Skip to main content
Substance Abuse and Rehabilitation logoLink to Substance Abuse and Rehabilitation
. 2013 Jan 23;4:11–27. doi: 10.2147/SAR.S25869

The effect of cannabis use on memory function: an update

Tabea Schoeler 1, Sagnik Bhattacharyya 1,
PMCID: PMC3931635  PMID: 24648785

Abstract

Investigating the effects of cannabis use on memory function appears challenging. While early observational investigations aimed to elucidate the longer-term effects of cannabis use on memory function in humans, findings remained equivocal and pointed to a pattern of interacting factors impacting on the relationship between cannabis use and memory function, rather than a simple direct effect of cannabis. Only recently, a clearer picture of the chronic and acute effects of cannabis use on memory function has emerged once studies have controlled for potential confounding factors and started to investigate the acute effects of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (Δ9-THC) and cannabidiol (CBD), the main ingredients in the extract of the cannabis plant in pharmacological challenge experiments. Relatively consistent findings have been reported regarding the acute impairments induced by a single dose of Δ9-THC on verbal and working memory. It is unclear whether they may persist beyond the intoxication state. In the long-term, these impairments seem particularly likely to manifest and may also persist following abstinence if regular and heavy use of cannabis strains high in Δ9-THC is started at an early age. Although still at an early stage, studies that employed advanced neuroimaging techniques have started to model the neural underpinnings of the effects of cannabis use and implicate a network of functional and morphological alterations that may moderate the effects of cannabis on memory function. Future experimental and epidemiological studies that take into consideration individual differences, particularly previous cannabis history and demographic characteristics, but also the precise mixture of the ingredients of the consumed cannabis are necessary to clarify the magnitude and the mechanisms by which cannabis-induced memory impairments occur and to elucidate underlying neurobiological mechanisms.

Keywords: cannabis, THC, CBD, memory, neuroimaging, fMRI

Introduction

Marijuana or Cannabis sativa (C. sativa) is the most widely used illicit drug,1,2 and its use often starts during teenage years.3 Cannabis contains more than 600 ingredients, including over 60 different cannabinoids,4 which are now recognized for both their toxic and potential therapeutic effects,5 and that are mediated through their effects on the endogenous cannabinoid system.6 Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (commonly known as Δ9-THC) is thought to be the principal psychoactive ingredient present in cannabis that is responsible for the acute and adverse effects of cannabis on various cognitive functions including memory and the induction of psychotic symptoms.715 In contrast, the other major cannabinoid that has attracted attention in recent years, cannabidiol (CBD), does not impair cognition,16 and may have anxiolytic and antipsychotic effects.1720 The potency of cannabis that is available on the street can vary and seems to have changed over time in many countries,2123 with strains that are high in Δ9-THC and low in CBD currently dominating the market.24,25 Hence, the acute effects as well as the effects of long-term use of cannabis on neurocognitive processes and psychological state are likely to vary depending on the precise mix of different cannabinoids present in the cannabis used.

Cannabis has historically been considered as a relatively harmless drug. However, the age of onset of cannabis use is decreasing and young people start to use cannabis at an earlier age,26,27 a time when they are likely to be more vulnerable to its impairing effects.28,29 Additionally, considering that the effects of Δ9-THC on memory appear to be dose-dependent,9,12,30,31 and increasingly potent forms of cannabis with higher Δ9-THC content are available on the street than before,25 the modern cannabis user may be at a particular risk for its adverse cognitive effects. Several factors are likely to influence the impact of cannabis on memory, particularly the duration and frequency of previous cannabis use,3133 as well as the strain of cannabis used,20 which complicate the interpretation of early observational studies investigating memory function in cannabis users. Only recently have the effects of cannabis been studied, employing controlled experimental designs in conjunction with neuroimaging techniques that allow one to draw conclusions regarding the acute effects of cannabis and the neurobiological mechanisms that may underlie the effects of cannabis on memory function. This update is organized into sections addressing the acute and chronic effects of cannabis on memory function at a behavioral level, but also provides an overview regarding the potential neural mechanisms underlying these processes, reflecting the focus of the recent literature.

Acute effects of cannabis on memory

When studying the effects of cannabis on memory function, it is important to be mindful that human memory is not a unitary measurable concept. Rather, it is a construct that considers multiple subsystems with different specializations and processes being involved – all of which are localized in overlapping but different brain regions.3436 In one approach, the stage model of memory refers to a temporal categorization that distinguishes between sensory memory, short-term memory, and long-term memory, and includes the processes of encoding (receiving and processing of information), followed by storage (creation of a permanent record of the encoded information), and recall (retrieval or recollection of information). As an alternative approach, the memory system is subdivided depending on its content, which refers to the discrimination between declarative and procedural memory systems. Procedural (implicit) memory is the storage of elements which are inaccessible to the consciousness (eg, the memory of skills such as playing a guitar). Most of the memory tests that have been employed when investigating the acute effects of cannabis assessed the declarative (explicit) memory system, which requires the conscious recollection of events and facts and is further divided into episodic (facts about the world) and semantic memory (storage of elements that have an autobiographical context),37 while working memory is defined as the temporary storage and manipulation of episodic information.38 The challenge to measure this complex construct is reflected in the variety of different memory tests that are proposed in the literature to measure similar domains (see Table 1). Therefore, it remains a matter of debate whether test performance is comparable across the different tests being employed, a point that needs to be considered when evaluating the studies that investigated the effects of cannabis on memory function.

Table 1.

Memory tests

Neurocognitive test Acronym Memory domain
Alphabet task AT Executive function
Benton visual retention test BVRT Visuospatial memory
Boston naming test BNT Verbal fluency (semantic memory)
Buschke selective reminding test BSRT Verbal learning and verbal memory
California verbal learning test CVLT Verbal learning and memory
Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery CANTAB Verbal and visual memory, learning, working memory, executive function
– ST: paired associate learning test – CANTAB-PAL – Visual associative memory and learning
– ST: rapid visual information processing task – CANTAB-RVIPT – Working memory
– ST: spatial span – CANTAB-SS – Spatial memory span
– ST: spatial working memory – CANTAB-SWM – Spatial working memory
– ST: one-touch stockings of cambridge – CANTAB-OTSC – Working memory
Controlled oral word association test COWAT Verbal fluency (semantic memory)
Delayed matching to sample DMTS Visual memory and working memory
Delayed recall DR Long-term episodic memory
Digit recall task DRT Visual memory
Digit span task DSP Working memory
Digit symbol substitution task DSST Visual memory
Face–name pair learning FNPL Verbal learning
Hopkins verbal learning test HVLT Verbal learning and memory
MicroCog: assessment of cognitive functioning MicroCog Logical memory and working memory
N-back task N-BACK Working memory
Omitted numbers ON Working memory
Paced auditory serial addition test PASAT Working memory
Repeated acquisition task RAT Learning
Perceptual priming task PPT Implicit memory
Pictorial memory task PMT Associative memory and retrieval
Prose recall PR Verbal memory
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test RAVLT Verbal learning and memory
Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test CFT Visuospatial memory
Short-delay response task SDRT Working memory
Spatial working memory task SWMT Spatial working memory
Subtracting serial sevens SS7 Working memory
Sternberg memory task SMT Working memory
Tower of London TOL Executive function
Verbal fluency task VFT Semantic memory
Verbal memory task VMT Verbal memory
Wechsler memory scale WMS Memory function
Wechsler adult intelligence scale (processing speed index) WAIS-PS Working memory
Wechsler adult intelligence scale (working memory index) WAIS-WM Working memory
Wechsler adult intelligence scale (verbal comprehension index) WAIS-VC Verbal memory
Wisconsin card sorting test WCST Executive function, working memory
Word recognition task WRT Verbal memory

Abbreviation: ST, subtest.

The acute effects of the different cannabinoids on memory function have been studied by directly administering cannabis or its different ingredients in order to examine its potential adverse or therapeutic effects.39 Several pharmacological challenge studies have consistently reported that the administration of Δ9-THC disrupted working and episodic memory in humans and animals.13,4048 However, when examining the literature regarding the acute effects of Δ9-THC, several factors that limit the direct comparability of the findings need to be considered. For instance, chronic exposure of cannabinoid drugs may result in the development of tolerance to the adverse effects of Δ9-THC or WIN 55,212-2 (a cannabinoid receptor agonist) on memory and executive functioning in humans and animals.45,4953 To illustrate, while 500 μg of Δ9-THC per kg body weight acutely decreased performance in the Tower of London Task (executive functioning) in occasional users,54 no such performance deficits were reported in heavy smokers following administration of the same dose.45 In accordance, acute Δ9-THC exposure has been associated with fewer impairments in working memory in heavy cannabis users than in occasional users,55 which is consistent with studies that reported no acute effects of Δ9-THC on memory function in frequent users.49,56

As summarized in Table 2, the majority of memory domains tested in heavy cannabis smokers were not affected by the acute administration of Δ9-THC or were only affected if the dose was high (eg, more than 3.9% Δ9-THC concentration in the smoked cigarette). In contrast, occasional users and regular users seem to show memory impairments after both high doses and low doses of Δ9-THC, with Δ9-THC impacting on memory function in a dose-dependent manner.8,9,12,40,49,54,57 For instance, increases in Δ9-THC concentration were associated with a linear decrease in performance in short-term memory tasks.9 Therefore, infrequent users may be more susceptible than heavy users to the acute effects of Δ9-THC on behavior,58 and hence it is important to take into account the history of previous cannabis use when investigating the acute effects of Δ9-THC on memory functioning.59 Encouragingly, despite the acute impairments caused by Δ9-THC, especially at high doses, a single dose of Δ9-THC is unlikely to have persistent effects on memory function. For instance, Curran et al8 found that Δ9-THC significantly impaired episodic memory and verbal recall 2 hours after the oral administration of Δ9-THC, with no residual effects being present 24 or 48 hours after administration.

Table 2.

Acute effects of Δ9-THC on memory

Author Subjects (gender ratio) Age in years (M) Previous cannabis exposure Dose of Δ9- THC (route) Memory testa
Low-dose Δ9-THC
Heavy users (≥joint/day)
 Hart et al49 n = 18 (10 m/8 f) 25 6.1 (M) days cannabis use/week
(±1.3); 4 joints per occasion (M)
1.8% (I) MicroCog
DRT
DSST
RAT
 Hart et al56 (EEG) n = 24 (13 m/11 f) 26 6.3 (M) days cannabis use/week
(±1.0); 4 joints per occasion (M)
1.8% (I) N-BACK
WRT
 Ramaekers et al45 n = 21 (15 m/6 f) 23 373.7 (M) times cannabis use/year, 5 (M) joints per occasion 400 μg/kg (I) TOL
Regular users (cannabis use > 1/month)
 Henquet et al42 n = 74 (56 m/18 f) 27 Cannabis use in past 12 months; 51% of the subjects had >1 time cannabis use/day 300 μg/kg (I) DMTS*
RAVLT*
 Ilan et al43 (EEG) n = 11 (6 m/5 f) 26 ≥10 × lifetime cannabis use; 15 joints (M)/month 1.8% (I) N-BACK*
WRT*
Occasional users (no or minimal previous exposure to cannabis)
 Bhattacharyya et al16 (fMRI) n = 15 (m only) 27 Lifetime cannabis use episodes ≤ 15 10 mg (O) WMS
 Bossong et al143 (fMRI) n = 13 (m only) 22 ≥4 × cannabis use/lifetime; <1/week 6 mg (I) PMT
 Bossong et al144 (fMRI) n = 17 (m only) 21 ≥4 × cannabis use/lifetime; <1/week 6 mg (I) SMT*
 Curran et al8 n = 15 (m only) 24 Prior experience; ≤1 cannabis exposure/week 7.5 mg (O) BSRT*
VFT*
PPT
RVIPT
SS7
PR
DR
 D’Souza et al12 n = 22 (14 m/8 f) 29 Prior experience; 6 subjects used cannabis > 100 times/lifetime 5 mg (IV) DMTS*
HVLT*
VFT
 Englund et al61 n = 26 (14 m/12 f) 26 Previous cannabis use ≥ 1 1.5 mg (IV) DSP*
HVLT*
 Morrison et al44 (EEG) n = 16 (7 m/6 f) 26 Previous cannabis use ≥ 1 1.25 mg (IV) N-BACK*
High-dose Δ9-THC
Heavy users (≥joint/day)
 Hart et al56 (EEG) n = 24 (13 m/11 f) 26 6.3 (M) days cannabis use/week
(±1.0); 4 joints per occasion (M)
3.9% (I) N-BACK
WRT
 Hart et al49 n = 18 (10 m/8 f) 25 6.1 (M) days cannabis use/week
(±1.3); 4 cigarettes per occasion (M)
3.9% (I) DRT*
MicroCog*
RAT
DSST
 Ramaekers et al55 n = 12 (9 m/3 f) 23 340 (M) joints/year, 2.3 (M) joints per occasion, 6.2 (M) years cannabis use 500 μg/kg (I) TOL
 Weinstein et al41 n = 14 (10 m/4 f) 27 ≥1 joint/day in the last 5 years 17 mg (I) WCST*
Regular users (cannabis use > 1/month)
 Ilan et al43 (EEG) n = 12 (6 m/6 f) 26 ≥10 × lifetime cannabis use; 13 joints (M) 3.6% (I) N-BACK*
WRT*
 Ilan et al142 (EEG) n = 10 (NR) 27 Cannabis use in past 12 months; cannabis use episodes ≥ 1/month, ≤1/week 3.45% (I) N-BACK*
WRT*
 Hunault et al9 n = 24 (m only) 24 7.7 (standard deviation 3.7) joints/month 69.4 mg (I) SMT*
 Ramaekers et al54 n = 12 (8 m/4 f) 23 55 (M) times cannabis use/year, 1.2 (M) joints per occasion, 7.4 (M) years of cannabis use 500 μg/kg (I) TOL
Occasional users (no or minimal previous exposure to cannabis)
 Böcker et al57 (EEG) n = 16 (m only) 18–45 >2 and <9 cigarettes/month 69.4 mg (I) SMT*
 Curran et al8 n = 15 (m only) 24 Prior experience; ≤1 cannabis exposure/week 15 mg (O) PR*
BSRT*
VFT*
DR*
PPT
RVIPT
SS7
 Lane et al40 n = 5 (3 m/2 f) 21 to 34 Cannabis use 2 to 10 times/month 2.20% (I) DMTS*
 Lane et al40 n = 5 (3 m/2 f) 21 to 34 Cannabis use 2 to 10 times/month 3.89% (I) DMTS*
 Morrison et al13 n = 22 (m only) 28 2–1000 prior cannabis use episodes, negative urine test 2.5 mg (IV) DSP*
RAVLT*
VFT
 Ramaekers et al54 n = 20 (14 m/6 f) 19–29 ≥5 times cannabis use in past year, not daily users 500 μg/kg (I) TOL*
 Weinstein et al41 n = 14 (10 m/4 f) 27 ≥1 joint/day in the last 5 years 13 mg (I) WCST*

Notes:

*

Δ9-THC induced deficits in performance;

Δ9-THC did not induce deficits in performance;

a

see Table 1.

Abbreviations: Δ9-THC, delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol; M, mean; n, number; m, male; f, female; I, inhalation (smoking); MicroCog, MicroCog Assessment of Cognitive Functioning; DRT, digit recall task; DSST, digit symbol substitution task; RAT, repeated acquisition task; N-BACK, N-back task; WRT, word recognition task; TOL, Tower of London; DMTS, delayed matching to sample; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; EEG, electroecephalography study; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging study; O, oral; WMS, Wechsler Memory Scale; PMT, pictorial memory task; SMT, Sternberg memory task; IV, intravenous; HVLT, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; PR, prose recall; BSRT, Buschke Selektive Reminding Test; VFT, verbal fluency task; DR, delayed recall; PPT, perceptual priming task; RVIPT, rapid visual information processing task; SS7, subtracting serial sevens; DSP, digit span task.

The other major component in the cannabis extract is CBD, a cannabinoid that has received less attention in pharmacological challenge studies investigating the effect of cannabis on memory function – probably due to its known nonharmful effects on cognition.43,60 However, considering that CBD may be protective for some aspects of memory function by inhibiting the Δ9-THC-induced impairments in episodic memory in humans and spatial memory in animals,20,61,62 but not in working memory,60,61 CBD may have therapeutic potential in reversing certain cognitive impairments induced by cannabis. Therefore, further experimental studies will be useful to understand the effects of the two main ingredients on memory function separately and in combination.

Studies have also investigated whether genetic variations may moderate variable sensitivity to the effects of Δ9-THC. For instance, a polymorphism in the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene has been shown to moderate the effect of Δ9-THC on verbal memory.42 Genetic variations may thus underlie some of the inconsistencies in results across studies depending on the genetic makeup of study participants. As indicated in Table 2, the overrepresentation of mainly adult male subjects in some of these study samples may also limit the generalizability of the results, especially because there are some suggestions that gender may be an important factor in mediating variability in the response to cannabinoids in animals and humans;6368 preliminary evidence also suggests an effect of gender on Δ9-THC-induced impairments in working memory and learning in humans and animals.6971

In summary, based on the recent literature, a relatively robust picture regarding the acute effects of cannabis emerged (ie, there is convincing evidence that the acute administration Δ9-THC impairs memory function), with several factors potentially moderating its effects, including previous exposure to cannabis, the dose of Δ9-THC administered, the precise Δ9-THC:CBD ratio, a genetic vulnerability to its effects, and the particular assessment of the multifactorial nature of memory function.

Long-term effects of cannabis on memory

In contrast to the acute effects, less consistency exists regarding the long-term effects of cannabis use on neurocognitive functioning. A major difficulty that appears when interpreting studies that compared cannabis users with control subjects regarding their memory performance is the heterogeneity of the selected samples. For instance, studies used different criteria to categorize their subjects (ie, allocated them to distinct but at times overlapping categories such as former cannabis users, current regular cannabis users, or heavy cannabis users, long-term and short-term cannabis users, or early-onset and late-onset cannabis users). Based on the methodological approaches in the literature, Table 3 summarizes the studies in terms of the subject selection criteria employed and the confounding factors that have been considered.

Table 3.

Effects of cannabis use on memory performance

Author Subjects Age in years (M) Age of onset of use (M) Cannabis use in years (M) Joints [episodes]/lifetime (M) Joints/month (M) Days of use/month (M) Abstinence period Controlled confoundera Memory testsb
Former cannabis users (history of cannabis use, ≤ 12 joints in the last 3 months)
Pope73 n = 45 41 NR 15 (median) 11,000 (median) ≤2 last 3 months NR 7 days DU, PC, PsyM, A, G, IQ BVRT
BSRT
Fried et al74 n = 16 18 14 2.2 2203 0 NR Day of testing (NS) DU, PC, PsyM, A, G, SES WAIS-PS
WMS
Current regular cannabis users (<30 joints/month or <20 days/month or <2000 joints/lifetime)
Jager et al77 (fMRI) n = 10 23 NR 7.1 1300 (median) NR NR 1 week DU, PC, MC, PsyM, A, G, IQ SMT
Jager et al78 (fMRI) n = 20 25 NR NR 1900 (median) NR NR NU DU, PC, MC, PsyM, A, G, IQ PMT
Grant et al111 n = 16 22 NR NR NR NR 12 NR DU, PC, A, G, SES, E CANTAB-OTSC* CANTAB-RVIPT
CANTAB-SWM
Harvey et al113 n = 34 16 NR NR NR 12 (median) 12 (median) 12 hours DU, PC, A, G CANTAB-RVIPT*
CANTAB-SWM*
RAVLT*
CANTAB-SS
WAIS-WM
CANTAB-PAL
Current heavy cannabis users (≥30 joints/month or ≥20 days/month or ≥2000 joints/lifetime)
Kanayama et al96 (fMRI) n = 12 38 NR NR 19,200 ≥28 NR NR DU, PC, MC, PsyM, G, E SDRT
Smith et al112 (fMRI) n = 10 20 NR 4.6 2697 46 NR <5 hours DU, PC, IQ, PF N-BACK
Messinis et al83 n = 20 24 NR 7 NR ≥16 21 123 hours (M) DU, PC, MC, PsyM, G, IQ, E RAVLT*
VFT*
BNT
Solowij et al33 n = 51 29 NR 10.2 NR NR 27 17 hours (M) DU, PC, A, G, IQ RAVLT
WCST
AT
ON
PASAT
Fontes et al82 n = 55 30 19 10.9 6790 51 NR 4.1 days (M) PC, MC, A, IQ, E WCST* WAIS-VC
Bolla et al30 n = 7 21 NR 5.3 NR 375 NR 28 days DU, PC, A, G, IQ CFT*
PAL*
WCST*
RAVLT*
WMS
Short-term cannabis users (<4 years of cannabis use)
Schweinsburg et al79 (fMRI) n = 15 17 NR 3.4 310 NR 13 48 hours DU, PC, MC, PsyM, A SWMT
Schweinsburg et al80 (fMRI) n = 13 17 NR 2.3 342 NR 14 3.3 days (M) DU, PC, MC, PsyM, A, G, IQ, SES SWMT
Schweinsburg et al80 (fMRI) n = 13 18 NR 2.7 515 NR 17 38 days (M) DU, PC, MC, PsyM, A, G, IQ, SES SWMTv
Medina92 n = 31 18 NR 2.91 541 57 NR 28 days PC, MC, A, G, SES, E WMS* CFT
CVLT
Fried et al74 n = 19 18 16 1.8 122 6 NR Day of testing (NS) DU, PC, PsyM, A, G, SES WMS
Block et al109 (PET) n = 18 NR NR 3.9 NR 72 NR 16 hours (M) DU, PC, PsyM, A, G, BV, HW BSRT* WAIS-PS
Long-term cannabis users (cannabis use > 15 years)
Pope73 n = 63 36 NR 19 (median) 18,720 (median) ≥28 NR 7 days DU, PC, PsyM, A, G, IQ BSRT* BVRT
Messinis et al83 n = 20 33 NR 15.6 NR ≥16 20 ≥24 hours DU, PC, MC, PsyM, G, IQ, E BNT*
RAVLT*
VFT*
Battisti et al84 n = 24 36 17 (median) 17.0 NR 435 (median) 30 (median) 12 hours PC, MC, A, G, E VMT*
Solowij et al33 n = 51 41 NR 23.9 NR NR 28 17 hours (M) DU, PC, A, G, IQ AT*
RAVLT*
ON
PASAT
WCST
Late-onset cannabis users (≥17 age of onset)
Fontes et al82 n = 49 30 21 8.7 5160 45 NR 3.8 days (M) PC, MC, A, IQ, E WCST
WAIS-VC
Pope et al85 n = 63 44 ≥17 NR 12,480 (median) NR NR 28 days DU, PC, PsyM, A, G WCST* BSRT
BVRT
WMS
Early-onset cannabis users (<17 age of onset)
Solowij et al31 n = 52 19 16 2.4 NR 17.5 14 (median) 20 hours (median) DU, PC, G, IQ, E RAVLT*
Fried et al74 n = 19 18 15 2.6 1884 12.4 NR Day of testing (NS) DU, PC, PsyM, A, G, SES WAIS-PS*
WMS*
Hanson et al75 n = 19 18 16 NR 465 NR 16 3 days PC, MC, A, G, IQ, SES HVLT*
WAIS-WM*
Ashtari et al97 (MRI) n = 14 19 13 5.3 NR 174 NR 6.7 months (M) DU, PC, A, G, IQ, SES, BV CVLT
Nestor et al138 n = 35 22 16.5 5.7 NR NR 23 15 hours (M) DU, PC, MC, PsyM, A, G, IQ FNPL*
Gruber et al32 n = 34 23 15.5 7.24 ≥2500 77 NR 12 hours DU, PC, MC, PsyM, A, IQ WCST* CFT
CVLT
COWAT
Pope et al85 n = 69 36 <17 NR 17,368 (median) NR NR 28 days DU, PC, PsyM, A, G BSRT*
WCST*
BVRT
WMS
Jager et al102 (fMRI) n = 21 17 13 NR 4006 NR NR 5.1 weeks (M) DU, PC, MC, PsyM, IQ PMT
SMT

Notes:

*

Poorer performance when compared to noncannabis users;

no differences in performance when compared to noncannabis users;

a

controlled though exclusion, matching, or statistical methods;

b

see Table 1.

Abbreviations: M, mean; n, number; NR, not reported; DU, other drug use; PC, psychiatric condition; PsyM, psychoactive medication; A, age; G, gender; IQ, Intelligence Quotient; BVRT, Benton Visual Retention Test; BSRT, Buschke Selective Reminding Test; NS, not further specified; SES, socioeconomic status; WAIS-PS, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Processing Speed Index); WMS, Wechsler Memory Scale; MC, medical condition; SMT, Sternberg Memory Task; NU, negative urine toxicology; PMT, pictorial memory task; E, education; CANTAB-OTSC, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery One-Touch Stockings of Cambridge; CANTAB-RVIPT, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery rapid visual information processing task; CANTAB-SWM, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery Spatial Working Memory; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; CANTAB-SS, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery spatial span; WAIS-WM, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Working Memory Index); CANTAB-PAL, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery Paired Associate Learning Test; SDRT, short-delay response task; PF, personality factors; N-BACK, N-back task; VFT, verbal fluency task; BNT, Boston naming test; WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; AT, alphabet task; ON, omitted numbers; PASAT, Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test; WAIS-VC, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (Verbal Comprehension Index); CFT, Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test; PAL, paired associate learning test; SWMT, spatial working memory task; CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test; PET, positron emission tomography study; BV, brain volume; HW, height and weight; VMT, verbal memory task; HVLT, Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging study; FNPL, face–name pair learning; COWAT, controlled oral word association test; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging study.

One crucial factor that has been suggested to interfere with the accuracy and interpretability of the results is the abstinence period. For instance, a previous meta-analysis implicated that regular cannabis users present with selective long-term memory impairments, including the domains of learning and retrieval,72 although the magnitude of the pooled effect sizes were modest, and uncertainty exists as to whether these deficits persist beyond the acute intoxication state. The question regarding the irreversibility of the effects of cannabis has specifically been addressed by Pope et al,73 who compared adult heavy and long-term cannabis users (smoked at least 5000 times in their lives and were daily smokers at study entry) and controls (who had smoked no more than 50 times in their lives) regarding their performance on neurocognitive measures at three different time-points after the first assessment. On days 0, 1, and 7, cannabis users significantly differed in terms of task performance compared to controls, and the learning and recall deficits across time points were related to Δ9-THC-creatinine ratios at first assessment. However, by day 28, the two groups did not differ any longer, and initial Δ9-THC concentrations were not related to task performance, suggesting that memory impairments in long-term cannabis users may not persist beyond the impact of Δ9-THC-induced “residual effects.” This is in line with more recent studies implicating that the severity of deficits in memory and immediate recall associated with cannabis use decreased over time following abstinence;58,7476 moreover, studies reported that normal memory functioning was observed in regular cannabis users when they were abstinent for between 48 hours up to a month.7780

In contrast, a more recent systematic review concluded that long-term cannabis use impairs memory, particularly verbal and working memory,81 reflecting the evidence from another set of studies that reported impairments in working memory and learning that persisted beyond the acute intoxication state in heavy cannabis users.30,8284 This body of evidence points to the idea that some individuals may recover from their memory impairments after a period of abstinence, while others continue exhibiting the memory impairments after the use of cannabis, implicating that additional factors (other than intoxication state) need to be identified in order to explain the memory deficits associated with cannabis use. To illustrate, it was reported that heavy smokers (who smoked on average 94 joints per week) that were abstinent for 28 days showed persistent impairments in a range of memory functions.30 In addition, a recent study suggested that neuropsychological deficits did not recover completely even 1 year after the cessation of cannabis use in those who started using it regularly before the age of 18 years,29 indicating that age of onset and frequency of cannabis use may be important moderators that need to be considered.

In fact, accumulating evidence suggests that deficits in memory performance in abstinent adolescent and adult cannabis users depend on the weekly dose of cannabis used,30 as well as on the frequency, duration, and age of onset of cannabis use.29,3133,81,83,85 For instance, in a group of adolescent cannabis users, the frequency of use explained 24.2% of the variance in word recall and a combined predictive model that included quantity and age of onset of cannabis use accounted for 31.5% of the variance in memory performance.31 In accordance, Meier et al29 showed that the decline in memory performance in cannabis users who began smoking in adolescence was independent of the frequency of cannabis use, whereas the decline in adult-onset users was dependent on the frequency of use.

Finally, at a neurobiological level, long-term and early-onset cannabis use appear to cause greater morphological and functional alterations in the still developing brain rather than in the mature human and animal brain.71,8691 Therefore, as summarized in Table 3, it appears that impairments in memory are unlikely to persist above and beyond the intoxication state in late-onset users and short-term users,74,79,80, 82,85,92 and if used only in an occasional manner.74 In contrast, early-onset and heavy cannabis use over the long-term seem to have a particularly adverse effect on memory function and may lead to persistent impairments in a range of memory domains beyond the acute intoxication state. This is also consistent with evidence from animal studies where it has been possible to investigate this issue under controlled experimental conditions with greater methodological rigor; it was found that the adverse effects of Δ9-THC on learning are more severe in adolescents than in adults.71,91 Similarly, chronic treatment with the cannabinoid receptor agonist, WIN55,212-2 (WIN), was associated with long-lasting impairments in object recognition memory in pubertal but not in adult rats.93,94

However, not all studies have reported memory impairments in heavy and early-onset cannabis users.86,9597 As indicated by pharmacological challenge studies, an important determinant of the long-term effects of cannabis use may be the precise mixture of the ingredients of the consumed cannabis. Although a naturalistic study reported that cannabis users who smoked cannabis high in CBD and low in Δ9-THC were protected from the memory-impairing effects associated with cannabis strains high in Δ9-THC,20 most of the observational studies to date did not include the Δ9-THC/CBD ratio (eg, by employing hair analysis as a proxy estimate of the amount of Δ9-THC or CBD in the cannabis used) as a potential confounder in statistical reporting (see Table 3). Given the impact of the Δ9-THC/CBD ratio on brain structures implicated in memory function and executive functions in cannabis users (while taking into account the variability of this ratio in street cannabis25),98,99 consideration of this factor in the design and analysis of studies may improve the replicability and interpretability of the results, particularly because simple and valid methods are now applicable for the quantitative detection of Δ9-THC and CBD.100,101

In light of the complexities in the assessment of memory function, some of the inconsistencies in results across studies may be related to the different memory paradigms that have been used in these studies, which may have investigated related but not completely overlapping aspects of memory and employed tasks with differing levels of complexity. For instance, studies that have employed functional neuroimaging techniques to investigate the neural substrates underlying memory impairments related to cannabis use have often used tasks that are fairly simple and specific to a memory-domain rather than more complex paradigms that can assess multiple domains. While this strategy has generally been employed to avoid potential confounding effects associated with differences in task performance, which can cloud the interpretation of brain activation patterns related to specific task demands, this restricts a direct comparison of the results of these studies with those employing more complex paradigms. Interestingly, neuroimaging studies have consistently reported similar performance levels in memory tasks (eg, throughout short delay response tasks, face-name cued recall tasks, and pictorial memory tasks) in heavy cannabis users when compared to controls,77,78,80,86,95,97,102104 suggesting perhaps that more challenging and multidimensional memory tasks may be required to discriminate chronic cannabis users from occasional users or nonusers. This is consistent with recent evidence from animal literature, which suggests that persistence of the adverse effects of chronic cannabinoid exposure [eg, the agonist WIN55,212-2 (WIN)] beyond the immediate withdrawal period may depend on the specific memory domain being tested, with some forms of hippocampal-dependent short-term memory (such as that measured by the object location task) being particularly sensitive, while deficits in other measures of short-term memory (such as the object recognition and water maze tasks) recover following a short period of abstinence.105

As shown in Table 1, a commonly employed and well-validated task is the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT),106108 a test that assesses a range of memory domains, including immediate and delayed recall, learning rate, recognition, proactive and retroactive interference, and primacy and recency effects. There is a greater degree of consistency with regard to poorer performance using the RAVLT in adolescent and adult heavy cannabis users when compared to controls,2931,33,73 unlike some of the other paradigms such as omitted numbers test, short-delay response task, or paced auditory serial addition test – all of which were tasks that were employed to measure working memory – with inconclusive results (see Table 3).8,33,96 Similarly, current long-term and heavy cannabis users, but not former or short-term cannabis users, performed poorer in the Buschke Selective Reminding Test (BSRT),73,85,109 a test that assesses multiple cognitive processes including learning and verbal memory, sensitivity to interference, recognition, and retrieval.106,110

In sum, early observational studies pointed to the idea of cannabis-induced impairments in memory and learning,72 although the extent of the irreversible neurotoxic effects of cannabis on working memory, verbal learning, and memory functions remained a matter of contention due to a number of potential confounders that have variably been considered. Although some subsequent studies are still of limited interpretability due to a lack of consideration of crucial confounding factors,96,111113 the majority of recent studies have generally attempted to address these methodological issues by considering factors such as the acute intoxication state, interindividual variability in past and current cannabis use, as well as the psychiatric and medical status of study participants (see Table 3), leading to a clearer picture of the long-term effects. There is now a growing body of evidence that tends to suggest that persistent memory deficits in a range of domains are more likely to occur in frequent and long-term cannabis users who start using cannabis early in life and consume cannabis strains high in Δ9-THC, while impairments become particularly salient when memory is measured as a complex multifactorial contract. These findings may be particularly worrying, given the increasing prevalence of cannabis use especially in young populations,3 particularly with high Δ9-THC concentration.25

Underlying neural mechanisms

Over the last couple of decades, advanced neuroimaging techniques have allowed investigation of the neural mechanisms underlying the effects of acute and chronic cannabis exposure in humans in vivo. While structural magnetic resonance imaging allows for the display of brain morphology in vivo, functional imaging techniques allow an indirect estimation of regional neural activity by utilizing the fact that neuronal activation results in regionally increased blood flow and metabolism. The application of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), single photon emission tomography, and positron emission tomography (PET) allow the investigation of brain activity changes in response to a study participant performing a specific cognitive task.

In pharmacological challenge studies, the acute effects of cannabinoids on memory function can be analyzed, while imaging studies comparing subjects with and without a history of cannabis use allow the investigation of changes in neural function associated with chronic cannabis exposure in humans.17,114 Cannabinoids exert their effects by binding to specific endogenous cannabinoid receptors, including the CB1 receptor and the CB2 receptor,24 both of which are G-protein-coupled receptors for cannabinoid ligands. However, the receptors differ in their structure, affinity for ligands, and distribution in the human body. CB2 receptors are mainly expressed in the immune system and in peripheral nerve tissues, and are not considered to affect cognition.115 Given the high densities of the CB1 receptors present in brain regions that are critically involved in learning and memory functions, particularly the prefrontal cortex (PFC), hippocampus, basal ganglia, anterior cingulate, and cerebellum,116,117 the endogenous cannabinoid (eCB) system is thought to modulate the neural substrates accompanying memory functioning after exposure to cannabis.118

Most of the major cannabinoids present in the extract of the cannabis plant act presynaptically as agonists at CB1 receptors,119 and thereby cause alterations in neuromodulator systems (eg, dopaminergic, cholinergic, serotonergic, GABAergic, and glutamatergic systems) that in turn affect molecular mechanisms relevant to cognitive processes and prevent long-term potentiation – a process that is widely believed to underlie learning and memory.120123 In contrast, CB1 antagonists/inverse agonists such as CBD or rimonabant may reverse some of the neurobiological and behavioral effects of Δ9-THC in animals and humans.19,20,124126 The particular vulnerability of the adolescent brain to the adverse cognitive effects of cannabis use may also be related to the increased sensitivity of the eCB system during this developmental stage due to the synaptic pruning and myelination that still occur, particularly in the frontal regions of the brain.118 Pharmacological studies have shown that both the acute and chronic administration of Δ9-THC or CB1 agonists caused morphological and functional changes in the hippocampus in rats,127,128 and the modulation of hippocampal CB1 receptors after the administration of Δ9-THC significantly disrupted memory performance in rats.129 Moreover, it was found that microinjection of a CB1 receptor antagonist into the hippocampus blocked spatial memory deficits caused by Δ9-THC administration, indicating that the disruptive effects of Δ9-THC on memory function are mediated through its effects on hippocampal CB1 receptors.130

Studies that have investigated human cannabis users employing structural imaging have reported morphological alterations in the parahippocampal, hippocampal, and thalamic areas,131,132 while larger hippocampus volumes have been related to higher memory scores in controls, but not in frequent cannabis users.97 Despite these structural imaging findings, there is modest evidence of a major effect of cannabis use on the brain structures relevant to memory function,114,133 and structural imaging studies lack the ability to display the dynamic neural processes underlying memory function. In this context, functional neuroimaging (single photon emission tomography, PET, and fMRI) studies of cannabis users have been used to assess neural activation during the resting state or during the performance of memory tasks. Resting state neuroimaging studies provide evidence of alterations in regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) and cerebral metabolism related to acute and chronic cannabis exposure,114,134136 while brain activation may normalize after a period of abstinence.137 Conversely, fMRI imaging studies using paradigms that engage memory and learning processes in cannabis users have reported both hypoactivation and hyperactivation in parahippocampal regions,78,96,102,103,138 despite nonsignificant differences in task performance between cannabis users and noncannabis users.

Another fMRI study reported working memory-related hypoactivation in the inferior frontal and temporal regions, including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and right inferior frontal and superior temporal areas; however, hyperactivation was noted in the medial temporal areas in cannabis users when compared to nonusers,79 despite similar task performance. Similarly, in a PET study, Eldreth et al139 found hypoactivation in the left perigenual ACC and the left lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) and bilateral hyperactivity in the hippocampus. While on the one hand, these studies support the notion of “hyperactivity,” indicating that increased brain activation reflects a higher neurophysiological effort or a compensatory mechanism to achieve similar performance levels, on the other hand they also tend to suggest that such compensatory mechanisms may no longer be sufficient during the performance of more challenging tasks.96,140 Moreover, parahippocampal activity appeared to be dependent on the frequency of previous cannabis use when performing an activation task.86 Jacobsen et al141 found that adolescent cannabis users performed poorer in the working memory task compared to controls, and that the former group failed to deactivate the right hippocampus, as opposed to controls. It has been suggested that this failure may reflect dysfunction of inhibitory interneurons within the hippocampus in cannabis users during mnemonic processing.

The acute administration of Δ9-THC and its neurocognitive correlates have only been studied recently, using sophisticated neuroimaging techniques with better spatial resolution.39 Bhattacharyya et al16 provided the first human evidence that memory impairments induced by Δ9-THC were mediated through its acute effects on brain activation in the medial temporal and prefrontal areas. In this study, a modest dose of orally administered Δ9-THC was found to augment task-related activity in the parahippocampal cortex during the encoding condition of a verbal learning task, thereby disrupting the linear decremental change in neural activity associated with repeated presentations of verbal stimuli and the relationship between parahippocampal activity and task performance in healthy occasional cannabis users, indicating that increased neurophysiological efforts are necessary under the acute effects of Δ9-THC in order to meet the demands of the task during encoding.

During the recall condition, Δ9-THC attenuated the task-related activity in the LPFC and ACC. These findings are in line with an electrophysiological study that reported an attenuation of stimulus-locked event related potentials (ERPs) in response to increased memory-task difficulty and impaired task performance after the acute administration of marijuana; additionally, a higher degree of intoxication (measured in terms of heart rate and subjective ratings) was related to a more dramatic reduction in ERP amplitude.142 In a recent fMRI study, Bossong et al143 reported that Δ9-THC attenuated activity in the insula and inferior frontal gyrus on the right side and in the middle occipital gyrus on the left side during the encoding condition of an associative memory task. These effects were unrelated to task performance, implicating that neural activity patterns associated with Δ9-THC administration could not be explained by differences in task performance and suggest that Δ9-THC may affect neural activity in brain regions critical to learning new information. During the recall condition, Δ9-THC resulted in a network-wide increase in activation, with the strongest effects being observed in the cuneus and precuneus. Given the opposite effects of Δ9-THC on brain activity during the encoding and recall conditions, the authors suggested that Δ9-THC affects memory more directly at an early stage (ie, during encoding), while changes during recall were more likely to be reflective of a mechanism to compensate for the effect of the drug during encoding.

Moreover, Bossong et al144 showed that a rising working memory load was related to a linear increase in activity in brain areas that mediate working memory in subjects under the placebo condition. In this way, Δ9-THC disrupted this linear relationship by inducing hyperactivity and decreasing task performance at low working memory load. In keeping with behavioral evidence suggesting differential vulnerability to the adverse effects of cannabis, preliminary evidence has emerged pointing to the neural mechanisms that may underlie this genetically mediated variable sensitivity to the effects of Δ9-THC.39 This study reported that the effects of Δ9-THC on medial temporal, striatal, and midbrain function during the encoding and recall conditions of a verbal memory task were moderated by variations in the AKT1 and the dopamine transporter (DAT1) genes that regulate central dopaminergic neurotransmission. Furthermore, the effects of Δ9-THC on brain activation during the encoding and recall condition were greater in those carrying the risk variants of both genes compared to the rest, indicating that the impact of Δ9-THC on the neural activity that may underlie memory function might depend on individual genetic profiles.

Another factor that potentially determines the level of vulnerability to the harmful effects of cannabis on cognitive functioning is the relative proportion of the different cannabinoids, particularly the relative proportion of Δ9-THC to CBD. While, at a behavioral level, the acute administration of Δ9-THC is thought to have adverse effects on memory and induces anxiety and psychotic-like symptoms in humans,100 CBD has not been associated with these adverse effects,39,60 and might even reverse the deficits induced by Δ9-THC.126 For instance, Morgan et al20 reported that impairments in episodic memory induced by Δ9-THC were prevented by a higher level of CBD. Moreover, at a neurobiological level, Bhattacharyya et al19 reported opposite effects of Δ9-THC and CBD on activation in the ACC, medial PFC, and the LPFC during verbal recall in healthy men with minimal previous cannabis exposure. These findings further implicate the involvement of CB1-rich brain regions on memory through the potential adverse effects of cannabinoid agonists such as Δ9-THC, and also propose protective effects of cannabinoid antagonists/inverse agonists such as CBD on memory function and learning in humans.

Conclusion

In conclusion, evidence regarding the acute impairments in memory function induced by cannabis is generally robust, particularly for those using cannabis with a lower proportion of CBD and higher proportion of Δ9-THC. In other words, the effects are likely to depend on the type of cannabis used (ie, the greater the dose of Δ9-THC, the greater the memory impairment).9 However, whether those with a history of frequent cannabis use may develop tolerance to the acute impairments caused by Δ9-THC is not entirely clear. Although some studies have reported recovery from cannabis-induced cognitive impairments in long-term users,73,74 deficits in verbal and working memory, as well as alterations in brain function and structure associated with cannabis use, are likely to persist beyond the acute intoxication state, particularly when heavy cannabis use is started at an early age. These findings emphasize the need for further investigations regarding the role of early-onset cannabis use on neurodevelopmental processes.

Since higher Δ9-THC concentrations are now dominating the market,23 and given that high prevalence rates exist particularly among teenagers,1,3 its consumption represents a major public health concern. Although the precise neurocognitive and neurochemical mechanisms underlying the marijuana-induced memory impairments remain to be elucidated, cognitive neurobiological studies suggest that cannabinoids may affect functioning in memory-relevant brain areas by interfering with the homeostatic role of the eCB system. While the hippocampus appears to have the highest density of CB1 receptors, and since this has been shown to play an important role in the disruptive effects of Δ9-THC on memory,130,145 studies have highlighted that these effects may also be related to the effects of Δ9-THC on prefrontal and parahippocampal function,16,46 implicating the involvement of a network of brain areas affected by Δ9-THC.143 In this context, the increased brain activation noted in cannabis users while performing hippocampus-dependent memory tasks may reflect compensatory mechanisms that could be activated in order to achieve similar performance levels comparable to those observed in nonusers. Moreover, these effects may depend on individual genetic profiles,39,42 suggesting interesting avenues for future genetic studies to investigate the modulatory role of susceptibility genes with regard to the memory impairments caused by cannabis.

Overall, further research elucidating the precise neural mechanisms underlying the heterogeneous and sometimes opposite effects of cannabinoids will be useful, not only in order to facilitate the development of treatments that may prevent the deleterious effects of cannabis, the most widely used illicit drug worldwide,2 but also to identify potential candidate targets for memory impairments in conditions such as schizophrenia or dementia. Given the therapeutic potential of cannabinoids such as Δ9-THC in the management of medical conditions including neuropathic pain or spasticity due to multiple sclerosis (for a review see Grant et al146), an understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying the effects of cannabis on memory function also has a wider implication beyond psychiatric disorders, as it may lead to the identification or synthesis of related molecules with the desired therapeutic effects, but without the undesired side effects.

Footnotes

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

References

  • 1.Degenhardt L, Chiu WT, Sampson N, et al. Toward a global view of alcohol, tobacco, cannabis, and cocaine use: findings from the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. PLoS Med. 2008;5(7):e141. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime World Drug Report 2012. [Accessed December 1, 2012]. Available from: http://www.unodc.org/documents/data-and-analysis/WDR2012/WDR_2012_web_small.pdf.
  • 3.Terry-McElrath YM, Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Yamaguchi R. Substance abuse counseling services in secondary schools: a national study of schools and students, 1999–2003. J Sch Health. 2005;75(9):334–341. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2005.00047.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Mechoulam R, Gaoni Y. The absolute configuration of delta-1- tetrahydrocannabinol, the major active constituent of hashish. Tetrahedron Lett. 1967;12:1109–1111. doi: 10.1016/s0040-4039(00)90646-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Murray RM, Morrison PD, Henquet C, Di Forti M. Cannabis, the mind and society: the hash realities. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(11):885–895. doi: 10.1038/nrn2253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Zanettini C, Panlilio LV, Alicki M, Goldberg SR, Haller J, Yasar S. Effects of endocannabinoid system modulation on cognitive and emotional behavior. Front Behav Neurosci. 2011;5:57. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Leonard BE. Therapeutic Uses of cannabis. British Medical Association (BMA). Harwood Academic Publishers, UK. 1997. £11.99. ISBN 90-5702-318-0. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental. 1998;13(8):592. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Curran HV, Brignell C, Fletcher S, Middleton P, Henry J. Cognitive and subjective dose-response effects of acute oral Delta 9- tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) in infrequent cannabis users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2002;164(1):61–70. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1169-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hunault CC, Mensinga TT, Böcker KB, et al. Cognitive and psychomotor effects in males after smoking a combination of tobacco and cannabis containing up to 69 mg delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2009;204(1):85–94. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1440-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bhattacharyya S, Atakan Z, Martin-Santos R, et al. Preliminary report of biological basis of sensitivity to the effects of cannabis on psychosis: AKT1 and DAT1 genotype modulates the effects of δ-9-tetrahydrocannabinol on midbrain and striatal function. Mol Psychiatry. 2012;17(12):1152–1155. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bhattacharyya S, Crippa JA, P Allen P, et al. Induction of psychosis by Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol reflects modulation of prefrontal and striatal function during attentional salience processing. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(1):27–36. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.D’Souza DC, Perry E, MacDougall L, et al. The psychotomimetic effects of intravenous delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in healthy individuals: implications for psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29(8):1558–1572. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Morrison PD, Zois V, McKeown DA, et al. The acute effects of synthetic intravenous Delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol on psychosis, mood and cognitive functioning. Psychol Med. 2009;39(10):1607–1616. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709005522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Large M, Sharma S, Compton MT, Slade T, Nielssen O. Cannabis use and earlier onset of psychosis: a systematic meta-analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(6):555–561. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Luzi S, Morrison PD, Powell J, di Forti M, Murray RM. What is the mechanism whereby cannabis use increases risk of psychosis? Neurotox Res. 2008;14(2–3):105–112. doi: 10.1007/BF03033802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Bhattacharyya S, Fusar-Poli P, Borgwardt S, et al. Modulation of mediotemporal and ventrostriatal function in humans by Delta9- tetrahydrocannabinol: a neural basis for the effects of Cannabis sativa on learning and psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(4):442–451. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Crippa JA, Lacerda AL, Amaro E, Busatto Filho G, Zuardi AW, Bressan RA. Brain effects of cannabis – neuroimaging findings. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2005;27(1):70–78. doi: 10.1590/s1516-44462005000100016. Portuguese. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Fusar-Poli P, Crippa JA, Bhattacharyya S, et al. Distinct effects of {delta}9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabidiol on neural activation during emotional processing. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(1):95–105. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2008.519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bhattacharyya S, Morrison PD, Fusar-Poli P, et al. Opposite effects of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabidiol on human brain function and psychopathology. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(3):764–774. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Morgan CJ, Schafer G, Freeman TP, Curran HV. Impact of cannabidiol on the acute memory and psychotomimetic effects of smoked cannabis: naturalistic study: naturalistic study [corrected] Br J Psychiatry. 2010;197(4):285–290. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.077503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.ElSohly MA. Quarterly Report Potency Monitoring Project #85. University, MS: National Center for Natural Products Research, University of Mississippi; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Pijlman FT, Rigter SM, Hoek J, Goldschmidt HM, Niesink RJ. Strong increase in total delta-THC in cannabis preparations sold in Dutch coffee shops. Addict Biol. 2005;10(2):171–180. doi: 10.1080/13556210500123217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Potter DJ, Clark P, Brown MB. Potency of delta 9-THC and other cannabinoids in cannabis in England in 2005: implications for psychoactivity and pharmacology. J Forensic Sci. 2008;53(1):90–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1556-4029.2007.00603.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ashton CH. Pharmacology and effects of cannabis: a brief review. Br J Psychiatry. 2001;178:101–106. doi: 10.1192/bjp.178.2.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Hardwick S, King L. Home Office Cannabis Potency Study 2008. Sandridge, UK: Home Office Scientific Development Branch; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Chen CY, O’Brien MS, Anthony JC. Who becomes cannabis dependent soon after onset of use? Epidemiological evidence from the United States: 2000–2001. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;79(1):11–22. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Smart RG, Ogborne AC. Drug use and drinking among students in 36 countries. Addict Behav. 2000;25(3):455–460. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(99)00013-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Arseneault L, Cannon M, Poulton R, Murray R, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Cannabis use in adolescence and risk for adult psychosis: longitudinal prospective study. BMJ. 2002;325(7374):1212–1213. doi: 10.1136/bmj.325.7374.1212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Meier MH, Caspi A, Ambler A, et al. Persistent cannabis users show neuropsychological decline from childhood to midlife. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(40):E2657–E2664. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206820109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bolla KI, Brown K, Eldreth D, Tate K, Cadet JL. Dose-related neurocognitive effects of marijuana use. Neurology. 2002;59(9):1337–1343. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000031422.66442.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Solowij N, Jones KA, Rozman ME, et al. Verbal learning and memory in adolescent cannabis users, alcohol users and non-users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011;216(1):131–144. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2203-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gruber SA, Sagar KA, Dahlgren MK, Racine M, Lukas SE. Age of onset of marijuana use and executive function. Psychol Addict Behav. 2011;26(3):496–506. doi: 10.1037/a0026269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Solowij N, Stephens RS, Roffman RA, et al. for Marijuana Treatment Project Research Group Cognitive functioning of long-term heavy cannabis users seeking treatment. JAMA. 2002;287(9):1123–1131. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.9.1123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kandel ER. In Search of Memory: The Emergence of a New Science of Mind. New York, NY: WW Norton & Company; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E. N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25(1):46–59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kim H. Neural activity that predicts subsequent memory and forgetting: a meta-analysis of 74 fMRI studies. Neuroimage. 2011;54(3):2446–2461. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Tulving E. Organization of Memory. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1972. Episodic and Semantic Memory; pp. 381–402. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Baddeley A. Working memory: the interface between memory and cognition. J Cogn Neurosci. 1992;4(3):281–288. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1992.4.3.281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bhattacharyya S, Atakan Z, Martin-Santos R, Crippa JA, McGuire PK. Neural mechanisms for the cannabinoid modulation of cognition and affect in man: a critical review of neuroimaging studies. Curr Pharm Des. 2012;18(32):5045–5054. doi: 10.2174/138161212802884636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lane SD, Cherek DR, Lieving LM, Tcheremissine OV. Marijuana effects on human forgetting functions. J Exp Anal Behav. 2005;83(1):67–83. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2005.22-04. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Weinstein A, Brickner O, Lerman H, et al. A study investigating the acute dose-response effects of 13 mg and 17 mg Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol on cognitive-motor skills, subjective and autonomic measures in regular users of marijuana. J Psychopharmacol. 2008;22(4):441–451. doi: 10.1177/0269881108088194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Henquet C, Rosa A, Krabbendam L, et al. An experimental study of catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met moderation of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol-induced effects on psychosis and cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(12):2748–2757. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ilan AB, Gevins A, Coleman M, ElSohly MA, de Wit H. Neurophysiological and subjective profile of marijuana with varying concentrations of cannabinoids. Behav Pharmacol. 2005;16(5–6):487–496. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200509000-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Morrison PD, Nottage J, Stone JM, et al. Disruption of frontal θ coherence by Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol is associated with positive psychotic symptoms. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(4):827–836. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ramaekers JG, Theunissen EL, de Brouwer M, Toennes SW, Moeller MR, Kauert G. Tolerance and cross-tolerance to neurocognitive effects of THC and alcohol in heavy cannabis users. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011;214(2):391–401. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2042-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Silva de Melo LC, Cruz AP, Rios Valentim SJ, Jr, Marinho AR, Mendonça JB, Nakamura-Palacios EM. Delta(9)-THC administered into the medial prefrontal cortex disrupts the spatial working memory. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;183(1):54–64. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0141-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lichtman AH, Varvel SA, Martin BR. Endocannabinoids in cognition and dependence. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids. 2002;66(2–3):269–285. doi: 10.1054/plef.2001.0351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Varvel SR, Hamm B, Martin BR, Lichtman AH. Differential effects of a delta 9-THC on spatial reference and working memory in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;157(2):142–150. doi: 10.1007/s002130100780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Hart CL, van Gorp W, Haney M, Foltin RW, Fischman MW. Effects of acute smoked marijuana on complex cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;25(5):757–765. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00273-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Theunissen EL, Kauert GF, Toennes SW, et al. Neurophysiological functioning of occasional and heavy cannabis users during THC intoxication. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012;220(2):341–350. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2479-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Deadwyler SA, Heyser CJ, Hampson RE. Complete adaptation to the memory disruptive effects of delta-9-THC following 35 days of exposure. Neurosci Res Commun. 1995;17:9–18. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Delatte MS, Winsauer PJ, Moerschbaecher JM. Tolerance to the disruptive effects of Delta(9)-THC on learning in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;74(1):129–140. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00966-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Hampson RE, Simeral JD, Kelly EJ, Deadwyler SA. Tolerance to the memory disruptive effects of cannabinoids involves adaptation by hippocampal neurons. Hippocampus. 2003;13(5):543–556. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Ramaekers JG, Moeller MR, van Ruitenbeek P, Theunissen EL, Schneider E, Kauert G. Cognition and motor control as a function of Delta9-THC concentration in serum and oral fluid: limits of impairment. Drug and Alcohol Depend. 2006;85(2):114–122. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Ramaekers JG, Kauert G, Theunissen EL, Toennes SW, Moeller MR. Neurocognitive performance during acute THC intoxication in heavy and occasional cannabis users. J Psychopharmacol. 2009;23(3):266–277. doi: 10.1177/0269881108092393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Hart CL, Ilan AB, Gevins A, et al. Neurophysiological and cognitive effects of smoked marijuana in frequent users. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2010;96(3):333–341. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2010.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Böcker KB, Hunault CC, Gerritsen J, Kruidenier M, Mensinga TT, Kenemans JL. Cannabinoid modulations of resting state EEG θ power and working memory are correlated in humans. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010;22(9):1906–1916. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Vadhan NP, Serper MR, Haney M. Effects of Δ-THC on working memory: implications for schizophrenia? Prim psychiatry. 2009;16(4):51–99. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Nordstrom BR, Hart CL. Assessing cognitive functioning in cannabis users: cannabis use history an important consideration. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(12):2798–2799. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Fadda P, Robinson L, Fratta W, Pertwee RG, Riedel G. Differential effects of THC- or CBD-rich cannabis extracts on working memory in rats. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47(8):1170–1179. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Englund A, Morrison PD, Nottage J, et al. Cannabidiol inhibits THC-elicited paranoid symptoms and hippocampal-dependent memory impairment. J Psychopharmacol. 2012 Oct 5; doi: 10.1177/0269881112460109. Epub. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Hayakawa K, Mishima K, Hazekawa M, et al. Cannabidiol potentiates pharmacological effects of Delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol via CB(1) receptor-dependent mechanism. Brain Res. 2008;1188:157–164. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.09.090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Farhang B, Diaz S, Tang SL, Wagner EJ. Sex differences in the cannabinoid regulation of energy homeostasis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(Suppl 1):S237–S246. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Tang SL, Tran V, Wagner EJ. Sex differences in the cannabinoid modulation of an A-type K+ current in neurons of the mammalian hypothalamus. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94(4):2983–2986. doi: 10.1152/jn.01187.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Craft RM. Sex differences in behavioral effects of cannabinoids. Life Sci. 2005;77(20):2471–2478. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2005.04.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Tseng AH, Craft RM. Sex differences in antinociceptive and motoric effects of cannabinoids. Eur J Pharmacol. 2001;430(1):41–47. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(01)01267-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Fattore L, Fratta W. How important are sex differences in cannabinoid action? Br J Pharmacol. 2010;160(3):544–548. doi: 10.1111/j.1476-5381.2010.00776.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Haney M. Opioid antagonism of cannabinoid effects: differences between marijuana smokers and nonmarijuana smokers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32(6):1391–1403. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Makela P, Wakeley J, Gijsman H, Robson PJ, Bhagwagar Z, Rogers RD. Low doses of delta-9 tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) have divergent effects on short-term spatial memory in young, healthy adults. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(2):462–470. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Pope HG, Jr, Jacobs A, Mialet JP, Yurgelun-Todd D, Gruber S. Evidence for a sex-specific residual effect of cannabis on visuospatial memory. Psychother Psychosom. 1997;66(4):179–184. doi: 10.1159/000289132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Cha YM, Jones KH, Kuhn CM, Wilson WA, Swartzwelder HS. Sex differences in the effects of delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol on spatial learning in adolescent and adult rats. Behav Pharmacol. 2007;18(5–6):563–569. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3282ee7b7e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Grant I, Gonzalez R, Carey CL, Natarajan L, Wolfson T. Non-acute (residual) neurocognitive effects of cannabis use: a meta-analytic study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003;9(5):679–689. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703950016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Pope HG, Jr, Gruber AJ, Hudson JI, Huestis MA, Yurgelun-Todd D. Neuropsychological performance in long-term cannabis users. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58(10):909–915. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.10.909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Fried PA, Watkinson B, Gray R. Neurocognitive consequences of marihuana – a comparison with pre-drug performance. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2005;27(2):231–239. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2004.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Hanson KL, Winward JL, Schweinsburg AD, Medina KL, Brown SA, Tapert SF. Longitudinal study of cognition among adolescent marijuana users over three weeks of abstinence. Addict Behav. 2010;35(11):970–976. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.06.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Tait RJ, Mackinnon A, Christensen H. Cannabis use and cognitive function: 8-year trajectory in a young adult cohort. Addiction. 2011;106(12):2195–2203. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03574.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Jager G, Kahn RS, Van Den Brink W, Van Ree JM, Ramsey NF. Long-term effects of frequent cannabis use on working memory and attention: an fMRI study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;185(3):358–368. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0298-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Jager G, Van Hell HH, De Win MM, et al. Effects of frequent cannabis use on hippocampal activity during an associative memory task. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2007;17(4):289–297. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2006.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Schweinsburg AD, Schweinsburg BC, Cheung EH, Brown GG, Brown SA, Tapert SF. fMRI response to spatial working memory in adolescents with comorbid marijuana and alcohol use disorders. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;79(2):201–210. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2005.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Schweinsburg AD, Schweinsburg BC, Medina KL, McQueeny T, Brown SA, Tapert SF. The influence of recency of use on fMRI response during spatial working memory in adolescent marijuana users. J Psychoactive Drugs. 2010;42(3):401–412. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2010.10400703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Solowij N, Battisti R. The chronic effects of cannabis on memory in humans: a review. Curr Drug Abuse Rev. 2008;1(1):81–98. doi: 10.2174/1874473710801010081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Fontes MA, Bolla KI, Cunha PJ, et al. Cannabis use before age 15 and subsequent executive functioning. Br J Psychiatry. 2011;198(6):442–447. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.077479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Messinis L, Kyprianidou A, Malefaki S, Papathanasopoulos P. Neuropsychological deficits in long-term frequent cannabis users. Neurology. 2006;66(5):737–739. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000201279.83203.c6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Battisti RA, Roodenrys S, Johnstone SJ, Respondek C, Hermens DF, Solowij N. Chronic use of cannabis and poor neural efficiency in verbal memory ability. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010;209(4):319–330. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1800-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Pope HG, Jr, Gruber AJ, Hudson JI, Cohane G, Huestis MA, Yurgelun-Todd D. Early-onset cannabis use and cognitive deficits: what is the nature of the association? Drug Alcohol Depend. 2003;69(3):303–310. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(02)00334-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Becker B, Wagner D, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Spuentrup E, Daumann J. Altered parahippocampal functioning in cannabis users is related to the frequency of use. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010;209(4):361–374. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1805-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Lopez-Larson MP, Bogorodzki P, Rogowska J, et al. Altered prefrontal and insular cortical thickness in adolescent marijuana users. Behav Brain Res. 2011;220(1):164–172. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Yücel M, Zalesky A, Takagi MJ, et al. White-matter abnormalities in adolescents with long-term inhalant and cannabis use: a diffusion magnetic resonance imaging study. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2010;35(6):409–412. doi: 10.1503/jpn.090177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Wilson W, Mathew R, Turkington T, Hawk T, Coleman RE, Provenzale J. Brain morphological changes and early marijuana use: a magnetic resonance and positron emission tomography study. J Addict Dis. 2000;19(1):1–22. doi: 10.1300/J069v19n01_01. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Moore NL, Greenleaf AL, Acheson SK, Wilson WA, Swartzwelder HS, Kuhn CM. Role of cannabinoid receptor type 1 desensitization in greater tetrahydrocannabinol impairment of memory in adolescent rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2010;335(2):294–301. doi: 10.1124/jpet.110.169359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Quinn HR, Matsumoto I, Callaghan PD, et al. Adolescent rats find repeated Delta(9)-THC less aversive than adult rats but display greater residual cognitive deficits and changes in hippocampal protein expression following exposure. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(5):1113–1126. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Medina KL, Hanson KL, Schweinsburg AD, Cohen-Zion M, Nagel BJ, Tapert SF. Neuropsychological functioning in adolescent marijuana users: subtle deficits detectable after a month of abstinence. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13(5):807–820. doi: 10.1017/S1355617707071032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Schneider M, Schömig E, Leweke FM. Acute and chronic cannabinoid treatment differentially affects recognition memory and social behavior in pubertal and adult rats. Addict Biol. 2008;13(3–4):345–357. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00117.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Schneider M, Koch M. Chronic pubertal, but not adult chronic cannabinoid treatment impairs sensorimotor gating, recognition memory, and the performance in a progressive ratio task in adult rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28(10):1760–1769. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Padula CB, Schweinsburg AD, Tapert SF. Spatial working memory performance and fMRI activation interaction in abstinent adolescent marijuana users. Psychol Addict Behav. 2007;21(4):478–487. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.4.478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Kanayama G, Rogowska J, Pope HG, Gruber SA, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Spatial working memory in heavy cannabis users: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;176(3–4):239–247. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1885-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Ashtari M, Avants B, Cyckowski L, et al. Medial temporal structures and memory functions in adolescents with heavy cannabis use. J Psychiatr Res. 2011;45(8):1055–1066. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Hermann D, Sartorius A, Welzel H, et al. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex N-acetylaspartate/total creatine (NAA/tCr) loss in male recreational cannabis users. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61(11):1281–1289. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Demirakca T, Sartorius A, Ende G, et al. Diminished gray matter in the hippocampus of cannabis users: possible protective effects of cannabidiol. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;114(2–3):242–245. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Morgan CJ, Curran HV. Effects of cannabidiol on schizophrenia-like symptoms in people who use cannabis. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;192(4):306–307. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.046649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Salomone A, Gerace E, D’Urso F, Di Corcia D, Vincenti M. Simultaneous analysis of several synthetic cannabinoids, THC, CBD and CBN, in hair by ultra-high performance liquid chromatography tandem mass spectrometry. Method validation and application to real samples. J Mass Spectrom. 2012;47(5):604–610. doi: 10.1002/jms.2988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Jager G, Block RI, Luijten M, Ramsey NF. Cannabis use and memory brain function in adolescent boys: a cross-sectional multicenter functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(6):561–572. 572.e1–e3. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Jacobsen LK, Pugh KR, Constable RT, Westerveld M, Mencl WE. Functional correlates of verbal memory deficits emerging during nicotine withdrawal in abstinent adolescent cannabis users. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61(1):31–40. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Schweinsburg AD, Nagel BJ, Schweinsburg BC, Park A, Theilmann RJ, Tapert SF. Abstinent adolescent marijuana users show altered fMRI response during spatial working memory. Psychiatry Res. 2008;163(1):40–51. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.04.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Abush H, Akirav I. Short- and long-term cognitive effects of chronic cannabinoids administration in late-adolescence rats. PLoS One. 2012;7(2):e31731. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Spreen O, Strauss E. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. 2nd ed. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Rey A. L’examen Clinique en Psychologie. [Clinical tests in psychology] Paris: Presses Universitaires de France; 1964. [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Powell JB, Cripe LI, Dodrill CB. Assessment of brain impairment with the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test: a comparison with other neuropsychological measures. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1991;6(4):241–249. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Block RI, O’Leary DS, Hichwa RD, et al. Effects of frequent marijuana use on memory-related regional cerebral blood flow. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;72(1–2):237–250. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(01)00771-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Buschke H. Selective reminding for analysis of memory and learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1973;12(5):543–550. [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Grant JE, Chamberlain SR, Schreiber L, Odlaug BL. Neuropsychological deficits associated with cannabis use in young adults. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012;121(1–2):159–162. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Smith AM, Longo CA, Fried PA, Hogan MJ, Cameron I. Effects of marijuana on visuospatial working memory: an fMRI study in young adults. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2010;210(3):429–438. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1841-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Harvey MA, Sellman JD, Porter RJ, Frampton CM. The relationship between non-acute adolescent cannabis use and cognition. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2007;26(3):309–319. doi: 10.1080/09595230701247772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Martín-Santos R, Fagundo AB, Crippa JA, et al. Neuroimaging in cannabis use: a systematic review of the literature. Psychol Med. 2010;40(3):383–398. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709990729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Davies SN, Pertwee RG, Riedel G. Functions of cannabinoid receptors in the hippocampus. Neuropharmacology. 2002;42(8):993–1007. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(02)00060-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Iversen L. Cannabis and the brain. Brain. 2003;126(Pt 6):1252–1270. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Herkenham M, Lynn AB, Little MD, et al. Cannabinoid receptor localization in brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1990;87(5):1932–1936. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.5.1932. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Pattij T, Wiskerke J, Schoffelmeer AN. Cannabinoid modulation of executive functions. Eur J Pharmacol. 2008;585(2–3):458–463. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2008.02.099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Pertwee RG. The pharmacology of cannabinoid receptors and their ligands: an overview. Int J Obes (Lond) 2006;30(Suppl 1):S13–S18. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Heifets BD, Castillo PE. Endocannabinoid signaling and long-term synaptic plasticity. Annu Rev Physiol. 2009;71:283–306. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.010908.163149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Howlett AC, Breivogel CS, Childers SR, Deadwyler SA, Hampson RE, Porrino LJ. Cannabinoid physiology and pharmacology: 30 years of progress. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47(Suppl 1):345–358. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.07.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Hoffman AF, Oz M, Yang R, Lichtman AH, Lupica CR. Opposing actions of chronic Delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabinoid antagonists on hippocampal long-term potentiation. Learn Mem. 2007;14(1–2):63–74. doi: 10.1101/lm.439007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Riedel G, Davies SN. Cannabinoid function in learning, memory and plasticity. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2005;(168):445–477. doi: 10.1007/3-540-26573-2_15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Hampson RE, Sweatt AJ, Goonawardena AV, et al. Memory encoding in hippocampal ensembles is negatively influenced by cannabinoid CB1 receptors. Behav Pharmacol. 2011;22(4):335–346. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3283473bfd. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Terranova JP, Storme JJ, Lafon N, et al. Improvement of memory in rodents by the selective CB1 cannabinoid receptor antagonist, SR 141716. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1996;126(2):165–172. doi: 10.1007/BF02246352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Zuardi AW, Crippa JA, Hallak JE, Moreira FA, Guimarães FS. Cannabidiol, a Cannabis sativa constituent, as an antipsychotic drug. Braz J Med Biol Res. 2006;39(4):421–429. doi: 10.1590/s0100-879x2006000400001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Robinson L, Goonawardena AV, Pertwee RG, Hampson RE, Riedel G. The synthetic cannabinoid HU210 induces spatial memory deficits and suppresses hippocampal firing rate in rats. Br J Pharmacol. 2007;151(5):688–700. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjp.0707273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Lawston J, Borella A, Robinson JK, Whitaker-Azmitia PM. Changes in hippocampal morphology following chronic treatment with the synthetic cannabinoid WIN 55,212-2. Brain Res. 2000;877(2):407–410. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)02739-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Han J, Kesner P, Metna-Laurent M, et al. Acute cannabinoids impair working memory through astroglial CB1 receptor modulation of hippocampal LTD. Cell. 2012;148(5):1039–1050. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.01.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Wise LE, Thorpe AJ, Lichtman AH. Hippocampal CB1 receptors mediate the memory impairing effects of Delta(9)-tetrahydrocannabinol. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34(9):2072–2080. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Matochik JA, Eldreth DA, Cadet JL, Bolla KI. Altered brain tissue composition in heavy marijuana users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2005;77(1):23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Yücel M, Solowij N, Respondek C, et al. Regional brain abnormalities associated with long-term heavy cannabis use. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(6):694–701. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.6.694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Quickfall J, Crockford D. Brain neuroimaging in cannabis use: a review. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2006;18(3):318–332. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2006.18.3.318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.O’Leary DS, Block RI, Koeppel JA, et al. Effects of smoking marijuana on focal attention and brain blood flow. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2007;22(3):135–148. doi: 10.1002/hup.832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Lundqvist T. Cognitive consequences of cannabis use: comparison with abuse of stimulants and heroin with regard to attention, memory and executive functions. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2005;81(2):319–330. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.02.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Bossong MG, van Berckel BN, Boellaard R, et al. Delta 9- tetrahydrocannabinol induces dopamine release in the human striatum. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;34(3):759–766. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Sneider JT, Pope HG, Jr, Silveri MM, Simpson NS, Gruber SA, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Differences in regional blood volume during a 28-day period of abstinence in chronic cannabis smokers. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;18(8):612–619. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.04.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Nestor L, Roberts G, Garavan H, Hester R. Deficits in learning and memory: parahippocampal hyperactivity and frontocortical hypoactivity in cannabis users. Neuroimage. 2008;40(3):1328–1339. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Eldreth DA, Matochik JA, Cadet JL, Bolla KI. Abnormal brain activity in prefrontal brain regions in abstinent marijuana users. Neuroimage. 2004;23(3):914–920. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Jansma JM, Ramsey NF, van der Wee NJ, Kahn RS. Working memory capacity in schizophrenia: a parametric fMRI study. Schizophr Res. 2004;68(2–3):159–171. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00127-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Jacobsen LK, Mencl WE, Westerveld M, Pugh KR. Impact of cannabis use on brain function in adolescents. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1021:384–390. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Ilan AB, Smith ME, Gevins A. Effects of marijuana on neurophysiological signals of working and episodic memory. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;176(2):214–222. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1868-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Bossong MG, Jager G, van Hell HH, et al. Effects of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol administration on human encoding and recall memory function: a pharmacological FMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2012;24(3):588–599. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Bossong MG, Jansma JM, van Hell HH, et al. Effects of δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol on human working memory function. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71(8):693–699. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Hampson RE, Deadwyler SA. Cannabinoids reveal the necessity of hippocampal neural encoding for short-term memory in rats. J Neurosci. 2000;20(23):8932–8942. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-23-08932.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Grant I, Atkinson JH, Gouaux B, Wilsey B. Medical marijuana: clearing away the smoke. Open Neurol J. 2012;6:18–25. doi: 10.2174/1874205X01206010018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Substance Abuse and Rehabilitation are provided here courtesy of Dove Press

RESOURCES