala BDNF to cocaine-based stimulus-outcome learning a promising area of potential study. The following experiments demonstrate the following: (1) both knockdown of the bdnf gene and inhibition of the TrkB receptor within the amygdala impair consolidation of associative memories; and (2) these manipulations similarly affect learning in both aversive and appetitive paradigms.
Materials and Methods
Animals
All mice were housed in standard group cages and given ad libitum access to both food and water. Ambient temperature remained at 20°C throughout the experiment. Experiments were performed during the light portion of a 12 h light/dark cycle. All experiments were approved by Emory University Institutional Review Board following Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee standards with accordance to the Yerkes Primate Research Center regulations.
LV-Cre.
Homozygous bdnf-floxed mice were originally obtained from The Jackson Laboratory (Bdnftm3Jae/J) and bred within our animal facility. These mice possessed loxP sites both upstream and downstream of exon 5 of the Bdnf gene. This strain was originated and maintained on a mixed B6, 129S4, BALB/c background and did not display any gross physical or behavioral abnormalities. Experiments were conducted on male mice between 5 and 10 weeks of age.
LV-TrkB.t1.
Adult (14–17 weeks old) male C57BL/6 mice weighing 20–30 g (The Jackson Laboratory) were used in these experiments. No important baseline differences were seen in the strains of mice C57BL/6 versus mixed strain used for the LV-TrkB.t1 versus LV-Cre lines, but for these reasons, littermates were always used as within experiment controls.
Lentiviral vectors
LV-GFP.
Viral vectors were derived from the HIV-based lentivirus backbone pLV-CMV-GFP-U3Nhe, which allows for virally mediated expression of GFP driven by a CMV promoter. Infected neurons remain intact and express the fluorescent marker.
LV-Cre.
We created a Cre-recombinase expressing viral vector (LV-Cre) by replacing the GFP coding sequence in pLV-CMV-GFP-U3Nhe with the coding sequence for Cre-recombinase. Viral production procedures were described in detail previously (Heldt et al., 2007).
LV-TrkB.t1.
Dominant-negative TrkB.t1 is a truncated version of TrkB that binds BDNF but lacks a catalytic, cytoplasmic kinase domain (Haapasalo et al., 2002). Lentiviral TrkB.t1 transfections inhibit BDNF signaling in vivo (Rattiner et al., 2004) and in vitro (Li et al., 1998; Haapasalo et al., 2002; Offenhäuser et al., 2002). The virus was tagged with HA, making it possible to confirm infusion sites.
Surgery
Mice received bilateral amygdala microinjections of LV-Cre, LV-TrkB.t1, or LV-GFP. Mice were anesthetized with a Ketastet (80 mg/kg)/Dormitor (1 mg/kg) solution and then mounted in a stereotaxic apparatus. Small holes were drilled in the skull above the injection sites, and a Hamilton microsyringe was lowered to the following coordinates from bregma: anteroposterior = −1.4, mediolateral = ±3.1, dorsoventral = −5.0. A total volume of 1.5 μl was administered at a rate of 0.1 μl/min (0.2 μl/min for LV-TrkB.t1). The needle remained in place for 10 min after the injection and was removed at a rate of 0.5 mm/min. The subject's skin was then sewn together using 1.5 metric polyglactin absorbable sutures. After recovery from anesthesia, mice were given a narcotic analgesic, returned to home cages, and monitored daily for 14 d before testing.
Histology
LV-Cre.
Mice were deeply anesthetized, and their brains were rapidly removed and frozen on dry ice. Coronal sections (20 μm) were cut on a cryostat, mounted on gelatin-coated slides, and stored at −80°C until processed. In situ hybridization was performed to examine the expression of mRNA as previously described (Ressler et al., 2002).
LV-TrkB.t1.
After ketamine overdose, animals were perfused intracardially using 4% paraformaldehyde in 1 × PBS; 55 μm sections were taken on a microtome, and sectioned tissue was blocked with 2% normal goat serum and 1% BSA in 1 × PBS+ (0.4% Triton X-100) and incubated in a 1:1000 dilution of HA rabbit polyclonal antibody (Sigma) overnight at 4°C. Sections were then washed in 1 × PBS and incubated in a 1:500 dilution of AlexaFluor-594 goat anti-rabbit secondary antibody for 1 h at room temperature. The sections were mounted and coverslipped.
LV-GFP.
After ketamine overdose, animals were perfused intracardially using 4% paraformaldehyde in 1 × PBS; 55 μm sections were taken on a microtome, mounted, coverslipped, and visualized for endogenous GFP fluorescence.
Conditioned place preference
Apparatus.
Three compartment CPP chambers (Med Associates, product MED-CPP-MS) were used for training and testing.
Pretest.
All animals were pretested for individual place preference over a 2 d period. Pretests consisted of placing the subject in the neutral compartment and allowing 20 min of free access to all chambers. Time spent in each chamber was recorded, and the chamber in which each animal spent the most time was codified as the “preferred side,” whereas the opposing chamber was the “nonpreferred side.”
Training.
Training used a biased design, in which subjects received cocaine pairings on the nonpreferred side. For 3 d, mice received one pairing session in the morning and one in the afternoon (each lasting 30 min). Intraperitoneal injections of cocaine hydrochloride (10 mg/kg, Sigma) were paired with the animal's nonpreferred chamber, whereas sterile 1 × PBS was paired with the preferred chamber. The cocaine pairing was counterbalanced between morning and afternoon to ensure that time of administration did not affect acquisition of CPP.
Testing.
Mice were allowed to freely explore all three chambers for 20 min. Time spent in each chamber was recorded.
Extinction testing.
The same protocol as the test day was used. Diminished preference for the cocaine-paired chamber following the unreinforced test day was considered extinction learning.
Fear training and testing
Fear-potentiated startle (FPS).
SR-LAB startle response systems (San Diego Instruments) were used for training and testing. During training, mice were placed in chambers and after 5 min received the first of 10 light + shock trials consisting of a 30 s CS light coterminating with a 0.5 s, 0.6 mA footshock. On each of 2 consecutive days after training, fear was assessed in the same context using the FPS paradigm. For each test, mice were presented with 4 startle stimuli at each of three different startle stimulus intensities (95, 105, and 115 dB). After these initial trials, mice were presented with 4 additional startle-alone trials and 4 light + startle trials at each of the three startle stimulus intensities. A percentage of FPS was computed by dividing the difference between these two trial types by the mean startle amplitude on startle-alone trials: percent FPS = [[(light + startle) − (startle-alone)]/startle-alone)] × 100.
Elevated plus maze
The elevated plus maze consisted of two open arms (50 × 6.5 cm) and two closed arms attached to a common central platform (6.5 × 6.5 cm) to form a cross. The percentage of open arm entries [open arm/(open + closed arm) entries] and percentage time in open arms [time in open arms/(open + closed arms)] were measured.
Results
Complementary site-specific Bdnf deletion and TrkB inhibition with lentiviral vector approaches
Although we have previously used the inducible knock-out technique with Cre-expressing lentivirus in several other brain regions (Heldt et al., 2007; Choi et al., 2010), we had not examined the effect of bdnf gene deletion within the amygdala. In these studies, we bilaterally infected the amygdala of homozygous floxed mice with lentivirus expressing Cre recombinase. We then performed behavioral studies followed by in situ hybridization to confirm amygdala-specific deletion. We found significant depletion of bdnf mRNA in the BLA of mice infected with the LV-Cre virus compared with LV-GFP-infected controls (Fig. 1A–C). The regions used for in situ analysis are depicted in Figure 1G. Figure 2A, B illustrates quantitative knockdown of bdnf mRNA in amygdala (t(26) = 2.6, p = 0.016), but not hippocampus.
We have previously shown that our dominant-negative TrkB virus with a truncated cytoplasmic tail (LV-TrkB.t1) impairs fear conditioning and extinction in the rat but that it has no effect on basal measures of anxiety-like behavior (Rattiner et al., 2004; Chhatwal et al., 2006). In the current studies, we verified that we could similarly infect the amygdala with LV-GFP or LV-TrkB.t1 in mice (Fig. 1D–F). To determine whether amygdala-specific bdnf deletion affects baseline anxiety, we infected bdnf-floxed mice bilaterally with LV-Cre or LV-GFP and examined their behavior on the elevated plus maze. We found no differences in time on open arms, percentage of open arm entries, or total distance traveled between these groups (Figs. 2C,D). Notably, we have previously shown that infection with LV-Cre has no behavioral effects on wild-type mice, indicating that our results are not the result of nonspecific effects of the virus (Heldt and Ressler, 2010). These data demonstrate that our inducible lentiviral vector approaches to either knock down bdnf gene expression or to impair TrkB functioning with a dominant-negative isoform are region-specific and do not affect locomotor behavior or baseline anxiety-like behaviors.
Bdnf knockdown in the amygdala impairs FPS
We next examined whether bdnf knockdown in the amygdala would result in impaired fear conditioning in the same manner as inhibition of amygdala TrkB function (Rattiner et al., 2004). After bilateral amygdala infection, animals were subjected to 10 light-shock pairings. The next 2 d, they were tested for FPS. We found that bdnf-floxed mice infected with LV-GFP demonstrated significant post-training FPS (Fig. 3A; repeated-measures ANOVA, comparing pre, post1, and post2 percentage FPS within the GFP group, F(2,25) = 5.28, p = 0.01), whereas the mice infected with LV-Cre, with bilateral amygdala bdnf deletion, did not (repeated-measures ANOVA as above, F(2,25) = 2.15, p > 0.1). Figure 3B demonstrates the extent of LV-Cre infusion in these animals. These data suggest that BDNF must be produced within the amygdala in order for fear memories to be established.
Bdnf knockdown and dominant-negative impairment of TrkB in the amygdala delay place preference and impair extinction
In addition to the contribution of amygdala BDNF to fear acquisition and consolidation, we also examined the effect of these manipulations on appetitive learning using CPP. We found that mice with Cre-mediated knockdown of bdnf in the amygdala failed to show significant preference for the cocaine-paired chamber over the saline-paired chamber, whereas LV-GFP-infected controls formed a robust preference, spending significantly more time in the cocaine-paired chamber (Fig. 4A,B). A mixed-model repeated-measures ANOVA was performed, examining a drug (cocaine vs saline chamber) × group (Cre vs GFP) interaction. This was significant for the interaction effect (F(1,27) = 4.1, p = 0.05). Additionally, when we examined the groups separately, we found no significant difference in the Cre group but a significant effect of preference for the cocaine chamber in the GFP group (F(1,14) = 17.0, p = 0.001).
Similarly, mice that received bilateral infusions of the dominant-negative TrkB.t1 virus in the amygdala did not demonstrate preference for the cocaine-paired chamber (Fig. 4C,D). A mixed-model repeated-measures ANOVA was performed, examining a drug (cocaine vs saline chamber) × group (LV-TrkB.t1 vs LV-GFP) interaction. We found a trend toward a significant interaction (F(1,39) = 1.99, one-tailed test, p = 0.08). Post hoc analyses revealed that the LV-GFP animals successfully acquired CPP (t(15) = 2.63, p = 0.02), whereas the LV-TrkB.t1 animals did not (t(16) = 0.96, p = 0.35). Together, these data suggest an influential role for amygdalar BDNF–TrkB interactions in the acquisition of cocaine-CPP.
Finally, these experiments were repeated in an entirely separate cohort of mice to further examine the effect of blocking TrkB in amygdala on CPP extinction (Fig. 4E,F). A mixed-model repeated-measures ANOVA was performed, examining a drug (cocaine vs saline chamber) × group (LV-TrkB.t1 vs LV-GFP) × test (post1 vs post2) interaction. We found a significant chamber × group × test interaction effect (F(1,18) = 3.36, one-tailed test, p < 0.05). Post hoc analyses revealed that, during Test 1, both TrkB.t1 and GFP groups demonstrated a significant place preference. However, after extinction of CPP associated with this first test, the GFP group no longer showed a place preference (t(9) = 1.07, p = 0.3), whereas the TrkB.t1 group continued to show a difference (t(9) = 2.54, p < 0.01), suggesting that appetitive extinction was impaired with this amygdala-TrkB.t1 manipulation.
Discussion
Synaptic plasticity in the amygdala plays a significant role in encoding representations of both appetitive and aversive learning events. We have demonstrated that either knocking down the expression of bdnf mRNA or sequestering BDNF with an inactive isoform of its principal TrkB receptor significantly impairs the consolidation of both aversive and appetitive conditioning. Bdnf-floxed mice that received bilateral infusions of LV-Cre into the amygdala failed to show fear-potentiated startle to a conditioned stimulus and did not exhibit significant preference for a cocaine-paired chamber over a saline-paired chamber. Likewise, bilateral infusion of a virus expressing the dominant-negative TrkB isoform, TrkB.t1, produced the same deficits in both tasks. Additionally, to complement prior work showing that LV-TrkB.t1 within amygdala impairs extinction of conditioned fear (Chhatwal et al., 2006), we also now show that the same manipulation impairs extinction of the appetitive CPP task. These data suggest that amygdala BDNF is critical for aversive and appetitive learning and that activation of amygdala TrkB receptors similarly enables acquisition and/or consolidation of emotionally salient events.
The promotion of synaptic plasticity (Rattiner et al., 2005; Chhatwal et al., 2006; Musumeci et al., 2009; Cowansage et al., 2010) and LTP (Li et al., 2011) in the amygdala via interaction between BDNF and the TrkB receptor has been shown to facilitate encoding of the CS-US representation during fear conditioning and/or enable effective consolidation of the event. When expression or function of either BDNF or its receptor is impeded in the amygdala, behavioral expression of a fear response to the CS in subsequent tests is significantly blunted, which indicates that training of the CS-US contingency was ineffectual. Inhibition of receptor function alone would not exclude the possibility that plasticity was induced principally by prefrontal cortical release of BDNF during acquisition or consolidation. However, when considered alongside the results from local knockdown of the bdnf gene, these data suggest that production of BDNF within the amygdala is also necessary for a lasting memory of the association to be established. Whether BDNF produced within the BLA is acting solely on local TrkB receptors in the BLA and/or central amygdala in a largely isolated amygdalar circuit or being transported to other downstream targets as well is still unknown. In the current experiments, bdnf knockdown in amygdala would affect BDNF release at terminals, whereas TrkB.t1 overexpression would not. It is therefore important to consider that, although the effects of TrkB inhibition are specific to the infected region, knockdown of bdnf in the amygdala potentially affects multiple behaviorally relevant postsynaptic target sites that receive BDNF via transmission from the BLA.
Although our infusions spanned multiple amygdalar nuclei, bdnf mRNA is absent from the CeA and expression is most prominent in the BLA (Krause et al., 2008). Some, but not all, of our infusions extended beyond the amygdala to surrounding cortical areas that are positive for bdnf mRNA (e.g., endopiriform nulei and, less frequently, the piriform cortex), but infection was not strong in these areas and did not seem to affect behavioral outcomes. We therefore suggest that knockdown of bdnf in the BLA, rather than other amygdalar nuclei or surrounding cortical areas, most likely generated behavioral differences. In contrast, TrkB is present throughout the amygdala and is also known to be expressed in glia (Kumar et al., 1993; Zhou et al., 1993); therefore, we cannot state, based on our data, which region of the amygdala is most reliant on TrkB activation for associative learning or whether expression of the virus in white matter tracts like the external capsule contributed to the behavioral differences.
Despite the opposing valences of fear- and incentive-based learning, there are clear advantages to studying the two motivations in concert. The lateral and basolateral nuclei of the amygdala have long been acknowledged as primary sites of associative learning in fear conditioning, but it is also widely understood that the amygdala codifies information about positively as well as negatively valenced stimuli. What is ostensibly an evolutionary advantage (having one structure that encodes a variety of emotionally disparate events) also potentially represents a risk for developing comorbid pathologies. For instance, in humans, the val66met Bdnf gene variant has been associated with increased risk for substance abuse (Cheng et al., 2005) as well as impaired fear extinction (Frielingsdorf et al., 2010; Lonsdorf et al., 2010; Soliman et al., 2010). This same variant has been shown to affect amygdalar activity during emotional memory formation (van Wingen et al., 2010, Soliman et al., 2010) and can also confer a risk for developing disorders that are based upon opposite motivations (i.e., addiction and post-traumatic stress disorder). There are many shared components in the neurocircuitry underlying aversive and appetitive learning, and exploring the extent to which these pathways overlap will permit a more comprehensive approach to studying healthy and maladaptive consolidation processes that are used in forming memories of emotionally relevant events.
Footnotes
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants R01DA01962 and RC1MH088467, the Center for Behavioral Neuroscience (National Science Foundation agreement IBN-987675), the Burroughs Wellcome Fund, and a National Institutes of Health/National Center for Research Resources base Grant P51RR000165 to Yerkes National Primate Research Center.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Bramham CR, Messaoudi E. BDNF function in adult synaptic plasticity: the synaptic consolidation hypothesis. Prog Neurobiol. 2005;76:99–125. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng CY, Hong CJ, Yu YW, Chen TJ, Wu HC, Tsai SJ. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (Val66Met) genetic polymorphism is associated with substance abuse in males. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 2005;140:86–90. doi: 10.1016/j.molbrainres.2005.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chhatwal JP, Stanek-Rattiner L, Davis M, Ressler KJ. Amygdala BDNF signaling is required for consolidation but not encoding of extinction. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:870–872. doi: 10.1038/nn1718. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi DC, Maguschak KA, Ye K, Jang SW, Myers KM, Ressler KJ. Prelimbic cortical BDNF is required for memory of learned fear but not extinction or innate fear. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:2675–2680. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0909359107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowansage KK, LeDoux JE, Monfils MH. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor: a dynamic gatekeeper of neural plasticity. Curr Mol Pharmacol. 2010;3:12–29. doi: 10.2174/1874467211003010012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis M. The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1992;15:353–375. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.15.030192.002033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Everitt BJ, Cardinal RN, Parkinson JA, Robbins TW. Appetitive behavior: impact of amygdala-dependent mechanisms of emotional learning. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003;985:233–250. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07085.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fanselow MS, LeDoux JE. Why we think plasticity underlying Pavlovian fear conditioning occurs in the basolateral amygdala. Neuron. 1999;23:229–232. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80775-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frielingsdorf H, Bath KG, Soliman F, Difede J, Casey BJ, Lee FS. Variant brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met endophenotypes: implications for posttraumatic stress disorder. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1208:150–157. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05722.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuchs RA, Weber SM, Rice HJ, Neisewander JL. Effects of excitotoxic lesions of the basolateral amygdala on cocaine-seeking behavior and cocaine conditioned place preference in rats. Brain Res. 2002;929:15–25. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)03366-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goosens KA, Maren S. Long-term potentiation as a substrate for memory: evidence from studies of amygdaloid plasticity and Pavlovian fear conditioning. Hippocampus. 2002;12:592–599. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haapasalo A, Sipola I, Larsson K, Akerman KE, Stoilov P, Stamm S, Wong G, Castren E. Regulation of TRKB surface expression by brain-derived neurotrophic factor and truncated TRKB isoforms. J Biol Chem. 2002;277:43160–43167. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M205202200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heldt SA, Ressler KJ. Amygdala-specific reduction of alpha1-GABAA receptors disrupts the anticonvulsant, locomotor, and sedative, but not anxiolytic, effects of benzodiazepines in mice. J Neurosci. 2010;30:7139–7151. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0693-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heldt SA, Stanek L, Chhatwal JP, Ressler KJ. Hippocampus-specific deletion of BDNF in adult mice impairs spatial memory and extinction of aversive memories. Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12:656–670. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001957. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Korte M, Carroll P, Wolf E, Brem G, Thoenen H, Bonhoeffer T. Hippocampal long-term potentiation is impaired in mice lacking brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1995;92:8856–8860. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.19.8856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krause S, Schindowski K, Zechel S, von Bohlen und Halbach O. Expression of trkB and trkC receptors and their ligands brain-derived neurotrophic factor and neurotrophin-3 in the murine amygdala. J Neurosci Res. 2008;86:411–421. doi: 10.1002/jnr.21490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar S, Peña LA, de Vellis J. CNS glial cells express neurotrophin receptors whose levels are regulated by NGF. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 1993;17:163–168. doi: 10.1016/0169-328x(93)90086-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li C, Dabrowska J, Hazra R, Rainnie DG. Synergistic activation of dopamine D1 and TrkB receptors mediate gain control of synaptic plasticity in the basolateral amygdala. PLoS One. 2011;6:e26065. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li YX, Xu Y, Ju D, Lester HA, Davidson N, Schuman EM. Expression of a dominant-negative TrkB receptor, T1, reveals a requirement for presynaptic signaling in BDNF-induced synaptic potentiation in cultured hippocampal neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95:10884–10889. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.18.10884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lonsdorf TB, Weike AI, Golkar A, Schalling M, Hamm AO, Ohman A. Amygdala-dependent fear conditioning in humans is modulated by the BDNFval66met polymorphism. Behav Neurosci. 2010;124:9–15. doi: 10.1037/a0018261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu L, Dempsey J, Liu SY, Bossert JM, Shaham Y. A single infusion of brain-derived neurotrophic factor into the ventral tegmental area induces long-lasting potentiation of cocaine seeking after withdrawal. J Neurosci. 2004;24:1604–1611. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5124-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meis S, Endres T, Lessmann V. Postsynaptic BDNF signaling regulates long-term potentiation at thalamo-amygdala afferents. J Physiol. 2012;590:193–208. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.220434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Musumeci G, Sciarretta C, Rodríguez-Moreno A, Al Banchaabouchi M, Negrete-Díaz V, Costanzi M, Berno V, Egorov AV, von Bohlen Und Halbach O, Cestari V, Delgado-García JM, Minichiello L. TrkB modulates fear learning and amygdalar synaptic plasticity by specific docking sites. J Neurosci. 2009;29:10131–10143. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1707-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Offenhäuser N, Muzio V, Biffo S. BDNF binding to truncated trkB.T1 does not affect gene expression. Neuroreport. 2002;13:1189–1193. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200207020-00024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ou LC, Gean PW. Regulation of amygdala-dependent learning by brain-derived neurotrophic factor is mediated by extracellular signal-regulated kinase and phosphatidylinositol-3-kinase. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:287–296. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pang PT, Teng HK, Zaitsev E, Woo NT, Sakata K, Zhen S, Teng KK, Yung WH, Hempstead BL, Lu B. Cleavage of proBDNF by tPA/plasmin is essential for long-term hippocampal plasticity. Science. 2004;306:487–491. doi: 10.1126/science.1100135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G, Franklin K. The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. New York: Academic; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Rattiner LM, Davis M, French CT, Ressler KJ. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and tyrosine kinase receptor B involvement in amygdala-dependent fear conditioning. J Neurosci. 2004;24:4796–4806. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5654-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rattiner LM, Davis M, Ressler KJ. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor in amygdala-dependent learning. Neuroscientist. 2005;11:323–333. doi: 10.1177/1073858404272255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ressler KJ, Paschall G, Zhou XL, Davis M. Regulation of synaptic plasticity genes during consolidation of fear conditioning. J Neurosci. 2002;22:7892–7902. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-18-07892.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogan MT, Stäubli UV, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature. 1997;390:604–607. doi: 10.1038/37601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Soliman F, Glatt CE, Bath KG, Levita L, Jones RM, Pattwell SS, Jing D, Tottenham N, Amso D, Somerville LH, Voss HU, Glover G, Ballon DJ, Liston C, Teslovich T, Van Kempen T, Lee FS, Casey BJ. A genetic variant BDNF polymorphism alters extinction learning in both mouse and human. Science. 2010;327:863–866. doi: 10.1126/science.1181886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Wingen G, Rijpkema M, Franke B, van Eijndhoven P, Tendolkar I, Verkes RJ, Buitelaar J, Fernández G. The brain-derived neurotrophic factor Val66Met polymorphism affects memory formation and retrieval of biologically salient stimuli. Neuroimage. 2010;50:1212–1218. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou XF, Parada LF, Soppet D, Rush RA. Distribution of trkB tyrosine kinase immunoreactivity in the rat central nervous system. Brain Res. 1993;622:63–70. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)90802-T. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]