Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Asia Pac Psychiatry. 2013 Aug 22;7(1):36–44. doi: 10.1111/appy.12093

Comparing schizophrenia symptoms in the Iban of Sarawak with other populations to elucidate clinical heterogeneity

Duncan McLean 1, Robert Barrett 2, Peter Loa 2,*, Rangaswamy Thara 3, Sujit John 3, John McGrath 1,4, Jake Gratten 1,4, Bryan Mowry 1,4
PMCID: PMC3932150  NIHMSID: NIHMS505712  PMID: 24038814

Abstract

Introduction

The symptom profile of schizophrenia can vary between ethnic groups. We explored selected symptom variables previously reported to be characteristic of schizophrenia in the Iban of Sarawak in transethnic populations from Australia, India and Sarawak, Malaysia. We tested site differences to confirm previous research, and to explore implications of differences across populations for future investigations.

Methods

We recruited schizophrenia samples in Australia (n=609), India (n=310) and Sarawak (n=205) primarily for the purposes of genetic studies. We analyzed seven identified variables and their relationship to site using logistic regression, including: global delusions, bizarre delusions, thought broadcast/insertion/withdrawal delusions, global hallucinations, auditory hallucinations, disorganized behavior, and prodromal duration.

Results

We identified a distinct symptom profile in our Sarawak sample. Specifically, the Iban exhibit: low frequency of thought broadcast/insertion/withdrawal delusions, high frequency of auditory hallucinations and disorganized behavior, with a comparatively short prodrome when compared with Australian and Indian populations.

Discussion

Understanding between-site variation in symptom profile may complement future transethnic genetic studies, and provide important clues as to the nature of differing schizophrenia expression across ethnically distinct groups. A comprehensive approach to subtyping schizophrenia is warranted, utilizing comprehensively ascertained transethnic samples to inform both schizophrenia genetics and nosology.

Keywords: psychotic disorders, schizophrenia, culture, diagnosis, population characteristics

Introduction

In the past, the syndrome of schizophrenia has been treated as a single entity diagnosed according to reliable, internationally accepted criteria consisting of symptoms, disability, duration, illness course, and exclusion of allied disorders (APA, 1994; WHO, 1992). However, there is now clear evidence that schizophrenia shares a genetic predisposition with bipolar disorder (ISC, 2009; Lichtenstein et al., 2009), which challenges the dichotomous view of functional psychosis (Craddock, O'Donovan & Owen, 2009). There is currently little consensus on whether the latent structure of schizophrenia is best represented as a single (continuous) entity, with clinical variation represented as dimensions within a single class, or as two or more distinct, separate entities, with variation indicative of a number of distinct classes grouped under the label ‘schizophrenia’ (Fiedorowicz, Epping & Flaum, 2008; Kendler, Karkowski & Walsh, 1998; Linscott, Lenzenweger & van Os, 2009).

While the field has been mindful of within-group heterogeneity of schizophrenia, the between-group, transethnic differences in the phenotypes have received less scrutiny. Examination of transethnic schizophrenia samples provides an opportunity to elucidate differences in schizophrenia expression, which are important to a comprehensive understanding of this disorder (Kleinman, 1988).

Previous anthropological (Barrett, 2004) and psychiatric (Barrett et al., 2005) research on the Iban of Sarawak has proposed the following symptoms as characteristic of schizophrenia in that population: low rates of bizarre delusions, specifically thought broadcast, insertion and withdrawal delusions; and high rates of auditory hallucinations, disorganized behavior, restlessness and insomnia. Additionally, a comparatively short prodrome has been consistently noted in case review, particularly in comparison with other studied populations.

The Genetics Research group at the Queensland Centre for Mental Health Research (QCMHR) and our collaborators recruited three cohorts of individuals with psychosis for genetic analyses: European Australians (n=821); Tamil Brahmin and proximal caste groups from Tamil Nadu, India (n=520); and the Iban of Sarawak, Malaysia (n=298). In this paper, we examined nine variables previously associated with schizophrenia in the Iban, and compared the frequencies of these variables in the Iban sample with our Australian and Indian cohorts.

Our available Iban sample is an extension of the sample described in a previous publication (Barrett et al., 2005). Their sample included individuals with schizophrenia identified through an initial medical records screen (n=275). We recontacted and comprehensively assessed 122 of these individuals plus an additional 23 individuals who were primarily new cases diagnosed after the initial screen. Thus we increased both sample size (of included individuals with schizophrenia) and quality of clinical information available. We aimed to confirm the previous research proposing these characteristic symptoms, and explore the implications of differences across populations.

We hypothesized that (a) hallucinations (specifically auditory), disorganized behavior, sleep disturbance, and psychomotor changes would occur more frequently in the Iban than in samples derived from Australian or Indian populations; (b) bizarre delusions (specifically broadcast, insertion and withdrawal delusions) would occur less frequently in the Iban than in Australia or India; and (c) the prodrome would be shorter in the Iban than in Australia or India.

Methods

Sample details

Sample recruitment across sites and clinical ascertainment are detailed elsewhere (see McLean et al., 2012). Briefly, recruitment at each site involved: Australia – sibling pairs and unrelated individuals were recruited as part of a major US/Australian collaboration (Molecular Genetics of Schizophrenia [MGS] Consortium) from multiple sources, including local treatment facilities, physician referrals, community organizations, supported accommodation facilities and advertisements; India – sibling pairs, trios and unrelated individuals were identified and invited to participate through The Schizophrenia Research Foundation India (SCARF); and Sarawak – Iban individuals were identified through Malaysian census data, an initial medical records assessment was undertaken, then a subset of individuals and families were contacted for in-depth follow-up. We included all individuals with a Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV) (APA, 1994) diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder who met the (self-reported) ethnicity inclusion criterion. Ethnicity was subsequently confirmed through genetic analysis (Australia: Shi et al., 2009; India and Sarawak: manuscripts in preparation). Exclusion criteria for individuals were: (i) inability to give informed consent; (ii) psychosis assessed as secondary to substance use or a neurological disorder; and (iii) severe intellectual disability. Informed consent was obtained across all sites, and local Institutional Ethics Committee approval was obtained for each study. This research conforms to the provisions of the Declaration of Helsinki (as revised in Edinburgh 2000).

Clinical ascertainment

Clinical ascertainment included five elements.

  1. Trained clinicians used the semi-structured Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies (DIGS) (Nurnberger et al., 1994) to obtain DSM-IV relevant diagnostic information.

  2. A family informant, when possible, or the proband was interviewed about the family psychiatric history using the Family Interview for Genetic Studies (FIGS) (Gershon et al., 1988; Maxwell, 1992). The cultural equivalence of the DIGS and FIGS was extensively addressed by the chief investigators at the three sites, and each instrument was translated into Iban (Sarawak) and Tamil (India) with appropriate back-translation procedures.

  3. All available medical records were retrieved for each participant and assessed by trained clinicians.

  4. A trained clinician prepared a case summary based on all available information, which facilitated diagnostic review.

  5. DIGS interview, case summary, available medical records, and FIGS reports formed the basis for diagnostic review. Diagnoses were assigned using the Best Estimate Final Diagnosis (BEFD) procedure (Leckman et al., 1982), with two experienced psychiatrists independently reviewing all available information then conferring to assign a consensus diagnosis. Approximately half the Sarawak sample had diagnoses formulated by one experienced psychiatrist, with a BEFD generated in a random subset (20 cases).

Inter-rater reliability was assessed within the Australian/US sample (Shi et al., 2009; Suarez et al., 2006); within the Indian sample (Thara et al., 2009); within the Sarawak sample; between the Australian and Indian samples (disagreement in one of 20 cases; κ=0.886); and between the Australian and Sarawak samples (disagreement in one of 20 cases; κ=0.828). One psychiatrist (BM) was a Principal Investigator on all studies, and has reviewed all included cases.

Data analysis

We analyzed nine clinical variables identified in previous research: (i) lifetime delusions; (ii) bizarre delusions; (iii) delusions of thought broadcast, insertion or withdrawal; (iv) lifetime hallucinations; (v) auditory hallucinations; (vi) lifetime disorganized or catatonic motor behavior; (vii) sleep disturbance; (viii) psychomotor changes; and (ix) length of prodromal period. We also included the following potentially confounding variables: (x) sex; (xi) age; and (xii) presence of lifetime major depressive episodes.

Prodromal duration was obtained from all available information (DIGS interview, case summary, medical records, FIGS reports), and was assessed as the period between when social and/or occupational decline was first observed and when definite psychotic symptoms were first recorded. It was coded as dichotomous: rapid=onset within 4 weeks; gradual=onset longer than 4 weeks. Two outcome variables (sleep disturbance and psychomotor changes) were dropped from the model due to a lack of equivalence across sites.

Sources of data were audited (both electronic and hard copy), and all potential cases were identified for whom comprehensive diagnostic information was available. Data were extracted from diagnostic interview databases, where possible, then responses were checked, corrected, and missing values retrieved from all available sources, including a detailed review of all narrative summaries.

Due to the potential confounding effect of including both related and unrelated individuals in our sample, we selected a conservative model. Individuals within families may be more similar with regard to demographic and symptom characteristics than unrelated individuals owing to shared socioeconomic and environmental circumstances. Given that between-site similarities and differences across these variables were a focus of this study, and the degree of inter-relatedness differed across sites, we included only unrelated individuals, with a single individual randomly selected from each of the 1124 families across the three sites.

Outcome variables were then assessed individually by logistic regression, fitting site, presence of lifetime depressive episode(s), age, and sex as explanatory variables, plus all second level effects (e.g. site-by-sex) for these variables. We performed model simplification based on AIC, with the least significant effect being removed from the model at each iteration, until all remaining effects were significant. The minimal adequate model is presented for each outcome variable. Statistical analyses used Proc Logistic in SAS software, version 9.3 for Windows (SAS Institute Inc.). We used Bonferroni correction to account for multiple testing.

The present study was drawn from 1831 individuals from Australia (n=821), India (n=524) and Sarawak (n=486). We excluded 192 individuals because their DSM-IV diagnosis was a psychotic disorder other than schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder (Australia, n=0; India, n=4; Sarawak, n=188). Of the resulting 1639 individuals we then excluded all individuals with missing data for any of the eleven variables (seven outcome, four explanatory) of interest (Australia, n=136; India, n=24; Sarawak, n=62), leaving 1417 individuals from 1124 independent families. One individual from each family was randomly selected to constitute the final sample of 1124 individuals (Australia, n=609; India, n=310; Sarawak, n=205). DIGS and FIGS data were available for over 90% of participants in Australia and India, and approximately 50% of participants in Sarawak. Case summaries were available for over 90% of participants in Australia and Sarawak, and for over 75% of participants in India. Best-estimate final diagnoses were available for all participants across the three sites (except as previously noted).

Results

A summary of symptom variable frequencies by site, including all outcome and explanatory variables, is provided in table 1.

Table 1. Demographic and symptom characteristics of affected individuals by site .

Variable Australia India Sarawak Total
N 609 (54.2%) 310 (27.6%) 205 (18.2%) 1124 (100%)

Demographic variables

Age, Mean ± SD 38.95 ± 11.55 36.57 ± 11.37 47.19 ± 14.67 39.80 ± 12.66

Sex
  Female 173 (28.4%) 128 (41.3%) 85 (41.5%) 386 (34.3%)
  Male 436 (71.6%) 182 (58.7%) 120 (58.5%) 738 (65.7%)

Symptom variables

Diagnosis
  Schizoaffective, depressed 22 (3.6%) 1 (0.3%) 30 (14.6%) 53 (4.7%)
  Schizoaffective, bipolar 26 (4.3%) 0 (0%) 16 (7.8%) 42 (3.7%)
  Schizophrenia 561 (92.1%) 309 (99.7%) 159 (77.6%) 1029 (91.6%)

Onset type
  Rapid (4 weeks or less) 109 (17.9%) 70 (22.6%) 90 (43.9%) 269 (23.9%)
  Gradual (longer than 4 weeks) 500 (82.1%) 240 (77.4%) 115 (56.1%) 855 (76.1%)

Presence of any delusions (lifetime) 599 (98.3%) 283 (91.3%) 164 (80.0%) 1046 (93.1%)

Presence of bizarre delusions (lifetime) 410 (67.3%) 113 (36.5%) 40 (19.5%) 563 (50.1%)

Presence of thought broadcast/insertion/withdrawal delusions (lifetime) 322 (52.9%) 44 (14.2%) 22 (10.7%) 388 (34.5%)

Presence of any hallucinations (lifetime) 566 (92.9%) 253 (81.6%) 194 (94.6%) 1013 (90.1%)

Presence of auditory hallucinations (lifetime) 551 (90.5%) 244 (78.7%) 192 (93.7%) 987 (87.8%)

Presence of disorganized behavior (lifetime) 464 (76.2%) 244 (78.7%) 177 (86.3%) 885 (78.7%)

Presence of major depressive episode(s) (lifetime) 261 (42.9%) 9 (2.9%) 44 (21.5%) 314 (27.9%)

One affected family member randomly generated from each family included in the sample.

A summary of the logistic regression performed is provided in table 2, with associated odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals provided in table 3.

Table 2. Logistic regression showing predictors of the presence of identified characteristic Iban symptoms .

Outcome Variable Explanatory Variables DF Wald Χ2 P-value §
Global Delusions
Intercept 1 19.85 <0.0001***
Site 2 23.78 <0.0001***
Age 1 0.84 0.36
Major depressive episodes 1 7.59 0.006**
Age by Site 2 4.62 0.10

Bizarre Delusions
Intercept 1 7.77 0.005**
Site 2 38.67 <0.0001***
Age 1 16.56 <0.0001***
Sex 1 3.56 0.06
Age by Site 2 9.63 0.008**
Sex by Site 2 5.17 0.08

Broadcast/Insertion/Withdrawal Delusions
Intercept 1 19.09 <0.0001***
Site 2 65.97 <0.0001***
Age 1 10.57 0.001**
Major depressive episodes 1 2.57 0.11
Age by Site 2 6.38 0.041*

Global Hallucinations
Intercept 1 51.18 <0.0001***
Site 2 24. 24 <0.0001***
Age 1 4.82 0.028*
Sex 1 5.68 0.017*
Major depressive episodes 1 0.96 0.33
Sex by Major depressive episodes 1 4.31 0.038*

Auditory Hallucinations
Intercept 1 90.76 <0.0001***
Site 2 65.40 <0.0001***
Age 1 4.89 0.027*
Sex 1 0.16 0.69
Sex by Site 2 9.39 0.009**

Disorganized Behavior
Intercept 1 60.33 <0.0001***
Site 2 6.38 0.041*
Age 1 3.10 0.08
Major depressive episodes 1 3.46 0.06

Rapid Onset Type
Intercept 1 2.52 0.11
Site 2 43.03 <0.0001***
Age 1 10.56 0.001**
Sex 1 6.90 0.009**
Major depressive episodes 1 1.42 0.23
Age by Site 2 5.18 0.08
Sex by Site 2 5.96 0.05
Age by Sex 1 8.06 0.005**
Sex by Major depressive episodes 1 2.81 0.09
*

p<0.05

**

p<0.01

***

p<0.001

Iteration with best AIC (model fit) that passes overdispersion test shown for each outcome variable

Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for relevant effect combinations presented as Table 3

§

Effects surpassing Bonferroni correction shown in bold

Table 3. Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals for all variable combinations presented in Table 2.

Outcome Variable Explanatory Variable Combinations Estimate (95% CI)
Global Delusions
Site (Australia vs. India) at mean age 5.06 (1.94 − 13.18)
Site (Australia vs. Sarawak) at mean age 15.57 (6.17 − 39.32)
Site (India vs. Sarawak) at mean age 3.08 (1.71 − 5.57)
Age in Australia 0.21 (0.04 − 1.05)
Age in India 0.22 (0.09 − 0.52)
Age in Sarawak 0.70 (0.33 − 1.49)
No major depressive episodes vs. one or more ma jor depressive episodes 0.26 (0.10 − 0.68)

Bizarre Delusions
Site (Australia vs. India) at mean age, males 3.64 (2.40 − 5.54)
Site (Australia vs. Sarawak) at mean age, males 11.91 (6.32 − 22.43)
Site (India vs. Sarawak) at mean age, males 3.27 (1.64 − 6.51)
Site (Australia vs. India) at mean age, females 4.40 (2.53 − 7.65)
Site (Australia vs. Sarawak) at mean age, females 4.61 (2.31 − 9.18)
Site (India vs. Sarawak) at mean age, females 1.05 (0.50 − 2.18)
Age in Australia 0.73 (0.50 − 1.07)
Age in India 0.49 (0.28 − 0.88)
Age in Sarawak 0.17 (0.07 − 0.39)
Sex (Male vs. Female) in Australia 1.15 (0.76 − 1.74)
Sex (Male vs. Female) in India 1.39 (0.81 − 2.38)
Sex (Male vs. Female) in Sarawak 0.44 (0.19 − 1.03)

Broadcast/Insertion/Withdrawal Delusions
Site (Australia vs. India) at mean age 6.29 (4.08 − 9.70)
Site (Australia vs. Sarawak) at mean age 9.15 (5.09 − 16.44)
Site (India vs. Sarawak) at mean age 1.45 (0.74 − 2.85)
Age in Australia 0.65 (0.46 − 0.93)
Age in India 0.79 (0.37 − 1.71)
Age in Sarawak 0.15 (0.05 − 0.47)
No major depressive episodes vs. one or more ma jor depressive episodes 0.76 (0.54 − 1.06)

Global Hallucinations
Site (Australia vs. India) 3.58 (2.00 − 6.39)
Site (Australia vs. Sarawak) 0.73 (0.32 − 1.66)
Site (India vs. Sarawak) 0.20 (0.09 − 0.47)
Age 0.58 (0.35 − 0.94)
Sex (Male vs. Female), no major depressive episodes 0.46 (0.25 − 0.87)
Sex (Male vs. Female), one or more major depressive episodes 1.68 (0.59 − 4.74)
Depressive episodes (none vs. one or more), males 0.66 (0.29 − 1.52)
Depressive episodes (none vs. one or more), females 2.39 (0.89 − 6.40)

Auditory Hallucinations
Site (Australia vs. India), males 3.73 (2.61 − 5.34)
Site (Australia vs. Sarawak), males 0.58 (0.31 − 1.10)
Site (India vs. Sarawak), males 0.16 (0.08 − 0.30)
Site (Australia vs. India), females 1.47 (0.86 − 2.50)
Site (Australia vs. Sarawak), females 0.62 (0.30 − 1.30)
Site (India vs. Sarawak), females 0.43 (0.20 −0.90)
Age 0.72 (0.54 − 0.96)
Sex (Male vs. Female) in Australia 1.12 (0.72 − 1.76)
Sex (Male vs. Female) in India 0.44 (0.28 − 0.70)
Sex (Male vs. Female) in Sarawak 1.19 (0.50 − 2.83)

Disorganized Behavior
Site (Australia vs. India) 0.96 (0.67 − 1.38)
Site (Australia vs. Sarawak) 0.57 (0.36 − 0.89)
Site (India vs. Sarawak) 0.59 (0.36 − 0.97)
Age 1.31 (0.97 − 1.76)
Depressive episodes (none vs. one or more) 1.38 (0.98 − 1.94)

Rapid Onset Type
Site (Australia vs. India) at mean age, males 0.80 (0.50 − 1.29)
Site (Australia vs. Sarawak) at mean age, males 0.20 (0.13 − 0.32)
Site (India vs. Sarawak) at mean age, males 0.25 (0.15 − 0.43)
Site (Australia vs. India) at mean age, females 0.34 (0.17 − 0.68)
Site (Australia vs. Sarawak) at mean age, females 0.23 (0.12 − 0.43)
Site (India vs. Sarawak) at mean age, females 0.66 (0.35 − 1.23)
Age, males in Australia 0.66 (0.42 − 1.05)
Age, males in India 0.88 (0.46 − 1.67)
Age, males in Sarawak 0.36 (0.19 − 0.66)
Age, females in Australia 1.62 (0.88 − 2.99)
Age, females in India 2.15 (1.14 − 4.07)
Age, females in Sarawak 0.87 (0.43 − 1.73)
Sex (Male vs. Female) at mean age, no major depressive episodes in Australia 1.59 (0.83 − 3.04)
Sex (Male vs. Female) at mean age, no major depressive episodes in India 0.68 (0.39 − 1.17)
Sex (Male vs. Female) at mean age, no major depressive episodes in Sarawak 1.76 (0.95 − 3.25)
Sex (Male vs. Female) at mean age, one or more ma jor depressive episodes in Australia 0.84 (0.48 − 1.47)
Sex (Male vs. Female) at mean age, one or more ma jor depressive episodes in India 0. 36 (0.15 − 0.88)
Sex (Male vs. Female) at mean age, one or more ma jor depressive episodes in Sarawak 0.93 (0.42 − 2.07)
Depressive episodes (none vs. one or more), males 0.77 (0.50 − 1.18)
Depressive episodes (none vs. one or more), females 0.41 (0.22 − 0.75)

Each of the seven outcome variables had significant site effects, while all outcome variables except global delusions and disorganized behavior had significant age effects. Both lifetime hallucinations and length of prodrome had significant sex effects, whereas only lifetime delusions had a significant depressive episode(s) effect.

Multiple testing correction

The site effect for six of the seven outcome variables remained significant using a Bonferroni corrected p-value=0.0007, with only the disorganized behavior effect (p=0.041) failing to reach this threshold. Age remained significant for bizarre delusions, with younger individuals having greater odds of reporting bizarre delusions. No other effects remained significant for any of the outcome variables.

Discussion

Consistent with previous reports (Barrett, 2004; Barrett et al., 2005), we found that individuals with schizophrenia from the Iban sample differed on key symptom variables. In addition, we identified a range of differences for these variables between the Iban, Australia and India.

Delusions

While the proportion of individuals reporting bizarre delusions and delusions of thought broadcast, insertion and withdrawal was lowest in the Iban, it is noteworthy that the frequencies in India were also low in contrast to Australia, and that India and Sarawak were similar in their reported frequencies of broadcast/insertion/withdrawal delusions (India 14.2%; Sarawak 10.7%). The difficulty of assessing bizarreness of beliefs is widely recognized (Kendler et al., 1983), particularly across cultures (APA, 1994). Moreover, the greater frequency of Schneiderian first-rank symptoms reported in Western populations has been attributed to bizarre experiences in Western cultures being considered normative in non-Western cultures (Barrett, 2004). However, several alternative explanations for these cross-cultural differences have also been proposed (for a summary see Barrett, 2004).

Younger individuals had significantly greater odds of experiencing lifetime bizarre delusions, after correction for multiple testing. This somewhat counter-intuitive result may reflect recall bias, with an under-reporting of bizarre delusions in older individuals, since age has been associated with decreased occurrence of delusions and hallucinations (Schultz et al., 1997).

Hallucinations

Auditory hallucinations were, as expected, reported most frequently in the Iban, although rates of both lifetime hallucinations and auditory hallucinations did not differ between Sarawak (lifetime 94.6%; auditory 93.7%) and Australia (lifetime 92.9%; auditory 90.5%), with Indian rates (lifetime 81.6%; auditory 78.7%) significantly lower. A ‘striking resemblance’ in the appearance of auditory hallucinations, between Australian and Iban samples with schizophrenia, has been noted (Barrett, 2004). This finding offers an interesting contrast to our delusion finding, in that any explanation relying on Western/non-Western cultural norms explaining site differences is not sufficient to explain the significant difference in rates of auditory hallucinations between the non-Western cultures of India and Sarawak.

Disorganized behavior

The frequency of disorganized behavior was marginally higher in the Iban (86.3%) than in Australia (76.2%) or India (78.7%), although this site difference was non-significant after correction for multiple testing.

Onset type

The clear site difference regarding greater frequency of rapid onset in Sarawak (43.9%) in contrast to Australia (17.9%) and India (22.6%) is of particular interest. A later age of onset (∼6 years) finding in the Iban compared with Australia and India has been previously reported (McLean et al., 2012), yet the greater proportion of Iban individuals with rapid onset type is equally stark. Because the majority of Iban live with close family contact, and there is a societal propensity to seek early medical treatment (Barrett et al., 2005), early changes in behavior would likely be readily noted; thus, a longer reported prodrome may be expected, with social/occupational decline prior to psychosis onset identified more readily than in other populations. Clearly, the data do not support this. Alternatively, help-seeking behavior may prompt earlier diagnosis, truncating the period regarded as prodromal, with definite psychosis detected earlier in the Iban, in contrast to other populations, where later detection may ‘artificially’ lengthen the period regarded as prodromal. Cultural considerations may also be fundamental to the identified site difference: e.g. the Iban may be more tolerant of aberrant behavior than other societies, and may not rate atypical behavior as ‘prodromal’ until immediately prior to recognition of definite psychosis.

Cultural considerations

Categorization of research participants by ethnicity and/or culture is problematic and highly contested (Egede, 2006; Ma et al., 2007). The fact that our cohorts are both ethnically homogeneous and geographically constrained enables assessment of cultural confounders because these two factors represent the best available proxy for culture (Azuonye, 1994). Our sample is large for a transethnic comparative schizophrenia study (n=1124), individuals were assessed using the same battery of instruments, and individuals were diagnosed using the best-estimate final diagnosis method, which offers consistently high diagnostic reliability and stability (Beckmann, Franzek & Stober, 1996; Calkins et al., 2007), and is the benchmark method available with current methods of classifying schizophrenia (McLean et al., 2012). We assessed ethnicity by self-report, which is considered the research ‘gold standard’ in transcultural research (Ma et al., 2007). Furthermore, we ascertained birthplace for individuals’ parents and grandparents, confirmed ethnic homogeneity genetically, and interviewed individuals in their home countries using local interviewers. These measures avoid many confounding factors frequently experienced in cross-cultural research, which can limit generalizability of findings (McKenzie & Crowcroft, 1996a, 1996b).

Methodological limitations

First, a lack of measurement equivalence was unavoidable. The samples were collected for specific genetic studies, thus there were differences in selection methods of included individuals. Both the Indian and Sarawak samples were chosen from ethnically homogeneous populations, whereas the Australian sample was not specifically recruited as such (although we did focus on Caucasian ethnicity). Furthermore, the Australian and Indian cohorts included sib-pairs as well as unrelated individuals recruited opportunistically, while only the Sarawak sample can be considered relatively epidemiologically sound (Barrett et al., 2005). As previously noted, we negated any effect of familial relatedness in our model by randomly selecting one individual per family for analyses. The different recruitment methods also resulted in DIGS and FIGS data being unavailable for approximately half the Sarawak sample (see McLean et al., 2012).

Second, caution must always be exercised when using diagnostic instruments across cultures, since converting thoughts, feelings, and concepts such as bizarreness across languages can be difficult (Barrett, 2004). Consequently, we addressed cultural equivalence extensively in the preliminary planning for the studies (for a cultural equivalence framework see Herdman, Fox-Rushby & Badia, 1998). We also employed state-of-the-art methods, translating and back-translating the DIGS, and using local interviewers who interviewed in the native language across the three sites, and recorded responses in Iban (Sarawak) and English (India and Australia). The reliability of the instrument (inter-rater reliability within and across the samples) was also tested across sites, although diagnostic inter-rater reliability was not assessed between India and Sarawak.

Third, the generalizability of our findings is limited, particularly given that both the Iban in Malaysia and the Tamil Brahmin and other geographically proximal castes in India are homogeneous groups within diverse societies. Furthermore, we did not specifically collect socioeconomic data across our samples, which further limits the generalizability of any findings attributed to ethnicity/culture, as socioeconomic position has been proposed as a stronger determinant of health outcomes than ethnicity (Egede, 2006).

Fourth, utilizing a sample of individuals meeting the DSM-IV criteria for either schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder to study the nature of psychosis and/or schizophrenia itself may be problematic, in that we are not able to make comparisons with other DSM-IV diagnoses such as bipolar disorder, which would give a ‘broader perspective’. Furthermore, restricting the sample to those meeting DSM-IV schizophrenia criteria excludes cases with psychosis demonstrating the most clinical variation; these may be important to cultural understanding of psychosis (Kleinman, 1988).

Finally, because the samples were collected for specific genetic studies, there are limitations to the assumptions and generalizations we can make regarding difficult-to-quantify culturally-sensitive concepts such as length of prodrome, notwithstanding our extensive cross-cultural equivalence work and use of carefully ascertained ethnicity as our distinguishing factor between sites. In order to validate and interpret our finding of significantly shorter prodrome in Sarawak, further qualitative in-depth interviews specifically addressing cultural interpretations of illness progression and associated concepts across sites should be undertaken. This follow-up research, while worthwhile, was beyond the scope of this study.

Conclusions

We have observed significant differences in the frequency of symptoms of schizophrenia across three ethnically different populations. A comprehensive search for clinical subtypes using ethnically distinct populations is warranted, as this may contribute to our understanding of between-group clinical variation, which may not only benefit future genetic studies, but also inform the ongoing nosological debate regarding schizophrenia.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (grant numbers 339454, 143027, 9937625, 496698) and the United States National Institute of Mental Health (grant number RO1 MH59588).

We thank participants and their families. We acknowledge the contributions of: Deborah Nertney; Dr Saha; Edward Jerah; Dr Padmavati and staff, SCARF, India; Sarawak Department of Health; University of Malaysia, Sarawak; hospital and clinic staff, Kuching and Sri Aman; Queensland Health; and the MGS Consortium.

Footnotes

Declaration of Interest No conflicts declared.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (APA) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4. American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  2. Azuonye IO. Ethnicity in epidemiological research. Ethnicity revolves around culture. BMJ (Clinical research ed) 1994;309:959. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barrett R, Loa P, Jerah E, Nancarrow D, Chant D, Mowry B. Rates of treated schizophrenia and its clinical and cultural features in the population isolate of the Iban of Sarawak: a tri-diagnostic approach. Psychol Med. 2005;35:281–293. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704002880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barrett RJ. Kurt Schneider in Borneo: Do first rank symptoms apply to the Iban? In: Jenkins JH, Barrett RJ, editors. Schizophrenia, Culture, and Subjectivity. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 2004. pp. 87–109. [Google Scholar]
  5. Beckmann H, Franzek E, Stober G. Genetic heterogeneity in catatonic schizophrenia: a family study. Am J Med Genet. 1996;67:289–300. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19960531)67:3<289::AID-AJMG5>3.0.CO;2-I. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, Cadenhead KS, et al. The Consortium on the Genetics of Endophenotypes in Schizophrenia: model recruitment, assessment, and endophenotyping methods for a multisite collaboration. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33:33–48. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Craddock N, O'Donovan MC, Owen MJ. Psychosis genetics: modeling the relationship between schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and mixed (or “schizoaffective”) psychoses. Schizophr Bull. 2009;35:482–490. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Egede LE. Race, ethnicity, culture, and disparities in health care. J Gen Intern Med. 2006;21:667–669. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.0512.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Fiedorowicz JG, Epping EA, Flaum M. Toward defining schizophrenia as a more useful clinical concept. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2008;10:344–351. doi: 10.1007/s11920-008-0055-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gershon ES, DeLisi LE, Hamovit J, et al. A controlled family study of chronic psychoses. Schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45:328–336. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800280038006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Herdman M, Fox-Rushby J, Badia X. A model of equivalence in the cultural adaptation of HRQoL instruments: the universalist approach. Qual Life Res. 1998;7:323–335. doi: 10.1023/a:1024985930536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. International Schizophrenia Consortium (ISC) Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature. 2009;460:748–752. doi: 10.1038/nature08185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Kendler KS, Glazer WM, Morgenstern H. Dimensions of delusional experience. Am J Psychiatry. 1983;140:466–469. doi: 10.1176/ajp.140.4.466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Walsh D. The structure of psychosis: latent class analysis of probands from the Roscommon Family Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:492–499. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.6.492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Kleinman A. Rethinking Psychiatry: From cultural category to personal experience. The Free Press; New York: 1988. [Google Scholar]
  16. Leckman JF, Sholomskas D, Thompson WD, Belanger A, Weissman MM. Best estimate of lifetime psychiatric diagnosis: a methodological study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1982;39:879–883. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1982.04290080001001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Lichtenstein P, Yip BH, Bjork C, et al. Common genetic determinants of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in Swedish families: a population-based study. Lancet. 2009;373:234–239. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60072-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Linscott RJ, Lenzenweger MF, van Os J. Continua or classes? Vexed questions on the latent structure of schizophrenia. In: Gattaz WF, Busatto G, editors. Advances in Schizophrenia Research. Springer Science + Business Media; New York: 2009. pp. 333–355. [Google Scholar]
  19. Ma IW, Khan NA, Kang A, Zalunardo N, Palepu A. Systematic review identified suboptimal reporting and use of race/ethnicity in general medical journals. J Clin Epidemiol. 2007;60:572–578. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2006.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Maxwell ME. Clinical Neurogenetics Branch, Intramural Research Program. NIMH; Bethesda, MD: 1992. Family Interview for Genetic Studies (FIGS): a manual for FIGS. [Google Scholar]
  21. McKenzie K, Crowcroft NS. Describing race, ethnicity, and culture in medical research. BMJ (Clinical research ed) 1996a;312:1054. doi: 10.1136/bmj.312.7038.1054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. McKenzie K, Crowcroft NS. Ethnicity, race, and culture: guidelines for research, audit, and publication. BMJ (Clinical research ed) 1996b;312:1094. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. McLean D, John S, Barrett R, et al. Refining clinical phenotypes by contrasting ethnically different populations with schizophrenia from Australia, India and Sarawak. Psychiatry Res. 2012;196:194–200. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Nurnberger JI, Jr, Blehar MC, Kaufmann CA, et al. Diagnostic interview for genetic studies. Rationale, unique features, and training. NIMH Genetics Initiative. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51:849–859. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950110009002. discussion 863-864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. SAS Institute Inc. Cary, NC: [Google Scholar]
  26. Schultz SK, Miller DD, Oliver SE, Arndt S, Flaum M, Andreasen NC. The life course of schizophrenia: age and symptom dimensions. Schizophr Res. 1997;23:15–23. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(96)00087-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Shi J, Levinson DF, Duan J, et al. Common variants on chromosome 6p22.1 are associated with schizophrenia. Nature. 2009;460:753–757. doi: 10.1038/nature08192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Suarez BK, Duan J, Sanders AR, et al. Genomewide linkage scan of 409 European-ancestry and African American families with schizophrenia: suggestive evidence of linkage at 8p23.3-p21.2 and 11p13.1-q14.1 in the combined sample. Am J Hum Genet. 2006;78:315–333. doi: 10.1086/500272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Thara R, Srinivasan T, John S, et al. Design and clinical characteristics of a homogeneous schizophrenia pedigree sample from Tamil Nadu, India. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2009;43:561–570. doi: 10.1080/00048670902873631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. World Health Organization (WHO) International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems: Tenth Revision. WHO; Geneva: 1992. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES