Abstract
Background
To assess how much of a public health problem emotional and instrumental feeding practices are, we explored the use of these feeding practices in a sample of Dutch mothers regarding their child’s food intake between main meals.
Methods
A cross-sectional questionnaire study was conducted among 359 mothers of primary school children aged 4–12 years. The questionnaires were completed online at home.
Results
Of the mothers, 29.5% reported using foods to reward, 18.1% to punish and 18.9% to comfort their child. Mothers most frequently offered energy-dense and nutrient-poor products such as candy in the context of emotional and instrumental feeding practices. The use of these practices was associated with a lower age of both mother and child and a higher educational level of the mother. Mothers living in neighborhoods with intermediate socioeconomic position used the practices less often than mothers from low and high socioeconomic position neighborhoods.
Conclusions
Our results show that mothers in our sample mainly used unhealthy products in the context of instrumental and emotional feeding practices. Research into the association between these practices and children’s dietary intake is warranted, since the use of unhealthy products in the context of these practices may not necessarily lead to an increased consumption of these products. Findings regarding the frequency of use of these practices among specific subgroups can be used to carefully determine the target population for interventions and tailor the content of interventions to specific target group characteristics. Besides examining associations between personal and family characteristics and the use of emotional and instrumental feeding practices, attempts should be made to understand parents’ reasons for using them.
Keywords: Parenting practices, Sweets and snacks, Primary school children
Background
The high consumption of unhealthy foods such as sweets, snacks and soft drinks between main meals among young people is a major lifestyle problem in Western society and has important implications for public health [1-3]. These between-meal snacks are often high in calories, sugar or fat and have a low nutritional value, resulting in unhealthy dietary intakes [4]. This can contribute to dental problems [4], the rapidly growing problem of overweight and obesity, and the risk of developing chronic diseases at a later age [1,4,5].
The Netherlands Nutrition Centre (NNC) recommends a maximum of four snacking breaks between the three main meals, with the exception of water and coffee and tea without milk or sugar [6]. The NNC also recommends eating core products such as fruit or vegetables between main meals and not exceeding certain age- and gender-specific amounts of kcals from non-core products like snacks and soft drinks [7]. The average energy intake from foods and drinks consumed between main meals among Dutch children aged 4–13 years is two to three times higher than recommended [2,3]. National Food Consumption Survey data show that 24 to 40% (the percentage varies between age groups) of children aged 7–14 years had 8 or more food consumption occasions a day [8].
An important category of factors influencing children’s dietary intake is that of parent-related factors, including general parenting styles [9,10] and food-specific parenting practices [11]. The present study focused on the food-specific parenting practices of using food in between meals to regulate a child’s emotions (emotional feeding) and using food as a reward or withholding food as a punishment (instrumental feeding).
There are widely held beliefs that emotional and instrumental feeding practices are undesirable parental behaviors [12]. The foods that are typically offered in the context of these practices are thought to be palatable and energy-dense [13,14]. Previous studies found that using food as a reward for consuming other foods may increase the preference for the reward food and decrease healthy preferences in the long run [15,16]. One possible explanation may relate to the overjustification hypothesis, which says that children develop a decreased preference for the rewarded behavior (e.g. eating fruit) [17]. Furthermore, Wardle et al. argue that emotional and instrumental feeding can both encourage the child to associate eating with cues other than hunger, thereby increasing the risk of eating at times when there is no physiological need [12], possibly through a reduced ability of the child to perceive signs of hunger and satiety [18]. This inability of children to identify whether they are hungry or suffering from other discomfort and the tendency to overeat in response to emotional arousal is defined as emotion eating [19]. The evidence that emotional and instrumental feeding practices are indeed undesirable has been inconsistent. On the one hand, a study exploring memories about food showed that obese patients reported emotional feeding practices of their parents during their youth [20]. In a similar qualitative study, a few adults who scored high on binge eating and restraint eating recalled instrumental feeding in childhood [21]. Furthermore, previous research into the relationship between emotional and instrumental feeding and children’s food intake has found instrumental feeding to be positively associated with children’s snacking behavior [22] and more specifically a higher sweets consumption [23]. In addition, Kröller and Warschburger found that rewarding significantly decreased the intake of fruit and vegetables among children aged 4–6 years [24]. By contrast, however, no associations between instrumental feeding and fruit, vegetable and soft drink consumption [23] or with problematic foods in general (including fastfood, snacks, sweets and soft drinks were found) [24]. In spite of the relatively weak evidence, experts have suggested that parents should for the time being be advised not to use emotional and instrumental feeding [12].
To assess how much of a public health problem emotional and instrumental feeding practices are, it is important to know the effects of these practices on foods actually eaten between main meals, but it might be even more important to first examine the actual use of these potential parental risk behaviors and the type of foods that are offered in this context within specific populations. Therefore, this study aimed to explore the use of emotional and instrumental feeding practices among a sample of mothers of primary school children aged 4–12 years in the Netherlands, and to find out what types of products are offered in this context. Assessing the association between maternal use of instrumental and emotional feeding practices and children’s actual intake was beyond the scope of our study.
So far, only one study assessing the use of instrumental and emotional feeding has been conducted in a Dutch population [21] and it found mean scores of 1.5 and 1.3 on a 5-point Likert scale for instrumental and emotional feeding practices among parents of children aged 6–7 years. A number of studies outside the Netherlands did assess the use of emotional and instrumental feeding, but there might be cultural differences in the prevalence of this type of parental behavior [25]. Moreover, these studies measured the prevalence by asking parents to indicate on 5- or 6-point scales to what extent they used emotional and instrumental feeding practices [12,13,16,23-25]. Only mean scores were reported; the studies by Saxton et al. [13] and Wardle et al. [12] reported scores of 1.9 and 2.0 for emotional feeding, and 2.3 for instrumental feeding, using five-point scales. Moreover, reported mean scores were not always accompanied by information about the scoring system (e.g. from 1 to 5 or from 0 to 4) [16,25]. These mean scores do not provide much information on the proportion of parents that use these practices. Therefore, we decided to use dichotomous measures to assess the use of emotional and instrumental feeding.
To date, mixed results have been reported regarding subgroup differences in the use of emotional and instrumental feeding practices. Emotional feeding was found to be more prevalent among higher income parents [26], mothers of overweight children [27], lower educated mothers [13], mothers with a higher level of emotional eating [12,25] and parents in less deprived neighborhoods [28]. Instrumental feeding was found to be more prevalent among mothers with a higher level of external eating [12,25] and among mothers with lower levels of family income [29]. Other studies found no associations between emotional and instrumental feeding practices and the BMI of the mother [12] or that of the child [12,16], though a recent study in Australia found that overweight and obese girls were more likely to be rewarded with sweets for good behavior [30].
This study explored the use of emotional and instrumental feeding practices among parents of primary school children aged 4–12 years in the Netherlands, and aimed to find out which type of products are offered in this context. The research questions were: (1) How many mothers use emotional and/or instrumental feeding practices? (2) For which foods do mothers use emotional/instrumental feeding practices? (3) Are there differences in emotional and instrumental feeding practices between different subgroups (in terms of demographics, perceived body size and familiarity with the national recommendations)?
Methods
Participants and recruitment
A cross-sectional design was used in which 680 mothers of primary school children aged 4–12 years were recruited for participation. Only mothers were recruited for the present study, since they usually have the primary responsibility for feeding the children [31]. Data collection took place through a research agency, which invited members of their existing research panel for online questionnaire surveys to fill in the web survey. The members received points for participation in surveys, which they could exchange for gift vouchers. The agency was chosen because of the comprehensiveness of their research panel and the presence of individuals from a range of socio-economic positions (SEP) included in the panel. The panel was stratified based on region (postal code) and educational level of the mother. A group of mothers of primary school children was selected from this panel, with a distribution in terms of region and educational level that was comparable to that in the Dutch population, based on information from Statistics Netherlands. Ethical approval for this study was not required under Dutch law [32].
Instrument development
The items used to assess instrumental and emotional feeding were created by the authors for the purpose of this study. The use of instrumental and emotional feeding practices was assessed using general dichotomous items to cover the full construct, unlike previous studies, which measured the practices on 5- or 6-point scales (e.g. [12,13,23,25]).
In depth-interviews with four mothers of primary school children were conducted to inform the questionnaire development. The printed questionnaire was piloted by four mothers, and the questionnaire was also discussed with three health promotion research experts, after which after adjustments were made. Subsequently, the digital questionnaire was piloted by two mothers and six health promotion research experts.
Measures
Mothers were asked to answer all child-related questions for their youngest child attending primary school. Personal and family characteristics included family situation (living together with the biological father of all children, living together with a partner who is not the biological father of all children, living alone with children, other); country of birth of mother and father; weight, height and perceived body size of the mother (I am too thin, I am a little bit too thin, I have a normal weight, I am a little bit too heavy, I am too heavy); level of education of the mother; and number of children attending primary school. Mothers were also asked to report their child’s age, gender, perceived body size and whether their child attended childcare. The research agency provided data on the mother’s age, postal code and total number of children.
Familiarity with the national recommendation regarding snacking breaks between main meals was measured with two items. One item assessed whether respondents knew the recommendation regarding the number of snacking breaks children are allowed to have (viz. a maximum of four snacking breaks between the three main meals, with the exception of water and coffee and tea without milk or sugar). If they answered yes, the mothers were asked to write down the recommended number.
Emotional feeding practices were measured with a set of items that started with one dichotomous question asking mothers whether they used foods to comfort their child (no/yes). If mothers indicated doing so, they were shown a list of 26 products and asked to specify which products they used for this practice (yes/no) (potato chips, nuts and/or savory snacks; cookies; pastry and cake; ginger cake; bread; crackers and biscuit rusks; breadsticks; chocolate; candy bars; candy; French fries; deep fried snacks; cheese; sausage; ice cream; yoghurt and soft curd cheese; pudding; fruit; raw vegetables; water; soft drinks; fruit juice; milk; chocolate milk and yoghurt drink; other drinks; other).
Instrumental feeding practices were measured with two dichotomous questions asking mothers (1) whether they withheld certain products from their children between main meals to punish them and (2) whether they used products in between main meals to reward their child. If the respondents reported instrumental feeding practices, they were presented with the same list of 26 products, and were asked to indicate which products they used for this practice (yes/no).
Data analysis
Body Mass Index (BMI) of the mother was calculated using weight and height, and divided into categories in accordance with the standard BMI classification by the World Health Organization [33]. The child’s ethnicity was used as a dichotomous variable (Dutch origin or not) based on the definition of ethnic minorities used by Statistics Netherlands, i.e., having at least one parent born abroad [34]. Postal code was recoded as 'socioeconomic position (SEP)’, based on a factor score (range -4 [high] to 4 [low]) calculated from four SEP indicators for all Dutch postal code areas, i.e. mean income, percentage of low income households, percentage of residents without a paid job and percentage of households with average or low education [35]. This factor score has a mean of 0.0 and scores of 1 and -1 should be interpreted as 1* the standard deviation, etc. [35]. The SEP variable was divided into tertiles (low-intermediate-high). Educational level was recoded as a categorical variable (low level [primary or basic vocational education], intermediate level [secondary vocational school or high school] and high level [higher professional education or university]), according to the definitions used by Statistics Netherlands [36]. Mother’s age was categorized into five groups: (1) ≤ 30, (2) 30–35, (3) 35–40, (4) 40–45, (5) ≥ 45. Child’s age was categorized into three groups: (1) ≤4–5, (2). 6–9; (3). 10–12. The cut-off points for these classifications were data-driven, and we aimed at obtaining groups with similar sizes.
Data were analyzed using SPSS 20.0. A multiple logistic regression analysis using the Enter method was conducted to determine associations between response (i.e. whether or not mothers participated) and the mother’s age, postal code, and number of children living at home as independent variables, in order to assess selective response. Multiple logistic regression analyses using the Enter method were performed to assess associations between using emotional or instrumental feeding (punishment and reward) practices and the mother’s age, educational level, SEP, BMI, perceived body size and familiarity with the recommendations, use of child care, and the child’s age, gender, perceived body size, and ethnicity as independent variables. Contrasts of the correlates of the above-mentioned dependent variables were tested by repeating the logistic regression analyses using a different reference group for each categorical independent variable each time. P-values <0.05 were considered to indicate statistical significance.
Results
Response and participants
Of the 680 mothers in the random sample, 24 were excluded since they did not have children in primary school. Of the remaining mothers, 359 (56%) responded within the requested one-week period. No significant differences in demographic characteristics were found between participants and non-participants.
Table 1 shows the personal and family characteristics of the participants and their youngest child. The mean age of the mothers was 38.4 (SD 5.6) years. Compared to the Dutch population, low-educated mothers were somewhat underrepresented, while highly educated mothers were somewhat overrepresented. Mean BMI was 25.8 kg/m2 and nearly half of the mothers (47.9%) would be classified as overweight or obese, which is representative of the general Dutch population [37]. The mean age of the children whom the mothers reported on was 7.0 (SD 2.7) and close to 90% of the children were of Dutch ethnicity, meaning that ethnic minorities were slightly underrepresented.
Table 1.
Characteristics | Mean (SD) | % |
---|---|---|
Characteristics of the mother
|
|
|
Age |
38.4 (5.6) |
|
≤ 30 |
|
7.8 |
30–35 |
|
22.0 |
35–40 |
|
33.1 |
40–45 |
|
27.6 |
≥ 45 |
|
9.5 |
Educational level
1
|
|
|
Low |
|
21.4 |
Intermediate |
|
46.8 |
High |
|
31.8 |
SEP
2
|
|
|
Low |
|
33.4 |
Intermediate |
|
31.8 |
High |
|
34.8 |
BMI (kg/m
2
) in categories
3
|
25.8 (5.1) |
|
Underweight ≤ 18,49 kg/m2 |
|
3.1 |
Normal 18,5–24,99 kg/m2 |
|
48.9 |
Overweight 25–29,99 kg/m2 |
|
28.2 |
Obese 30 kg/m2 and above |
|
19.8 |
Perceived body size |
|
|
(A little bit) too thin |
|
4.8 |
Normal weight |
|
33.1 |
(A little bit) too heavy |
|
62.1 |
Family situation |
|
|
Living together with biological father of all children |
|
76.9 |
Living together with partner (not biological father of all children) |
|
8.1 |
Living alone with children |
|
12.8 |
Other |
|
2.2 |
Number of children living at home |
|
|
1 |
|
22.6 |
2 |
|
56.3 |
3 |
|
16.4 |
4 or more |
|
4.7 |
Child care use |
|
|
0 days |
|
61.6 |
≥ 1 day |
|
38.4 |
Familiar with NNC recommendation |
|
|
Yes |
|
20.3 |
No |
|
79.7 |
Characteristics of the child
|
|
|
Age in years |
7.0 (2.7) |
|
≤ 4 |
|
21.7 |
5 |
|
17.8 |
6 |
|
11.4 |
7 |
|
11.1 |
8 |
|
7.8 |
9 |
|
6.7 |
10 |
|
7.5 |
11 |
|
8.4 |
≥12 |
|
7.5 |
Gender |
|
|
Boys |
|
55.2 |
Girls |
|
44.8 |
Perceived body size |
|
|
(A little bit) too thin |
|
20.9 |
Normal weight |
|
73.3 |
(A little bit) too heavy |
|
5.8 |
Ethnicity
4
|
|
|
Dutch origin |
|
89.7 |
Non-Dutch origin | 10.3 |
1Educational level (low [primary or basic vocational education], intermediate [secondary vocational school or high school] and high [higher professional education or university]) [36].
2Socioeconomic position (SEP) is based on a factor score (range -4 [high] to 4 [low]) calculated from four indicators of SEP for all Dutch postal code areas, i.e. mean income, percentage of low-income households, percentage of residents without a paid job and percentage of households with intermediate or low education [35]. The SEP was divided into tertiles with cut-off points: (low [2.62–0.27], intermediate [0.27– -0.44], high [-0.44– -2.68]).
3According to standard BMI classification by the World health Organization [33].
4Non-Dutch origin: having at least one parent born abroad [34].
One out of five mothers reported being familiar with the recommendations for the number of snacking breaks a child is allowed to have between main meals. Of these 73 mothers, 17.8% (N = 13) answered in line with the NNC recommendations (i.e. four moments), meaning that 3.6% of all mothers were familiar with the recommendations (not tabulated).
Emotional and instrumental feeding
Table 2 shows that the use of foods to reward a child was reported more often (29.5%) than the use of foods to comfort (18.1%) or punish the child (18.9%). With regard to subgroup differences in the use of emotional and instrumental feeding, we found that older mothers and intermediate-SEP mothers were less likely to use foods in between main meals to reward or comfort their child than younger mothers and high- and low-SEP mothers (Table 2). Mothers with an intermediate educational level were less likely to use foods to reward than highly educated mothers. Mothers were less likely to use foods to punish or reward older children than younger children, and less likely to use foods to reward girls than boys. No significant differences regarding the use of emotional and instrumental feeding were found for mother’s perceived body size, use of child care, child’s perceived body size, child’s ethnicity, and mother’s familiarity with the recommendations.
Table 2.
|
Instrumental feeding |
Emotional feeding |
|
---|---|---|---|
Groups | Punishment (%, CI) | Reward (%, CI) | Comfort (%, CI) |
Total group |
18.9 (15–23) |
29.5 (25–34) |
18.1 (14–22) |
Age of mother |
|
|
|
(1). ≤ 30 (n = 28) |
28.6 (11–46) |
39.3 (20–59) |
32.1 (14–51) |
(2). 30–35 (n = 79) |
26.6 (17–37) |
44.3 (33–56) |
26.6 (17–37) |
(3). 35–40 (n = 119) |
15.1 (9–22) |
23.5 (16–31) |
17.6 (11–25) |
(4). 40–45 (n = 99) |
14.1 (7–21) |
21.2 (13–29) |
8.1 (3–14) |
(5). ≥ 45 (n = 34) |
18.2 (6–35) |
30.3 (16–49) |
18.2 (4–31) |
Sign. contrasts |
|
3,4 < 2 |
4 < 1,2,3 |
Education of mother
1
|
|
|
|
(1). Low (n = 77) |
19.5 (10–29) |
26.0 (16–36) |
19.5 (10–29) |
(2). Moderate (n = 168) |
16.8 (11–23) |
24.0 (18–31) |
16.2 (10–22) |
(3). High (n = 114) |
21.1 (13–29) |
39.5 (30–49) |
20.2 (13–28) |
Sign. contrasts |
|
2 < 3 |
|
SEP
2
|
|
|
|
(1). Low (n = 120) |
21.7 (14–29) |
33.3 (25–42) |
22.5 (15–30) |
(2). Intermediate (n = 114) |
20.2 (13–28) |
21.9 (14–30) |
12.3 (6–18) |
(3). High (n = 125) |
15.2 (9–22) |
32.8 (24–41) |
19.2 (12–26) |
Sign. contrasts |
|
2 < 1,3 |
2 < 1,3 |
BMI of mother (kg/m
2
)
3
|
|
|
|
(1). Underweight (n = 11) |
36.4 (2–70) |
27.3 (4–59) |
0.0 |
(2). Normal weight (n = 175) |
17.1 (12–23) |
32.6 (26–40) |
17.7 (12–23) |
(3). Overweight (n = 101) |
20.0 (13–29) |
25.0 (17–34) |
22.0 (14–30) |
(4). Obese (n = 71) |
18.3 (9–28) |
26.8 (16–37) |
16.9 (8–26) |
Sign. contrasts |
|
|
|
Age of child |
|
|
|
(1). ≤4–5 (n = 142) |
24.6 (17–32) |
39.4 (31–48) |
20.4 (14–27)) |
(2). 6–9 (n = 133) |
20.3 (13–27) |
27.1 (19–35) |
19.5 (13–26 |
(3). 10–12 (n = 84) |
7.1 (2–13) |
16.7 (9–25) |
11.9 (5–19) |
Sign. contrasts |
3 < 1,2 |
3 < 1 |
|
Gender of child |
|
|
|
(1). Boys (n = 198) |
18.7 (13–24) |
33.3 (27–40) |
20.7 (15–26) |
(2). Girls (n = 161) |
19.3 (13–25) |
24.8 (18–32) |
14.9 (9–20) |
Sign. Contrasts | 2 < 1 |
Note: this table only shows the independent variables for which significant contrasts were found; significant contrasts were retrieved by comparisons made among the numbered subgroups by repeating the logistic regression analysis using a different reference group for each independent variable each time.
1Educational level (low [primary or basic vocational education], intermediate [secondary vocational school or high school] and high [higher professional education or university]) [36].
2Socioeconomic position (SEP) is based on a factor score (range -4 [high] to 4 [low]) calculated from four indicators of SEP for all Dutch postal code areas, i.e. mean income, percentage of low income households, percentage of residents without a paid job and percentage of households with intermediate or low education [35].
3According to standard BMI classification by the World health Organization [33].
The type of food most frequently used to withhold as a punishment, to reward or comfort children was candy (Table 3). In addition, withholding cookies and chocolate was frequently used to punish while ice cream was often used to reward or comfort the child. On average, 5 food products were used to punish the child, while foods used to reward and to comfort the child included 3 and 2 products, respectively.
Table 3.
Practices | Most frequently applied using | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Instrumental feeding |
|
|
|
|
|
Products withheld as punishment |
Candy (82.4%) |
Cookies (52.9%), |
Candy bars (41.2%) |
Ice cream (36.8%) |
Pastry and cake (26.5%) |
|
Chocolate (52.9%) |
Chips, nuts and savory snacks (41.2%) |
|
|
|
Products used as reward |
Candy (59.4%) |
Ice cream (43.4%) |
Chocolate (26.4%) |
Chips, nuts and savory snacks (25.5%) |
Cookies (24.5%) |
Emotional feeding |
|
|
|
|
|
Products used to comfort | Candy (66.2%) | Ice cream (30.8%) | Cookies (29.2%) | Chocolate (15.4%) | Chips, nuts and savory snacks (12.3%) |
Discussion
This study explored the use of emotional and instrumental feeding practices among parents of primary school children aged 4–12 years in the Netherlands, and aimed to find out which type of products are offered in this context. A substantial proportion of the mothers used emotional and instrumental feeding practices. Mothers most frequently offered energy-dense and nutrient-poor products such as candy, cookies, chocolate, ice cream and candy bars in the context of their emotional and instrumental feeding practices, while core products like bread, fruit and vegetables were least often used in this context. Comparison of our results with those of previous studies is limited by the fact that these studies reported the use of the practices in mean scores, which are difficult to compare with our dichotomous measures. Moreover, previous studies did not assess the practices specifically in relation to consumption in between meals. Our findings indicate that mothers mainly used unhealthy products in the context of emotional and instrumental feeding practices. This underlines the need to further investigate the potential negative effects of these practices on children’s food intake and health, and to develop interventions.
We found emotional and instrumental feeding practices to be associated with mother’s age, SEP, and educational level, and the child’s age and gender. In contrast to earlier studies, which reported that highly educated mothers make less use of food as a reward for positive behavior and make less use of emotional feeding [13], our results showed that mothers with an intermediate educational level were less likely to use foods as reward than highly educated mothers. Furthermore, older mothers appeared to be less likely to use foods to reward or comfort their child. We did not find an association between emotional and instrumental feeding practices and the BMI of the mother or child, which is in line with previous studies [12,25]. These studies were, however, conducted among parents with young children (3–7 years) and it might be that the parent’s behavior had not yet impacted on their children’s weight [16].
The majority of the mothers were not familiar with the NNC recommendation about the maximum number of snacking breaks children are advised to have between main meals. Moreover, the majority of mothers who reported being familiar with the recommendations were wrong about the recommended maximum number of snacking breaks. Since we did not find relevant associations between familiarity with the recommendation and the use of instrumental or emotional feeding practices, our study does not indicate that it is important for parents to know these recommendations.
A strength of the current study is that we used a sample that was representative of the Dutch population in terms of BMI and regional variation, which increases the potential for generalizing the results of the current study to the entire population of Dutch mothers of primary school children. Some limitations must, however, be acknowledged. The validity and reliability of the composite instrument we used have not yet been established. However, the use of our dichotomous measures to assess the use of instrumental and emotional feeding practices seems to provide some added value compared to previous studies that reported mean scores, since it forced participants to decide whether or not they used the feeding practices, and as such clearly revealed how many parents were actually using the practices. A limitation of these dichotomous measures is that we were not able to quantify the use of these feeding practices, which could be relevant especially in the case of negative consequences of these practices. Another limitation is that our results are based on self-reported data, which may have been subject to bias (e.g. through social desirability). In addition, we asked mothers to report only on their youngest child, while almost 80% of the mothers reported having more than one child, so this may have biased our results. The sample included relatively larger numbers of younger children, resulting in a relatively low mean age. The age limitation probably means that the use of instrumental feeding was overestimated, as fewer mothers reported using foods to punish or reward older children.
Conclusion
Our findings indicate that research into the harmfulness of emotional and instrumental feeding practices is warranted. Our results provide a first indication of specific subgroups in which these practices are more likely to be used (viz. younger mothers, intermediate SEP mothers and higher educated mothers). These findings can be used to inform the development of future interventions to reduce these potentially harmful parental behaviors among the identified risk groups. Besides examining associations between personal and family characteristics and the use of emotional and instrumental feeding practices, we also consider it useful to understand parent’s reasons for using them. For instance, Campbell, Crawford, and Hesketh explored Australian parents’ views on their children’s food choices and gained some insight into parent’s thoughts about using foods as rewards [38]. It is possible that parents perceive rewarding with food as an effective strategy to have their child eat more healthy foods, since rewarding may be effective in the short term [39]. Therefore interventions to reduce these undesirable behaviors should also address long-term consequences.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors’ contributions
LR drafted the manuscript. All authors made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the study. LR, DT and PvA made substantial contributions to data acquisition. All authors made substantial contributions to the analysis and interpretation of the data and have read and approved the final version of the manuscript.
Pre-publication history
The pre-publication history for this paper can be accessed here:
Contributor Information
Lieke GM Raaijmakers, Email: lieke.raaijmakers@maastrichtuniversity.nl.
Dorus WM Gevers, Email: dorus.gevers@maastrichtuniversity.nl.
Dorit Teuscher, Email: d.teuscher@maastrichtuniversity.nl.
Stef PJ Kremers, Email: s.kremers@maastrichtuniversity.nl.
Patricia van Assema, Email: p.vanassema@maastrichtuniversity.nl.
References
- Currie C, Roberts C, Morgan A, Smith R, Settertobulte W, Samdal O, Barnekow Rasmussen V. Young people’s health in context. Health behaviour in school-aged children (HBSC) study: international report from the 2001/2002 survey. Geneva: WHO; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- TNO Voeding. Basisrapportage en tabellen voedselconsumptiepeiling 1997-1998 en gegevens op het niveau van voedingsmiddelen en voedingsstoffen over een periode van tien jaar. (Basic report and tables on the 1997-1998 food consumption survey and results on the level of food products and nutrients over a 10-year period) http://www.zuivelengezondheid.nl/tno/Start.htm.
- Ocké MC, Van Rossum CTM, Fransen HP, Buurma EJM, de Boer EJ, Brants HAM, Niekerk EM, Van der Laan JD, Drijvers JJMM, Ghameslou Z. Dutch National Food Consumption Survey - Young Children 2005/2006. Bilthoven: National Institute for Public Health and the Environment; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. Diet, Nutrition and the Prevention of Chronic Diseases. Geneva: WHO; 2003. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ministry of Health Welfare and Sport. Preventienota Kiezen voor gezond leven (Prevention memorandum: Choosing a healthy lifestyle) The Hague: Ministry of Health Welfare and Sport; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Netherlands Nutrition Centre. Richtlijnen voedselkeuze (Guidelines on food choice) The Hague: Netherlands Nutrition Centre; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Netherlands Nutrition Centre. Tabel hoeveelheden calorieën voor extra’s (Table of amounts of calories for extras) The Hague: Netherlands Nutrition Centre; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- van Rossum CTM, Fransen HP, Verkaik-Kloosterman J, Buurma-Rethans EJM, Ocke MC. Dutch National Food Consumption Survey 2007–2010: Diet of children and adults aged 7 to 69 years. Bilthoven: National Institute for Public Health and the Environment; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Kremers SP, Brug J, de Vries H, Engels RC. Parenting style and adolescent fruit consumption. Appetite. 2003;41(1):43–50. doi: 10.1016/S0195-6663(03)00038-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown R, Ogden J. Children’s eating attitudes and behaviour: a study of the modelling and control theories of parental influence. Health Educ Res. 2004;19(3):261–271. doi: 10.1093/her/cyg040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birch L, Savage JS, Ventura A. Influences on the development of children’s eating behaviours: from infancy to adolescence. Can J Diet Pract Res. 2007;68(1):1–56. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wardle J, Sanderson S, Guthrie CA, Rapoport L, Plomin R. Parental feeding style and the inter-generational transmission of obesity risk. Obes Res. 2002;10:453–462. doi: 10.1038/oby.2002.63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saxton J, Carnell S, van Jaarsveld CH, Wardle J. Maternal education is associated with feeding style. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109(5):894–898. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2009.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans A, Seth JG, Smith S, Harris KK, Loyo J, Spaulding C, Van Eck M, Gottlieb N. Parental feeding practices and concerns related to child underweight, picky eating, and using food to calm differ according to ethnicity/race, acculturation, and income. Matern Child Health J. 2011;15(7):899–909. doi: 10.1007/s10995-009-0526-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman J, Taylor A. Effect of a means-end contingency on young children’s food preferences. J Exp Child Psychol. 1992;53(2):200–216. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(92)90049-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carnell S, Wardle J. Associations between multiple measures of parental feeding and children’s adiposity in United Kingdom preschoolers. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2007;15(1):137–144. doi: 10.1038/oby.2007.513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birch LL, Birch D, Marlin DW, Kramer L. Effects of instrumental consumption on children’s food preference. Appetite. 1982;3(2):125–134. doi: 10.1016/S0195-6663(82)80005-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faith MS, Scanlon KS, Birch LL, Francis LA, Sherry B. Parent–child feeding strategies and their relationships to child eating and weight status. Obes Res. 2004;12(11):1711–1722. doi: 10.1038/oby.2004.212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kroller K, Jahnke D, Warschburger P. Are maternal weight, eating and feeding practices associated with emotional eating in childhood? Appetite. 2013;65:25–30. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.11.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brink PJ, Ferguson K, Sharma A. Childhood memories about food: the Successful Dieters Project. J Child Adolesc Psychiatr Nurs. 1999;12(1):17–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6171.1999.tb00037.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puhl RM, Schwartz MB. If you are good you can have a cookie: how memories of childhood food rules link to adult eating behaviors. Eat Behav. 2003;4(3):283–293. doi: 10.1016/S1471-0153(03)00024-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sleddens EF, Kremers SP, De Vries NK, Thijs C. Relationship between parental feeding styles and eating behaviours of Dutch children aged 6–7. Appetite. 2010;54(1):30–36. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vereecken CA, Keukelier E, Maes L. Influence of mother’s educational level on food parenting practices and food habits of young children. Appetite. 2004;43(1):93–103. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2004.04.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kröller K, Warschburger P. Associations between maternal feeding style and food intake of children with a higher risk for overweight. Appetite. 2008;51(1):166–172. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.01.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Lauzon-Guillain B, Musher-Eizenman D, Leporc E, Holub S, Charles MA. Parental feeding practices in the United States and in France: relationships with child’s characteristics and parent’s eating behavior. J Am Diet Assoc. 2009;109(6):1064–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2009.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Musher-Eizenman D, Holub S. Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire: validation of a new measure of parental feeding practices. J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32(8):960–972. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsm037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baughcum AE, Powers SW, Johnson SB, Chamberlin LA, Deeks CM, Jain A, Whitaker RC. Maternal feeding practices and beliefs and their relationships to overweight in early childhood. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2001;22(6):391. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200112000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark HR, Goyder E, Bissell P, Blank L, Walters SJ, Peters J. A pilot survey of socioeconomic differences in child-feeding behaviours among parents of primary-school children. Public Health Nutr. 2007;11(10):1030–1036. doi: 10.1017/S1368980007001401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hendy HM, Williams KE. Mother’s feeding practices for children 3–10 years of age and their associations with child demographics. Appetite. 2012;58(2):710–716. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hardy LL, King L, Hector D, Lloyd B. Weight status and weight-related behaviors of children commencing school. Prev Med. 2012;55(5):433–437. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2012.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dekovic M, Rispens J. Vaders en de opvoeding en ontwikkeling van kinderen Een inleiding. (Fathers and child rearing and evelopment – an introduction) Kind en adolescent. 1998;19(1):39–40. doi: 10.1007/BF03060680. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Central Committee on Research Inv. Human Subjects. [ http://www.ccmo.nl/nl/uwonderzoek-wmo-plichtig-of-niet]
- BMI classification. [ http://apps.who.int/bmi/index.jsp?introPage=intro_3.html]
- Keij I. Standaarddefinitie allochtonen. (Standard definition of ethnic minorities) Index. 2000;10:24–25. [Google Scholar]
- Sociaal Cultureel Planbureau (SCP) (The Netherlands Institute for Social Research) Statusscores (Status scores) http://www.scp.nl/content.jsp?objectid=default:20133.
- Verweij A. Scholing en opleiding: indeling opleidingsniveau. (Education and training: categorization of educational levels) Bilthoven: National Institute for Public Health and the Environment; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Brink van den CL, Blokstra A. Hoeveel mensen hebben overgewicht of ondergewicht? (How many people are overweight or underweight?) 2013. [ http://www.nationaalkompas.nl/gezondheidsdeterminanten/persoonsgebonden/overgewicht/hoeveel-mensen-hebbenovergewicht/]
- Campbell KJ, Crawford DA, Hesketh KD. Australian parents’ view on their 5–6-year old children’s food choices. Health Promot Int. 2006;22(1):11–18. doi: 10.1093/heapro/dal035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birch LL. Development of food preferences. Annu Rev Nutr. 1999;19:41–62. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nutr.19.1.41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]