Abstract
Background
The effect of the interaction between long-term mental stress and eating habits on weight gain has not been confirmed in humans.
Methods
A population of 1080 healthy Japanese male local government employees without lifestyle-related diseases were studied. Height and weight were measured and perception of mental stress and the frequency of eating to satiety, drinking, smoking, and exercise were surveyed by means of a questionnaire in both 1997 and 2002. Exposure patterns during this 5-year period were classified as low or high. Information on daily food and energy intake was collected in 2002. The effect of the interaction between stress and the frequency of eating to satiety on change in BMI (ΔBMI) during this 5-year period was examined by 2-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and covariance (ANCOVA) adjusted for age, BMI at baseline, and other lifestyle habits. The association between satiation eating and ΔBMI was compared between participants with high and low levels of stress.
Results
Stress and satiation eating were not significantly mutually correlated. Two-way ANCOVA showed a significant interaction (F = 4.90, P = 0.03) between mental stress and satiation eating. Among participants with a high level of stress, BMI gain was significantly larger in those who ate to satiety than in those who ate moderately, when ΔBMI was unadjusted or adjusted for covariates (adjusted mean [SE]: 0.34 ± 0.06 kg/m2 vs. 0.12 ± 0.07 kg/m2, P = 0.002). Among participants with a low level of stress no such difference was observed. These results were unchanged after further adjustment for energy intake in 2002.
Conclusion
In this population, eating pattern interacted with long-term mental stress to produce a larger body mass gain in satiation eaters than in moderate eaters among participants with a high level of stress, independent of energy intake or other lifestyle habits.
Key words: mental stress, eating to satiety, interaction, body mass index gain, energy intake
INTRODUCTION
Abdominal obesity is a key factor in metabolic syndrome, and is linked with a number of life-threatening diseases. Various lifestyle factors are known to cause primary obesity.1 Although the effect of mental stress on obesity has been investigated in both humans2–12 and animals,13–19 the results vary depending on the species of animal studied, and on whether the mental stress was acute or chronic, participants were male or female, the stress was subjective or objective, and on whether the stressor was measured by a standardized method like the Karasek model20 or the Effort–Reward Imbalance Model.21
Recent animal experiments have indicated that the caloric efficiency of energy-dense food that is responsible for weight gain was higher in stressed, as compared to unstressed, rats due to the interaction of stress and food,18,22 and that stressed rats consumed larger proportions of “comfort food” containing lard and sugar.22 Although a growing number of studies that were done on the basis of an individual-difference model8 support the hypothesis that stress-induced eating eventually results in obesity, none has examined the effect of the interaction between mental stress and eating pattern on obesity gain. Furthermore, in most studies, mental stress status was determined by information collected on only one occasion, rather than on multiple occasions years apart.
In this study, we assessed each participant’s self-perception of mental stress, eating pattern, and related lifestyle factors by using questionnaires administered 5 years apart and investigated whether, over this 5-year period, the interaction between mental stress and eating pattern, defined as satiation eating or moderate eating, was associated with body mass gain in a population of Japanese working men without lifestyle-related diseases.
METHODS
Study population
The participants were members of a cohort study on cardiovascular disease that began in 1997 for local government employees in Aichi Prefecture, Japan aged 40 years or older. This cohort comprised 7414 persons (6114 men and 1300 women), 4003 of whom also participated in a self-administered questionnaire survey of lifestyle and disease history in 2002, among whom 2814 (2324 men and 490 women) provided written informed consent for the use of information obtained from the questionnaire and from health check-up data collected in 1997 and 2002. Among 2144 men with no missing data, 1064 men who had a history of cancer, heart disease, stroke, diabetes, hypertension, hyperlipidemia, or hyperuricemia were excluded so that any effects due to medication and intensive lifestyle modification on stress, BMI change, and lifestyle habits would be eliminated. After these exclusions, data from 1080 men remained for analysis. Data from all women were also excluded because there were too few women available for analysis. The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Review Committee of Nagoya University School of Medicine, Nagoya, Japan.
Anthropometry
At the site of the annual health check-up, height and weight were measured to the nearest 0.1 cm and 0.1 kg, without shoes and with light clothing, and body mass index (BMI) was defined as weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters and was recorded every year. Change in BMI (ΔBMI) was determined by subtracting the value in 1997 from that recorded in 2002.
Assessment of lifestyle factors and exposure to stress
Perception of mental stress was assessed by means of a self-administered questionnaire survey in 1997 and 2002. In response to the question, “Do you have much stress in your life?”, participants answered “very much”, “much,” “ordinary,” or “little”. Mental stress was defined as low if a participant answered “ordinary” or “little” in both years; for those who answered “very much” or “much” in at least 1 year, mental stress was defined as high. In a previous study, the validity of these responses was checked against questionnaires on job stressors and other lifestyle factors, including sleeping, exercise, and hours of overtime, and a satisfactory relation was confirmed.23–25
To determine the respondent’s eating pattern, participants were asked: “Do you eat to satiety?” The responses listed in 1997 were: “usually eat to satiety,” “usually eat moderately,” and “don’t know”. The responses were then dichotomized into “usually eat moderately” or “not,” which included the other 2 responses. The choices in 2002 were: “usually eat to satiety,” “tend to eat to satiety,” “tend to eat moderately,” and “usually eat moderately”. For our analysis, the first 2 and second 2 responses were divided to produce 2 dichotomous categories. Based on the combination of dichotomous answers in both years, participants were classified in the manner described above for mental stress as moderate eaters if they chose the second category response in both years or as satiation eaters if they chose the first category response in at least 1 year.
Frequency of physical exercise in each survey year was assessed based on participant agreement with one of the following statements: (1) “Not including job-related activities, I exercise once a month or more.” (2) “I exercise less than once a month or not at all.”
In both years, the choices for smoking status were: “do not smoke,” “have quit smoking,” and “smoke”. These were dichotomized into non-smoker (for the first 2 responses) and current smoker (for the third response). The items regarding alcohol were frequency and amount of ethanol per drink. This information was combined to produce an alcohol intake level of “none,” “less than 23 g/day of ethanol,” and “23 g/day or more,” as described in a previous report,24 and was dichotomized into those who drank less than 23 g/day and those who drank 23 g/day or more. Five-year exposure to each of these habits was assessed in the same manner as that used for mental stress. Statistical differences in these rates between the groups in question were examined by using the chi-square test.
Other measurements considered
Daily energy intake (kcal/day), as well as intakes (g/day) of animal fat and sweets, was obtained individually by using a brief self-administered diet history questionnaire (BDHQ) in 2002 only. Responses were used to evaluate quantitatively the respondent’s eating pattern. The BDHQ is a 4-page, structured questionnaire previously developed by one of the authors.26–29 Estimates of dietary intake of 48 food and beverage items, energy, and 42 kinds of nutrients were calculated based on the Standard Tables of Food Composition in Japan.30 The reliability of the BDHQ itself,26 as well as the short-term test-retest reliability of eating habits,31 has been reported.
Statistical analyses
To examine the effect of the interaction between mental stress and eating patterns on ΔBMI over a 5-year period, we performed 2-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and covariance (ANCOVA) with 2 fixed factors, ie, mental stress and eating pattern, together with the interaction term between them, as well as covariates for adjustment in the ANCOVA. The items used as essential covariates were age, BMI at baseline (1997), and dichotomized 5-year exposure levels of alcohol, smoking, and exercise habits. In additional analysis, daily energy intake in 2002 was further adjusted to exclude the effect of its variation within and between groups.
Then, to examine the effects of mental stress and eating pattern on body mass, ΔBMI was compared between participants classified as moderate eaters and those classified as satiation eaters in the low and high mental stress strata, under conditions unadjusted and adjusted for the covariates listed above. Differences in mean age, BMI, and daily intake of energy, animal fat, and sweets between 2 groups were tested by using the t-test. Intakes of sweets were logarithmically converted for normalization. Differences in the proportions of participants with respective 5-year exposure levels of smoking, drinking, and exercise, and the association between mental stress and eating pattern were tested by using the chi-square test. The difference between mean ΔBMI and unity was tested using the paired t-test. All statistical analyses were performed with the SPSS statistical package, version 15.0. Statistical significance was defined as P < 0.05 for 2-sided testing.
RESULTS
The characteristics of participants are shown in Table 1. The mean age (± SD) was 46.6 ± 4.1 years and mean BMI significantly increased by 0.22 ± 1.18 kg/m2, from 22.62 ± 2.50 kg/m2 in 1997 to 22.84 ± 2.58 kg/m2 in 2002 (P < 0.001, paired t-test). The proportion of participants with high mental stress was 61.9%, and 64.2% reported eating to satiety. In participants, mean intakes of energy, animal fat, and sweets were slightly lower than the mean per capita intakes (2235 kcal/day, 29.3 g/day, and 15.8 g/day, respectively) of males in the same age group in the National Health and Nutrition Survey of the same year. Although the proportion of satiation eaters in the high-stress stratum (65.6% [438/668]; Table 2) was slightly higher than that in the low-stress stratum (61.9% [255/412]), the rates did not significantly differ (P = 0.2, chi-square test), which indicates that eating pattern was not associated with mental stress in this population.
Table 1. Characteristics of participants (N = 1080).
Items | Mean (SD) or Number (%) |
Age in 1997, Mean (SD), years | 46.6 (4.1) |
BMI 1997, Mean (SD), kg/m2 | 22.62 (2.50) |
BMI 2002, Mean (SD), kg/m2 | 22.84 (2.58)* |
ΔBMI, Mean (SD), kg/m2 | 0.22 (1.18) |
Perceived high mental stress, Number (%) | 668 (61.9%) |
Satiation eater, Number (%) | 693 (64.2%) |
Low exerciser, Number (%) | 306 (28.3%) |
Nondrinker or light drinker, Number (%) | 501 (46.4%) |
Non-smoker, Number (%) | 570 (52.8%) |
Quantitative indices from BDHQ in 2002 | |
Energy intake, Mean (SD), kcal/day | 2064 (679) |
Animal fat intake, Mean (SD), g/day | 28.5 (12.8) |
Sweets intake, Mean (SD), g/day | 13.5 (8.1) |
SD: standard deviation; BMI: body mass index; BDHQ: brief dietary habit questionnaire
*: P < 0.001 by Student paired t-test, as compared to the value in 1997.
Table 2. Characteristics of eating groups stratified by level of perceived stress.
Low-stress stratum | High-stress stratum | |||||
Moderate eaters | Satiation eaters | P value | Moderate eaters | Satiation eaters | P value | |
(n = 157) | (n = 255) | (n = 230) | (n = 438) | |||
Age 1997, Mean (SE), years | 47.6 (0.3) | 46.5 (0.3) | 0.007 | 46.5 (0.3) | 46.4 (0.2) | 0.7 |
Low exerciser, Number (%) | 42 (26.8%) | 82 (32.2) | 0.2 | 76 (33.0) | 106 (24.2) | 0.02 |
Nondrinker or light drinker, Number (%) | 78 (49.7%) | 116 (45.5) | 0.4 | 111 (48.3) | 196 (44.7) | 0.4 |
Non smoker, Number (%) | 78 (49.7%) | 140 (54.9) | 0.3 | 112 (48.7) | 240 (54.8) | 0.1 |
BMI 1997, Mean (SE), kg/m2 | 21.83 (0.19) | 22.92 (0.16) | <0.001 | 21.78 (0.14) | 23.17 (0.12) | <0.001 |
BMI 2002, Mean (SE), kg/m2 | 22.05 (0.19) | 23.06 (0.16) | <0.001 | 21.90 (0.15) | 23.50 (0.13) | <0.001 |
ΔBMI, Mean (SE), kg/m2 | 0.23 (0.09) | 0.14 (0.08) | 0.5 | 0.12 (0.07) | 0.33 (0.06) | 0.03 |
Adjusted ΔBMI-1*, Mean (SE), kg/m2 | 0.23 (0.09) | 0.13 (0.08) | 0.8 | 0.12 (0.07) | 0.34 (0.06) | 0.002 |
Adjusted ΔBMI-2†, Mean (SE), kg/m2 | 0.23 (0.09) | 0.13 (0.08) | 0.8 | 0.12 (0.07) | 0.34 (0.06) | 0.002 |
SE: standard error; n: number; BMI: body mass index
*: ΔBMI estimated by adjusting for essential covariates, ie, age, BMI in 1997, and exercise, drinking, and smoking habits
†: ΔBMI estimated by adjusting for essential covariates and energy intake in 2002
The effect of the interaction between mental stress and eating pattern on unadjusted ΔBMI approached significance on 2-way ANOVA (F = 3.76; degrees of freedom: 1, 1076; P = 0.053); however, the effect on ΔBMI after adjustment for essential factors (F = 4.90, P = 0.03) and after further adjustment for energy intake (F = 4.91, P = 0.03) were both significant on ANCOVA.
The ΔBMI and other characteristics of the moderate eaters and satiation eaters in each stratum of perceived stress are shown in Table 2. No group experienced a decrease in BMI during the 5-year period, and mean BMI gain (± SE) was largest among satiation eaters in the high-stress stratum (0.33 ± 0.06 kg/m2) and smallest among moderate eaters in the high-stress stratum (0.12 ± 0.07 kg/m2).
In the low-stress stratum, the mean age (± SE) of satiation eaters was significantly lower than that of moderate eaters (46.5 ± 0.3 vs 47.6 ± 0.3 years, respectively; P = 0.007), and BMI in both 1997 and 2002 was significantly higher among satiation eaters, as compared to moderate eaters (P < 0.001 for both 1997 and 2002). Mean ΔBMI, whether unadjusted or adjusted, and proportions of respondents with a given lifestyle characteristic, did not differ by eating pattern.
In the high-stress stratum, mean age and lifestyle characteristics did not significantly differ between moderate and satiation eaters, except that the latter exercised significantly less often (Table 2). Mean BMI in 1997 and in 2002 were significantly higher among satiation eaters, as compared to moderate eaters, as was the case in the low-stress stratum (P < 0.001 for both 1997 and 2002). ΔBMI was significantly greater among satiation eaters, as compared to moderate eaters, whether unadjusted (0.33 ± 0.06 vs 0.12 ± 0.07 kg/m2, respectively; P = 0.03), adjusted for essential covariates (0.34 ± 0.06 vs 0.12 ± 0.07 kg/m2, respectively; P = 0.002), or adjusted further for energy intake (0.34 ± 0.06 vs 0.12 ± 0.07 kg/m2, respectively; P = 0.002), which was not the case among the low-stress stratum.
The quantitative indices for eating patterns in 2002 are shown in Table 3. In an analysis with both strata combined, satiation eaters had significantly higher mean intakes of energy and sweets. When stratified by stress level, however, only mean energy intake, among the indices examined, was significantly higher among satiation eaters, as compared to moderate eaters (2123 ± 32 vs 2005 ± 45 kcal/day, respectively; P = 0.03), in the high-stress stratum. By contrast, among the low-stress stratum, only intake of sweets was significantly higher in satiation eaters than in moderate eaters (14.1 ± 1.2 vs 7.8 ± 1.2 g/day, respectively; P = 0.01); however, the difference in energy intake did approach statistical significance.
Table 3. Energy, animal fat, and sweets intakes in 2002.
Low-stress stratum | High-stress stratum | Unstratified | |||||||
Moderate eaters | Satiation eaters | P value | Moderate eaters | Satiation eaters | P value | Moderate eaters | Satiation eaters | P value | |
(n = 157) | (n = 255) | (n = 230) | (n = 438) | (n = 387) | (n = 693) | ||||
Energy, Mean (SE), kcal/day | 1964 (54) | 2079 (42) | 0.09 | 2005 (45) | 2123 (32) | 0.03 | 1988 (32) | 2107 (27) | 0.006 |
Animal fat, Mean (SE), g/day | 29.0 (1.1) | 28.1 (0.9) | 0.5 | 27.9 (0.8) | 29.0 (0.6) | 0.3 | 28.3 (0.7) | 28.7 (0.5) | 0.7 |
Sweets, Mean (SE), g/day | 7.8 (1.2) | 14.1 (1.2) | 0.01 | 13.4 (1.1) | 16.1 (1.1) | 0.2 | 10.8 (1.1) | 15.3 (1.1) | 0.008 |
SE: standard error
DISCUSSION
The mean BMI of this population increased over a 5-year period, as it did in the general Japanese population during the same period, as indicated by a series of National Health and Nutrition Surveys.32 This suggests that the increase in body weight was not particular to the present study population. Stress was not related to eating pattern in this healthy male population; thus, the pattern of stress-induced eating that has been observed in women8 was not seen.
In the present analysis, perception of mental stress and eating pattern over a 5-year period had a statistically significant interaction effect on BMI gain during that period, ie, satiation eating was associated with a significantly higher BMI gain than moderate eating among participants who were highly stressed; this association was not observed in those who were less stressed. These results remained unchanged even after ΔBMI was adjusted for age, lifestyle factors, and BMI at baseline.
In animal experiments, stress resulted in an increase in either,19 or both,13,14,18 energy intake and weight, or a reduction in body weight,15–17 depending on the nature and duration of the stress (acute or chronic), diet, and species. Chronic stress caused by isolation housing, however, has been consistently shown to increase both eating and weight.8 Meanwhile, in humans, the association between chronic stress and BMI was independent and positive,4,7 inverse,9 or absent.3,5,6 Thus, the effect of chronic stress on eating habits and weight remains controversial. Gender differences in stress-induced eating8 may partly explain this disagreement. The effect of an interaction between eating pattern and stress on long-term ΔBMI, which has not been previously examined, but was observed in the present study, might also be a factor.
In most studies on humans, chronic mental stress was assessed by using data obtained from one investigation rather than from successive observations. In the present study, data on lifestyle factors were obtained on two occasions 5 years apart, with the exception of energy and food intakes. Hence, the association between lifestyle and ΔBMI over this period should reflect long-term phenomena better than data based on just one data collection, as noted in the study of the association between chronic stress and metabolic syndrome by Chandola et al.33
Among highly stressed participants, energy intake was significantly higher in satiation eaters than in moderate eaters, and this difference in energy intake might have contributed to the difference in BMI gain between these groups. However, this is not consistent with the absence of a difference in ΔBMI between these 2 groups in the low-stress stratum, where the difference in energy intake was of borderline significance and nearly the same as that observed in the high-stress stratum. Moreover, the presence of a significant interaction after adjustment for energy intake suggests that some mechanism other than simple differences in calorie intake and the lifestyle factors examined in the present study might better explain the variation in ΔBMI.
Although elucidating the mechanism of interaction is beyond the scope of this article, recent animal experiments indicate that the caloric efficiency of high energy food, with respect to weight gain, was higher in stressed rats than in control rats,18,22 and that stress enhanced weight gain in genetically predisposed rats that were kept on a high-energy diet.18 Furthermore, in stressed, as compared to unstressed, rats a larger proportion of energy intake was attributable to consumption of comfort food.22 Stressed humans also consumed more high-fat foods.5,10 In the present study, the intake of sweets among the satiation eaters exceeded that of the moderate eaters in the stratum with low stress; however, this tendency was less apparent among those with high stress, a finding that conflicts with those of previous reports.5,10 When this result is considered together with the finding that a significantly greater gain in BMI was seen only in the high-stress stratum, despite the fact that the energy increase related to satiation eating was almost the same in the high- and low-stress strata, it appears that caloric efficiency is somehow increased by stress, and that this mechanism might be the basis of the interaction.
Limitations
This study is subject to the following limitations. Because the present results were obtained in a population with a homogeneous social environment (ie, public servants) and no lifestyle-related disease, the results thus obtained may not necessarily be applicable to the general population. However, any associations observed would be less likely to have been affected by confounders such as medication use and intensive lifestyle modifications.
Second, although so-called stress-induced eating is reported to be more prevalent in women than in men,8 gender differences could not be examined in the present study due to the small number of potential female participants.
Third, although all data on lifestyle factors were collected twice, 5 years apart, quantitative data on energy and food intake were obtained in 2002 only. Because participants with lifestyle-related disease were not included in the analysis, we assumed that energy and food intakes in 2002 were the same as those at baseline (1997). However, a long-term association between change in BMI and energy or food intakes might have been less clear than an association between change in BMI and lifestyle factors, which were assessed twice.
Fourth, the true interaction effect might have been distorted by the coexistence of participants who increased or decreased their exposure to the 2 factors of concern and those who maintained a high exposure. This distorting effect needs to be examined in a larger sample, so as to confirm the interaction between exposures to more refined factors.
Lastly, although we gathered data longitudinally over a 5-year period, the analysis itself was cross-sectional. Thus, a cause-effect relationship cannot be ascertained. To take one example from the present study, the group that had the greatest increase in BMI had the highest mean BMI at baseline, in accordance with the results of a recent study34 of the effect of stress on weight gain in women. Although we adjusted for the difference in BMI in 1997 in our analysis of the effect on ΔBMI, it can still be argued that obesity itself may have been responsible for the greater BMI gain, higher mental stress, and satiation eating. A well-designed longitudinal study to confirm these associations is warranted, in order to examine their applicability and underlying mechanisms in a population that is either older or has a higher prevalence of lifestyle-related diseases.
In conclusion, satiation eaters showed a significantly greater increase in body mass than did moderate eaters only under high-level mental stress, independently of other lifestyle habits as well as energy intake. These findings indicate the presence of an interaction between eating pattern and long-term mental stress in middle-aged healthy male workers.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
We cordially thank all the participants in the study, the healthcare personnel at the local government office, and Drs. Takaaki Kondo, Yoko Hori, and Shankuan Zhu for their cooperation in data acquisition at baseline and thereafter. This study was supported by funds from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (09470112, 13470087 and 17390185 to H.T., 16590499 to K.T., 15689011 and 17790384 to H.Y); the Japan Atherosclerosis Prevention Fund; and a Research Grant for Longevity Sciences from the Ministry of Health, Labor and Welfare of Japan (15C-7 to H.T.).
REFERENCES
- 1.Expert Panel on Detection Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure of High Blood Cholesterol in Adults . Executive Summary of the Third Report of the National Cholesterol Education Program (NCEP) Expert Panel on Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Cholesterol in Adults (Adult Treatment Panel III). JAMA. 2001;285:2486–97. 10.1001/jama.285.19.2486 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Overgaard D , Gyntelberg F , Heitmann BL. Psychological workload and body weight: is there an association? A review of the literature. Occup Med. 2004;54:35–41. 10.1093/occmed/kqg135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kornitzer M , Kittel F. How does stress exert its effects-smoking, diet and obesity, physical activity? Postgrad Med J. 1986;62:695–6. 10.1136/pgmj.62.729.695 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Georges E , Wear ML , Mueller WH. Body fat distribution and job stress in Mexican-American men of the Hispanic Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Am J Hum Biol. 1992;4:657–67. 10.1002/ajhb.1310040512 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hellerstedt WD , Jeffery RW. The association of job strain and health behaviours in men and women. Int J Epidemiol. 1997;26:575–83. 10.1093/ije/26.3.575 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Landsbergis PA , Schnall PL , Deitz DK , Warren K , Pickering TG , Schwartz JE. Job strain and health behaviours: results of a prospective study. Am J Health Promot. 1998;12:237–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Overgaard D , Gamborg M , Gyntelberg F , Heitmann BL. Psychological workload is associated with weight gain between 1993 and 1999: analyses based on the Danish Nurse Cohort Study. Int J Obes. 2004;28:1072–81. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802720 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Greeno CG , Wing RR. Stress-induced eating. Psychol Bull. 1994;115:444–64. 10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.444 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Amick B , Kawachi I , Coakley EH , Lerner D , Levine S , Colditz GA. Relationship of job strain to health status in a cohort of women in the United States. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1998;24:54–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ng DM , Jeffery RW. Relationship between perceived stress and health behaviors in a sample of working adults. Health Psychol. 2003;22:638–42. 10.1037/0278-6133.22.6.638 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kouvonen A , Kivimaki M , Cox SJ , Cox T , Vahtera J. Relationship between work stress and body mass index among 45,810 female and male employees. Psychosom Med. 2005;67:577–83. 10.1097/01.psy.0000170330.08704.62 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Nishitani N , Sakakibara H. Relationship of obesity to job stress and eating behavior in male Japanese workers. Int J Obes. 2006;30:528–33. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rowland NE , Antelman SM. Stress-induced hyperphagia and obesity in rats: A possible model for understanding human obesity. Science. 1976;191:310–2. 10.1126/science.1246617 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Meisel RL , Hays TC , Del Paine SN , Luttrel VR. Induction of obesity by group housing in female Syrian hamsters. Physiol Behav. 1990;47:815–7. 10.1016/0031-9384(90)90002-L [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Levine AS , Morley JE. Stress-induced eating in rats. Am J Physiol. 1981;241:R72–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bhatnagar S , Vining C , Iyer V , Kinni V. Changes in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal function, body temperature, body weight and food intake with repeated social stress exposure in rats. J Neuroendocrinol. 2006;18:13–24. 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2005.01375.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Michel C , Duclos M , Cabanac M , Richard D. Chronic stress reduces body fat content in both obesity-prone and obesity-resistant strains of mice. Horm Behav. 2005;48:172–9. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Michel C , Levin BE , Dunn-Meynell AA. Stress facilitates body weight gain in genetically predisposed rats on medium-fat diet. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2003;285:R791–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sterritt GM , Shemberg K. Inhibition and facilitation of eating by electric shock: II. Shock level, shock schedule and strain of rats. J Psychosom Res. 1963;7:215–22. 10.1016/0022-3999(63)90005-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Karasek R, Theorell T. Healthy work-stress, productivity, and the reconstruction of working life. New York: Basic Books, 1990: 1–381. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Siegrist J. Adverse health effects of high-effort/low reward conditions. J Occup Health Psychol. 1996;1:27–41. 10.1037/1076-8998.1.1.27 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Pecoraro N , Reyes F , Gomez F , Bhargava A , Dallman M. Chronic stress promotes palatable feeding, which reduces signs of stress: Feedforward and feedback effects of chronic stress. Endocrinology. 2004;145:3754–62. 10.1210/en.2004-0305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Otsuka R , Toyoshima H , Tamakoshi K , Wada K , Matsushita K , Takefuji S , et al. . Validity of perceived mental stress evaluated from job stressors and the association between perceived mental stress and lifestyle. Jap J Cardiovasc Dis Prev. 2006;41:62–9 (in Japanese with English abstract). [Google Scholar]
- 24.Otsuka R , Yatsuya H , Tamakoshi K , Matsushita K , Wada K , Toyoshima H. Perceived psychological stress and serum leptin concentration in Japanese men. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2006;14:1832–7. 10.1038/oby.2006.211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Murata C , Yatsuya H , Tamakoshi K , Otsuka R , Wada K , Toyoshima H. Psychological factors and insomnia among male civil servants in Japan. Sleep Med. 2007;8:209–14. 10.1016/j.sleep.2007.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sasaki S. Development and evaluation of dietary assessment methods using biomarkers and diet history questionnaires for individuals. In: Research for evaluation methods of nutrition and dietary lifestyle programs held on Healthy Japan 21 (head investigator: Tanaka H). Summary report. Ministry of Health, Welfare, and Labor, 2004: 10–44. (in Japanese). [Google Scholar]
- 27.Sasaki S , Yanagibori R , Amano K. Self-administered diet history questionnaire developed for health education: a relative validation of the test-version by comparison with 3-day diet record in women. J Epidemiol. 1998;8:203–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Sasaki S , Yanagibori R , Amano K. Validity of a self-administered diet history questionnaire for assessment of sodium and potassium. Comparison with single 24-hour urinary excretion. Jpn Circ J. 1998;62:431–5. 10.1253/jcj.62.431 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Sasaki S , Ushio F , Amano K , Morihara M , Todoroki T , Uehara Y. Serum biomarker–based validation of a self-administered diet history questionnaire for Japanese subjects. J Nutr Sci Vitaminol (Tokyo). 2000;46:285–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Science and Technology Agency. Standard tables of food composition in Japan 5th ed. Tokyo, Printing Bureau of the Ministry of Finance, 2000 (in Japanese). [Google Scholar]
- 31.Yatsuya H , Ohwaki A , Tamakoshi K , Wakai K , Koide K , Otsuka R , et al. . Reproducibility and validity of a simple checklist-type questionnaire for food intake and dietary behavior. J Epidemiol. 2003;13:235–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Yatsuya H , Matsushita K , Tamakoshi K , Otsuka R , Wada K , Toyoshima H. Obesity and its comorbidities; review of current understanding of the epidemiology, etiology, treatment and prevention. Occup Health Rev. 2005;18:71–116 (in Japanese). [Google Scholar]
- 33.Chandola T , Brunner E , Marmot M. Chronic stress at work and the metabolic syndrome: prospective study. BMJ. 2006;332:521–5. 10.1136/bmj.38693.435301.80 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Roberts C , Troop N , Connan F , Treasure J , Campbell C. The effects of stress on body weight: Biological and psychological predictors of change in BMI. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2007;5:3045–55. 10.1038/oby.2007.363 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]