Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2014 Feb 18.
Published in final edited form as: J Alzheimers Dis Parkinsonism. 2013 Aug 26;3(2):124. doi: 10.4172/2161-0460.1000124

Poor Sleep as a Precursor to Cognitive Decline in Down Syndrome : A Hypothesis

Fabian Fernandez 1,*, Jamie O Edgin 2
PMCID: PMC3928031  NIHMSID: NIHMS552297  PMID: 24558640

Abstract

We propose that sleep disruption is a lever arm that influences how cognition emerges in development and then declines in response to Alzheimer disease in people with Down syndrome. Addressing sleep disruptions might be an overlooked way to improve cognitive outcomes in this population. This article is a contribution to a Special Issue on Down Syndrome curated by the editors of the Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Parkinsonism.

Keywords: Down syndrome, Alzheimer’s disease, Sleep disruption, Circadian arrhythmia


Trisomy for human chromosome 21 (Hsa21; Down syndrome, DS) results in a unique trajectory of developmental outcomes for the brain and craniofacial skeleton. In those with DS, the brain’s overall complexity is reduced owing to fewer cells and narrowing within the sulci and gyri that form its outer contour [1, 2]. Local and interregional connectivity may be altered due to weaker insulation of long-range projections by myelin [36], and to differences in how synapses and dendrites mature in response to behavioral experience [7, 8]. Infants with DS exhibit smaller neurocraniums at birth and altogether different morphometric relationships between individual bones of the craniofacial skeleton [9]. These divergent craniofacial features impact breathing [10]. Specifically, tissue crowding produced by midface hypoplasia and realigned soft tissue (i.e., constriction of the pharynx and palate, posterior displacement of the tongue, enlarged tonsilsadenoids) curtails airflow through the upper respiratory tract [1117]. Propensity toward obesity and reduced muscle tone put further strains on the upper airways [18]. As a result of these medical issues, virtually all people with DS will present symptoms associated with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome (OSAS) and sleep fragmentation [1824]. The loss of sleep quality wrought by OSAS is predicted to contribute to at least some of the everyday intellectual difficulties experienced by people with DS, presenting a case study in how developmental pathways affected by an extra copy of Hsa21 interact at a macro-level to influence cognitive outcomes later in life.

In this review, we discuss recent evidence linking sleep disruption to Alzheimer disease (AD) and cognitive decline in the typical non-DS population, and stipulate that DS is a population in which these elements might converge. Given the high prevalence of OSAS and AD in DS, we propose that sleep disruption could predispose individuals with DS to earlier onset or faster deterioration with dementia. These factors may be inextricably connected to the DS phenotype-to the extent that all future studies on cognitive development from infancy or cognitive decline in DS must take problematic sleep into account.

Intellectual Disability and Sleep Disturbance: Doubling Down

The neurocognitive profile of DS is characterized, in part, by deficits in the structure-function of an episodic memory network that unifies the frontal and medial temporal lobes [2533]. Pathology weighs most heavily at the metaphoric poles of this system: the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and hippocampus [3439]. The frontal lobes of children with DS show truncated growth within the first 6 months of life [1, 2], fewer convolutions in adulthood [36], and shrinkage within the PFC and surrounding tissue during aging [40]. Concomitant with these volume reductions are failures in subcomponents of working memory, including the phonological loop, central executive, and episodic buffer [34, 4143]. Several neuroimaging and psychometric studies have confirmed that the hippocampus never fully develops in children with DS [4446] and then atrophies with aging [4749]. The selectivity with which the hippocampus is disproportionately affected by trisomy 21 is striking given the otherwise global consequences expected of chromosomal triplication. This emphasis suggests that the hippocampus and the circuitry that binds it within the episodic memory network are sites where multiple atypical developmental processes ferment to steer impaired intellectual function in DS. Below we suggest that sleep disturbance and overexpression of the amyloid precursor protein (APP) gene on Hsa21 are two factors that further erode the integrity of this memory system.

In the non-DS population, OSAS has been shown to impair performance in neuropsychological assessments of learning and memory. Children or adolescents meeting polysomnographic (PSG) criteria for OSAS score significantly lower on full scale tests of IQ and particular measures of executive function [5052], while demonstrating reduced metabolic markers in the hippocampus and frontal cortex [52]. Young or middle-aged adults with OSAS also show compromised working memory and attention, and fairly specific deactivation or atrophy in the same brain regions [5362]. While these data raise the possibility that OSAS contributes to intellectual disabilities in people with DS, research on the relationship between sleep fragmentation and cognitive outcomes in this population is scarce.

Evidence for OSAS (e.g., elevated apnea-hypopnea index, oxygen desaturation) appears as early as infancy in DS, with the majority of toddlers demonstrating sleep fragmentation and arousals by age 2–4 years [1823]. Longitudinal EEG analyses from birth indicate that infants with DS spend less time in slow wave sleep (SWS) than non-DS infants and exhibit less spindle activity [63]. Miano et al. found that a group of children with DS experienced more fragmented sleep and reduced stage 2 non-rapid eye movement (nREM) sleep relative to a group with Fragile X syndrome (FXS) and controls [64]. Both FXS and DS displayed increased Stage 1 and decreased REM sleep [64]. More recent studies have confirmed the pattern of excessive sleep fragmentation in DS in comparison to individuals with Williams syndrome and typical controls [65]. These data are consistent with those from DS mouse models (e.g., Ts65dn), which show increased wake at the expense of non-REM periods [66]. Declines in SWS have not been consistently reported in DS, however, with one study in adults suggesting greater SWS than typical adult control subjects presenting with OSAS [67]. In order for links to be made between altered neural function in these stages to cognitive outcomes, more work is needed to track the developmental progression of sleep architecture in DS.

Ongoing studies by Edgin et al. [26] have found that deficiencies in SWS are prevalent during childhood and adolescence in those with DS comorbid for OSAS, and that OSAS might explain some of the variability associated with verbal IQ, memory, and executive function in an unreferred DS community cohort (submitted). In this cohort verbal IQ related to SWS. Two other investigations have related sleep disruption to cognitive outcomes in adults with DS, revealing correlations between OSAS and frontally mediated neuropsychological tasks, such as that measured on a nonverbal IQ test (e.g., Raven’s Progressive Matrices) and other assessments of executive function [68, 69].

How does OSAS weaken cognition in DS and non-DS populations? One possibility is that OSAS limits SWS-dependent memory consolidation, a phenomenon where information from recent episodic memories is integrated into preexisting knowledge [70, 71]. The unique brain oscillations witnessed during SWS are thought to provide windows of plasticity that allow for efficient hippocampal-cortical dialogue. When concurrent with reactivation of nascent experiences, they support the strengthening and integration of memory representations within neocortex [70, 71]. In one tangible example of this process, Gais et al. [72] demonstrated that sleep after word-pair learning in typical adults strengthens functional connectivity between the hippocampus and medial PFC 48-h after encoding, but correlates with preferential PFC activation at recall 6 months later [72]. Boosting SWS oscillations artificially with transcranial stimulation enhances retention of paired-associates learning [73]. Similar findings have been documented in children, suggesting that sleep provides a special state that facilitates memory consolidation across development [7476]. This body of work highlights the importance of SWS and REM periods for consolidating and abstracting information in children and adults. The mechanisms by which poor sleep impairs cognition in DS plausibly reside here.

OSAS might also impact cognition by inhibiting restorative sleep or perturbing homeostatic synaptic scaling, a phenomenon where synaptic strength built-up over the day is downregulated in the evening [77]. Energy substrates like ATP surge during SWS and provide the energetics necessary for immune and nervous system maintenance, and somatic growth and repair [78]. Suppression of SWS interferes with growth hormone secretion and decreases the body’s ability to store glucose effectively, which can indirectly affect learning and memory [79, 80]. Loss of restorative sleep is likely to influence cognitive outcomes in those with DS who are already at risk for a number of metabolic disorders, including obesity and hypothyroidism [8184]. But this influence is hard to quantify and is not mutually exclusive from many of the other factors discussed in the current review. An OSAS mechanism that has received decidedly more clinical attention for its role in precipitating cognitive decline is brain injury via recurrent intermittent hypoxemia [85]. Hypoxia has been invoked to explain how OSAS accelerates the progression of Alzheimer neuropathology, which first localizes to the frontal cortex and medial temporal lobe, and quickens the transition from mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to dementia recently documented in several large cohorts of typical adults [8688]. While sleep-disordered breathing and hypoxemia presage the risk of cognitive impairment in older community-dwelling women as measured by the Mini-Mental State Exam [88], a dementia scale, other studies in middle-aged adults suggest that sleep fragmentation (e.g., arousal index, sleep duration) might be just as significant a variable in OSAS related memory problems [62]. Whether sleep fragmentation or hypoxemia accelerates MCI-AD in DS as it does in the general population remains an important unexplored question.

Experiments using in vitro assays and mouse models of AD show that hypoxia promotes AD by driving production of Aβ. Hypoxic neurons upregulate the enzymatic activities of β- and γ-secretases, leading to higher APP cleavage, increased Aβ secretion, and self-aggregation of Aβ into amyloid fibrils and thicker plaque deposits (i.e., the classic amyloid hypothesis of AD) [reviewed in 85]. This cascade from hypoxemia → Aβ generation is likely at work in people with DS, and aside from direct hypoxic injury, provides some added functional insight into how OSAS could further debilitate cognition. An evolving understanding of the interactions between sleep and Aβ metabolism in the non-DS brain suggests that the interplay between APP gene dosage and OSAS in DS might set in motion cycles of progressively worse developmental and cognitive outcomes.

Intellectual Disability, Sleep Fragmentation, and Aβ: Spiraling Down

In an elegant series of investigations that have stretched from “bench-to-bedside,” Kang et al. [89] have shown that interstitial fluid levels of Aβ in the hippocampus of wildtype mice and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) levels in healthy human volunteers oscillate with the circadian or “sleep-wake” cycle [89, 90]. Aβ secretion is highest during the active period and lowest during rest, scaling proportionately to the total time spent awake [89]. Negative correlations between Aβ and time spent asleep are most pronounced during SWS [89]. The association of Aβ with wakefulness stems from the fact that Aβ production in the brain normally occurs in an activity-dependent fashion [9194]. Local synaptic activity or regional energy use during the active period triggers APP internalization and processing of Aβ for exocytotic release [9193]. Not surprisingly, parts of the brain exhibiting the most functional connectivity or those that are repetitively tapped like the default mode network tend to demonstrate the highest amyloid plaque burdens upon AD onset [94, 95].

Acute and chronic sleep deprivation studies performed by the Holtzman laboratory in human APP transgenic (Tg2576) or double transgenic (APPswe/PS1δE9) mice reveal that prolonged wakefulness over 24 h significantly enhances Aβ levels in the hippocampus, and over 3 weeks, greater plaque deposition in the MTL [89, 90]. Via a destructive positive feedback loop, aggregation of Aβ for longer stretches will then disrupt the sleep-wake cycle, increase time spent awake, and decrease SWS [90]. People with mutations in the presenilin (PS) AD susceptibility gene and confirmed Aβ deposition display circadian arrhythmia, and provide some preliminary data hinting that the feedback loop highlighted in mice is also active in humans [90]. The prospect that sleep fragmentation and Aβ may feed off one another in the non-DS population to prime AD is sobering considering that individuals with DS are genetically predisposed to secrete more Aβ and to present with OSAS from the moment they are born.

It is widely understood that those with DS are trisomic for the APP gene and overproduce Aβ in quantities that accelerate the formation of plaques in the brain relative to the non-DS population [96]. The developmental timing of these events is sometimes less appreciated. Immunoreactivity for soluble Aβ1–42 has routinely appeared in the youngest children with DS evaluated, from 5 months gestation to 3–4 years of age in the temporal cortex [97, 98], to 8 years of age in the parahippocampal gyrus and other areas of the MTL [99]. Enlarged endosomes, a morphological signature of AD connected to Aβ generation in neural cells, are also noted as early as 7 months gestation [100]. Inevitably, diffuse and senile plaque deposits materialize during adolescence and the early twenties in the hippocampus and PFC [101103].

What becomes prima facie obvious is that people affected by DS are possibly AD-presymptomatic their entire lives. The developmental trajectories in the brain that guide expansion of cognitive skill sets and language, already set astray by Hsa21 trisomy, overlap with trajectories that have been more historically associated with mild cognitive impairment. The overlap is conspicuous in slow developing systems like the episodic memory network, which might not even be fully mature by the time “late-life” degenerating processes start that erode tissue integrity and the quality with which information is acquired and consolidated. Because of the biological complexity of DS, changes in other organs such as the skeleton unexpectedly influence these dynamics. Compression of the mid-face triggers OSAS, hypoxemia, and sleep fragmentation during childhood, which might fuel the production of Aβ from more ample sources of APP. The escalating loss of sleep quality that ensues might help to explain the precipitous drop in intellectual function that occurs in adults with DS once AD goes symptomatic. Although theoretical at the moment, this interplay suggests that sleep problems might weigh on cognitive outcomes more significantly in people with DS relative to those with other forms of intellectual disability such as Fragile X or Williams syndrome.

Conclusion: Down But Not Out

Several interdependent factors undermine the typical establishment of cognitive skills in children with DS and cause their eventual decline into AD. Poor sleep is one of those factors. Here, we provide a hypothesis regarding the extent to which sleep disruption might alter cognitive development and contribute to pathological aging in DS. We point out several potential mechanisms that may relate to neurocognitive dysfunction across the lifespan, including the effects of sleep fragmentation and hypoxemia on Aβ deposition. While sleep disruptions have been widely reported in DS, only recently has OSAS been specifically linked to learning and memory variability. Given the neural penalties that likely accrue from OSAS and sleep fragmentation in DS, more research is needed to determine exactly how and when sleep contributes to cognitive outcomes. OSAS is treatable with surgery to remove the tonsil-adenoids and nCPAP (nasal continuous positive airway pressure). These treatments vary in efficacy and are sometimes difficult to implement. The advent of medical devices that can adapt nCPAP to neonates and young children with DS might be an important advance in addressing sleep-related cognitive outcomes. Garnering hope for treatment promotion, some centers have reported higher levels of adherence to nCPAP [104]. Research programs examining the effects of nCPAP and other medical interventions are now needed at all ages, but especially in younger children, when sleep disruptions might affect developmental trajectories in the brain.

Our main goal in this review is to stir the field to consider sleep when considering life-long cognitive decline in DS. The evidence presented here creates a cause for concern regarding the negative consequences of poor sleep, including the possibility that feedback loops only serve to exacerbate the influence of these factors. In light of the fact that OSAS and sleep fragmentation could possibly result in neural consequences in DS different than those currently targeted by pharmacological and behavioral treatments, the role of poor sleep, and approaches to treating it, merit further attention.

Acknowledgements

This report would not have been possible without generous support from the Thrasher Research Fund, Research Down Syndrome, the Down Syndrome Research and Treatment Foundation, and the National Institutes of Health (R01HD07434601A1 to J.O.E.). We would also like to thank Lynn Nadel for comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Schmidt-Sidor B, Wisniewski KE, Shepard TH, Sersen EA. Brain growth in Down syndrome subjects 15 to 22 weeks of gestational age and birth to 60 months. Clin Neuropathol. 1990;9:181–190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Wisniewski KE. Down syndrome children often have brain with maturation delay, retardation of growth, and cortical dysgenesis. Am J Med Genet. 1990;7:274–281. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320370755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wisniewski KE, Schmidt-Sidor B. Postnatal delay of myelin formation in brains from Down syndrome infants and children. Clin Neuropathol. 1989;8:55–62. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Koo BK, Blaser S, Harwood-Nash D, Becker LE, Murphy EG. Magnetic resonance imaging evaluation of delayed myelination in Down syndrome: a case report and review of the literature. J Child Neurol. 1992;7:417–421. doi: 10.1177/088307389200700417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ábrahám H, Vincze A, Veszprémi B, Kravják A, Gömöri É, et al. Impaired myelination of the human hippocampal formation in Down syndrome. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2012;30:147–158. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2011.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Anderson JS, Nielsen JA, Ferguson MA, Burback MC, Cox ET, et al. Abnormal brain synchrony in Down syndrome. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2013;2:703–715. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Golden JA, Hyman BT. Development of the superior temporal neocortex is anomalous in trisomy 21. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 1994;53:513–520. doi: 10.1097/00005072-199409000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Becker LE, Armstrong DL, Chan F. Dendritic atrophy in children with Down’s syndrome. Ann Neurol. 1986;20:520–526. doi: 10.1002/ana.410200413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Richtsmeier JT, Baxter LL, Reeves RH. Parallels of craniofacial maldevelopment in Down syndrome and Ts65Dn mice. Dev Dyn. 2000;217:137–145. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0177(200002)217:2<137::AID-DVDY1>3.0.CO;2-N. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Fung E, Witmans M, Ghosh M, Cave D, El-Hakim H. Upper airway findings in children with Down syndrome on sleep nasopharyngoscopy: case-control study. J Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2012;41:138–144. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Shapiro BL, Gorlin RJ, Redman RS, Bruhl HH. The palate and Down’s syndrome. N Engl J Med. 1967;276:1460–1463. doi: 10.1056/NEJM196706292762603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Uong EC, McDonough JM, Tayag-Kier CE, Zhao H, Haselgrove J, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging of the upper airway in children with Down syndrome. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2001;163:731–736. doi: 10.1164/ajrccm.163.3.2004231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ardran GM, Harker P, Kemp FH. Tongue size in Down’s syndrome. J Ment Defic Res. 1972;16:160–166. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1972.tb01172.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Guimaraes CV, Donnelly LF, Shott SR, Amin RS, Kalra M. Relative rather than absolute macroglossia in patients with Down syndrome: implications for treatment of obstructive sleep apnea. Pediatr Radiol. 2008;38:1062–1067. doi: 10.1007/s00247-008-0941-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Donnelly LF, Shott SR, LaRose CR, Chini BA, Amin RS. Causes of persistent obstructive sleep apnea despite previous tonsillectomy and adenoidectomy in children with down syndrome as depicted on static and dynamic cine MRI. AJR Am J Roentgenol. 2004;183:175–181. doi: 10.2214/ajr.183.1.1830175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Fricke BL, Donnelly LF, Shott SR, Kalra M, Poe SA, et al. Comparison of lingual tonsil size as depicted on MR imaging between children with obstructive sleep apnea despite previous tonsillectomy and adenoidectomy and normal controls. Pediatr Radiol. 2006;36:518–523. doi: 10.1007/s00247-006-0149-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Pilcher ES. Dental Care for the Patient with Down Syndrome. Downs Syndr Res Pract. 1998;5:111–116. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Rosen D. Management of obstructive sleep apnea associated with Down syndrome and other craniofacial dysmorphologies. Curr Opin Pulm Med. 2011;17:431–436. doi: 10.1097/MCP.0b013e32834ba9c0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Dyken ME, Lin-Dyken DC, Poulton S, Zimmerman MB, Sedars E. Prospective polysomnographic analysis of obstructive sleep apnea in down syndrome. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2003;157:655–660. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.157.7.655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Southall DP, Stebbens VA, Mirza R, Lang MH, Croft CB, et al. Upper airway obstruction with hypoxaemia and sleep disruption in Down syndrome. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1987;29:734–742. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1987.tb08818.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Marcus CL, Keens TG, Bautista DB, von Pechmann WS, Ward SL. Obstructive sleep apnea in children with Down syndrome. Pediatrics. 1991;88:132–139. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Shott SR, Amin R, Chini B, Heubi C, Hotze S, et al. Obstructive sleep apnea: Should all children with Down syndrome be tested? Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2006;132:432–436. doi: 10.1001/archotol.132.4.432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Levanon A, Tarasiuk A, Tal A. Sleep characteristics in children with Down syndrome. J Pediatr. 1999;134:755–760. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(99)70293-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Capone GT, Aidikoff JM, Taylor K, Rykiel N. Adolescents and young adults with down syndrome presenting to a medical clinic with depression: Co-morbid obstructive sleep apnea. Am J Med Genet A. 2013;161:2188–2196. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.36052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Pennington BF, Moon J, Edgin J, Stedron J, Nadel L. The neuropsychology of Down syndrome: evidence for hippocampal dysfunction. Child Dev. 2003;74:75–93. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Edgin JO, Pennington BF, Mervis CB. Neuropsychological components of intellectual disability: the contributions of immediate, working, and associative memory. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2010;54:406–417. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01278.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Carlesimo GA, Marotta L, Vicari S. Long-term memory in mental retardation: evidence for a specific impairment in subjects with Down’s syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35:71–79. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(96)00055-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Menghini D, Costanzo F, Vicari S. Relationship between brain and cognitive processes in Down syndrome. Behav Genet. 2011;41:381–393. doi: 10.1007/s10519-011-9448-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wang PP. A neuropsychological profile of Down syndrome: Cognitive skills and brain morphology. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 1996;2:102–108. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Carducci F, Onorati P, Condoluci C, Di Gennaro G, Quarato PP, et al. Whole-brain voxel-based morphometry study of children and adolescents with Down syndrome. Funct Neurol. 2013;28:19–28. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Jernigan TL, Bellugi U, Sowell E, Doherty S, Hesselink JR. Cerebral morphologic distinctions between Williams and Down syndromes. Arch Neurol. 1993;50:186–191. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1993.00540020062019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.White NS, Alkire MT, Haier RJ. A voxel-based morphometric study of nondemented adults with Down Syndrome. Neuroimage. 2003;20:393–403. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00273-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Krasuski JS, Alexander GE, Horwitz B, Rapoport SI, Schapiro MB. Relation of medial temporal lobe volumes to age and memory function in nondemented adults with Down’s syndrome: implications for the prodromal phase of Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:74–81. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.1.74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Baddeley A, Jarrold C. Working memory and Down syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2007;51:925–931. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2007.00979.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ikeda K, Kasugai H, Yamaguchi A, Uchino Y, Katase H, et al. Visual perceptual strengths and weaknesses in adults with intellectual disabilities compared with a birth year-matched norm. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2013;57:67–79. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2011.01516.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Lögdberg B, Brun A. Prefrontal neocortical disturbances in mental retardation. J Intellect Disabil Res. 1993;37:459–468. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1993.tb00316.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Sylvester PE. The hippocampus in Down’s syndrome. J Ment Defic Res. 1983;27:227–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1983.tb00294.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Guidi S, Bonasoni P, Ceccarelli C, Santini D, Gualtieri F, et al. Neurogenesis impairment and increased cell death reduce total neuron number in the hippocampal region of fetuses with Down syndrome. Brain Pathol. 2008;18:180–197. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2007.00113.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ferrer I, Gullotta F. Down’s syndrome and Alzheimer’s disease: dendritic spine counts in the hippocampus. Acta Neuropathol. 1990;79:680–685. doi: 10.1007/BF00294247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Raz N, Torres IJ, Briggs SD, Spencer WD, Thornton AE, et al. Selective neuroanatomic abnormalities in Down’s syndrome and their cognitive correlates: evidence from MRI morphometry. Neurology. 1995;45:356–366. doi: 10.1212/wnl.45.2.356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kanno K, Ikeda Y. Word-length effect in verbal short-term memory in individuals with Down’s syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2002;46:613–618. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.2002.00438.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Jarrold C, Baddeley AD, Phillips CE. Verbal short-term memory in Down syndrome: a problem of memory, audition, or speech? J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2002;45:531–544. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/042). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Costanzo F, Varuzza C, Menghini D, Addona F, Gianesini T, et al. Executive functions in intellectual disabilities: a comparison between Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34:1770–1780. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Pinter JD, Brown WE, Eliez S, Schmitt JE, Capone GT, et al. Amygdala and hippocampal volumes in children with Down syndrome: a high-resolution MRI study. Neurology. 2001;56:972–974. doi: 10.1212/wnl.56.7.972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Smigielska-Kuzia J, Bockowski L, Sobaniec W, Sendrowski K, Olchowik B, et al. A volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study of brain structures in children with Down syndrome. Neurol Neurochir Pol. 2011;45:363–369. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3843(14)60107-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Aylward EH, Li Q, Honeycutt NA, Warren AC, Pulsifer MB, et al. MRI volumes of the hippocampus and amygdala in adults with Down’s syndrome with and without dementia. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156:564–568. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.4.564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kesslak JP, Nagata SF, Lott I, Nalcioglu O. Magnetic resonance imaging analysis of age-related changes in the brains of individuals with Down’s syndrome. Neurology. 1994;44:1039–1045. doi: 10.1212/wnl.44.6.1039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Teipel SJ, Schapiro MB, Alexander GE, Krasuski JS, Horwitz B, et al. Relation of corpus callosum and hippocampal size to age in nondemented adults with Down’s syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1870–1878. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.10.1870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Teipel SJ, Hampel H. Neuroanatomy of Down syndrome in vivo: a model of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Behav Genet. 2006;36:405–415. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9047-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.O’Brien LM, Mervis CB, Holbrook CR, Bruner JL, Smith NH, et al. Neurobehavioral correlates of sleep-disordered breathing in children. J Sleep Res. 2004;13:165–172. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00395.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bourke R, Anderson V, Yang JS, Jackman AR, Killedar A, et al. Cognitive and academic functions are impaired in children with all severities of sleep-disordered breathing. Sleep Med. 2011;12:489–496. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Halbower AC, Degaonkar M, Barker PB, Earley CJ, Marcus CL, et al. Childhood obstructive sleep apnea associates with neuropsychological deficits and neuronal brain injury. PLoS Med. 2006;3:e301. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Bartlett DJ, Rae C, Thompson CH, Byth K, Joffe DA, et al. Hippocampal area metabolites relate to severity and cognitive function in obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep Med. 2004;5:593–596. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2004.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Torelli F, Moscufo N, Garreffa G, Placidi F, Romigi A, et al. Cognitive profile and brain morphological changes in obstructive sleep apnea. Neuroimage. 2011;54:787–793. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Macey PM, Henderson LA, Macey KE, Alger JR, Frysinger RC, et al. Brain morphology associated with obstructive sleep apnea. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2002;166:1382–1387. doi: 10.1164/rccm.200201-050OC. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Morrell MJ, McRobbie DW, Quest RA, Cummin AR, Ghiassi R, et al. Changes in brain morphology associated with obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep Med. 2003;4:451–454. doi: 10.1016/s1389-9457(03)00159-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Lal C, Strange C, Bachman D. Neurocognitive impairment in obstructive sleep apnea. Chest. 2012;141:1601–1610. doi: 10.1378/chest.11-2214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Bucks RS, Olaithe M, Eastwood P. Neurocognitive function in obstructive sleep apnoea: a meta-review. Respirology. 2013;18:61–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1843.2012.02255.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Beebe DW, Gozal D. Obstructive sleep apnea and the prefrontal cortex: towards a comprehensive model linking nocturnal upper airway obstruction to daytime cognitive and behavioral deficits. J Sleep Res. 2002;11:1–16. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2002.00289.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Greenberg GD, Watson RK, Deptula D. Neuropsychological dysfunction in sleep apnea. Sleep. 1987;10:254–262. doi: 10.1093/sleep/10.3.254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Alchanatis M, Deligiorgis N, Zias N, Amfilochiou A, Gotsis E, et al. Frontal brain lobe impairment in obstructive sleep apnoea: a proton MR spectroscopy study. Eur Respir J. 2004;24:980–986. doi: 10.1183/09031936.04.00127603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Thomas RJ, Rosen BR, Stern CE, Weiss JW, Kwong KK. Functional imaging of working memory in obstructive sleep-disordered breathing. J Appl Physiol. 2005;98:2226–2234. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01225.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Ellingson RJ, Peters JF. Development of EEG and daytime sleep patterns in Trisomy-21 infants during the first year of life: longitudinal observations. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1980;50:457–466. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(80)90012-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Miano S, Bruni O, Elia M, Scifo L, Smerieri A, et al. Sleep phenotypes of intellectual disability: a polysomnographic evaluation in subjects with Down syndrome and Fragile-X syndrome. Clin Neurophysiol. 2008;119:1242–1247. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Ashworth A, Hill CM, Karmiloff-Smith A, Dimitriou D. Cross syndrome comparison of sleep problems in children with Down syndrome and Williams syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34:1572–1580. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Colas D, Valletta JS, Takimoto-Kimura R, Nishino S, Fujiki N, et al. Sleep and EEG features in genetic models of Down syndrome. Neurobiol Dis. 2008;30:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2007.07.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Trois MS, Capone GT, Lutz JA, Melendres MC, Schwartz AR, et al. Obstructive sleep apnea in adults with Down syndrome. J Clin Sleep Med. 2009;5:317–323. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Andreou G, Galanopoulou C, Gourgoulianis K, Karapetsas A, Molyvdas P. Cognitive status in Down syndrome individuals with sleep disordered breathing deficits (SDB) Brain Cogn. 2002;50:145–149. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(02)00019-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Chen CC, Spanò G, Edgin JO. The impact of sleep disruption on executive function in Down syndrome. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34:2033–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Gais S, Born J. Declarative memory consolidation: mechanisms acting during human sleep. Learn Mem. 2004;11:679–685. doi: 10.1101/lm.80504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Urbain C, Galer S, Van Bogaert P, Peigneux P. Pathophysiology of sleep-dependent memory consolidation processes in children. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013 doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Gais S, Albouy G, Boly M, Dang-Vu TT, Darsaud A, et al. Sleep transforms the cerebral trace of declarative memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:18778–18783. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705454104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Marshall L, Helgadóttir H, Mölle M, Born J. Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature. 2006;444:610–613. doi: 10.1038/nature05278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Gómez RL, Bootzin RR, Nadel L. Naps promote abstraction in language-learning infants. Psychol Sci. 2006;17:670–674. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01764.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Backhaus J, Hoeckesfeld R, Born J, Hohagen F, Junghanns K. Immediate as well as delayed post learning sleep but not wakefulness enhances declarative memory consolidation in children. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2008;89:76–80. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Wilhelm I, Diekelmann S, Born J. Sleep in children improves memory performance on declarative but not procedural tasks. Learn Mem. 2008;15:373–377. doi: 10.1101/lm.803708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: a hypothesis. Brain Res Bull. 2003;62:143–150. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Robles TF, Carroll JE. Restorative biological processes and health. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2011;5:518–537. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00368.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Van Cauter E, Leproult R, Plat L. Age-related changes in slow wave sleep and REM sleep and relationship with growth hormone and cortisol levels in healthy men. JAMA. 2000;284:861–868. doi: 10.1001/jama.284.7.861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Tasali E, Leproult R, Ehrmann DA, Van Cauter E. Slow-wave sleep and the risk of type 2 diabetes in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:1044–1049. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706446105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Van Cauter E, Spiegel K, Tasali E, Leproult R. Metabolic consequences of sleep and sleep loss. Sleep Med. 2008;9(Suppl 1):S23–S28. doi: 10.1016/S1389-9457(08)70013-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Luke A, Roizen NJ, Sutton M, Schoeller DA. Energy expenditure in children with Down syndrome: correcting metabolic rate for movement. J Pediatr. 1994;125:829–838. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3476(94)70087-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Roos A, Bakker SJ, Links TP, Gans RO, Wolffenbuttel BH. Thyroid function is associated with components of the metabolic syndrome in euthyroid subjects. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2007;92:491–496. doi: 10.1210/jc.2006-1718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Rosen D. Severe hypothyroidism presenting as obstructive sleep apnea. Clin Pediatr (Phila) 2010;49:381–383. doi: 10.1177/0009922809351093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Daulatzai MA. Death by a thousand cuts in Alzheimer2019s disease: hypoxia--the prodrome. Neurotox Res. 2013;24:216–243. doi: 10.1007/s12640-013-9379-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Lim AS, Kowgier M, Yu L, Buchman AS, Bennett DA. Sleep Fragmentation and the Risk of Incident Alzheimer’s Disease and Cognitive Decline in Older Persons. Sleep. 2013;36:1027–1032. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Tranah GJ, Blackwell T, Stone KL, Ancoli-Israel S, Paudel ML, et al. Circadian activity rhythms and risk of incident dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older women. Ann Neurol. 2011;70:722–732. doi: 10.1002/ana.22468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Yaffe K, Laffan AM, Harrison SL, Redline S, Spira AP, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing, hypoxia, and risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in older women. JAMA. 2011;306:613–619. doi: 10.1001/jama.2011.1115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Kang JE, Lim MM, Bateman RJ, Lee JJ, Smyth LP, et al. Amyloid-beta dynamics are regulated by orexin and the sleep-wake cycle. Science. 2009;326:1005–1007. doi: 10.1126/science.1180962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Roh JH, Huang Y, Bero AW, Kasten T, Stewart FR, et al. Disruption of the sleep-wake cycle and diurnal fluctuation of beta-amyloid in mice with Alzheimer’s disease pathology. Sci Transl Med. 2012;4:150ra122. doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3004291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Cirrito JR, Yamada KA, Finn MB, Sloviter RS, Bales KR, et al. Synaptic activity regulates interstitial fluid amyloid-beta levels in vivo. Neuron. 2005;48:913–922. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.10.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Cirrito JR, Kang JE, Lee J, Stewart FR, Verges DK, et al. Endocytosis is required for synaptic activity-dependent release of amyloid-beta in vivo. Neuron. 2008;58:42–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Bero AW, Yan P, Roh JH, Cirrito JR, Stewart FR, et al. Neuronal activity regulates the regional vulnerability to amyloid-beta deposition. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:750–756. doi: 10.1038/nn.2801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Bero AW, Bauer AQ, Stewart FR, White BR, Cirrito JR, et al. Bidirectional relationship between functional connectivity and amyloid-beta deposition in mouse brain. J Neurosci. 2012;32:4334–4340. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5845-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Sheline YI, Raichle ME, Snyder AZ, Morris JC, Head D, et al. Amyloid plaques disrupt resting state default mode network connectivity in cognitively normal elderly. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:584–587. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Mann DM, Esiri MM. The pattern of acquisition of plaques and tangles in the brains of patients under 50 years of age with Down’s syndrome. J Neurol Sci. 1989;89:169–179. doi: 10.1016/0022-510x(89)90019-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Teller JK, Russo C, DeBusk LM, Angelini G, Zaccheo D, et al. Presence of soluble amyloid beta-peptide precedes amyloid plaque formation in Down’s syndrome. Nat Med. 1996;2:93–95. doi: 10.1038/nm0196-93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Mori C, Spooner ET, Wisniewsk KE, Wisniewski TM, Yamaguch H, et al. Intraneuronal Abeta42 accumulation in Down syndrome brain. Amyloid. 2002;9:88–102. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Leverenz JB, Raskind MA. Early amyloid deposition in the medial temporal lobe of young Down syndrome patients: a regional quantitative analysis. Exp Neurol. 1998;150:296–304. doi: 10.1006/exnr.1997.6777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Cataldo AM, Peterhoff CM, Troncoso JC, Gomez-Isla T, Hyman BT, et al. Endocytic pathway abnormalities precede amyloid beta deposition in sporadic Alzheimer’s disease and Down syndrome: differential effects of APOE genotype and presenilin mutations. Am J Pathol. 2000;157:277–286. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9440(10)64538-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Lemere CA, Blusztajn JK, Yamaguchi H, Wisniewski T, Saido TC, et al. Sequence of deposition of heterogeneous amyloid beta-peptides and APO E in Down syndrome: implications for initial events in amyloid plaque formation. Neurobiol Dis. 1996;3:16–32. doi: 10.1006/nbdi.1996.0003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Motte J, Williams RS. Age-related changes in the density and morphology of plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in Down syndrome brain. Acta Neuropathol. 1989;77:535–546. doi: 10.1007/BF00687256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Hof PR, Bouras C, Perl DP, Sparks DL, Mehta N, et al. Age-related distribution of neuropathologic changes in the cerebral cortex of patients with Down’s syndrome. Quantitative regional analysis and comparison with Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol. 1995;52:379–391. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1995.00540280065020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.O’Donnell AR, Bjornson CL, Bohn SG, Kirk VG. Compliance rates in children using noninvasive continuous positive airway pressure. Sleep. 2006;29:651–658. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES