Abstract
A central issue in biology concerns the presence, timing and nature of phylotypic periods of development, but whether, when and why species exhibit conserved morphologies remains unresolved. Here, we construct a developmental morphospace to show that amniote faces share a period of reduced shape variance and convergent growth trajectories from prominence formation through fusion, after which phenotypic diversity sharply increases. We predict in silico the phenotypic outcomes of unoccupied morphospaces and experimentally validate in vivo that observed convergence is not due to developmental limits on variation but instead from selection against novel trajectories that result in maladaptive facial clefts. These results illustrate how epigenetic factors such as organismal geometry and shape impact facial morphogenesis and alter the locus of adaptive selection to variation in later developmental events.
Keywords: Neural crest, Evolvability, Cleft lip
INTRODUCTION
Mammals, reptiles and avians exhibit tremendous phenotypic diversity as adults but are often remarkably similar to each other earlier in their embryonic development (Slack et al., 1993; Hall, 1996; Raff, 1996). The presence of such a ‘phylotypic’ stage of development suggests a causal relationship between conserved developmental mechanisms and mid-embryonic phenotypic convergence (Domazet-Loso and Tautz, 2010); but whether, when and why such a period exists remains highly contested (Duboule, 1994; Raff, 1996; Hall, 1997; Bininda-Emonds et al., 2003; Kalinka and Tomancak, 2012). Resolving this issue is essential to understanding how development contributes to evolutionary change (Hallgrímsson et al., 2009), yet despite evidence for conserved genome and transcriptome-level expression within and between species (Roux and Robinson-Rechavi, 2008; Irie and Kuratani, 2011; Quint et al., 2012), comparable quantitative phenotypic data has lagged, hindering effective comparisons. Here, we use geometric morphometric analyses of shape to address this issue in the morphogenesis and growth of the amniote face.
The amniote face has its developmental origins in a shared embryonic organization or ‘bauplan’. At its most basic, this bauplan consists of the frontonasal prominence (FNP) and the maxillary component of the first branchial arch (MxP), which first appear adjacent to the primordial brain, itself a simple flexed tube. Cranial neural crest (CNC) cells migrate from the dorsal neural tube to form the majority of the mesenchymal tissue of the MxP and FNP. Under the influence of reciprocal epithelial-mesenchymal signaling, the prominences grow in a highly choreographed manner, come into contact and fuse to form the primary palate, failure of which results in a cleft (Diewert and Lozanoff, 2002; Szabo-Rogers et al., 2008). The CNC differentiates into the connective tissues that comprise the facial cartilages and skeleton (Hall and Horstadius, 1988), and although the number, shape and size of facial elements varies widely among amniotes, their developmental origin and organization reflects this shared bauplan. In particular, the maxillary, zygomatic, jugal and quadratojugal bones are thought to be MxP derived, whereas the frontal, premaxilla and nasals are FNP derived (Morriss-Kay, 2001; Lee et al., 2004).
To address the presence, timing and nature of a conserved ‘phylotypic’ stage in amniote facial development, we first sampled embryos from each of the major amniote lineages (mammals, lizards and snakes, turtles, crocodilians and avians) and measured the location of anatomical landmarks that tracked the relative contributions of the FNP and MxP to the upper jaw of the adult skeleton (supplementary material Fig. S1). From these data, we next generated a shape ‘morphospace’ to describe variation in phenotype spanning the earliest period of embryonic facial morphogenesis through postnatal growth of the upper jaw, and quantified and compared divergence in shape across lineages and developmental time.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
Multivariate analysis of embryonic data indicated that species could be discriminated from one another at the earliest stages of facial morphogenesis, suggesting significant embryonic shape diversity (Fig. 1; supplementary material Tables S3, S4). When analyzed as a developmental sequence, the majority of shape variation (78.0%) could be decomposed into three principal axes: a common pattern of progressive anterior facial outgrowth and mediolateral narrowing [PC1, 53.8% total shape variation (TSV)]; divergence in the relative proportions of maxilla to frontonasal (PC2, 15.5% TSV); and anteroventral extension of the FNP (PC3, 8.7% TSV) (Fig. 2A; supplementary material Fig. S2A-D). To compare developmental trajectories, we first noted that PC2 and PC3 were nonlinear with respect to PC1, a shape-defined measure associated with developmental age, and individual species had a better fit with a quadratic equation than a single linear regression [i.e. variance in residuals was significantly lower (Levene’s test, P<0.001)]. Avian trajectories also appeared to progressively diverge from non-avians, such that average adult PC2 scores were significantly different (t-test, P<0.001). To estimate when these trajectories diverge, we estimated the convergence point from the quadratic equations of mean avian and non-avian trajectories (PC1∼0.21), and the inflection point in the mean avian trajectory (PC1=0.07±0.01). This timeframe coincided with the period of facial fusion in avians (supplementary material Fig. S2A-C). Plotted against PC1, shape divergence is initially high, declines to its lowest point as trajectories converge around fusion (PC1=0.05-0.08) and then subsequently increases during fetal growth to reach its highest levels in adults (Fig. 2B; supplementary material Fig. S3).
From this analysis, amniote facial development can be divided into four common stages (Fig. 2A). In the first stage, the MxP is small and posteriorly located relative to the distal aspect of the face, and the nasal placodes are arranged on the lateral-ventral side of the flexed telencephalon. The MxP subsequently grows anteromedially, whereas the FNP becomes relatively smaller and the globular process rotates ventrally and posteriorly towards the distal MxP. During the next stage, these shape changes continue to drive the prominences towards each other such that lateral and distal most regions approximate, consistent with fusion of the primary palate. In the subsequent post-fusion stage of fetal growth, the entire face continues to elongate, but the trend in relative proportions of MxP and FNP (PC2), and ventral movement of the FNP (PC3) reverse. In adults, this distinction is most extreme in avians, where facial length is almost exclusively determined by the premaxilla, while the maxilla is highly reduced. By contrast, in the non-avian facial skeleton the premaxilla proportions vary from relatively small (e.g. turtles, humans) to proportionately large (e.g. ungulates), but facial length is always dominated by the maxilla (supplementary material Fig. S4).
This comparative analysis suggests that the most conserved period of amniote facial shape coincides with prominence fusion. To better understand this relationship, and in particular why avians diverge from non-avians around this developmental event but not earlier, we next used the comparative morphospace to infer rules of facial growth and predict potential but unrealized shapes. We found that deviations from conserved early trajectories resulted in increasing mismatch of relative prominence shape and size to facial length, and in avian lineages predicted a precocious outgrowth of the frontonasal, both of which increase the likelihood of clefts (Diewert and Lozanoff, 2002; Young et al., 2007) (Fig. 2A; supplementary material Fig. S2D). This result suggests that early trajectories are dictated in part by the geometric configuration and shape of constituent parts (i.e. maxillary, frontonasal, brain), particularly in the apposition of globular process of the frontonasal and the distal tip of the maxillary, and thus represent a potential epigenetic mechanism for phenotypic convergence. If this were the case, then prominences should be relatively intolerant to shape variation around the time of facial fusion, whether unilaterally (asymmetric) or bilaterally (symmetric) expressed.
To test this idea, we used the known function of the sonic hedgehog (SHH) signaling pathway in facial growth as a tool to experimentally manipulate relative prominence size and shape prior to fusion (Chai and Maxson, 2006). Although a number of pathways, such as bone morphogenetic protein (BMP), fibroblast growth factor (FGF) and Wingless (WNT) (Brugmann et al., 2007), may contribute to differential growth, SHH plays a crucial role in dorsoventral patterning, midline width variation, anterior outgrowth and palatal fusion (Marcucio et al., 2005; Hu and Marcucio, 2009; Young et al., 2010). When we compared embryonic stages corresponding to the period of maximal shape convergence, we found that Shh expression was localized to the distal tip and margins of the frontonasal, maxillary and primitive oral cavity (Fig. 3A-E). We hypothesized that selectively modulating Shh activity in one prominence, either by implanting a bead soaked in exogenous SHH-N protein to increase signaling or by electroporating a Ptc-Δ-loop construct to inhibit receptor activity and decrease signaling, would alter relative prominence growth and proportions, thereby increasing the incidence and severity of primary palatal clefts. As predicted, enhanced signaling increased maxillary or frontonasal proportions on the treated side, while decreased signaling had the opposite effect (Fig. 3F-O; supplementary material Fig. S5). Moreover, all experimental combinations altered the developmental trajectory into morphospace previously unoccupied by normal chickens and induced clefts of the primary palate (Fig. 3P).
Together these results demonstrate that amniote faces exhibit maximal phenotypic convergence at the mid-embryonic branchial arch stage, consistent with an hourglass model of development (Kalinka and Tomancak, 2012). Consistent with qualitative observations (Richardson, 1995), there is significant variation early in facial shape, notably in the location and orientation of the nasal placodes and MxP (Fig. 1; supplementary material Tables S2, S3). However, a full morphospace analysis suggests the shared amniote facial bauplan of initially independent outgrowths dictates that each species must confront the problem of fusion in order to generate an integrated and functional upper jaw. There are limited solutions to this problem; therefore, embryos must follow a developmental trajectory that maximizes the likelihood that distal facial prominences will contact at the appropriate time and place.
Experimental results similarly demonstrate that fusion is likely to be a strong selective filter against developmental shape variation. Normal variation at or near the time of fusion underestimates potential variation, as evidenced by the large number of genes known to contribute to primary cleft incidence and severity in humans (Dixon et al., 2011), many of which modulate the growth of prominences (Suzuki et al., 2009; Suazo et al., 2011). We targeted SHH signaling because of its known role in controlling mesenchymal growth zones, but similar predictions could be made of other genes or pathways that affect the relative growth of facial prominences and are implicated in cleft-lip etiology (Brugmann et al., 2007; Dixon et al., 2011). The relatively high incidence of clefting in humans (Diewert and Lozanoff, 2002) further suggests that natural variation at this crucial time period occurs, but is actively selected against. Observed phenotypic convergence at this stage is therefore the net result of selection against genetic variations that would induce both stage-inappropriate shape combinations and consequent structural defects detrimental to short-term jaw function and long-term individual fitness.
The divergence in both overall shape and growth trajectories after fusion supports the idea that the potential for clefts influences both phenotypic convergence, and when and how evolutionary diversity is generated (Fig. 4). We speculate that after the prominences fuse, the relative proportions of developmental components are free to vary more because the potential for clefting is no longer an issue. This post-fusion divergence is most obvious in the comparison between non-avian snouts, which are both maxillary and frontonasal derived, and avian beaks, which are primarily frontonasal derived. Associated with this divergent pattern is the location of the external nares, remnant markers of the nasal pits. In avians, the nares remain posteriorly located and facial length is primarily a result of premaxillary growth, whereas in non-avians the nasal aperture tracks the distal tip of the snout and length is dominated by the maxilla. Although some non-avians such as ungulates may have a proportionately large premaxilla, the modern avian beak occupies a novel and discontinuous region of morphospace. Even in non-avians, where skeletal correlates of the nares are posteriorly located, the underlying maxillary/frontonasal skeletal proportions remain in the primitive maxillary-dominant configuration (e.g. in whales, dolphins, tapirs, elephants), suggesting the avian condition is uniquely derived.
Together, these results demonstrate a period of convergent shape in the development of the amniote embryonic face associated with functional demands of prominence fusion. From a morphogenetic perspective, this period reflects epigenetic factors imposed by earlier developmental events rather than intrinsic limits to developmental variation. This suggests that periods of conserved gene expression during this developmental period may result from the way in which a shared bauplan impacts the selective landscape of morphospace. Although this analysis focuses on the face as a single organ system, we predict that epigenetic factors such as geometry and shape similarly constrain variation in other organs where the growth of simple structures must be coordinated to produce more complex shapes.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Sample
We collected embryonic and fetal data for the following species (supplementary material Table S1). (1) Mammals: Mus musculus (mouse) (n=41), Meriones unguiculatus (gerbil) (n=2), Rattus norvegicus (rat) (n=4), Mesocricetus auratus (hamster) (n=7), Homo sapiens (human) (n=6). (2) Snakes and lizards: Elaphe guttata (python royal) (n=2), Chamaeleo calyptratus (veiled chameleon) (n=6). (3) Turtles: Trachemys scripta (red-eared slider) (n=21). (4) Crocodilians: Alligator mississippiensis (alligator) (n=24). (5) Avians: Gallus gallus (chicken) (n=22), Coturnix coturnix (quail) (n=21) and Anas platyrhynchos (white pekin duck) (n=24). With the exception of humans, embryos were incubated (reptiles, avians) or gestated (mammals) for a set time, then dissected and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA)/5% glutaraldehyde. Samples were scanned at a resolution of 5-25 μm in a ScanCo 40 micro-computed tomography scanner. Human shape data were reconstructed from scanned serial sections of the Carnegie series of human embryonic development at an effective resolution of 5-20 μm. For each specimen, a three-dimensional (3D) model was reconstructed in the software Amira 5.4 (Mercury Systems).
Morphospace analysis
We identified 17 landmarks on the maxillary and frontonasal prominences in prenatal samples and associated locations on skeletal derivatives of a broad sample of adult crania (supplementary material Fig. S1, Table S2). Three-dimensional landmark coordinates (x,y,z) were identified and recorded in Landmark (v. 3.0) (Wiley et al., 2005) and analyzed in MorphoJ (v.1.05d) (Klingenberg, 2011). Intraobserver error from repeated measurement of the same specimens was not significant. Procrustes superimposition transformed landmark configurations into a common scale-free shape space. Procrustes data of early embryonic samples (prominence stage before fusion) were ordinated via Canonical Variates Analysis (CVA) to estimate distance measures among species with significance of shape differences determined via resampling. Principal Components Analysis (PCA) of the full developmental series was used to describe a morphospace of coordinated shape variation. To visualize the morphospace and generate shape predictions, a thin-plate spline algorithm was used to warp a 3D object fitted to the mean landmark configuration in EVAN toolbox v. 1.40 (http://www.evan-society.org/node/23). We calculated shape divergence as the average Procrustes distance from the mean shape of the total sample.
Gene expression
Dioxigenin (DIG)-labeled riboprobes for Sonic hedgehog (Shh) were synthesized for each species (mouse, python, turtle, alligator and chicken). The mouse probe was used for all rodent species, whereas the chicken probe was used on all avian species. Whole-mount in situ hybridization was performed following established protocols (Moustakas, 2007; Hu and Marcucio, 2009).
Experimental manipulations
To upregulate SHH signaling, SHH-N protein beads (800 mg/ml) were placed into one side of the mesenchyme of the maxillary or frontonasal prominence of a chicken embryo at HH20. To downregulate SHH signaling, a constitutively active Patched (Ptc)-Δ-loop construct was unilaterally electroporated into the ectoderm of the maxillary prominence at stage HH15-16. Embryos were collected from both 48-72 hours post-treatment and at 12 days to assess for skeletal clefts. For morphometric analysis, each side (treated and normal) was treated as a single individual and compared with the original dataset (right and left).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank R. Elsey and the Louisiana Department of Wildlife and Fisheries for alligator embryos, and J. Moustakas for alligator and turtle Shh riboprobe plasmids. J. Cork provided data from the Virtual Human Embryo. The authors thank the three anonymous reviewers, members of the Marcucio laboratory, D. Noden and K. Katsura for feedback, suggestions and comments that improved the manuscript.
Footnotes
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Author contributions
N.Y., B.H. and R.M. designed the research. N.Y. and B.H. analyzed the data. N.Y., R.M. and D.H. conceived the experiments. D.H. performed the experiments. N.Y., A.L., F.S. and R.D. collected the data. A.T., P.T. and R.S. contributed embryological specimens and reagents. All authors contributed to the writing of the paper.
Funding
This research was funded by National Institutes of Health/The National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research (NIH/NIDCR) [F32DE018596 to N.M.Y., R01DE019638 and R01DE021708 to R.S.M. and B.H., R01DE016402 to R.A.S. and R01DE016082 to P.A.T.], by the Stowers Institute for Medical Research (P.A.T.) and by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) [238992-12 to B.H.]. Deposited in PMC for release after 12 months.
Supplementary material
Supplementary material available online at http://dev.biologists.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1242/dev.099994/-/DC1
References
- Bhullar B. A., Marugán-Lobón J., Racimo F., Bever G. S., Rowe T. B., Norell M. A., Abzhanov A. (2012). Birds have paedomorphic dinosaur skulls. Nature 487, 223–226 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bininda-Emonds O. R. P., Jeffery J. E., Richardson M. K. (2003). Inverting the hourglass: quantitative evidence against the phylotypic stage in vertebrate development. Proc. Biol. Sci. 270, 341–346 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boback S. M., Dichter E. K., Mistry H. L. (2012). A developmental staging series for the African house snake, Boaedon (Lamprophis) fuliginosus. Zoology 115, 38–46 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brugmann S. A., Goodnough L. H., Gregorieff A., Leucht P., ten Berge D., Fuerer C., Clevers H., Nusse R., Helms J. A. (2007). Wnt signaling mediates regional specification in the vertebrate face. Development 134, 3283–3295 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chai Y., Maxson R. E., Jr (2006). Recent advances in craniofacial morphogenesis. Dev. Dyn. 235, 2353–2375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diewert V. M., Lozanoff S. (2002). Animal models of facial clefting: experimental, congenital, and transgenic. In Understanding Craniofacial Anomalies: Etiopathogenesis of Craniosynostoses and Facial Clefting (ed. Mooney M. M., Siegel M. I.) pp. 251–272 New York, NY: Wiley-Liss; [Google Scholar]
- Dixon M. J., Marazita M. L., Beaty T. H., Murray J. C. (2011). Cleft lip and palate: understanding genetic and environmental influences. Nat. Rev. Genet. 12, 167–178 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Domazet-Loso T., Tautz D. (2010). A phylogenetically based transcriptome age index mirrors ontogenetic divergence patterns. Nature 468, 815–818 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duboule D. (1994). Temporal colinearity and the phylotypic progression: a basis for the stability of a vertebrate Bauplan and the evolution of morphologies through heterochrony. Dev. Suppl. 1994, 135–142 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson M. J. (1985). Development. In Biology of the Reptilia (ed. Gans C., Billet F., Maderson P. F. A.), pp. 329–491 New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons; [Google Scholar]
- Hall B. K. (1996). Bauplane, phylotypic stages, and constraint: Why there are so few types of animals. Evol. Biol. 29, 215–261 [Google Scholar]
- Hall B. K. (1997). Phylotypic stage or phantom: is there a highly conserved embryonic stage in vertebrates? Trends Ecol. Evol. 12, 461–463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall B. K., Horstadius S. (1988). The Neural Crest. London: Oxford University Press; [Google Scholar]
- Hallgrímsson B., Jamniczky H., Young N. M., Rolian C., Parsons T. E., Boughner J. C., Marcucio R. S. (2009). Deciphering the Palimpsest: Studying the Relationship Between Morphological Integration and Phenotypic Covariation. Evol. Biol. 36, 355–376 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamburger V., Hamilton H. L. (1951). A series of normal stages in the development of the chick embryo. J. Morphol. 88, 49–92 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu D., Marcucio R. S. (2009). Unique organization of the frontonasal ectodermal zone in birds and mammals. Dev. Biol. 325, 200–210 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Irie N., Kuratani S. (2011). Comparative transcriptome analysis reveals vertebrate phylotypic period during organogenesis. Nat. Commun. 2, 248–254 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalinka A. T., Tomancak P. (2012). The evolution of early animal embryos: conservation or divergence? Trends Ecol. Evol. 27, 385–393 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klingenberg C. P. (2011). MorphoJ: an integrated software package for geometric morphometrics. Mol. Ecol. Resour. 11, 353–357 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee S. H., Bédard O., Buchtová M., Fu K., Richman J. M. (2004). A new origin for the maxillary jaw. Dev. Biol. 276, 207–224 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Louchart A., Viriot L. (2011). From snout to beak: the loss of teeth in birds. Trends Ecol. Evol. 26, 663–673 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcucio R. S., Cordero D. R., Hu D., Helms J. A. (2005). Molecular interactions coordinating the development of the forebrain and face. Dev. Biol. 284, 48–61 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marugán-Lobón J., Buscalioni A. D. (2004). Geometric morphometrics in macroevolution: morphological diversity of the skull in modern avian forms in contrast to some theropod dinosaurs. In Morphometrics: Applications in Biology and Paleontology (ed. Elewa A. M. T.), pp. 157–173 Berlin: Springer-Verlag; [Google Scholar]
- Morriss-Kay G. M. (2001). Derivation of the mammalian skull vault. J. Anat. 199, 143–151 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moustakas J. E. (2007). The Evolution and Development of Skeletogenic Mesenchyme in Reptiles. PhD dissertation, University of California Berkeley, CA, USA: [Google Scholar]
- Quint M., Drost H.-G., Gabel A., Ullrich K. K., Bönn M., Grosse I. (2012). A transcriptomic hourglass in plant embryogenesis. Nature 490, 98–101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raff R. A. (1996). The Shape of Life: Genes, Development and the Evolution of Animal Form. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; [Google Scholar]
- Richardson M. K. (1995). Heterochrony and the phylotypic period. Dev. Biol. 172, 412–421 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roux J., Robinson-Rechavi M. (2008). Developmental constraints on vertebrate genome evolution. PLoS Genet. 4, 1–10 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slack J. M., Holland P. W. H., Graham C. F. (1993). The zootype and the phylotypic stage. Nature 361, 490–492 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suazo J., Tapia J. C., Santos J. L., Castro V. G., Colombo A., Blanco R. (2011). Risk variants in BMP4 promoters for nonsyndromic cleft lip/palate in a Chilean population. BMC Med. Genet. 12, 163–171 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki S., Marazita M. L., Cooper M. E., Miwa N., Hing A., Jugessur A., Natsume N., Shimozato K., Ohbayashi N., Suzuki Y., et al. (2009). Mutations in BMP4 are associated with subepithelial, microform, and overt cleft lip. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 84, 406–411 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szabo-Rogers H. L., Geetha-Loganathan P., Nimmagadda S., Fu K. K., Richman J. M. (2008). FGF signals from the nasal pit are necessary for normal facial morphogenesis. Dev. Biol. 318, 289–302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Theiler K. (1989). The House Mouse: Atlas of Embryonic Development. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag; [Google Scholar]
- Wiley D. F., Amenta N., Alcantara D. A., Ghosh D., Kil Y. J., Delson E., Harcourt-Smith W., Rohlf F. J., St. John K., Hamann B. (2005). Evolutionary Morphing. In Proceedings of IEEE Visualization 2005, pp.431–438 doi: 10.1109/VISUAL.2005.1532826 [Google Scholar]
- Yntema C. L. (1968). A series of stages in the embryonic development of Chelydra serpentina. J. Morphol. 125, 219–251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young N. M., Wat S., Diewert V. M., Browder L. W., Hallgrímsson B. (2007). Comparative morphometrics of embryonic facial morphogenesis: implications for cleft-lip etiology. Anat. Rec. (Hoboken) 290, 123–139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young N. M., Chong H. J., Hu D., Hallgrímsson B., Marcucio R. S. (2010). Quantitative analyses link modulation of sonic hedgehog signaling to continuous variation in facial growth and shape. Development 137, 3405–3409 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.