Skip to main content
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience logoLink to Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
. 2014 Feb 18;8:33. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00033

Change in Brainstem Gray Matter Concentration Following a Mindfulness-Based Intervention is Correlated with Improvement in Psychological Well-Being

Omar Singleton 1, Britta K Hölzel 2, Mark Vangel 1, Narayan Brach 3, James Carmody 4, Sara W Lazar 1,*
PMCID: PMC3927233  PMID: 24600370

Abstract

Individuals can improve their levels of psychological well-being (PWB) through utilization of psychological interventions, including the practice of mindfulness meditation, which is defined as the non-judgmental awareness of experiences in the present moment. We recently reported that an 8-week-mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) course lead to increases in gray matter concentration in several brain areas, as detected with voxel-based morphometry of magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo MRI scans, including the pons/raphe/locus coeruleus area of the brainstem. Given the role of the pons and raphe in mood and arousal, we hypothesized that changes in this region might underlie changes in well-being. A subset of 14 healthy individuals from a previously published data set completed anatomical MRI and filled out the PWB scale before and after MBSR participation. PWB change was used as the predictive regressor for changes in gray matter density within those brain regions that had previously shown pre- to post-MBSR changes. Results showed that scores on five PWB subscales as well as the PWB total score increased significantly over the MBSR course. The change was positively correlated with gray matter concentration increases in two symmetrically bilateral clusters in the brainstem. Those clusters appeared to contain the area of the pontine tegmentum, locus coeruleus, nucleus raphe pontis, and the sensory trigeminal nucleus. No clusters were negatively correlated with the change in PWB. This preliminary study suggests a neural correlate of enhanced PWB. The identified brain areas include the sites of synthesis and release of the neurotransmitters, norepinephrine and serotonin, which are involved in the modulation of arousal and mood, and have been related to a variety of affective functions as well as associated clinical dysfunctions.

Keywords: brain stem, mindfulness, well-being, stress, psychological, raphe nuclei

Introduction

Mindfulness meditation, a practice with origins in ancient Buddhist meditation traditions, has long been reported to produce positive effects on psychological well-being (PWB) that extend beyond the time the individual is actually meditating (Ekman and Davidson, 1994; Baer, 2003; Ekman et al., 2005). Taking advantage of these benefits, mindfulness practices have been increasingly incorporated into psychotherapeutic programs over the last three decades (Kabat-Zinn, 1990; Linehan, 1993; Roemer and Orsillo, 2002; Segal et al., 2002; Luoma et al., 2008). Mindfulness is defined as the purposeful and non-judgmental awareness of present-moment experience (Kabat-Zinn, 2003).

Research has shown that interventions incorporating mindfulness training positively affect symptoms of a variety of disorders including anxiety (Roemer et al., 2009; Hofmann et al., 2010), depression (Teasdale et al., 2000; Hofmann et al., 2010), and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Zylowska et al., 2008; van de Weijer-Bergsma et al., 2012). Furthermore, preliminary evidence suggests that mindfulness-based interventions can positively influence sleep and dietary patterns in clinical populations (Baer et al., 2005; Winbush et al., 2007; Dalen et al., 2010).

Recently, neuroimaging studies have begun to explore changes in neural structure and function associated with meditation practice (Davidson, 2003; Lazar et al., 2005; Brefczynski-Lewis et al., 2007; Farb et al., 2007; Pagnoni and Cekic, 2007; Slagter et al., 2007; Hölzel et al., 2008; Lutz et al., 2008). A number of anatomical MRI studies have demonstrated that individuals who have regularly practiced meditation for several years exhibit a different gray matter morphometry in multiple brain regions when compared to demographically matched controls (Lazar et al., 2005; Pagnoni and Cekic, 2007; Hölzel et al., 2008; Luders et al., 2009; Vestergaard-Poulsen et al., 2009). Recently, we reported the first longitudinal study of gray matter changes following an 8-week-mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) course (Hölzel et al., 2011). One region with enhanced gray matter concentration following the MBSR course was in the cerebellar vermis, reaching into a region of the brain stem that included the locus coeruleus, nucleus raphe pontis, pontine tegmentum, and the sensory trigeminal nucleus (Naidich et al., 2008). The locus coeruleus, a site of synthesis of norepinephrine, has been implicated in conditions such as depression and anxiety (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005). Furthermore, this region may play a role in modulating serotonin release (Plaznik and Kostowski, 1983; Grenhoff et al., 1993; Ressler and Nemeroff, 1999). The modulation of levels of serotonin, which is synthesized in the raphe nuclei, has been shown to be one of the most effective treatments for mood and anxiety disorders (Masand and Gupta, 2002). The pontine tegmentum, part of the cholinergic system, is implicated in regulating selective attention, wakefulness, learning, reward, and sleep (Kobayashi and Okada, 2007; Wang and Morales, 2009).

Given that these regions are well-known to modulate several systems, including the serotonin, dopamine, and norepinephrine systems, as well as play central roles in processes such as mood, arousal, sleep, and appetite (Ressler and Nemeroff, 1999; Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005; Winn, 2006; Kobayashi and Okada, 2007; Wang and Morales, 2009; Bailer and Kaye, 2011), we reasoned that gray matter changes in these regions might contribute to enhanced well-being following mindfulness practice. A subset of individuals in our previous study had completed a questionnaire to assess PWB. Therefore, in order to test this hypothesis, we re-analyzed this subgroup of the previous data set and investigated correlations between changes in gray matter concentration and changes in self-report measures of PWB.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The PWB scale was administered to a subsample of 14 participants from our previous study (Hölzel et al., 2011). As described previously (Hölzel et al., 2011), participants were recruited from MBSR courses held at the Center for Mindfulness at the University of Massachusetts Medical School. Individuals were included if they presented as physically and psychologically healthy, scored ≥1 SD above the population mean on the four-item Perceived Stress Scale (PSS; Cohen and Williamson, 1988), had no significant previous meditation experience, were between 25 and 50 years old, had no contra-indications for MRI scanning (i.e., metallic implants, claustrophobia, pregnancy), and made a verbal commitment to attend all eight classes and perform the prescribed daily meditation exercises.

The participants were healthy, right-handed individuals [five male and nine female; mean age: 37.9 years (SD: 4.3 years; age range: 29–44 years)]. Participants had an average of 17.5 years of education (SD: 1.9 years). Ethnicities were: 11 Caucasians, 1 South Asian, 1 African American, 1 multi-ethnic. Participants received a $300 discount in the MBSR course fee (which costs between $475 and $630, depending on the household income) for their participation in the study. Additional analyses that included data from this sample have been reported elsewhere (Hölzel et al., 2010, 2011). The study protocol was approved by the IRBs of Massachusetts General Hospital and the University of Massachusetts Medical School and written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

MRI data collection and analysis

Participants were scanned at the Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging in Charlestown, MA, USA, during the 2 weeks before (Pre) and after (Post) participation in MBSR. High-resolution MRI data were acquired with a Siemens Magnetom Avanto 1.5 T scanner with standard head coil. Data sets of the whole brain were collected using a T1 weighted, magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (MPRAGE) sequence, consisting of 128 sagittal slices (voxel size: 1.0 mm × 1.0 mm × 1.3 mm, TI = 1000 ms; TE = 3.39 ms; TR = 2730 ms, flip angle 7°, matrix 256 mm × 256 mm). Image analysis was performed with VBM tools within the SPM5 neuroimaging statistical software (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm5/) based in MATLAB 7.1, release 14 (Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA, USA), using default settings unless otherwise specified. Images were manually aligned to the anterior commissure and then segmented into gray and white matter in native space (i.e., before normalization, using the “Native Space” segmentation option implemented in SPM5). For each individual, the (unmodulated) gray matter segmentations of the Pre and Post images were spatially coregistered. Normalization parameters were calculated for the Pre scan and were applied to both time-points (trilinear interpolation, 2 mm × 2 mm × 2 mm), to make sure that regional differences between the images were not removed by scan-specific spatial normalization (Driemeyer et al., 2008; Ilg et al., 2008). Images were smoothed using an 8-mm full width at half maximum Isotropic Gaussian Kernel.

Psychological well-being

Psychological well-being was assessed using the 54-item version of the PWB scale (PWB) by Ryff (1989). The PWB is based on a model comprising six factors of PWB (Ryff, 1989; Ryff and Keyes, 1995): self-acceptance (positive attitude toward oneself even while aware of one’s own limitations), positive relations with others (developing and maintaining warm and trusting interpersonal relationships), environmental mastery (managing one’s environment so as to meet personal needs and desires), autonomy (sense of self-determination and personal authority), purpose in life (sense of meaning in one’s effort and challenges), and personal growth (view of self as growing and developing, openness to new experiences). These six factors integrate into a single second-order factor (Ryff and Keyes, 1995). The 54-item version of the PWB scale has been shown to have good psychometric properties (Sewell et al., 2004). Items were rated on a six-point continuum ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. The total score is derived by summing the scores on the six factors.

In a regression analysis using SPM5, Pre- to Post-intervention changes in the PWB total score were correlated with changes in gray matter concentration in regions that have previously been identified as showing an increase in gray matter concentration over the 8-week-MBSR course (Hölzel et al., 2011). In the previous study, we had identified these regions by performing a paired t-test within the group that had undergone the MBSR program, choosing a cluster-size threshold that was corrected for multiple comparisons across the entire brain (i.e., in order to exceed the threshold of p < 0.05, clusters had to exceed a size of 250 voxels) and based on statistical parametric maps with an initially, uncorrected, thresholded of p = 0.01. For the current study, we created a mask that contained the result of that previous study as our new region of interest. To be conservative, we included into this mask all the clusters that displayed a significant increase in gray matter concentration from Pre- to Post-intervention (cf. Table 2 and Figure 2 in Hölzel et al., 2011), namely the clusters within the brainstem/cerebellum, PCC, and left TPJ, i.e., four clusters with a total of 1537 voxels. To obtain images representing the change in gray matter concentration, the Pre-intervention scan was subtracted from the Post-intervention scan. Cluster level statistics for the current analysis are reported on an alpha level of <0.05, multiple comparisons corrected for the search region (height threshold: p = 0.01).

Results

Improvements in psychological well-being

A paired-samples t-test revealed a significant increase in PWB from Pre- to Post-intervention (Pre mean: 224.64, SD: 28.62; Post mean: 252.75, SD: 26.89; t = −4.03; p = 0.001). Pre-Post changes for five of the six scales were also significant: self-acceptance (mean pre: 34.18, SD: 8.18, mean post: 40.93, SD: 6.67, t = −4.21, p = 0.001), environmental mastery (mean pre: 31.68, SD: 5.78, mean post: 36.68, SD: 6.47, t = −2.90, p = 0.012), autonomy (mean pre: 40.07, SD: 7.41, mean post: 44.14, SD: 6.15, t = −2.97, p = 0.011), purpose in life (mean pre: 38.14, SD: 8.02, mean post: 43.14, SD: 4.93, t = −2.66, p = 0.020), and personal growth (mean pre: 43.21, SD: 4.98, mean post: 47.21, SD: 4.93, t = −3.61, p = 0.003). The sixth scale, positive relations with others, revealed a trend toward significance (mean pre: 37.36, SD: 8.85, mean post: 40.64, SD: 7.04, t = −1.87, p = 0.084). When applying the very conservative Bonferroni multiple comparison correction for the six sub-tests, pre-post changes for the scales self-acceptance and personal growth remained significant, but all other subscales missed significance.

Correlation between changes in psychological well-being and changes in gray matter concentration

To address the question of whether increase in gray matter concentration were related to improvements in well-being, the change in the total PWB score was regressed against changes in gray matter concentration within the regions identified in Hölzel et al. (2011). Within the chosen mask, two clusters in the brainstem were identified to be positively correlated with changes in PWB [Figure 1; right cluster: cluster-size k: 43 voxels; p = 0.024; MNI coordinates of peak voxel (x, y, z): 12, −36, −30; left cluster: cluster-size k: 37 voxels; p = 0.040; MNI coordinates of peak voxel (x, y, z): −14, −42, −32]. The more the participants’ PWB improved over the 8-week-MBSR course, the more increase in gray matter concentration was observed in these regions. According to the atlas by Naidich et al. (2008), these clusters appear to contain the area of the pontine tegmentum, locus coeruleus, nucleus raphe pontis, and the sensory trigeminal nucleus bilaterally. For illustrative purposes, values were extracted and averaged across each cluster and plotted with the change in PWB total score (Figure 2). The Pearson coefficients were 0.72 (p = 0.004) for the correlation between PWB change and change in the left brainstem cluster, and 0.76 (p = 0.002) for the correlation between PWB change and change in the right brainstem cluster. Importantly, these numbers are reported only for comparative purposes, and should not be interpreted by themselves, since clusters were derived through searching for correlations with PWB scores in the first place. Using the Tukey-criterion of defining outliers as those values that are further than 1.5 times the interquartile range away from the upper or lower quartile (Tukey, 1977), we identified one single outlier, namely the individual with the highest change in PWB total score. When excluding this outlier from the analysis, the correlation coefficients dropped slightly, but remained significant for the right cluster (r = 0.716, p = 0.006) and almost significant for the left cluster (r = 0.553, p = 0.050). When additionally excluding the individual with the second highest change in PWB total score, the correlation with the left brainstem cluster was no longer significant (r = 0.165, p = 0.609), but the correlation with the cluster in the right side of the brainstem remained significant (r = 0.59, p = 0.043). No clusters or voxels were negatively correlated with the change in PWB. No clusters were negatively correlated with the change in PWB.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Correlation of improvements in psychological well-being and increase in gray matter concentration in the brainstem. Axial slices from z = −38 to −24, with an image every 2 voxels. Significant clusters are overlaid over the group averaged normalized structural MPRAGE image. This analysis includes N = 14 participants.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Correlation of improvement of psychological well-being (PWB) and change in gray matter concentration (GMC). (A) Left brainstem cluster (peak x, y, z: −14, −42, −32; cluster-size k: 37), Pearson correlation coefficient: r = 0.72), (B) right brainstem cluster (peak x, y, z: 12, −36, −30; cluster size k: 43; Pearson correlation coefficient: r = 0.76). For the two regions displayed in Figure 1, values were averaged across the cluster and plotted against the change in PWB. Units indicate the percent change relative to the Pre-intervention baseline.

Discussion

We identified a positive correlation between improvement in PWB and increase in gray matter concentration within regions of the brainstem, suggesting that these morphological changes might be part of a mechanism underlying the changes in PWB. Regions within the brainstem were found to increase in gray matter concentration over the 8 weeks (Hölzel et al., 2011), and the increase within a sub-region of the original area of change was correlated with improvements in PWB. These regions appear to include the area of the locus coeruleus, pontine tegmentum, nucleus raphe pontis, and the sensory trigeminal nucleus (DeArmond et al., 1989; Naidich et al., 2008). Several previous cross-sectional studies have investigated the impact of meditation practice on brain morphology by comparing groups of experienced meditators to non-meditators (Lazar et al., 2005; Pagnoni and Cekic, 2007; Hölzel et al., 2008; Luders et al., 2009; Vestergaard-Poulsen et al., 2009). None of these previous studies assessed the participants’ PWB, and all used a cross-sectional study design, which is usually not as sensitive as a longitudinal design and which suffers from well-known limitations (i.e., the possibility of pre-existing conditions or other life-style differences which may confound results).

The raphe nuclei are a major site of serotonergic neurons (Michelsen et al., 2007), which project widely throughout the brain. Serotonin is implicated in numerous functions including sleep (Monti and Monti, 2000; Monti, 2011), mood, appetite, and conditioned fear. Further, stress has been shown to downregulate serotonin receptors in the raphe nuclei (Fuchs and Flügge, 2003). Modulation of the serotonin system has been profoundly effective for the treatment of a wide range of mood and anxiety disorders (Masand and Gupta, 2002) and the serotonergic neurons of the dorsal raphe nuclei have been implicated in eating disorders (Bailer et al., 2007; Bailer and Kaye, 2011). Interestingly, mindfulness training has been shown to improve a number of conditions for which altered serotonin levels have been implicated, including anxiety and depression (Teasdale et al., 2000; Baer, 2003; Kuyken et al., 2008; Roemer et al., 2009), insomnia (Kuyken et al., 2008; Ong et al., 2009), eating disorders (Kristeller et al., 2006), as well as improvements in sleep patterns (Carlson et al., 2003; Carlson and Garland, 2005; Ong et al., 2009), and attention (Jha et al., 2007). Considering the importance of serotonin for a number of factors that contribute to PWB including mood and sleep patterns, our findings of increased gray matter concentration in the raphe following MBSR that is correlated with improved well-being is highly suggestive.

The locus coeruleus is the site of synthesis and release of the neurotransmitter norepinephrine, and is thought to optimize behavioral performance by modulating arousal, regulating the interplay between focused vs. flexible responding to environmental demands, or selective vs. scanning attention (Aston-Jones et al., 2000, 2007; Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005). The neurons of the locus coeruleus are important in a variety of cognitive, affective, and other behavioral functions, as well as associated clinical dysfunctions (e.g., depression, anxiety, sleep, and circadian disorders; for discussion, see Aston-Jones et al., 2007). It is also one of the primary sites mediating the stress response as well as a site of action of antidepressant drugs (Brady, 1994). This may be related to the influence of the norepinephrine system on perceptions of personal control and autonomy (Bandura et al., 1985; Ryff et al., 2006), which our results (autonomy and environmental mastery subscales) show are improved following MBSR participation. Compared to healthy controls, depressed individuals display reduced gray matter density in this region (Chan-Palay and Asan, 1989; Arango et al., 1996; Ressler and Nemeroff, 1999). Norepinephrine is thought to act as a modulatory agent, modulating serotonin and dopamine release through projections into the ventral tegmental area and the dorsal raphe nuclei (Plaznik and Kostowski, 1983; Grenhoff et al., 1993; Ressler and Nemeroff, 1999). Furthermore, there is evidence that bias toward negative memories and emotions in depression may be related to norepinephrine, and that potentiation of norepinephrine results in increased recognition of positive emotions and more positive emotional bias (Harmer et al., 2003). Several studies have found changes in serum concentration of serotonin and norepinephrine, particularly decrease in norepinephrine and increase in serotonin in meditators (Infante et al., 2001; Solberg et al., 2004; Curiati et al., 2005; Yu et al., 2011).

The pontine tegmentum and its nuclei, the pedunculopontine nucleus and the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus, are also part of the brain’s cholinergic system, and have been indicated as working as a modulatory system influencing learning, reward, sleep/wakefulness, motor function, and attention (Kobayashi and Okada, 2007; Wang and Morales, 2009). The pedunculopontine nucleus and laterodorsal tegmental nucleus neurons send axons to dopamine-containing areas of the ventral tegmental area and the substantia nigra, as well as to the lateral hypothalamus, thalamus, and basal ganglia, wherein glutamine and acetylcholine may act to modulate reward and learning (Yeomans et al., 1993; Steiniger and Kretschmer, 2003). In addition, the pedunculopontine nucleus may play a role in associative learning and reward as a relay for contextual information to midbrain dopamine neurons (Pan and Hyland, 2005). Executive control processes contribute to PWB (e.g., autonomy, environmental mastery; Ryff et al., 2006) and the pontine tegmentum has also been implicated in REM sleep (Fuller et al., 2007), which also correlates positively with well-being (Ryff et al., 2006).

This study comes with several important limitations: first, the sample size is extremely small, and findings are therefore unreliable. Second, with a relatively low resolution of the acquired images and additional smoothing, spatial sensitivity is limited, which is especially relevant when looking at small nuclei. Third, it has been discussed in the literature that segmentation and normalization of the brainstem is particularly problematic (Beissner et al., 2011). The exact localization of the regions identified here therefore needs to be confirmed. Fourth, the brainstem search territory was defined by our previous between-group analysis, and as such might be considered “non-independent” from the current correlation analysis (e.g., Vul et al., 2009, but see also Lieberman et al., 2009; Poldrack and Mumford, 2009). As a consequence, we would like the results of this study to be understood as purely speculative, and hope that they might be used to generate hypotheses for future rigorous research.

An extensive body of research during the last decade has established that MBSR leads to improvements in psychological health and well-being (Grossman et al., 2004; Nyklícek and Kuijpers, 2008; Shapiro et al., 2008). Interestingly, the data presented here suggest well-being is associated with brainstem regions that are the primary production sites of several neurotransmitters and which modulate basic functions of survival (sleep, appetite) and mood/arousal. Given that chronic stress increases the likelihood of developing future psychopathology, knowledge of the neurobiological mechanisms of behavioral interventions used in reducing stress and promoting well-being will be of great clinical interest.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We wish to express our gratitude to all participants for their cooperation. We thank the Center for Mindfulness for conducting the Mindfulness-based stress reduction courses, and Nik Olendzki and Christina Congleton for support in data collection. This research was funded by the National Institutes of Health-NCCAM (R21-AT003425-01A2) and the British Broadcasting Company (BBC). Britta K. Hölzel was supported by a Marie Curie International Outgoing Fellowship within the Seventh European Community Framework Programme and the Kusala Foundation. Sara W. Lazar was supported by National Institutes of Health funding K01AT00694. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  1. Arango V., Underwood M. D., Mann J. J. (1996). Fewer pigmented locus coeruleus neurons in suicide victims: preliminary results. Biol. Psychiatry 39, 112–120 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00107-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aston-Jones G., Cohen J. (2005). An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 403–450 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aston-Jones G., Iba M., Clayton E., Rajkowski J., Cohen J. (2007). “The locus coeruleus and regulation of behavioral flexibility and attention: clinical implications”, in Brain Norepinephrine: Neurobiology and Therapeutics, eds Ordway G. A., Schwartz M. A., Frazer A. (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; ), 196–235 [Google Scholar]
  4. Aston-Jones G., Rajkowski J., Cohen J. (2000). Locus coeruleus and regulation of behavioral flexibility and attention. Prog. Brain Res. 126, 165–182 10.1016/S0079-6123(00)26013-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baer R. A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: a conceptual and empirical review. Clin. Psychol. 10, 125–143 10.1093/clipsy/bpg015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baer R. A., Fischer S., Huss D. B. (2005). Mindfulness and acceptance in the treatment of disordered eating. J. Ration. Emot. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 23, 281–300 10.1007/s10942-005-0015-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bailer U. F., Frank G. K., Henry S. E., Price J. C., Meltzer C. C., Becker C., et al. (2007). Serotonin transporter binding after recovery from eating disorders. Psychopharmacology 195, 315–324 10.1007/s00213-007-0896-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bailer U. F., Kaye W. H. (2011). Serotonin: imaging findings in eating disorders. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 6, 59–79 10.1007/7854_2010_78 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bandura A., Taylor C. B., Williams S. L., Mefford I. N., Barchas J. D. (1985). Catecholamine secretion as a function of perceived coping self-efficacy. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 53, 406–414 10.1037/0022-006X.53.3.406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Beissner F., Deichmann R., Baudrexel S. (2011). fMRI of the brainstem using dual-echo EPI. Neuroimage. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811911000772 [DOI] [PubMed]
  11. Brady L. S. (1994). Stress, antidepressant drugs, and the locus coeruleus. Brain Res. Bull. 35, 545–556 10.1016/0361-9230(94)90168-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brefczynski-Lewis J. A., Lutz A., Schaefer H. S., Levinson D. B., Davidson R. J. (2007). Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 11483–11488 10.1073/pnas.0606552104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Carlson L. E., Garland S. N. (2005). Impact of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on sleep, mood, stress and fatigue symptoms in cancer outpatients. Int. J. Behav. Med. 12, 278–285 10.1207/s15327558ijbm1204_9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Carlson L. E., Speca M., Patel K. D., Goodey E. (2003). Mindfulness-based stress reduction in relation to quality of life, mood, symptoms of stress, and immune parameters in breast and prostate cancer outpatients. Psychosom. Med. 65, 571–581 10.1097/01.PSY.0000074003.35911.41 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chan-Palay V., Asan E. (1989). Quantitation of catecholamine neurons in the locus coeruleus in human brains of normal young and older adults and in depression. J. Comp. Neurol. 287, 357–372 10.1002/cne.902870307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cohen S., Williamson G. M. (1988). “Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States,” in The Social Psychology of Health: The Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology, eds Spacapan S., Oskamp S. (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.), 31–67 [Google Scholar]
  17. Curiati J. A., Bocchi E., Freire J. O., Arantes A. C., Braga M., Garcia Y., et al. (2005). Meditation reduces sympathetic activation and improves the quality of life in elderly patients with optimally treated heart failure: a prospective randomized study. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 11, 465–472 10.1089/acm.2005.11.465 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dalen J., Smith B. W., Shelley B. M., Sloan A. L., Leahigh L., Begay D. (2010). Pilot study: Mindful Eating and Living (MEAL): weight, eating behavior, and psychological outcomes associated with a mindfulness-based intervention for people with obesity. Complement. Ther. Med. 18, 260–264 10.1016/j.ctim.2010.09.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Davidson R. J. (2003). Alterations in brain and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation. Psychosom. Med. 65, 564–570 10.1097/01.PSY.0000077505.67574.E3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. DeArmond S. J., Fusco M. M., Dewey M. M. (1989). Structure of the Human Brain: A Photographic Atlas. Oxford University Press; Available at: http://www.amazon.com/Structure-Human-Brain-Photographic-Atlas/dp/019504357X [Google Scholar]
  21. Driemeyer J., Boyke J., Gaser C., Büchel C., May A. (2008). Changes in gray matter induced by learning – revisited. PLoS ONE 3:e2669. 10.1371/journal.pone.0002669 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ekman P., Davidson R. J. (1994). The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions (Series in Affective Science). Oxford University Press, 512 Available at: http://www.amazon.com/Nature-Emotion-Fundamental-Questions-Affective/dp/0195089448 [Google Scholar]
  23. Ekman P., Davidson R. J., Ricard M., Alan Wallace B. (2005). Buddhist and psychological perspectives on emotions and well-being. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 14, 59–63 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00335.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Farb N. A. S., Segal Z. V., Mayberg H., Bean J., McKeon D., Fatima Z., et al. (2007). Attending to the present: mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 313–322 10.1093/scan/nsm030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fuchs E., Flügge G. (2003). Chronic social stress: effects on limbic brain structures. Physiol. Behav. 79, 417–427 10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00161-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Fuller P. M., Saper C. B., Lu J. (2007). The pontine REM switch: past and present. J. Physiol. 584(Pt 3), 735–741 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.140160 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Grenhoff J., Nisell M., Ferr S., Aston-Jones G., Svensson T. H. (1993). Noradrenergic modulation of midbrain dopamine cell firing elicited by stimulation of the locus coeruleus in the rat. J. Neural Transm. 93, 11–25 10.1007/BF01244934 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Grossman P., Niemann L., Schmidt S., Walach H. (2004). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and health benefits: a meta-analysis. J. Psychosom. Res. 57, 35–43 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00573-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Harmer C. J., Hill S. A., Taylor M. J., Cowen P. J., Goodwin G. M. (2003). Toward a neuropsychological theory of antidepressant drug action: increase in positive emotional bias after potentiation of norepinephrine activity. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 990–992 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.5.990 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hofmann S. G., Sawyer A. T., Witt A. A., Oh D. (2010). The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and depression: a meta-analytic review. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 78, 169–183 10.1037/a0018555 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hölzel B. K., Carmody J., Evans K. C., Hoge E. A., Dusek J. A., Morgan L., et al. (2010). Stress reduction correlates with structural changes in the amygdala. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 5, 11–17 10.1093/scan/nsp034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hölzel B. K., Carmody J., Vangel M., Congleton C., Yerramsetti S. M., Gard T., et al. (2011). Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry Res. 191, 36–43 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.08.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hölzel B. K., Ott U., Gard T., Hempel H., Weygandt M., Morgen K., et al. (2008). Investigation of mindfulness meditation practitioners with voxel-based morphometry. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 3, 55–61 10.1093/scan/nsm038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Ilg R., Wohlschläger A. M., Gaser C., Liebau Y., Dauner R., Wöller A., et al. (2008). Gray matter increase induced by practice correlates with task-specific activation: a combined functional and morphometric magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 28, 4210–4215 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5722-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Infante J. R., Torres-Avisbal M., Pinel P., Vallejo J. A., Peran F., Gonzalez F., et al. (2001). Catecholamine levels in practitioners of the transcendental meditation technique. Physiol. Behav. 72, 141–146 10.1016/S0031-9384(00)00386-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Jha A. P., Krompinger J., Baime M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 109–119 10.3758/CABN.7.2.109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kabat-Zinn J. (1990). Full Catastrophe Living: Using the Wisdom of Your Body and Mind to Face Stress, Pain, and Illness. New York, NY: Delacorte Press [Google Scholar]
  38. Kabat-Zinn J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clin. Psychol. Sci.Pract. 10, 144–156 10.1093/clipsy.bpg016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kobayashi Y., Okada K.-I. (2007). Reward prediction error computation in the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus neurons. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1104, 310–323 10.1196/annals.1390.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kristeller J. L., Baer R. A., Quillian-Wolever R. (2006). “Mindfulness-based approaches to eating disorders,” in Mindfulness-Based Treatment Approaches: Clinician’s Guide to Evidence Base and Applications, ed. Baer R. (San Diego, CA: Academic Press; ), 1–32 [Google Scholar]
  41. Kuyken W., Byford S., Taylor R. S., Watkins E., Holden E., White K., et al. (2008). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy to prevent relapse in recurrent depression. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 76, 966–978 10.1037/a0013786 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lazar S. W., Kerr C. E., Wasserman R. H., Gray J. R., Greve D. N., Treadway M. T., et al. (2005). Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport 16, 1893–1897 10.1097/01.wnr.0000186598.66243.19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lieberman M. D., Berkman E. T., Wager T. D. (2009). Correlations in social neuroscience aren’t voodoo: commentary on Vul et al. (2009). Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4, 299–307 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01128.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Linehan M. M. (1993). Cognitive-Behavioral Treatment of Borderline Personality Disorder. New York: Guilford Press [Google Scholar]
  45. Luders E., Toga A. W., Lepore N., Gaser C. (2009). The underlying anatomical correlates of long-term meditation: larger hippocampal and frontal volumes of gray matter. Neuroimage 45, 672–678 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.061 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Luoma J. B., Kohlenberg B. S., Hayes S. C., Bunting K., Rye A. K. (2008). Reducing self-stigma in substance abuse through acceptance and commitment therapy: model, manual development, and pilot outcomes. Addict. Res. Theory 16, 149–165 10.1080/16066350701850295 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lutz A., Brefczynski-Lewis J., Johnstone T., Davidson R. J. (2008). Regulation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: effects of meditative expertise. PLoS ONE 3:e1897. 10.1371/journal.pone.0001897 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Masand P., Gupta S. (2002). Long-term side effects of newer-generation antidepressants: SSRIS, venlafaxine, nefazodone, bupropion, and mirtazapine. Ann. Clin. Psychiatry 14, 175–182 10.3109/10401230209147454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Michelsen K. A., Schmitz C., Steinbusch H. W. M. (2007). The dorsal raphe nucleus – from silver stainings to a role in depression. Brain Res. Rev. 55, 329–342 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Monti J. M. (2011). Serotonin control of sleep-wake behavior. Sleep Med. Rev. 15, 269–281 10.1016/j.smrv.2010.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Monti J. M., Monti D. (2000). Role of dorsal raphe nucleus serotonin 5-HT1A receptor in the regulation of REM sleep. Life Sci. 66, 1999–2012 10.1016/S0024-3205(99)00649-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Naidich T. P., Duvernoy H. M., Delman B. N., Sorensen A. G., Kollias S. S., Haacke E. M. (2008). Duvernoy’s Atlas of the Human Brain Stem and Cerebellum: High-Field MRI, Surface Anatomy, Internal Structure, Vascularization and 3 D Sectional Anatomy. Springer, 888 Available at: http://www.amazon.com/Duvernoys-Atlas-Human-Brain-Cerebellum/dp/321173970X [Google Scholar]
  53. Nyklícek I., Kuijpers K. F. (2008). Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction intervention on psychological well-being and quality of life: is increased mindfulness indeed the mechanism? Ann. Behav. Med. 35, 331–340 10.1007/s12160-008-9030-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ong J. C., Shapiro S. L., Manber R. (2009). Mindfulness meditation and cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia: a naturalistic 12-month follow-up. Explore (NY) 5, 30–36 10.1016/j.explore.2008.10.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Pagnoni G., Cekic M. (2007). Age effects on gray matter volume and attentional performance in Zen meditation. Neurobiol. Aging 28, 1623–1627 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.06.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Pan W.-X., Hyland B. I. (2005). Pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus controls conditioned responses of midbrain dopamine neurons in behaving rats. J. Neurosci. 25, 4725–4732 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0277-05.2005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Plaznik A., Kostowski W. (1983). The interrelationship between brain noradrenergic and dopaminergic neuronal systems in regulating animal behavior: possible clinical implications. Psychopharmacol. Bull. 19, 5–11 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Poldrack R. A., Mumford J. A. (2009). Independence in ROI analysis: where is the voodoo? Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 4, 208–213 10.1093/scan/nsp011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ressler K. J., Nemeroff C. B. (1999). Role of norepinephrine in the pathophysiology and treatment of mood disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 46, 1219–1233 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00127-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Roemer L., Lee J. K., Salters-Pedneault K., Erisman S. M., Orsillo S. M., Mennin D. S. (2009). Mindfulness and emotion regulation difficulties in generalized anxiety disorder: preliminary evidence for independent and overlapping contributions. Behav. Ther. 40, 142–154 10.1016/j.beth.2008.04.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Roemer L., Orsillo S. M. (2002). Expanding our conceptualization of and treatment for generalized anxiety disorder: integrating mindfulness/acceptance-based approaches with existing cognitive-behavioral models. Clin. Psychol. 9, 54–68 10.1093/clipsy.9.1.54 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  62. Ryff C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1069–1081 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Ryff C. D., Dienberg Love G., Urry H. L., Muller D., Rosenkranz M. A., Friedman E. M., et al. (2006). Psychological well-being and ill-being: do they have distinct or mirrored biological correlates? Psychother. Psychosom. 75, 85–95 10.1159/000090892 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Ryff C. D., Keyes C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological well-being revisited. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 719–727 10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.719 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Segal Z. V., Teasdale J. D., Williams J. M., Gemar M. C. (2002). The mindfulness-based cognitive therapy adherence scale: inter-rater reliability, adherence to protocol and treatment distinctiveness. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 9, 131–138 10.1002/cpp.320 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. Sewell W. H., Hauser R. M., Springer K. W., Hauser T. S. (2004). As we age: the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study, 1957–2001. Res. Soc. Stratif. Mobil. 20, 3–111 10.1016/S0276-5624(03)20001-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Shapiro S. L., Oman D., Thoresen C. E., Plante T. G., Flinders T. (2008). Cultivating mindfulness: effects on well-being. J. Clin. Psychol. 64, 10.1002/jclp.20491 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Slagter H. A., Lutz A., Greischar L. L., Francis A. D., Nieuwenhuis S., Davis J. M., et al. (2007). Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biol. 5:e138. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Solberg E. E., Holen A., Ekeberg Ø., Østerud B., Halvorsen R., Sandvik L. (2004). The effects of long meditation on plasma melatonin and blood serotonin. Med. Sci. Monit. 10, CR96–CR101 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Steiniger B., Kretschmer B. D. (2003). Glutamate and GABA modulate dopamine in the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus. Exp. Brain Res. 149, 422–430 10.1007/s00221-003-1382-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Teasdale J. D., Segal Z. V., Williams J. M. G., Ridgeway V. A., Soulsby J. M., Lau M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68, 615–623 10.1037/0022-006X.68.4.615 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Tukey J. W. (1977). Exploratory Data Analysis. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley [Google Scholar]
  73. van de Weijer-Bergsma E., Formsma A. R., de Bruin E. I., Bögels S. M. (2012). The effectiveness of mindfulness training on behavioral problems and attentional functioning in adolescents with ADHD. J. Child Fam. Stud. 21, 775–787 10.1007/s10826-011-9531-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Vestergaard-Poulsen P., van Beek M., Skewes J., Bjarkam C. R., Stubberup M., Bertelsen J., et al. (2009). Long-term meditation is associated with increased gray matter density in the brain stem. Neuroreport 20, 170–174 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328320012a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Vul E., Harris C., Winkielman P., Pashler H. (2009). Puzzlingly high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4, 274–290 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01125.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Wang H.-L., Morales M. (2009). Pedunculopontine and laterodorsal tegmental nuclei contain distinct populations of cholinergic, glutamatergic and GABAergic neurons in the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 340–358 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06576.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Winbush N. Y., Gross C. R., Kreitzer M. J. (2007). The effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on sleep disturbance: a systematic review. Explore (NY) 3, 585–591 10.1016/j.explore.2007.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Winn P. (2006). How best to consider the structure and function of the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus: evidence from animal studies. J. Neurol. Sci. 248, 234–250 10.1016/j.jns.2006.05.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Yeomans J. S., Mathur A., Tampakeras M. (1993). Rewarding brain stimulation: role of tegmental cholinergic neurons that activate dopamine neurons. Behav. Neurosci. 107, 1077–1087 10.1037/0735-7044.107.6.1077 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Yu X., Fumoto M., Nakatani Y., Sekiyama T., Kikuchi H., Seki Y., et al. (2011). Activation of the anterior prefrontal cortex and serotonergic system is associated with improvements in mood and EEG changes induced by Zen meditation practice in novices. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 80, 103–111 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Zylowska L., Ackerman D. L., Yang M. H., Futrell J. L., Horton N. L., Hale T. S., et al. (2008). Mindfulness meditation training in adults and adolescents with ADHD: a feasibility study. J. Atten. Disord. 11, 737–746 10.1177/1087054707308502 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Human Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES